Abstract
Expressive language impairment is one of the most frequently associated clinical features of 16p11.2 copy number variations (CNV). However, our understanding of the language profiles of individuals with 16p11.2 CNVs is still limited. This study builds upon previous work in the Simons Variation in Individuals Project (VIP, now known as Simons Searchlight), to characterize language abilities in 16p11.2 deletion and duplication carriers using comprehensive assessments. Participants included 110 clinically ascertained children and family members (i.e., siblings and cousins) with 16p11.2 BP4-BP5 deletion and 58 with 16p11.2 BP4-BP5 duplication between the ages of 2–23 years, most of whom were verbal. Regression analyses were performed to quantify variation in language abilities in the presence of the 16p11.2 deletion and duplication, both with and without autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and cognitive deficit. Difficulties in pragmatic skills were equally prevalent in verbal individuals in both deletion and duplication groups. NVIQ had moderate quantifiable effects on language scores in syntax and semantics/pragmatics (a decrease of less than 1 SD) for both groups. Overall, language impairments persisted even after controlling for ASD diagnosis and cognitive deficit. Language impairment is one of the core clinical features of individuals with 16p11.2 CNVs even in the absence of ASD and cognitive deficit. Results highlight the need for more comprehensive and rigorous assessment of language impairments to maximize outcomes in carriers of 16p11.2 CNVs.
Keywords: 16p11.2 deletion, 16p11.2 duplication, autism, genetics, language profiles
1 |. INTRODUCTION
Expressive language impairment is one of the most frequently described clinical features associated with copy number variation (CNV) of the recurrent ~600 kb BP4-BP5 region on 16p11.2 (BP4–BP5). Past studies have identified marked expressive language impairments (Bijlsma et al., 2009; Fedorenko et al., 2016; Ghebranious, Giampietro, Wesbrook, & Rezkalla, 2007; Hanson et al., 2015; Weiss et al., 2008), which can occur in the absence of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) or intellectual disability (ID) (Sebat et al., 2007; Weiss et al., 2008; Zufferey et al., 2012). In fact, language impairment is a commonly documented feature in many genetic syndromes, copy number variants and monogenetic conditions, such as Down syndrome, Fragile X, and Williams syndrome (Rice, Warren, & Betz, 2005), FOXP2 and CNTNAP2 variants (Feuk et al., 2006; Shriberg et al., 2006; Vernes et al., 2008) and CNVs of chromosome 7q (Alarcón, Cantor, Liu, Gilliam, & Geschwind, 2002; Folstein & Mankoski, 2000). Impairments are observed in these conditions relative to cognitive and receptive levels, and across the domains of syntax, lexicon, semantics and social use of language (pragmatics), and persist well beyond early development (Feuk et al., 2006; Finestack & Abbeduto, 2010; Laws & Bishop, 2003; Miller, 1999; Philofsky, Hepburn, Hayes, Hagerman, & Rogers, 2004).
There are few descriptions of language profiles across domains in 16p11.2 deletion and duplication, and few detailed comparisons between the two syndromes. An earlier study demonstrated that language delays were equally pervasive in both groups (Shinawi et al., 2010). Our previous work suggested that delays in duplication carriers were largely associated with global ID, while diagnoses of language disorders were more prevalent in pediatric deletion carriers (Green-Snyder et al., 2016; Hanson et al., 2010, 2015; Hippolyte et al., 2016), yet these findings warranted more careful examination.
First, ID and ASD have been linked to 16p11.2 deletion and duplication with relatively high prevalence (up to 25% for ASD; up to 80% for ID; Fernandez et al., 2010; Girirajan et al., 2013; Green-Snyder et al., 2016; Hanson et al., 2010, 2015; Kim, Paul, et al., 2014; Kumar et al., 2009; Kumar & Christian, 2009; Marshall et al., 2008; McCarthy et al., 2009; Sanders et al., 2011; Sebat et al., 2007; Walsh & Bracken, 2011; Weiss et al., 2008; Pinborough-Zimmerman et al., 2007; Zufferey et al., 2012), and these factors can affect language development in children.
Second, the limited scope of currently available standardized language measures is a significant barrier to the timely and accurate detection of language impairments in clinical populations, including 16p11.2 carriers. Many existing standardized measures are designed to determine the presence and severity of language impairments by gauging answers that are dependent upon pre-determined responses in a highly structured setting. Therefore, impairments in certain areas of language, such as spontaneous use of language in a natural environment or functional language in everyday contexts, may not readily be recognized by those instruments (Condouris, Meyer, & Tager-Flusberg, 2003). This highlights the need for a more comprehensive approach including assessment of language samples and pragmatics when studying language phenotypes in 16p11.2 (Tager-Flusberg et al., 2009).
To address these gaps in current knowledge, we attempted to thoroughly characterize the pattern of quantitative expressive language impairments in 16p11.2 deletion versus duplication carriers, specifically in syntax, semantics, pragmatics and functional communication (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013; Bishop et al., 2017; Bishop, Snowling, Thompson, & Greenhalgh, 2016). We examined the prevalence and severity of language impairments in the context of ASD and cognitive ability, using multiple, complementary language measures. These included clinician observations of syntax, pragmatics, and semantics in structured (Comprehensive Assessment of Spoken Language [CASL]; Carrow-Woolfolk, 1999) and natural contexts (Observation of Spontaneous Expressive Language [OSEL]; Kim, Paul, et al., 2014), as well as parent reports of communication (Children’s Communication Checklist-2 [CCC-2]; Bishop, 2003), including everyday functional language use (Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale-II [VABS]; Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti, 2005).
2 |. METHODS
2.1 |. Participants
Participants with the recurrent ~600 kb 16p11.2 BP4-BP5 deletion or duplication identified through clinical diagnostic evaluations were invited to participate in Simons Searchlight. All deletion and duplication carriers had the same recurrent CNV and no additional pathogenic CNV or known monogenic disorders. See Simons VIP Consortium (2012), Hanson et al. (2015) and Green Snyder et al. (2016) for more details.
