Abstract
With the eye as a window to the brain, non-invasive fast screening of retinal nerve fibre layer thickness poses the opportunity for early detection of cognitive decline leading to dementia. Our objective is to determine whether performance in various neurocognitive tests has an association with itemized retinal nerve fibre layer thickness. Detailed investigation of associations factored in sex and eye-side. The large population-based LIFE-Adult study (Leipzig Research Centre for Civilization Diseases) was conducted at Leipzig University, Germany from 2011 to 2014. Randomly selected participants (N = 10 000) were drawn from population registry in an age- and gender-stratified manner, focusing on 40–80 years. Cognitive function was examined with the CERAD-NP Plus test-battery (Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease), Stroop-Test, Reading the Mind in the Eyes-Test and Multiple-Choice Vocabulary Intelligence Test. Circumpapillary retinal nerve fibre layer thickness was measured with Optical Coherence Tomography. Subjects with reliable measurements (≥50 B-scan repetitions, signal-to-noise-ratio ≥20 dB, ≤5% missing A-scans) and without clinical eye pathology (sample A) and additional exclusion due to conditions of the central nervous system (sample B) were evaluated. The relationship between cognitive function and retinal nerve fibre layer thickness was investigated for six segments: temporal, temporal-superior, temporal-inferior, nasal, nasal-superior and nasal-inferior. For comparison with other studies, global mean is given. Brain-side projection analysis links results to the corresponding brain hemisphere. We analysed 11 124 eyes of 6471 subjects [55.5 years of age (19.1–79.8 years), 46.9% male]. Low cognitive performance was predominantly associated with thinner retinal nerve fibre layer thickness. Correlation analysis indicated emphasis on global and temporally located effects. Multivariable regression analysis with adjustments (age, sex and scan radius) presented individual results for each test, differentiating between sex and eye-side. For instance, verbal fluency tests and Trail Making Test-B show stronger association in females; Trail Making Test-A shows right-eye dominance. Findings in Trail-Making-Test-A projected to left brain hemisphere, and the ratio incongruent to neutral in the Stroop test projected to right brain-hemisphere. Separate assessment for sex and eye-side is presented for the first time in a population-based study. Location-specific sectorial retinal nerve fibre layer thickness was found to be an indicator for cognitive performance, giving an option for early detection of cognitive decline and the potential of early treatment. The eye as a window to the brain was studied with optical coherence tomography and connected to cognition. Girbardt et al. report that thinner retinal nerve fibre layer thickness was found to be a meaningful index for poorer cognitive performance which presents the potential for prediction of future cognitive decline.
Keywords: retinal nerve fibre layer, cognition, dementia, the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD), population-based
The eye as window to the brain was studied with optical coherence tomography and connected to cognition. Girbardt et al. report that thinner retinal nerve fiber layer thickness was found to be a meaningful index for poorer cognitive performance which presents the potential for prediction of future cognitive decline.
Graphical Abstract
Introduction
To measure cognitive abilities, extensive tests and test batteries have been developed. Modern imaging techniques are used in cognitive neuroscience to examine the neuroanatomical and functional correlates of cognitive abilities such as attention, language or memory.1,2 In the eye, as ‘window to the brain’, it is possible to examine early degeneration of the central nervous system fast and non-invasively at micrometre resolution using optical coherence tomography (OCT). As all retinal nerve fibres throughout the eye project to the optic nerve head, the circumpapillary area is the most informative region to detect nerve fibre damage in the eye. Retinal nerve fibre layer thickness (RNFLT) is an important measure to diagnosing optic neuropathies like glaucoma and has been shown to decrease with age in healthy individuals.3–6 Furthermore, there is evidence that RNFLT decreases with poorer cognitive abilities.7,8 Over 46 million people around the world live with dementia.9 As therapeutic options are very limited so far, there is a need to screen for the disease in an early stage to be able to develop new forms of treatment. An inexpensive and non-invasive marker like RNFLT which also serves other diagnostic purposes would be most suitable.
Previous population-based studies provide coarse results on retinal layers and cognition. Investigation of ‘macular’ ganglion cell inner plexiform layer in the large-scale ‘Rotterdam study’ (n = 3289) showed association with prevalent dementia.10 Less macular ganglion cell layer volume was associated with worse processing speed and worse verbal episodic memory in the ‘Rhineland study’ (n = 2483).11
Investigation of circumpapillary retinal nerve fibre layer thickness (cpRNFLT) in the Rotterdam study revealed association of thinner global values with low performance in several cognitive tests, this was supported by a trend in the Rhineland study.10,11
The ‘Three-City-Alienor study’ (n = 483) did not observe a significant association between cognitive performance and global RNFLT in baseline measurement, but sectorial analysis in follow-up showed a significant association of decreased RNFLT with memory decline (temporal, temporal-superior, temporal-inferior, nasal).12 The inferior area of RNFL was significantly related to general processing speed in the ‘Lothian Birth Cohort 1936’ (n = 96) in subjects aged 72 years.13 These two studies motivated this study, in which we investigated sectorial RNFLT in a much larger sample.
Other population-based studies without OCT technology have estimated RNFLT with scanning laser polarimetry and in vivo confocal microscopy (Heidelberg retinal tomography, HRT). ‘Erasmus Rucphen Family study’ (n = 1485) and ‘EPIC Norfolk Cohort study’ (n = 5563) found a positive association of global RNFLT with cognitive performance in several tests.14,15 An association between thinner global RNFLT and self-reported Alzheimer’s disease was found in the ‘Northern Ireland Cohort for the Longitudinal Study of Ageing’ (NICOLA) (n = 3221).16
Analyses on averaged macular RNFLT with the ‘UK Biobank data’ (n = 32 038) indicate ‘sector-specific’ rather than global relations when comparing with circumpapillary data. As macular nerve fibres project towards the optic nerve head via the inferior-temporal and temporal segments, these two segments can be compared to our data.17 These results motivated our study to specifically analyse not only the inferior-temporal and temporal segment but also ‘all’ segments around the optic nerve head.
Here, we aim to investigate RNFLT and cognitive performance in a large population-based sample by presenting age-, sex-, education- and scan radius-adjusted analyses separately for male and female subjects. The latter is important due to sex-dependent differences in RNFLT18 and cognition.19 While most previous studies summarized RNFLT globally or at most divided into coarse segments, we analysed RNFLT at greater precision of ‘six segments’. Furthermore, we consider effects of ocular laterality motivated by projections into different brain hemispheres for the first time in a population-based study.
We hypothesize that RNFLT can provide diagnostic information on cognitive decline. We specifically hypothesize thinner RNFLT to be associated with lower cognitive performance in a sector-specific manner. Additionally to previous studies focusing especially on Alzheimer’s disease (AD), we consider the entire spectrum of cognitive decline and focus on different neuropsychological tests in particular. Unlike case–control studies, our population-based approach is ideal to obtain a broad and sizable sample of individuals and to adjust for potential confounders.
Materials and methods
Study population
Participants were investigated in the framework of a large population-based cohort study conducted by the Leipzig Research Centre for Civilization Diseases (LIFE-Adult Study) at Leipzig University, from August 2011 to November 2014. LIFE-Adult includes 10 000 randomly selected participants from the population registry of over half a million inhabitants of Leipzig, Germany. Subjects were recruited in an age and gender stratified manner, focusing on the age range 40–80 years. More specifically, 9600 subjects between 40 and <80 years of age and 400 subjects between 19 and <40 years of age were recruited. All participants provided written informed consent prior to participation. The institutional ethics board of the Medical Faculty of Leipzig University approved the study and the research followed the Declaration of Helsinki.
Of the 10 000 LIFE subjects, 9069 had cpRNFLT available. Of those, 271 subjects were excluded from the analysis sample because of unreliable OCT scanning quality. Another 2327 subjects were excluded due to relevant ophthalmic findings classified either based on current ophthalmological standards and/or medical history (i.e. optic nerve disorders including glaucoma, retinal disorders including age-related macular degeneration (AMD), inflammatory disease, substantial visual impairment, severe ocular trauma, orbital disease and tumours of the eye). See also Fig. 1 and Supplementary material 1 ‘exclusion criteria’ for more details.
We present two analyses. In the first sample A, we included all subjects based on exclusion criteria described above (11 124 eyes of 6471 subjects). Considering that several neurological or psychiatric conditions are known to affect brain function, and potentially RNFLT, we additionally excluded the following conditions: tumour, stroke, Parkinson’s disease, multiple sclerosis, epilepsy, depression and centrally acting drugs. This data set is labelled sample B, including 9226 eyes of 5347 subjects.
The final numbers of included subjects per cognitive assessment are depicted in Table 1 ‘characteristics of the study population’. The number of subjects varies between tests because some cognitive tests were performed on subsets of the LIFE participants.
Table 1.
Eyes, no. | Subjects, no. | Age, mean (2.5%, 97.5%) | Male, no. (%) | Right eyes, no. (%) | Test score (2.5%, 97.5%) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sample A
| |||||||
| |||||||
Entire sample Aa | 11 124 | 6471 | 55.5 (28.6, 76.3) | 46.9 | 50.3 | NA | |
Boston Naming Testa | 2169 | 1323 | 69.1 (60.7, 78.3) | 52.3 | 50.3 | 14.5 (12, 15) | |
MMSEa | 2160 | 1318 | 69.1 (60.7, 78.3) | 52.5 | 50.2 | 28.2 (24, 30) | |
MWT-Ba | 10 996 | 6392 | 55.4 (28.5, 76.3) | 46.9 | 50.3 | 104.8 (72, 129) | |
Phonemic fluency ‘S’a | 2171 | 1324 | 69.1 (60.7, 78.3) | 52.3 | 50.3 | 12.8 (5, 22) | |
RMETa | 2362 | 1378 | 56.6 (23.4, 77.1) | 55.3 | 50.4 | 23.1 (15, 30) | |
Semantic fluencya | 11 091 | 6450 | 55.5 (28.6, 76.3) | 46.9 | 50.3 | 24.2 (13.0, 38.0) | |
Stroop neutralb | 10 667 | 6196 | 55.3 (28.5, 76.3) | 46.7 | 50.3 | 1669 (1065, 2704) | |
Stroop inc.b | 10 667 | 6196 | 55.3 (28.5, 76.3) | 46.7 | 50.3 | 1975 (1137, 3341) | |
Stroop inc./neut. | 10 671 | 6198 | 55.3 (28.5, 76.3) | 46.7 | 50.3 | 1.2 (0.9, 1.5) | |
TMT-Ab | 11 093 | 6453 | 55.5 (28.6, 76.3) | 46.9 | 50.3 | 35.2 (17, 70) | |
TMT-Bb | 11 024 | 6413 | 55.4 (28.5, 76.3) | 46.8 | 50.3 | 83.7 (38, 209) | |
TMT B/A | 11 016 | 6408 | 55.4 (28.5, 76.3) | 46.8 | 50.3 | 2.4 (1.3, 4.8) | |
Visuoconstruction copya | 2161 | 1318 | 69.1 (60.7, 78.3) | 52.4 | 50.3 | 10.7 (8, 11) | |
Visuoconstruction recalla | 2142 | 1306 | 69.1 (60.7, 78.3) | 52.5 | 50.4 | 10.8 (5, 14) | |
Wordlist learninga | 3369 | 1993 | 59.6 (24.1, 77.1) | 52.2 | 50.3 | 22.6 (14, 29) | |
Wordlist recalla | 3369 | 1993 | 59.6 (24.1, 77.7) | 52.2 | 50.3 | 8.0 (4, 10) | |
Wordlist recognitiona | 3357 | 1985 | 59.6 (24.2, 77.7) | 52.1 | 50.4 | 19.7 (18, 20) | |
| |||||||
Sample B | |||||||
Entire sample B | 9226 | 5347 | 54.8 (27.7, 76.0) | 49 | 50.2 | NA | |
Boston Naming Testa | 1679 | 1020 | 68.9 (60.6, 78.3) | 55.2 | 50.7 | 14.5 (12, 15) | |
MMSEa | 1672 | 1016 | 69.0 (60.7, 78.3) | 55.4 | 50.7 | 28.3 (25, 30) | |
MWT-Ba | 9126 | 5285 | 54.7 (27.7, 75.9) | 49.0 | 50.2 | 105.4 (75, 129) | |
Phonemic fluency ‘S’a | 1681 | 1021 | 68.9 (60.6, 78.3) | 55.1 | 50.7 | 12.9 (5, 22) | |
RMETa | 1922 | 1119 | 55.5 (23.3, 76.9) | 56.8 | 50.7 | 23.2 (15, 30) | |
Semantic fluencya | 9199 | 5330 | 54.8 (27.7, 76.0) | 49.0 | 50.2 | 24.4 (13.0, 38) | |
Stroop neutralb | 8863 | 5129 | 54.6 (27.7, 75.9) | 48.9 | 50.3 | 1649 (1062, 2651) | |
Stroop inc.b | 8861 | 5128 | 54.6 (27.7, 75.9) | 48.9 | 50.3 | 1954 (1136, 3305) | |
Stroop inc./neut. | 8863 | 5129 | 54.6 (27.7, 75.9) | 48.9 | 50.3 | 1.2 (0.9, 1.5) | |
TMT-Ab | 9200 | 5332 | 54.8 (27.7, 76.0) | 49.0 | 50.2 | 34.2 (17, 67) | |
TMT-Bb | 9155 | 5306 | 54.7 (27.7, 76.0) | 49.0 | 50.2 | 81.1 (38, 198) | |
TMT B/A | 9147 | 5301 | 54.7 (27.7, 76.0) | 49.0 | 50.2 | 2.4 (1.3, 4.7) | |
Visuoconstruction copya | 1673 | 1016 | 69.0 (60.7, 78.3) | 55.3 | 50.7 | 10.7 (8, 11) | |
Visuoconstruction recalla | 1661 | 1009 | 68.9 (60.7, 78.3) | 55.5 | 50.8 | 10.9 (5, 14) | |
Wordlist learninga | 2719 | 1604 | 58.6 (23.8, 77.5) | 54.2 | 50.6 | 22.8 (15, 29) | |
Wordlist recalla | 2719 | 1604 | 58.6 (23.8, 77.5) | 54.2 | 50.6 | 8.1 (4, 10) | |
Wordlist recognitiona | 2707 | 1596 | 58.6 (24.0, 77.6) | 54.1 | 50.6 | 19.7 (18, 20) |
inc. = incongruent; neut. = neutral; MMSE = Mini-Mental state examination; NA = not applicable; RMET = Reading the Mind in the Eyes test; TMT = Trail Making Test.