Following screening, families participated in data collection at one of three Simons Searchlight phenotyping sites at Boston Children’s Hospital, Baylor College of Medicine or University of Washington for a comprehensive and standardized multiday evaluation. Data from participants were obtained from three core sites trained in the language battery. The study was approved by the institutional review board at each participating institution. Standardization of measurements across sites included mandatory formalized and standardized training on all measures through in-person training sessions and webinars for all clinicians, and maintenance of cross-site reliability with independent consultants.
One hundred ten (110) participants with the 16p11.2 deletion (53 females, 57 males) and 58 participants with 16p11.2 duplication (34 males, 24 females) aged 2–23 years were included. Fourteen (14) percent of the carrier sample were full-biological siblings (n = 16; 8 deletion and 8 duplication carriers), half-siblings (n = 2 duplication carriers) or cousins (n = 2 duplication carriers) of initially identified probands, identified through cascade genetic testing. Due to the small proportion of these cases, we decided to include them in the analyses because their presence yielded no differences in results (data available upon request). Out of the total 168 individuals, 26 deletion and 13 duplication carriers received a diagnosis of ASD based on DSM-IV-TR criteria (see Section 2.3 below for more details).
The number of individuals able to be included in analyses varied for each language measure according to their language level, and most analyses were conducted upon verbal individuals with at least phrase speech (see below).
2.2 |. Measures
2.2.1 |. Vineland-II
The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale-2nd Edition (Vineland-II; Sparrow et al., 2005) is a measure of adaptive behavior for individuals from birth to adulthood. Even though the Vineland-II is not typically used as a core measure of language, we were interested in examining impairments in the use of language skills in an everyday context. For the present study, we used the V-scale score from the expressive communication domain, which is standardized by age, with a mean of 15 and a standard deviation of 3. The Vineland-II was available for all but one case across a wide range of language level including minimally verbal and verbal individuals.
2.2.2 |. Comprehensive Assessment of Spoken Language
The CASL (Carrow-Woolfolk, 1999) is a measure designed to assess oral language abilities in the Syntactic domain for children age 3–10 years and the Pragmatic domain for individuals age 3–21 years. For the present study, standard scores and age equivalents were examined. The standard score has a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15. Seventeen deletion carriers and 7 duplication carriers could not complete the Syntactic subtests, and 15 deletion carriers and 8 duplication carriers could not complete the Pragmatic subtests, due to language limitations (lack of phrase or sentence speech), despite being within age range for the test (See Table S1).
2.2.3 |. Children’s Communication Checklist-2
The CCC-2 (Bishop, 2003) is a parent-report measure designed to assess communication skills in the areas of pragmatics, syntax, and semantics for individuals from 4 to 16 years. For this study, we used scaled scores (with an average of 10 and SD of 3) for 9 domains targeting Syntax (grammar), Semantics (vocabulary), Coherence (discourse), and various pragmatic skills (Initiation, Scripted Language, Context, Nonverbal Communication, Social Relations, Interests). Eleven (11) deletion carriers and 1 duplication carrier had language levels that were too low to receive the instrument (below the 4-year-old level; See Table S1).
2.2.4 |. Observation of Spontaneous Expressive Language
The OSEL (Kim, Junker, & Lord, 2014) is a 30–45 min observational assessment which focuses on children’s spontaneous expressive language use in standardized but natural contexts. The OSEL is intended to be used with children with ASD and other communication disorders from 2 to 12 years old whose expressive language levels are equivalent to typically developing children between 2 and 5 years. Thirty-seven (37) deletion carriers and 19 duplication carriers were unable to complete the measure due to limited language (below the 5-year-old level; See Table S1). OSEL syntax totals are computed by combining grammatical usages of 24 items, including different types of pronouns [e.-g., subjective/objective/possessive], verb tenses [e.g., regular/irregular past, be + progressive “-ing”], and sentence forms [e.g., coordination/subordination]. The OSEL pragmatic and semantic profile (PSP) totals include three domains: (a) Initiation of Reciprocal Communication (e.g., Verbal request and Asks for information about others’ experiences); (b) Narrative Skills (e.g., Reporting main ideas and Reporting sequence of events); (c) Unusual Features (e.g., Dominates conversations and Stereo-typed/idiosyncratic language). The OSEL provides age equivalents based on established norms. A language quotient (LQ = [age equivalent/chronological age] × 100) was derived from the age equivalents following the convention for ratio quotients (Kim, Junker, et al., 2014). Because the age equivalent scores for the OSEL only go up to 60 months, LQ scores were not derived for individuals above 59 months who achieved ceiling scores, in order to minimize the risk of underestimating skills for this particular group of children (n = 12 deletion and n = 10 duplication carriers for Syntax; n = 10 deletion and 3 duplication carriers for the Initiation of Reciprocal Communication domain, and n = 17 deletion and n = 9 duplication carriers for the Narrative Skills domain).
2.2.5 |. Cognitive measures
All participants were administered a developmentally appropriate cognitive measure: Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL; Mullen, 1995) or Differential Abilities Scale, Second Edition (DAS; Elliott, 2007). When available, standard scores were used for Full-Scale IQ (FSIQ), Verbal IQ (VIQ), and Nonverbal IQ (NVIQ). For MSEL, deviation IQ scores were extrapolated using Visual Reception and Fine Motor T scores for NVIQ, and Receptive and Expressive Language T scores for VIQ. MSEL ratio IQ scores were computed based on the age equivalent scores from the Visual Reception and Fine Motor domains for the NVIQ and from the Receptive and Expressive Language Scores for the VIQ (average of the age equivalents divided by chronological age; Bishop, Farmer, & Thurm, 2015).