Score in points.
Time in seconds.
Ophthalmologic assessments
Ophthalmological imaging included spectral-domain OCT (Spectralis, Heidelberg Engineering, Heidelberg, Germany, Spectralis HRA + OCT, Acquisition Module 5.4.7.0). OCT was performed to obtain cpRNFLT scans with a resolution of 768 equidistant measurement points placed on a circle around the optic nerve head. The circle location and its coordinate system are illustrated by Wang et al. RNFLT are evaluated in six segments (T: temporal, TS: temporal-superior, TI: temporal-inferior, N: nasal, NS: nasal-superior, NI: nasal-inferior) and overall average (G). These sectorial data are analysed in comparison with cognitive assessments for the current analysis.
We included both eyes per subject wherever possible. We have previously described for the same dataset in a high level of detail the similarities and differences between right and left eyes within the same person.20 Given the existing interocular symmetries on subject-specific level, a mixed linear regression model with random effects for subjects was considered but proved to be too complex given the large number of different subjects as a ratio of the total number of samples (each subject had at most two measurement samples: right and left eye). Being aware of this issue, we performed separate data analyses for right and left eyes to explicitly investigate potential ocular laterality effects on RNFLT. Reliability criteria for the OCT scan were based on: (i) image quality with signal-to-noise-ratio ≥20 dB; (ii) average number of B-scans ≥50; and (iii) no more than 5% missing or unreliable cpRNFLT segmentations among the 768 A-scans which are the basis of the average sectorial data. All evaluated scans were adequately centred on the optic nerve head.
Previous studies have demonstrated strong relationships between RNFLT and age21 and sex.18 In addition, it has been shown that ocular magnification effects, due to corneal curvature, lens-related myopia or axial length, have a substantial impact on the true size of the OCT scanning circle.21 The Spectralis machine estimates the true scan radius from the individual focus settings of each scan. We used this estimated true scan radius as an additional covariate.
Neuropsychological Assessment
A detailed description of the performance and intention concerning the following cognitive assessments are described in Supplementary material 2 ‘neuropsychological assessment’. Note that the individual cognitive phenotyping included various cognitive dimensions, including memory, attention, executive and language functions, social cognition and others. We used the authorized German version of the CERAD Plus test battery (The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease), an established battery for phenotyping AD and related disorders, which included the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE), Boston Naming Test (BNT), Phonemic fluency, Semantic fluency animals, Trail Making Tests (TMT), Visuoconstruction copy, Visuoconstruction delayed recall, Wordlist learning, Wordlist delayed recall and Wordlist recognition.22,23 Further tests were the Reading the Mind in the Eyes test (RMET),24 Multiple choice German vocabulary test (MWT-B)25 and the Stroop-Test.26,27 Education is known to have an influence on cognitive testing,28,29 accordingly, we included education as a covariate in our analyses.
Statistical analysis
We analysed associations between RNFLT, separately for each of the six segments as well as globally, and performance in the several cognitive tests in two different ways: First, by separate Pearson’s correlations with each cognitive score, and second, by multivariable linear regression with RNFLT as outcome and each cognitive test score as well as age, the estimated true scan radius, sex and education as regressors: RNFLT = β0 + β1 (cognitive test) + β2 (age)+ β3 (sex) + β4 (scan radius)+ β5 (education). We investigated males and females separately in additional sub-analyses. Moreover, the data analyses were performed separately for right and left eyes to investigate possible relationships with brain laterality, as explained below. Significant relationships (P < 0.05) were determined after adjusting the sectorial and global results for multiple comparisons by false discovery rate. All statistical analyses were performed in R environment using version 3.6 (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria).
Brain hemisphere-specific correlation by evaluating nasal, nasal superior and temporal cpRNFL
Retinal nerve fibres originating from the macular bundle proceed to the temporal peripapillary segment. These and the peripapillary nasal and nasal superior located fibres decussate to the contralateral brain hemisphere. Therefore, we correlated the measurements of these three segments to one brain hemisphere. Regarding the remaining peripapillary segments, direct relationship with one brain hemisphere is not possible because in these segments, axons from the superior and inferior nasal ganglion cells as well as the arcuating temporal axons are intermingled, see Fig. 2 for details.
Data availability
The authors confirm that the data supporting the findings of this study are available within the article and its Supplementary materials. All presented data are available as Supplementary information for download. Raw data cannot be shared publicly because of consent restrictions of LIFE-Adult participants. Data are available after an approved project agreement from the LIFE Leipzig Research Centre for Civilization Diseases. Please contact Dr Matthias Nüchter (Head of LIFE Managing Office, contact via matthias.nuechter[at]life.uni-leipzig.de) for data access requests.
Results
Demographic characteristics
The final analysis included 11 124 eyes from 6471 participants (mean age 55.5 years, range 19.1–79.8 years, 46.86% male). Table 1 shows the number of subjects per test and demographic characteristics. The study population is almost exclusively of European descent (0.86% in sample A and 1.2% in sample B were non-Europeans). Education was classified on the basis of the CASMIN-scale (Comparative Analysis of Social Mobility in Industrial Nations)31: 7.24% in sample A and 6.47% in sample B had lower education, 58.64% in A and 58.25% in B had intermediate education and 34.12% in A and 35.28% in B had high education.
Age was found to be inversely related to performance in the following cognitive measures: MMSE, Wordlist learning, recall, recognition, RMET, Semantic fluency, TMT, Stroop neutral-, and -incongruent. Mean RNFLT was 97.3 µm (SD 9.3 µm) and 97.4 µm (SD 9.3 µm) for sample A and sample B; see Supplementary material 3 ‘Results’ for details.
Association between RNFLT and performance in cognitive tests
Figure 3 presents RNFLT in six segments (T: temporal; TS: temporal-superior; TI: temporal-inferior; N: nasal; NS: nasal-superior; NI: nasal-inferior, G: global, overall average) and its statistically significant associations with cognitive tests. Numerical results are shown in Supplementary material 3 ‘Results’.