2.2.6 |. ADOS-2
The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-2 (ADOS-2; Lord et al., 2012) is a play-based diagnostic instrument for core symptoms of ASD in the social communication (SC) and restricted and repetitive behaviors (RRBs) domains. Out of 168 individuals, 5 individuals did not have ADOS data available (e.g., individuals with mental ages below 15 months for whom the ADOS could not be validly administered). The Social Affect calibrated severity score (CSS; or Comparison scores; Hus, Gotham, & Lord, 2014) derived from the algorithm score was used to measure social communication impairment.
2.3 |. Diagnostic procedure
Experienced, licensed clinicians gave best-estimate, clinical DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2013) diagnoses using all information obtained during the research evaluation. Diagnoses were based on information obtained from the standardized interview, questionnaires, and observations, as well as results from standardized administration of the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (Shaffer, Fisher, Lucas, Dulcan, & Schwab-Stone, 2000) and review of available medical records and prior testing. For ASD diagnosis, information from the Autism Diagnostic Observation Scale (Lord et al., 2012) and the Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised (Le Couteur, Lord, & Rutter, 2003) was used to reach the clinical best-estimate diagnosis.
2.4 |. Data analysis
Because of inherent differences in the nature of deletion versus duplication of the 16p11.2 chromosomal region, deletion and duplication carriers were examined separately in analyses.
To examine potential sampling bias caused by the uneven numbers of deletion versus duplication carriers excluded on each instrument, we compared characteristics of included and excluded participants using t tests and chi-square analyses. For individuals with at least phrase speech, we examined the presence and severity of language impairments. Functional communication was examined for all individuals, including minimally verbal cases (e.g., no words or using single words).
First, we determined the proportion of participants with a delay in each language domain, defined as a score more than 1 SD below the mean. We compared the proportion of deletion versus duplication carriers with a delay in each measure, using chi-square tests. Mean language scores were also compared between the two groups using t tests. Effect sizes were calculated for the comparisons (using odds ratios for chi-square tests and Cohen’s d for t tests).
Second, we conducted regression analyses on standardized language scores with ASD diagnosis and NVIQ as covariates. In these analyses, NVIQ was centered at 100 and divided by 10, such that estimates were interpreted as the change in a given language score with every 10-point unit change in NVIQ. Given that the 16p11.2 deletion and duplication are distinct in mechanism and phenotype, we conducted regression analyses separately for the two groups. Beta weights were examined separately to examine the magnitude of specific ASD and NVIQ effects on language outcomes. We also examined the significant effects of the intercepts in the regression models to determine whether a participant’s predicted language score without ASD and with average NVIQ is significantly lower than the average scores based on the test norms (e.g., a standard score of 100 on CASL, or ratio quotient of 100 in the OSEL). This was possible by using the centered normed scores as outcome variables (all centered at the mean: 100 for standard scores, 10 for V-scaled scores).
All results were corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni correction.
3 |. RESULTS
3.1 |. Sample characteristics and language profiles for deletion and duplication carriers
Sample characteristics can be found in Table 1. Age ranged from 2 to 23 years with a mean age of 8 years (SD = 4.5), depending upon each language instrument (see below). VIQ and NVIQ ranged 13–122 (M = 80, SD = 21) and 26–130 (M = 82, SD = 18), respectively. Consis tent with findings reported previously, duplication carriers showed significantly lower NVIQ compared to deletion carriers (p < .05).
TABLE 1.
Deletion | Duplication | |
---|---|---|
N | 110 | 58 |
ASD, n | 26 | 13 |
Age in months M (SD) | 101.6 (49.7) | 99.6 (62.8) |
Range | 24–250 | 24–281 |
Nonverbal IQa M (SD) | 86.2 (15.3) | 74.8 (20.3) |
Range | 48–130 | 26–116 |
Verbal IQ M (SD) | 79.1 (18.8) | 79.1 (25.3) |
Range | 23–116 | 13–122 |
ADOS calibrated severity score M (SD) | 3.3 (2.3) | 3.4 (2.8) |
Range | 1–10 | 1–10 |
Vineland-II socialization standard score M (SD) | 82.6 (15.4) | 79.9 (15.8) |
Range | 34–131 | 42–118 |
Vineland-II daily living skills standard score M (SD) | 83.3 (14.6) | 78.7 (16.3) |
Range | 34–120 | 33–111 |
Vineland-II communication standard score M (SD) | 78.6 (12.7) | 80.8 (16.9) |
Range | 45–122 | 43.118 |
Abbreviations: ADOS, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; Vineland-II, Vineland Adaptive Behavioral Scale.
Significant differences emerged between the two groups (p < .05).
As expected, for each given measure, individuals who were unable to complete testing had significantly lower verbal or nonverbal IQ scores and were more likely to be diagnosed with ASD compared to those who completed testing (See Table S1). This was true for both deletion and duplication carriers and for nearly all measures. For those individuals who were within valid age range for each instrument (besides Vineland-II), 12–46% of deletion and 2–41% of duplication carriers did not receive the language testing due to limited language levels. There was no statistically significant difference in the proportions of individuals who were unable to complete the testing between deletion and duplication carriers across all measures.
As seen in Table 2, in verbal individuals, deletion carriers showed significantly lower scores than duplication carriers in the two language instruments targeting syntax (CASL and CCC-2; p < .05). No significant difference emerged between the two groups in the pragmatics domain. The proportion of individuals with delays on quantitative measures was also significantly higher in deletion than duplication carriers for all domains except for pragmatics (p < .05).
TABLE 2.
Deletion | Duplication | t (chi-square) | p | Effect size (Cohen’s d/odds ratios) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Reported functional expressive communication | ||||||
Vineland-II | N | 109 | 58 | |||
Expressive communication v-scale scores, M (SD) | 10.6 (2.7) | 11.7 (3.5) | −1.947 | .055 | d = 0.35 | |
% with delays | 67% | 48% | (5.536) | .019 | OR = 0.45 | |
Syntax | ||||||
CASL | N | 58 | 35 | |||
Syntax standard scores, M (SD) | 72.8 (16.6) | 84.1 (15.5) | −3.311 | .001 | d = 0.70 | |
% with delays | 78% | 46% | (9.825) | .002 | OR = 0.24 | |
CCC-2 | N | 81 | 41 | |||
Syntax scaled scores, M (SD) | 5.5 (3.8) | 8.0 (3.8) | −3.325 | .001 | d = 0.66 | |
% with delays | 84% | 56% | (11.141) | .001 | OR = 0.24 | |
OSEL | N | 43 | 27 | |||
OSEL syntax totals, M (SD) | 62.4 (15.1) | 67.6 (17.7) | −1.307 | .196 | d = 0.32 | |
% with delays | 78% | 41% | (6.793) | .009 | OR = 0.27 | |
Pragmatics and semantics | ||||||
CASL | N | 91 | 48 | |||
Pragmatic standard scores, M (SD) | 77.2 (16.7) | 82.6 (17.0) | −1.776 | .079 | d = 0.32 | |
% with delays | 63% | 48% | (2.787) | .095 | OR = 0.55 | |
CCC-2 | N | 81 | 41 | |||
Initiation scaled scores, M (SD) | 7.4 (2.8) | 6.3 (3.4) | 1.820 | .071 | d = 0.35 | |
Scripted language scaled scores, M (SD) | 6.5 (3.3) | 7.7 (3.0) | −1.848 | .067 | d = 0.38 | |
Context scaled scores, M (SD) | 5.8 (2.9) | 6.6 (3.3) | −1.391 | .167 | d = 0.27 | |
Nonverbal communication scaled scores, M (SD) | 5.8 (3.1) | 6.7 (3.6) | −1.538 | .127 | d = 0.27 | |
Interests scaled scores, M (SD) | 8.0 (2.9) | 7.7 (3.2) | 0.510 | .611 | d = 0.10 | |
Semantics scaled scores, M (SD) | 5.8 (3.2) | 7.4 (2.9) | −2.635 | .010a | d = 0.52 | |
% with delays | 79% | 76% | (0.183) | .952 | OR = 0.82 | |
OSEL | N | 43 | 27 | |||
Initiation of reciprocal totals, M (SD) | 3.5 (2.8) | 3.7 (2.8) | −0.124 | .902 | d = 0.07 | |
% with delays | 98% | 96% | (0.113) | .738 | OR = 0.62 | |
Narrative skills totals, M (SD) | 5.7 (3.4) | 6.4 (4.7) | −0.667 | .507 | d = 0.17 | |
% with delays | 95% | 93% | 0.023 | .629 | OR = 0.61 |
Note: t test with equal variance not assumed.
Bold values represents p-values <0.05.
Abbreviations: CASL, Comprehensive Assessment of Spoken Language; CCC-2, Children’s Communication Checklist-2; OSEL, Observation of Spontaneous Expressive Language; Vineland-II, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales.
No longer significant with Bonferroni correction.
3.2 |. The effects of ASD diagnosis and NVIQ on the severity of language impairments
Tables 3 and 4 display results of regression models in which the intercept, or constant, predict each language domain and the effects of ASD and cognitive deficit.
TABLE 3.
B | SE | t | Sig. | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Reported functional expressive communication | |||||
Vineland-II | Constant | −3.303 | .344 | −9.606 | <.001 |
ASD diagnosis | −1.654 | .551 | −3.000 | .003 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | .517 | .153 | 3.372 | .001 | |
Syntax | |||||
CASL | Constant | −18.884 | 2.662 | −7.093 | <.001 |
ASD diagnosis | 3.787 | 5.611 | .675 | .503 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | 6.680 | 1.437 | 4.648 | <.001 | |
CCC-2 | Constant | −3.815 | .600 | −6.363 | <.001 |
ASD diagnosis | −.980 | .982 | −.998 | .220 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | .365 | .295 | 1.236 | .321 | |
OSEL | Constant | −44.752 | 4.731 | −9.459 | <.001 |
ASD diagnosis | −7.054 | 6.490 | −1.087 | .286 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | 4.097 | 2.243 | 1.826 | .078 | |
Pragmatics and semantics | |||||
CASL | Constant | −14.267 | 2.108 | −6.768 | <.001 |
ASD diagnosis | −5.842 | 3.617 | −1.615 | .110 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | 6.105 | 1.073 | 5.687 | <.001 | |
CCC-2 initiation | Constant | −1.799 | .430 | −4.183 | <.001 |
ASD diagnosis | −1.858 | .704 | −2.638 | .010a | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | .309 | .212 | 1.461 | .148 | |
CCC-2 scripted language | Constant | −2.755 | .507 | −5.439 | <.001 |
ASD diagnosis | .166 | .249 | .665 | .508 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | −2.161 | .830 | −2.605 | .011a | |
CCC-2 context | Constant | −3.272 | .438 | −7.477 | <.001 |
ASD diagnosis | −2.105 | .717 | −2.937 | .004 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | .380 | .216 | 1.764 | .082 | |
CCC-2 nonverbal communication | Constant | −3.743 | .476 | −7.866 | <.001 |
ASD diagnosis | .010 | .234 | .042 | .967 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | −2.147 | .779 | −2.755 | .007 | |
CCC-2 interests | Constant | −1.563 | .440 | −3.550 | .001 |
ASD diagnosis | −1.736 | .720 | −2.412 | .018a | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | .046 | .216 | .212 | .833 | |
CCC-2 semantics | Constant | −1.181 | .599 | −1.971 | .056 |
ASD diagnosis | .761 | 1.069 | .712 | .481 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | .790 | .208 | .3.803 | <.001 | |
OSEL initiation of reciprocal social interaction | Constant | −48.656 | 3.813 | −12.760 | <.001 |
ASD diagnosis | −1.738 | 5.696 | −.305 | .762 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | 4.118 | 1.930 | 2.133 | .041a | |
OSEL narrative skills | Constant | −28.416 | 6.176 | −4.601 | .000 |
ASD diagnosis | −10.760 | 8.328 | −1.292 | .209 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | 10.046 | 3.006 | 3.342 | .003 |
Note: Significant results are bolded. NVIQ is centered at 100 and divided by 10 such that the beta estimates can be interpreted as increases/decreases in scores for every 10-point unit increase in NVIQ scores.