In the majority of the implemented tests, better performance was associated with thicker RNFLT, while lower scores were related to thinner RNFLT, respectively. MWT-B and Figure Recall show associations of better test performance with thinner RNFL in predominantly nasally located segments. RNFLT was not associated with performance in BNT and Visuoconstruction copy.
Sample A and B present similar results in all six analyses. In sample B, additional significant segments were found in Phonemic fluency (slope TS), Semantic fluency (females TI), TMT-A (females T), TMT B/A (males G) Stroop incongruent (right eye G). Correlation analysis in both samples revealed predominant associations globally and in temporally located segments.
After multivariable regression analysis with adjustment for age, sex, scan radius and education, five tests still show statistically significant positive associations globally in both samples. In this analysis, the previously predominant associations in temporally located segments are considerably reduced. The maximum of segments with statistically significant association is found for TMT-A in both samples (G, T, TS, TI, N and NS).
Sex-specific analyses
Separate analyses for females and males, see Fig. 3, demonstrate that, in both samples, some associations occur predominantly in one sex and other associations are distributed asymmetrically. Female-only associations (positive) were found in semantic fluency, MMSE, TMT-B and phonemic fluency, mainly concerning sectors G and TS. In men, associations (positive) were solely found globally in Stroop neutral and incongruent condition and ratio TMT B/A.
Eye laterality
Five cognitive tests investigated have asymmetrical results between right and left eyes, see Fig. 3. For the right eye, associated segments of samples A and B are identical.
Noticeably, in sample B, except for TMT-A, all statistically significant associations concern the right eye.
Brain hemisphere-specific analysis
In our separate analysis of unilateral associations of segments N, NS and T, where fibres mainly cross to the contralateral brain hemisphere (see Fig. 2), when adjusting for sex, age and scan radius only, Semantic fluency and TMT-A in both samples, and TMT-B in sample A were positively associated with RNFLT of the right eye, projecting to the left hemisphere. Ratio Stroop incongruent/neutral was negatively associated with the left eye in sample A. After additional adjustment for education, the mentioned associations of TMT-A and ratio Stroop incongruent/neutral were still present. MWT-B in sample A had positive temporal association in both eyes with RNFLT but additional negative association in N and NS for the left eye.
We found associations of better cognitive test performance with both thicker RNFLT in some and thinner RNFLT in other segments only for the MWT-B, where thickening was located temporally and thinning in segments N and NS.
Interestingly, the segment NI shows the least effects apart from MWT-B in both samples.
Additionally explained variance by cognitive test scores (effect size). As detailed elsewhere in this work, age, scan radius and sex have previously been shown to have substantial impacts on RNFLT and, therefore, explain considerable amounts of RNFLT variance. Furthermore, education as a parameter is known to be correlated with the scores of many cognitive tests. Associations between RNFLT and cognitive test scores become particularly relevant if the test score explains any additional variance which is not already explained by these available covariates. To quantify this additionally explained variance, we first cumulatively calculated the entire variance explained by the four parameters age, scan radius, sex and education and then determined the remaining explained variance, which can then solely be attributed to the test score. Figures 4 and 5 show the ratios of variance explained by each respective cognitive test score (red) that is not explained by the covariates (blue) age, radius, sex and education relative to the total explained variance for sample A (Fig. 4) and sample B (Fig. 5). In other words, these figures illustrate how much additional variance each cognitive test explains for each sector and each covariate.
Discussion
In this large population-based cohort study, we found significant statistical association of poor cognitive performance in several neuropsychological tests predominantly with thinner cpRNFLT. Mean RNFLT (see Supplementary material 3 ‘Results’) is in line with normative data of European and Asian populations.32,33 We previously found an annual decrease of RNFLT of 0.17 µm when adjusting for scan radius.21 We further demonstrated both sex-specific differences18 as well as differences in laterality20 of RNFLT. Comparable to other data, cognitive performance for most tests was lower with higher age.34
As detailed in the Results and further discussed below, in this study, we deliberately analysed associations between RNFLT and cognitive performance at an unprecedented level of detail, which resulted in a large number of different conditions together with their respective subgroups and thereby in a multitude of statistical comparisons. Some of the effects robustly appear under multiple conditions, such as the association of RNFLT with attention (TMT) or the focus on retinal locations in superior sectors. Other findings are restricted to only single conditions or subgroups and are therefore more difficult to interpret. Nevertheless, these effects survived our adjustments for multiple comparisons, which is why we report them here as well. These seemingly isolated effects, many of which could not have been found in previous studies as the respective combination of conditions had not been investigated before, may inspire specifically designed future studies which could help to better understand and interpret them and reveal potential underlying conditions for them.
Effects, which occur only in sample B, indicate that our decision to exclude possible brain-related confounders was justified as it highlights associations with RNFLT despite the smaller sample size.
In many neuropsychological tests, we found effects in temporal segments (T, TS and TI), which were considerably reduced when adjusting for age, sex, scan radius and education. Similarly, Feuer et al.35 found the biggest thinning per decade in segments NS, TS, N, TI for their combined cohorts and Parikh et al.36 found the main thinning per decade in segments NS, TS, TI, adapted from publication of 12 segments. As we previously demonstrated, thinning of RNFLT is underestimated in higher ages when not adjusting for scan radius.21
We found remarkable sex differences. Female eyes exclusively presented effects in MMSE, Fluency tasks and TMT-B in the dominant segments G, TS and TI. Only in men, Stroop neutral and incongruent had global effects. As we recently published regarding the LIFE cohort, global RNFLT is 1 µm larger in females with location-specific differences up to 9.98 µm nasally. All over, we found areas with thicker RNFLT in females within the segments T, TS, TI, N and NI and within segments ST and NS for males.18 Analogously, in the Gobi Desert Children Eye study, girls had thicker RNFL than boys in segments T, TI, N, NS.37 In addition, females of the LIFE cohort performed better than men in many cognitive tests besides BNT and Constructional praxis.19 Differences between females and males as observed in our study might be related to these factors, that is, sex-specific differences in RNFLT and cognition.18,37 Accordingly, separate analysis of female and male eyes is generally advisable in future studies, matching a recent focus to consider sex-specific analyses in neuroscience in general.38–41 Previous population studies included sex as covariate but did not publish analyses for single tests stratified by sex.
Concerning eye laterality, we found effects in sample B almost only for the right eye. In TMT-A, for the right eye in both samples, we found significant effects in 6 of 7 segments whereas in the left eye only the global mean is significant. Global RNFLT is slightly thicker in right eyes. As we previously showed, the thickness profile of cpRNFL is location-specific with thicker areas for right eyes in segment N and thicker areas for left eyes in segment NS.20 To our knowledge, the impact of cognition on RNFLT with differentiation of eye laterality has not been demonstrated in a population-based study before.