Abbreviations: CASL, Comprehensive Assessment of Spoken Language; CCC-2, Children’s Communication Checklist-2; OSEL, Observation of Spontaneous Expressive Language; Vineland-II, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales.
No longer significant with Bonferroni correction.
TABLE 4.
B | SE | t | Sig. | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Reported functional expressive communication | |||||
Vineland-II | Constant | −.644 | .570 | −1.129 | .264 |
ASD diagnosis | −1.58 | .898 | −1.759 | .084 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | .952 | .186 | 5.13 | <.001 | |
Syntax | |||||
CASL | Constant | −3.086 | 2.857 | −1.080 | .288 |
ASD diagnosis | 1.141 | 5.506 | .226 | .823 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | 6.261 | 1.064 | 5.884 | <.001 | |
CCC-2 | Constant | −.051 | .743 | −.0.68 | .946 |
ASD diagnosis | 1.300 | 1.326 | .981 | .333 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | 1.146 | .258 | 4.448 | <.001 | |
OSEL | Constant | −3.288 | 6.736 | −.488 | .634 |
ASD diagnosis | −37.161 | 18.239 | −2.037 | .064 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | 8.683 | 2.173 | 3.996 | .002 | |
Pragmatics and semantics | |||||
CASL | Constant | −4.701 | 3.018 | −1.558 | .126 |
ASD diagnosis | −8.115 | 5.112 | −1.587 | .119 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | 5.186 | 1.086 | 4.776 | <.001 | |
CCC-2 initiation | Constant | −2.954 | .815 | −3.625 | .001 |
ASD diagnosis | −1.609 | 1.455 | −1.106 | .276 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | .280 | .283 | .992 | .328 | |
CCC-2 scripted language | Constant | −1.306 | .659 | −1.981 | .055 |
ASD diagnosis | −1.105 | 1.177 | −.938 | .354 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | .469 | .229 | 2.049 | .048a | |
CCC-2 context | Constant | −2.346 | .743 | −3.159 | .003 |
ASD diagnosis | −.496 | 1.327 | −.374 | .711 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | .560 | .258 | 2.173 | .036a | |
CCC-2 nonverbal communication | Constant | −2.290 | .775 | −2.956 | .005 |
ASD diagnosis | −2.101 | 1.383 | −1.519 | .138 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | .429 | .269 | 1.596 | .138 | |
CCC-2 interests | Constant | −1.528 | .746 | −2.049 | .048a |
ASD diagnosis | −2.053 | 1.332 | −1.542 | .132 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | .274 | .259 | 1.061 | .296 | |
CCC-2 semantics | Constant | −1.015 | .556 | −1.825 | .076 |
ASD diagnosis | .733 | 1.056 | .694 | .492 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | .834 | .199 | 4.200 | <.001 | |
OSEL initiation of reciprocal social interaction | Constant | −31.209 | 5.037 | −6.196 | <.001 |
ASD diagnosis | −16.973 | 9.228 | −1.839 | .082 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | 4.682 | 1.686 | 2.776 | .012a | |
OSEL narrative skills | Constant | −22.944 | 7.518 | −2.976 | .004 |
ASD diagnosis | −15.627 | 14.029 | −1.114 | .284 | |
NVIQ (centered at 100) | 7.170 | 2.121 | 3.380 | .004 |
Note: Significant results are bolded. NVIQ is centered at 100 and divided by 10 such that the beta estimates can be interpreted as increases/decreases in scores for every 10-point unit increase in NVIQ scores.
Abbreviations: CASL, Comprehensive Assessment of Spoken Language; CCC-2, Children’s Communication Checklist-2; CTOPP, Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing; OSEL, Observation of Spontaneous Expressive Language; Vineland-II, Vineland Adaptive Behavioral Scale.
No longer significant with Bonferroni correction.
3.2.1 |. Deletion carriers
As shown in Table 3, in verbal deletion carriers, a small but statistically significant association of ASD diagnosis emerged for pragmatic skills (CCC-2 Context) in deletion carriers, in which the presence of ASD was associated with lower language scores. A significant association of NVIQ also emerged for most language measures, including syntax (CASL, CCC-2), and pragmatic skills (CASL, CCC-2 Scripted Language, Nonverbal Communication, OSEL Narrative Skills), in which NVIQ scores were positively associated with language scores. The presence of ASD and lower NVIQ also predicted more impairments in functional communication (Vineland-II Expressive Communication), including in minimally verbal deletion carriers. All p values were <.05. Other subtests showed effects of ASD and NVIQ which were not statistically significant following Bonferroni correction (CCC-2 Initiation and Interests domains, and OSEL Initiation, respectively). The largest estimated effects in verbal deletion carriers were those of NVIQ, upon CASL pragmatic and OSEL Narrative pragmatic scores, with an additive 7-point and 10-point increase respectively per 10 point-NVIQ increase, and upon CASL syntax, with an additive 6-point increase per 10-point NVIQ increase.