Most fibres of the segments N, NS and T cross to the contralateral brain hemisphere. Analogously, in patients with posterior cerebral artery infarction, the lesions in RNFL due to transsynaptic retrograde degeneration were least in segments N and NS of ipsilateral eyes while contralateral thinning was found in all segments.42 This indicates that performance of TMT-A might correlate particularly with changes in the left brain hemisphere. This finding is in line with results from lesion studies which provide evidence for association of TMT with several locations within the left frontal and parietal lobe.43,44 TMT-B correlated only with RNFLT when not adjusting for education. The ratio Stroop incongruent/neutral correlated in segment NS of the left eye from where fibres are connected with the right brain hemisphere. Although this finding in a verbal interference task might contradict the fMRI and brain lesion model literature at first glance,45 several studies reported also bilateral brain activations46 strengthened by a recent lesion model.47 The Stroop task is related to bilateral frontal region and the core network for tasks requiring supervisory control for the suppression of a routine action in favour of another, non-routine action was even related specifically to the right anterior insula and inferior frontal junctions.48
The MWT-B which estimates crystallized intelligence is the only test where better results were associated with thicker RNFLT in temporal segments and simultaneously with thinner RNFLT in nasal segments. Accordingly, global RNFL showed no association in 10 of 12 subanalyses because the effects were lost due to averaging. For this reason, it is important to analyse detailed segments. For Visuoconstruction recall, we also found opposite effects in correlation of better test results with nasal thinning. An explanation for these needs to be investigated in further studies. Few case-control studies found an augmentation of RNFLT in patients with mild cognitive impairment compared to healthy adults and discussed this as a result of gliosis which precedes axonal degeneration and tissue thinning.49,50
Even though the ratios of variance explained in addition to the covariates (Figures 4 and 5) show that the global RNFLT is in many cases almost fully explained by age, at many of the single separate RNFLT segments, more variance is explained by the cognitive test versus age. This underlines the benefit of analysing cpRNFLT at a higher spatial resolution compared to the global average. Another noteworthy conclusion from these figures is the large additional variance explanation of the cognitive tests compared to education.
Several previous population-based studies have investigated the impact of cognition on RNFLT, focusing on different aspects. Five studies present data for global RNFLT alone.10,11,13–15 The Lothian Birth cohort13 gives results for four segments. Only the Three-City-Alienor study12 provides a spatial resolution of six segments as our study, however, for much less participants and fewer cognitive tests. The UK Biobank17 analyses macular RNFL which can be matched to our segments T, TI, in the Erasmus Rucphen study,14 scanning laser polarimetry is used and in the Epic Norfolk Cohort,15 RNFL is derived with HRT. Those are different technologies and their results might not be easily comparable to the OCT-derived data of the other studies.
‘TMT’ as common assessment for attention, cognitive speed (TMT-A), and flexibility (TMT-B) is frequently impaired in brain disorders and, therefore, our outstanding associations of this test with RNFLT give meaningful evidence for OCT-investigation for cognitive screening. Additionally to cognitive challenge, TMT is also visually demanding.
For ‘TMT-A’, other population-based studies also found an association of thinner RNFLT with worse results for cognitive performance: Rotterdam Study10 globally using letter-digit substitution test, UK-Biobank17 for segments T, TI with timed test of symbol matching (macular RNFLT). In the Epic Norfolk study,51 the related letter-cancellation test was associated with RNFLT measured with HRT but only when not fully adjusting; in the Lothian Birth Cohort,13 general processing speed was associated with the inferior quadrant, and in the Rhineland Study,11 a statistically non-significant trend in TMT-A was found.
Concerning ‘TMT-B’, in the Erasmus Rucphen Family study,14 better performance was associated with thicker global RNFLT, as in our study. Astonishingly, this concerned predominantly younger individuals in their study. The Rotterdam Study10 showed association of RNFLT with letter-digit substitution test and in the Rhineland Study,11 a positive trend for TMT-B was found.
‘MMSE’, as general cognitive screening test, had only effects in global RNFLT in our analysis of females in sample A. The test has a ceiling effect as the majority of our participants had no dementia. In the Rotterdam Study,10 a positive association was only found for baseline measurements and the Epic Norfolk Study15 had a positive association of HRT-derived RNFLT with the short-form MMSE. In the Lothian Birth Cohort,13 general cognitive ability was negatively associated with RNFLT, however, this was only the case when including former IQ at age 11 as covariate. They discussed the relatively good cognitive health of their cohort as possible reason for this converse result. However, our population sample with a mean MMSE of 28 (95% CI: 24–30) was also in good cognitive health.
As verbal fluency can be impaired very early in AD,52 these test results, relating to several processes for example language and flexibility are of particular interest. We found global association for ‘semantic fluency’53 in both samples, analogous to the Erasmus Rucphen Family Study.14 Rotterdam Study,10 Three-City-Alienor Study12 and Epic Norfolk Study15 found no association of semantic fluency with RNFLT. In our analysis, ‘phonemic fluency’ had effects on RNFLT especially in females; other population-based studies did not present comparable tests.
As AD is the most prevalent type of dementia, we had expected to see an effect in ‘Delayed recall’; this might not be the case due to few cognitively impaired subjects. Memory tests in our analysis were only associated in correlation- but not in regression analysis. In the Erasmus Rucphen Family Study14 and the Epic Norfolk Cohort (HRT),15 memory tests were associated with global RNFLT, in UK Biobank17 in T and TI. In the Three-City-Alienor Study,12 RNFLT was not associated with memory at baseline. However, baseline RNFL was associated with lower decline in follow-up data for Free Delayed Recall (T, TS, TI) and Free plus Cued Delayed Recall (TI, N), both testing episodic memory. The Rhineland Study11 again found a trend, in the Rotterdam Study10 and Lothian Birth Cohort13 no association was found.
The ‘Stroop’ test is regarded as a measure of effectiveness of focused attention and additionally tests executive function, requiring inhibitory/interference control.54 We found global association in both samples likewise to the Rotterdam Study10 and the Erasmus Rucphen Family Study.14 Additionally, we found segment NS to be associated with sample A.
‘MWT-B’, differing from the other used tests, is a measure of crystallized intelligence. The Rhineland Study11 performed the identical test in which better performance was non-significantly associated with thicker RNFL but in the Epic Norfolk Study,15 where the short form of the National Adult Reading Test (NART) was evaluated, the results were statistically significant.
For the ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes-Test’ as a measure of social cognition, we found no effects. To our knowledge, impact of this test on RNFLT was not investigated in a population-based setting before.
Summing up our results and those of other population-based studies, RNFLT might be useful as a biomarker for cognitive speed, executive functions and cognitive interference.
Choice of RNFLT as an outcome
Most likely, both RNFL thinning and cognitive decline are indicators of general, systemic decline within the nervous system. Hence, correlation analyses between cognitive parameters and cpRNFLT first provided a quantification of this association regardless of any causal relationships. Second, multivariate regression analyses were carried out with a goal to establish if cpRNFLT, while correlated, would be indeed a useful variable to be associated with cognitive performance or if cognitive parameters would not add a significant amount of explained variance in addition to those parameters which were previously identified as the strongest impact factors on cpRNFLT. In our own previous studies on cpRNFLT, we identified age and scan radius influence cpRNFLT, with a particularly strong impact of scan radius (an eye-specific parameter, with cpRNFLT as outcome variable).21 Thus, we investigated if adding cognitive parameters to this set of previously identified strong covariates of cpRNFLT would explain any additional variance. Our rationale was: If cognitive parameters were to add any explained variance in addition to these known strong covariates, a typical ophthalmic OCT scan (which should include at least age and scan radius) has the potential to be associated with cognitive performance. In other words, we aimed to mimic a typical clinical ophthalmic measurement procedure, including standard covariates, and studied if this instrument could be sensibly related to cognitive parameters. Our design, therefore, investigates necessary conditions to associate cpRNFLT with cognitive decline.
To explicitly study cognitive decline, longitudinal studies are necessary. For this, the longitudinal course of cognitive test scores should indeed be the outcome variable and the choice of the covariates should not contain eye-specific parameters (e.g. scan radius) but rather a set of previously identified impacts on cognitive performance. Our cross-sectional dataset with eye-focused analyses provides the foundations to justify such longitudinal studies with cognitive performance over time as an outcome.
Pathophysiological mechanisms of RNFLT alteration
A number of studies addressed RNFLT alteration by the influence of neurological diseases like stroke,55 multiple sclerosis56 or Parkinson’s disease.57 Causes for these might overlap with reasons for reduced RNFLT in cognitive decline: Changes of RNFL can concern all comprised components measured by OCT, for example, axons of the ganglion cells, glia cells, blood vessels.58 Damage can arise locally in the retina or originate more distantly as a consequence of axonal damage. Locally, deposits of amyloid beta plaques which are typically associated with AD, have been histologically found in the superior segment,59,60 where we also found significant effects. Damage in the optic tract can lead to thinning of RNFL as direct retrograde degeneration61 and cortical damage can even lead to transsynaptic retrograde degeneration.62–64 In a histological examination of the primary visual cortex in patients with AD, a greater density of senile plaques and neurofibrillary tangles was found in the cuneal gyrus (which corresponds to the superior retina) than in the lingual gyrus.65 This could explain the previously described inferior visual field defects in AD patients.66
As RNFLT is correlated with vessel density, it is also possible that RNFL thinning in our study is the result of vascular pathology. For instance, a study in patients with subcortical vascular cognitive impairment using OCT angiography found decreased temporal peripapillary capillary density.67
Limitations
Subjects with severe cognitive or physical limitations might not have responded to an invitation to participate in the LIFE study. This might present a selection bias towards cognitively more healthy participants, however, this concerns all population-based studies. Our subjects were predominantly of European descent and our results are not necessarily generalizable.
As it is common practice in population-based studies on this topic, we excluded participants with glaucoma, since this optic neuropathy might confound the associations studied here. Therefore, our results are not fully implementable for glaucoma patients.
Most CERAD tests had a smaller sample size than other tests, which reduces the power of our data analyses. It might be possible that this reduced power contributed to fewer significant results for these tests.
While our results are promising in indicating RNFLT as an early marker for cognitive decline, our study is restricted to cross-sectional data and lacking longitudinal data to investigate this effect over time.
Strengths
Among the strengths of our study is the large number of participants on which we performed extensive neurocognitive testing in a standardized setting. Furthermore, the high spatial resolution of six RNFL-segments enables us to present findings, which are not visible when analysing fewer sectors or the global mean alone. We clearly demonstrate the benefits of this high spatial resolution, and our results might motivate future studies with even higher sectorial resolutions as well as studies including larger retinal areas, such as a combination of peripapillary and macular volume scans at high spatial resolutions.
Moreover, to our knowledge, the impact of cognition on RNFLT with separate analysis of right and left eyes and furthermore distinguished analysis separated by sex has not been accounted for in a population-based study so far.
Conclusions
We associate cpRNFLT using a spatial resolution of six segments with a considerable number of cognitive tests based on the largest population-based sample to date. We demonstrate highly location-specific effects of RNFLT segments with lower cognitive performance in several dimensions, particularly for trail making, stroop, and vocabulary tests. As common ophthalmic OCT devices analyse cpRNFLT in sector-specific ways, our findings are easily applicable in clinical practice. To summarize, our study not only confirms the previously reported general relationship between cognitive performance and RNFLT but also reveals the specific cpRNFLT regions that are most informative for each cognitive test. This improves the high potential for ocular OCT to be used as a possible adjunct or screening tool for detecting the onset of cognitive decline or monitoring its progression.
Supplementary material
Supplementary material is available at Brain Communications online.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
The authors gratefully acknowledge the help of Dr Jens Grosche, Effigos AG, Leipzig, Germany with the visual artwork of Fig. 2. The authors also thank Dr Kerstin Wirkner and her team at the Leipzig Research Centre for Civilization Diseases (LIFE ADULT), Leipzig University, Leipzig, Germany for data acquisition. The authors acknowledge support for the Article Processing Charge by the Open Access Publication Fund of Leipzig University, Germany.
Funding
This publication is supported by Leipzig Research Centre for Civilization Diseases (LIFE), an organizational unit affiliated with the Medical Faculty of Leipzig University. LIFE is funded by means of the European Union, by the European Regional Development Fund (ERDF) and by funds of the Free State of Saxony within the framework of the excellence initiative (project numbers: 713-241202, 14505/2470, 14575/2470). This work is also supported by the following organizations and grants: BrightFocus Foundation (M.W. and T.E.), Lions Foundation (M.W. and T.E.), Grimshaw-Gudewicz Foundation (M.W. and T.E.), Research to Prevent Blindness Unrestricted Grant (M.W. and T.E.), Alice Adler Fellowship (T.E.), Harvard Glaucoma Center of Excellence (M.W. and T.E.), National Institutes of Health K99EY028631 (M.W.), National Institutes of Health (R00 EY028631 to M.W.), National Institutes of Health (R21EY030142), National Institutes of Health (R21EY030631), National Institutes of Health (R01EY030575 to T.E.), National Institutes of Health and National Eye Institute Core Grant (P30EY003790 to M.W. and T.E.), German Federal Ministry of Education and Research: i:DSem (031L0026 to F.G.R.), German Research Foundation DFG: SCHR 774/5-1 (M.L.S.), Max Planck International Research Network on Aging (J.K.).
Competing interests
The authors report no competing interests.
Author contributions
Conceptualization: FGR, TE, MLS Data curation: BW, JG, CG, FGR, MW, TL, FSR, JK, MP Formal analysis: TE, JG Funding acquisition: ML, CE, JT, KW, AV, SGR-H, MLS Investigation: KW, FGR, TL, FSR Methodology: TE, FGR, TL, JK, FSR, AVW Project administration: FGR Resources: ML, AV, SGR-H, MLS Software: TE, FGR Supervision: MLS, SGR-H, FGR Visualization: TE, JG Writing -original draft: JG, FGR, TE Writing -review and editing: JG, FGR, MLS, TL, FSR, KW, JK, MW, CG, MP, AVW.