3.2.2 |. Duplication carriers
As shown in Table 4, similar to the results from verbal deletion carriers, in verbal duplication carriers, a significant association of NVIQ emerged for syntax (CASL, CCC-2 and OSEL) and pragmatics (CASL; CCC-2 Semantics; OSEL Narrative Skills), in which NVIQ scores were positively associated with language scores. However, unlike in deletion carriers, no significant effect of ASD diagnosis emerged in verbal duplication carriers. This pattern was true of functional communication on the Vineland-II as well, including in minimally verbal duplication carriers. All p values were <.05. Other subtests showed effects of NVIQ which were not significant with Bonferroni correction (CCC-2 Scripted Language and Context; OSEL Initiation). The largest estimated effects in verbal duplication carriers were seen for NVIQ in the OSEL syntax language quotient, with an additive 9-point increase per 10-point NVIQ increase, and in the CASL Pragmatic and OSEL Narrative Skills scores, with an additive 5–7-point increase per 10-point NVIQ increase.
3.3 |. Presence of language impairment after controlling for the effects of ASD diagnosis and cognitive deficit
3.3.1 |. Deletion Carriers
As shown in Table 3, in verbal individuals with deletion, the coefficients for the intercepts (beta) indicated that each estimated mean language score for a deletion carrier without ASD and with an NVIQ of 100 would be significantly lower than the average score based upon test norms (Standard Score of 100). Across nearly all language domains, the model estimates indicated that a deletion carrier without an ASD diagnosis or cognitive deficit would still show a language score that is at least 1 SD below what would be expected. This was true also of minimally verbal individuals on the Vineland-II. Syntax and pragmatics showed the greatest predicted decrements controlling for ASD and cognitive deficit, with an approximate 20- to 45-point decrease in CASL and OSEL syntax scores, and 28- to 49-point decrease in the OSEL pragmatic language quotients, relative to those expected.
3.3.2 |. Duplication Carriers
As shown in Table 4, in verbal duplication carriers, intercepts in the regression models were significant for some pragmatic and semantic domains when adjusting for ASD and NVIQ (CCC-2 Initiation, Context, Nonverbal Communication; OSEL Initiation of Reciprocal Social Communication and Narrative Skills). Other pragmatic measures showed differences that were not significant with Bonferroni correction (CCC-2 Interests). Intercepts for syntax and Vineland-II functional communication were not significant. Using this method, the magnitude of the predicted differences in verbal duplication carriers with average IQ and no ASD were smaller and more varied than in verbal deletion carriers, with many language test scores falling within 1 SD of the mean. Pragmatic skills, especially for the Initiation of Social Communication domain of the OSEL, showed the greatest predicted decrements controlling for ASD and cognitive deficit, with a 31-point decrease in OSEL language quotients relative to those expected.
4 |. DISCUSSION
The current study aimed to characterize the pattern of quantitative impairments in language measures in a sample of mostly verbal 16p11.2 deletion and duplication carriers with and without ASD diagnosis and cognitive deficits. Not surprisingly, cognitive deficit was a significant predictor of lower functional communication in everyday settings in both deletion and duplication carriers, including minimally verbal individuals. Among verbal individuals, cognitive skills also showed a significant effect on syntactic and pragmatic skills in both groups, with beta estimates suggesting 5- to 10-point increases on language indices for every 10-point increase in NVIQ. There was little to no effect of ASD diagnosis upon language in the current sample, but it is important to note that nonverbal ASD cases were excluded from most analyses, and so conclusions about the effects of ASD are limited.
Expressive language profiles in 16p11.2 duplication and deletion carriers continue to show impairments across different domains even after the effects of ASD diagnosis and NVIQ are controlled. Taken with the findings above, this lends support to the idea that at least in verbal individuals, language impairment is associated with the effects of 16p11.2 rearrangements themselves, and is neither entirely explained nor significantly exacerbated by other developmental diagnoses, especially ASD. This is consistent with previous research (Sebat et al., 2007; Weiss et al., 2008). These results may suggest that researchers characterizing genetic changes should keep in mind that not all language features traditionally considered “core” to ASD, such as pragmatic difficulties, are attributable to ASD but will be found in the absence of ASD as well.
4.1 |. Clinical implications
It is important to note that results from the current study are likely most clinically relevant to individuals capable of a minimum level of expressive language, namely, phrase speech, and do not represent all 16p11.2 CNV carriers or those with the most severe impairments. In fact, 12–46% of deletion carriers and 2–41% of duplication carriers did not receive the language testing due to limited language levels. There was no significant difference between the deletion and duplication carriers in the proportion of individuals who did not receive the language testing. Further, the current measures of language ability required not only expressive language but also receptive demands (e.g., CASL Pragmatic domain, and CCC-2). Nonetheless, we found that the severity and type of language impairment varied between verbal deletion and duplication carriers. More specifically, impairments were quite prominent for deletion carriers, with scores for most language domains falling more than 1 SD below the mean even after controlling for ASD diagnosis and NVIQ. Duplication carriers showed less severe difficulties in pragmatic and semantic domains than deletion carriers. This was especially striking given that cognitive impairment can be relatively severe for the duplication carriers, and could be an artifact of having to exclude the most severely cognitively and linguistically impaired individuals who could not complete testing.
Clinically it is important to note that both deletion and duplication carriers showed a wide range of language impairments in domains that varied by group. The presence of delays in pragmatic skills in verbal individuals were especially high (48–100%) for both deletion and duplication carriers, but have traditionally not been recognized in formal language diagnosis (e.g., with only 27% of duplication carriers having a clinical language disorder diagnosis in our previous reports). Therefore, it will be important for clinicians to consider if impairments warrant a separate diagnosis of language disorder to inform treatment planning with a more focused treatment goal on pragmatic skills.