Glossary
- AMD =
age-related macular degeneration
- BNT =
Boston Naming Test
- CERAD =
The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease
- 95% CI =
95% confidence interval
- cpRNFLT =
circumpapillary retinal nerve fibre layer thickness
- dB =
decibel
- G =
global
- HRT =
Heidelberg retinal tomography
- IQ =
intelligence quotient
- LIFE =
Leipzig Research Centre for Civilization Diseases
- MMSE =
mini mental state examination
- MWT-B =
Multiple choice German vocabulary test
- N =
nasal
- NI =
nasal-inferior
- NS =
nasal-superior
- OCT =
optical coherence tomography
- RMET =
Reading the Mind in the Eyes test
- T =
temporal
- TI =
temporal-inferior
- TMT =
Trail Making Test
- TS =
temporal-superior
References
- 1.Solso RL. Cognitive psychology. 6th ed. Allyn and Bacon, Boston, USA; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Andrewes DG. Neuropsychology: From theory to practice. 2nd ed. Routledge, London, UK; New York, USA; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hirasawa H, Tomidokoro A, Araie M, et al. Peripapillary retinal nerve fiber layer thickness determined by spectral-domain optical coherence tomography in ophthalmologically normal eyes. Arch Ophthalmol. 2010;128(11):1420–1426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Zhao L, Wang Y, Chen CX, Xu L, Jonas JB.. Retinal nerve fibre layer thickness measured by Spectralis spectral-domain optical coherence tomography: The Beijing Eye Study. Acta Ophthalmol. 2014;92(1):e35–e41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Sung KR, Wollstein G, Bilonick RA, et al. Effects of age on optical coherence tomography measurements of healthy retinal nerve fiber layer, macula, and optic nerve head. Ophthalmology. 2009;116(6):1119–1124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Jonas JB, Schmidt AM, Müller-Bergh JA, Schlötzer-Schrehardt UM, Naumann GO.. Human optic nerve fiber count and optic disc size. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1992;33(6):2012–2018. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Haan J, Verbraak FD, Visser PJ, Bouwman FH.. Retinal thickness in Alzheimer’s disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2017;6(1):162–170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Moreno-Ramos T, Benito-León J, Villarejo A, Bermejo-Pareja F.. Retinal nerve fiber layer thinning in dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease, dementia with Lewy bodies, and Alzheimer's disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2013;34(3):659–664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Prince MJ, Wimo A, Guerchet MM, Ali GC, Wu YT, Prina M.. The global impact of dementia: An analysis of prevalence, incidence, cost and trends. London:Alzheimer’s Disease International; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Mutlu U, Colijn JM, Ikram MA, et al. Association of retinal neurodegeneration on optical coherence tomography with dementia: A population-based study. JAMA Neurol. 2018;75(10):1256–1263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Ward DD, Mauschitz MM, Bönniger MM, Merten N, Finger RP, Breteler MMB.. Association of retinal layer measurements and adult cognitive function: A population-based study. Neurology. 2020;95(9):e1144–e1152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Méndez-Gómez JL, Rougier MB, Tellouck L, et al. Peripapillary retinal nerve fiber layer thickness and the evolution of cognitive performance in an elderly population. Front Neurol. 2017;8:93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Laude A, Lascaratos G, Henderson RD, Starr JM, Deary IJ, Dhillon B.. Retinal nerve fiber layer thickness and cognitive ability in older people: The Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 study. BMC Ophthalmol. 2013;13(1):28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.van Koolwijk LME, Despriet DDG, van Duijn CM, et al. Association of cognitive functioning with retinal nerve fiber layer thickness. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2009;50(10):4576–4580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Khawaja AP, Chan MPY, Yip JLY, et al. Retinal nerve fiber layer measures and cognitive function in the EPIC-Norfolk cohort study. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2016;57(4):1921–1926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.McCann P, Hogg R, Wright DM, et al. Intraocular pressure and circumpapillary retinal nerve fibre layer thickness in the Northern Ireland Cohort for the Longitudinal Study of Ageing (NICOLA): distributions and associations. Br J Ophthalmol. 2020;105:948–956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Ko F, Muthy ZA, Gallacher J, et al. ; The UK Biobank Eye & Vision Consortium. Association of retinal nerve fiber layer thinning with current and future cognitive decline: A study using optical coherence tomography. JAMA Neurol. 2018;75(10):1198–1205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Li D, Rauscher FG, Choi EY, et al. Sex-specific differences in circumpapillary retinal nerve fiber layer thickness. Ophthalmology. 2020;127(3):357–368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Luck T, Pabst A, Rodriguez FS, et al. Age-, sex-, and education-specific norms for an extended CERAD Neuropsychological Assessment Battery-Results from the population-based LIFE-Adult-Study. Neuropsychology. 2018;32(4):461–475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Baniasadi N, Rauscher FG, Li D, et al. Norms of interocular circumpapillary retinal nerve fiber layer thickness differences at 768 retinal locations. Transl Vis Sci Technol. 2020;9(9):23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Wang M, Elze T, Li D, et al. Age, ocular magnification, and circumpapillary retinal nerve fiber layer thickness. J Biomed Opt. 2017;22(12):1–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Welsh KA, Butters N, Mohs RC, et al. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part V. A normative study of the neuropsychological battery. Neurology. 1994;44(4):609–614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC, et al. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1989;39(9):1159–1165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I.. The “reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42(2):241–251. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lehrl S. Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest: MWT-B. 5, unveränd. Aufl. Spitta; 2005.