In addition, clinicians working with individuals with 16p11.2 CNVs may readily notice more obvious abnormalities, such as deficits in grammatical structures. However, pragmatic impairments, including initiation of reciprocal social interaction (OSEL, CCC-2; e.g., requesting, commenting on one’s own experiences), and narrative skills (OSEL, CCC-2; e.g., reporting main ideas from a story, sequencing ideas in conversation) in a more natural setting, as well as other pragmatic and semantic skills in a more structured context (CASL), may not be identified by clinicians, especially for deletion and duplication carriers who do not meet criteria for ASD.
Finally, it is important to note that some patterns in our findings varied by the type of instrument used. Although some of these instruments target similar domains of language (e.g., pragmatics), they vary in the source of information (e.g., parent-report vs. clinician observation) and the context in which the target skill is measured (e.g., home vs. lab-settings). For instance, instruments such as the CASL gauge the level of language skills based on pre-determined answers elicited in a highly structured setting. In contrast, language instruments such as the OSEL are intended to capture spontaneous use of language in a semi-structured setting, without many direct prompts, while parent report measures such as Vineland-II and CCC-2 are designed to target language skills that are functional and generalized in day-to-day settings. Further, profiles will vary within and between individuals. This highlights the need to take a comprehensive approach to language assessment across domains, and to combine different sources of information for individuals with 16p11.2 CNVs.
4.2 |. Limitations and future directions
This study included a subset of individuals from a larger study focused on characterization of 16p11.2 deletion and duplication carriers (Green-Snyder et al., 2016; Hanson et al., 2015). We focused our observations on initially identified probands and their siblings and cousins but did not include other older family members, including parents and grandparents. Very young children under the age of 2 were not included in most analyses. In addition, while our sample included a wide age range, one of the instruments, the OSEL, was given to mostly younger children, or older children whose language skills were equivalent to 2- to 5-year-old typically developing children. Therefore, generalization of the results may warrant caution.
One of the strengths of our study was the multi-measurement approach to profiling language in 16p11.2 CNVs. However, as these instruments were each validated for specific age groups and language levels, varying numbers of individuals falling below the basal or above the ceiling on each measure were excluded from analyses. In fact, those who were excluded were often minimally verbal, had significantly lower IQ and were more likely to be diagnosed with ASD compared to those who were included. Therefore, conclusions regarding effects of ASD and NVIQ on language are limited, and results should be considered specific to verbal carriers and to the instruments under study. Future studies also should examine interaction effects of NVIQ and ASD, in a larger and more inclusive sample. Studies could consider instruments appropriate for minimally verbal, younger children (e.g., Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales; CSBS34) that focus on impairments in pre-verbal communication, to expand the scope of research in this area.
Finally, our results are based on a clinically ascertained sample that was brought to attention by their developmental and language delays, and are not necessarily reflective of the full spectrum of the 16p11.2 population. This problem is somewhat mitigated by the fact that we included family members (e.g., siblings) of clinically ascertained probands and have observations of language for individuals with varying levels of cognitive abilities. However, these limitations underscore the need for replication with other independent samples.
5 |. CONCLUSION
Our findings demonstrate that specific types of language impairments in 16p11.2 deletion and duplication carriers can persist even in the absence of an ASD diagnosis and cognitive delay. These findings suggest that language impairments may be one of the core clinical features of 16p11.2 CNVs, even for those without ASD or cognitive deficits. Therefore, it is critical not to overlook pragmatic and other language impairments, even in the presence of more intact social and cognitive skills. Our results also confirm, not surprisingly, that the presence of cognitive deficit further exacerbates language impairments in both 16p11.2 deletion and duplication carriers in predictable and quantifiable ways. Careful, comprehensive examination of language is critical while considering other clinical features, such as ASD and cognitive deficits in these populations, in order to provide specific support for optimal development.
Supplementary Material
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This study was funded by a grant from the Simons Foundation (SFARI award #198677 to C. L., R. B., E. H., R. G. K., W. K. C.) and NICHD (5R01HD081199-06 awarded to CL for her effort). We are grateful to all of the families at the participating Simons Searchlight sites (formerly, Simons VIP), as well as the Simons VIP consortium (Neuron, 73[6]:1063-1067, 2012). We are also grateful for the guidance and support from Helen Tager-Flusberg on the development of this project. We appreciate obtaining access to phenotypic data on SFARI Base.
Funding information
Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, Grant/Award Number: 5R01HD081199-06; Simons Foundation, Grant/Award Number: SFARI award #198677
Footnotes
Approved researchers can obtain the Simons Searchlight dataset described in this study by applying at https://base.sfari.org. The Simons VIP Consortium includes: Hanalore Alupay, Benjamin Aaronson, Sean Ackerman, Katy Ankenman, Ayesha Anwar, Constance Atwell, Alexandra Bowe, Arthur L Beaudet, Marta Benedetti, Jessica Berg, Jeffrey Berman, Leandra N Berry, Audrey L Bibb, Lisa Blaskey, Jonathan Brennan, Christie M Brewton, Randy Buckner, Polina Bukshpun, Jordan Burko, Phil Cali, Bettina Cerban, Yishin Chang, Maxwell Cheong, Vivian Chow, Zili Chu, Darina Chudnovskaya, Lauren Cornew, Corby Dale, John Dell, Allison G Dempsey, Trent Deschamps, Rachel Earl, James Edgar, Jenna Elgin, Jennifer Endre Olson, Yolanda L Evans, Anne Findlay, Gerald D Fischbach, Charlie Fisk, Brieana Fregeau, Bill Gaetz, Leah Gaetz, Silvia Garza, Jennifer Gerdts, Orit Glenn, Sarah E Gobuty, Rachel Golembski, Marion Greenup, Kory Heiken, Katherine Hines, Leighton Hinkley, Frank I Jackson, Julian Jenkins III, Rita J Jeremy, Kelly Johnson, Stephen M Kanne, Sudha Kessler, Scrah Y Khan, Matthew Ku, Emily Kuschner, Anna L Laakman, Peter Lam, Morgan W Lasala, Hana Lee, Kevin LeGuerre, Susan Levy, Alyss Lian Cavanagh, Ashlie V Llorens, Katherine Loftus Campe, Tracy L Luks, Elysa J Marco, Stephen Martin, Alastair J Martin, Gabriela Marzano, Christina Masson, Kathleen E McGovern, Rebecca McNally Keehn, David T Miller, Fiona K Miller, Timothy J Moss, Rebecca Murray, Srikantan S Nagarajan, Kerri P Nowell, Julia Owen, Andrea M Paal, Alan Packer, Patricia Z Page, Brianna M Paul, Alana Peters, Danica Peterson, Annapurna Poduri, Nicholas J Pojman, Ken Porche, Monica B Proud, Saba Qasmieh, Melissa B Ramocki, Beau Reilly, Timothy PL Roberts, Dennis Shaw, Tuhin Sinha, Bethanny Smith-Packard, Anne Snow Gallagher, Vivek Swarnakar, Tony Thieu, Christina Triantafallou, Roger Vaughan, Nicole Visyak, Mari Wakahiro, Arianne Wallace, Tracey Ward, Julia Wenegrat, and Anne Wolken.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
Drs Lord and Bishop have received royalties from Western Psychological Services for publication of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS). All other authors declare no conflict of interest.