- 26.Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18(6):643–662. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Zysset S, Muller K, Lohmann G, von Cramon DY.. Color-word matching stroop task: Separating interference and response conflict. Neuroimage. 2001;13(1):29–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Ardila A, Ostrosky-Solis F, Rosselli M, Gomez C.. Age-related cognitive decline during normal aging: The complex effect of education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2000;15(6):495–513. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Harada CN, Natelson Love MC, Triebel KL.. Normal cognitive aging. Clin Geriatr Med. 2013;29(4):737–752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.JansoniusNM, , SchieferJ, , NevalainenJ, , PaetzoldJ, , Schiefer U.. A mathematical model for describing the retinal nerve fiber bundle trajectories in the human eye: average course, variability, and influence of refraction, optic disc size and optic disc position. Experimental Eye Research. 2012;105:70–78. 10.1016/j.exer.2012.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Brauns H, Scherer S, Steinmann S.. The CASMIN educational classification in international comparative research. In: Hoffmeyer-Zlotnik JHP, Wolf C, eds. Advances in cross-national comparison: A European working book for demographic and socio-economic variables. Springer, Boston, USA; 2003:221–244. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Mauschitz MM, Bonnemaijer PWM, Diers K, et al. ; European Eye Epidemiology (E3) Consortium. Systemic and ocular determinants of peripapillary retinal nerve fiber layer thickness measurements in the European Eye Epidemiology (E3) population. Ophthalmology. 2018;125(10):1526–1536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Ho H, Tham Y-C, Chee ML, et al. Retinal nerve fiber layer thickness in a multiethnic normal Asian population: The Singapore epidemiology of eye diseases study. Ophthalmology. 2019;126(5):702–711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Salthouse T. Consequences of age-related cognitive declines. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63:201–226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Feuer WJ, Budenz DL, Anderson DR, et al. Topographic differences in the age-related changes in the retinal nerve fiber layer of normal eyes measured by Stratus optical coherence tomography. J Glaucoma. 2011;20(3):133–138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Parikh RS, Parikh SR, Sekhar GC, Prabakaran S, Babu JG, Thomas R.. Normal age-related decay of retinal nerve fiber layer thickness. Ophthalmology. 2007;114(5):921–926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Wang CY, Zheng YF, Liu B, et al. Retinal nerve fiber layer thickness in children: The Gobi desert children eye study. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2018;59(12):5285–5291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Galea LAM, Choleris E, Albert AYK, McCarthy MM, Sohrabji F.. The promises and pitfalls of sex difference research. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2020;56:100817. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Cahill L. Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7(6):477–484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Shansky RM, Murphy AZ.. Considering sex as a biological variable will require a global shift in science culture. Nat Neurosci. 2021;24(4):457–464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Pawluski JL, Kokras N, Charlier TD, Dalla C.. Sex matters in neuroscience and neuropsychopharmacology. Eur J Neurosci. 2020;52(1):2423–2428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Anjos R, Vieira L, Costa L, et al. Macular ganglion cell layer and peripapillary retinal nerve fibre layer thickness in patients with unilateral posterior cerebral artery ischaemic lesion: An optical coherence tomography study. Neuroophthalmology. 2016;40(1):8–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Barbey AK, Colom R, Solomon J, Krueger F, Forbes C, Grafman J.. An integrative architecture for general intelligence and executive function revealed by lesion mapping. Brain. 2012;135(Pt 4):1154–1164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Gläscher J, Adolphs R, Damasio H, et al. Lesion mapping of cognitive control and value-based decision making in the prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109(36):14681–14686. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Schroeter ML, Vogt B, Frisch S, et al. Executive deficits are related to the inferior frontal junction in early dementia. Brain. 2012;135(Pt 1):201–215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Schroeter ML, Zysset S, Kupka T, Kruggel F, Yves von Cramon D.. Near-infrared spectroscopy can detect brain activity during a color-word matching Stroop task in an event-related design. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;17(1):61–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Schroeter ML, Eickhoff SB, Engel A.. From correlational approaches to meta-analytical symptom reading in individual patients: Bilateral lesions in the inferior frontal junction specifically cause dysexecutive syndrome. Cortex. 2020;128:73–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Cieslik EC, Mueller VI, Eickhoff CR, Langner R, Eickhoff SB.. Three key regions for supervisory attentional control: Evidence from neuroimaging meta-analyses. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;48:22–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Kwon JY, Yang JH, Han JS, Kim DG.. Analysis of the retinal nerve fiber layer thickness in Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Korean J Ophthalmol. 2017;31(6):548–556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Knoll B, Simonett J, Volpe NJ, et al. Retinal nerve fiber layer thickness in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: Case-control study and meta-analysis. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2016;4:85–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Khawaja AP, Chan MPY, Garway-Heath DF, et al. Associations with retinal nerve fiber layer measures in the EPIC-Norfolk Eye Study. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2013;54(7):5028–5034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Amieva H, Mokri H, Le Goff M, et al. Compensatory mechanisms in higher-educated subjects with Alzheimer’s disease: A study of 20 years of cognitive decline. Brain. 2014;137(Pt 4):1167–1175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Hilal S, Saini M, Tan CS, et al. Ankle-brachial index, cognitive impairment and cerebrovascular disease in a Chinese population. Neuroepidemiology. 2014;42(2):131–138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Bigler ED, Tranel D.. Neuropsychological assessment. 5th ed. Oxford University Press, New York, USA; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Gunes A, Inal EE, Demirci S, Tok L, Tok O, Demirci S.. Changes in retinal nerve fiber layer thickness in patients with cerebral infarction: Evidence of transneuronal retrograde degeneration. Acta Neurol Belg. 2016;116(4):461–466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Petzold A, Balcer LJ, Calabresi PA, et al. Retinal layer segmentation in multiple sclerosis: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Neurol. 2017;16(10):797–812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Chrysou A, Jansonius NM, van Laar T.. Retinal layers in Parkinson’s disease: A meta-analysis of spectral-domain optical coherence tomography studies. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2019;64:40–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Hood DC, Kardon RH.. A framework for comparing structural and functional measures of glaucomatous damage. Prog Retinal Eye Res. 2007;26(6):688–710. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.La Morgia C, Ross-Cisneros FN, Koronyo Y, et al. Melanopsin retinal ganglion cell loss in Alzheimer disease. Ann Neurol. 2016;79(1):90–109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Koronyo Y, Biggs D, Barron E, et al. Retinal amyloid pathology and proof-of-concept imaging trial in Alzheimer’s disease. JCI Insight. 2017;2(16):e93621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Dinkin M. Trans-synaptic retrograde degeneration in the human visual system: Slow, silent, and real. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2017;17(2):16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Beatty RM, Sadun AA, Smith L, Vonsattel JP, Richardson EP.. Direct demonstration of transsynaptic degeneration in the human visual system: A comparison of retrograde and anterograde changes. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1982;45(2):143–146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Park HYL, Park YG, Cho AH, Park CK.. Transneuronal retrograde degeneration of the retinal ganglion cells in patients with cerebral infarction. Ophthalmology. 2013;120(6):1292–1299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Vries-Knoppert WD, Baaijen JC, Petzold A.. Patterns of retrograde axonal degeneration in the visual system. Brain. 2019;142(9):2775–2786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Armstrong RA. Alzheimer’s disease and the eye⋆. J Optom. 2009;2(3):103–111. [Google Scholar]
- 66.Trick GL, Trick LR, Morris P, Wolf M.. Visual field loss in senile dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Neurology. 1995;45(1):68–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Lee JY, Kim JP, Jang H, et al. Optical coherence tomography angiography as a potential screening tool for cerebral small vessel diseases. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2020;12(1):73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The authors confirm that the data supporting the findings of this study are available within the article and its Supplementary materials. All presented data are available as Supplementary information for download. Raw data cannot be shared publicly because of consent restrictions of LIFE-Adult participants. Data are available after an approved project agreement from the LIFE Leipzig Research Centre for Civilization Diseases. Please contact Dr Matthias Nüchter (Head of LIFE Managing Office, contact via matthias.nuechter[at]life.uni-leipzig.de) for data access requests.