SUPPORTING INFORMATION
Additional supporting information may be found online in the Supporting Information section at the end of this article.
REFERENCES
- Alarcón M, Cantor RM, Liu J, Gilliam TC, & Geschwind DH (2002). Evidence for a language quantitative trait locus on chromosome 7q in multiplex autism families. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 70(1), 60–71. 10.1086/338241 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, (DSM-5®). American Psychiatric Publishing. Retrieved from http://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=-JivBAAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PT18&dq=dsm-5&ots=cdQK11JExd&sig=iOElOWoRZ3IQFhlSqAmWBh_O7A8 [Google Scholar]
- Bijlsma EK, Gijsbers ACJ, Schuurs-Hoeijmakers JHM, van Haeringen A, Fransen van de Putte DE, Anderlid B-M, … Ruivenkamp CAL (2009). Extending the phenotype of recurrent rearrangements of 16p11.2: Deletions in mentally retarded patients without autism and in normal individuals. European Journal of Medical Genetics, 52(2–3), 77–87. 10.1016/j.ejmg.2009.03.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop DV (2003). The Children’s Communication Checklist: CCC-2. San Antonio, TX: ASHA. [Google Scholar]
- Bishop DV, Snowling MJ, Thompson PA, & Greenhalgh T (2016). CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study. Identifying language impairments in children. PLoS One, 11(7), e0158753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop DV, Snowling MJ, Thompson PA, Greenhalgh T, Consortium C-2, Adams C, … Bellair J (2017). Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(10), 1068–1080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop SL, Farmer C, & Thurm A (2015). Measurement of nonverbal IQ in autism spectrum disorder: Scores in young adulthood compared to early childhood. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(4), 966–974. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrow-Woolfolk E (1999). CASL: Comprehensive assessment of spoken language, Torrance, CA: American Guidance Service. [Google Scholar]
- Condouris K, Meyer E, & Tager-Flusberg H (2003). The relationship between standardized measures of language and measures of spontaneous speech in children with autism. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12, 349–358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Consortium SV (2012). Simons variation in individuals project (Simons VIP): A genetics-first approach to studying autism spectrum and related neurodevelopmental disorders. Neuron, 73(6), 1063–1067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott CD (2007). Differential ability scales. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment; Retrieved from http://www.myschoolpsychology.com/wp-content/uploads/2014/02/Differential-Ability-Scales.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Fedorenko E, Morgan A, Murray E, Cardinaux A, Mei C, Tager-Flusberg H, Fisher SE, Kanwisher N (2016). A highly penetrant form of childhood apraxia of speech due to deletion of 16p11.2. European Journal of Human Genetics, 24(2), 302–306. 10.1038/ejhg.2015.149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez BA, Roberts W, Chung B, Weksberg R, Meyn S, Szatmari P, Scherer SW (2010). Phenotypic spectrum associated with de novo and inherited deletions and duplications at 16p11.2 in individuals ascertained for diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Medical Genetics, 47, (3), 195–203. 10.1136/jmg.2009.069369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feuk L, Kalervo A, Lipsanen-Nyman M, Skaug J, Nakabayashi K, Finucane B, … Hannula-Jouppi K (2006). Absence of a paternally inherited FOXP2 gene in developmental verbal dyspraxia. American Journal of Human Genetics, 79(5), 965–972. 10.1086/508902 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finestack LH, & Abbeduto L (2010). Expressive language profiles of verbally expressive adolescents and young adults with down syndrome or fragile X syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(5), 1334–1348. 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0125 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Folstein SE, & Mankoski RE (2000). Chromosome 7q: Where autism meets language disorder? American Journal of Human Genetics, 67(2), 278–281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghebranious N, Giampietro PF, Wesbrook FP, & Rezkalla SH (2007). A novel microdeletion at 16p11.2 harbors candidate genes for aortic valve development, seizure disorder, and mild mental retardation. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A, 143A(13), 1462–1471. 10.1002/ajmg.a.31837 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Girirajan S, Johnson RL, Tassone F, Balciuniene J, Katiyar N, Fox K, … Selleck SB (2013). Global increases in both common and rare copy number load associated with autism. Human Molecular Genetics, 22(14), 2870–2880. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green-Snyder L, D’Angelo D, Chen Q, Bernier R, Goin-Kochel RP, Wallace AS, … S. V Consortium. (2016). Autism spectrum disorder, developmental and psychiatric features in 16p11.2 duplication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(8), 2734–2748. 10.1007/s10803-016-2807-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanson E, Bernier R, Porche K, Jackson FI, Goin-Kochel RP, Snyder LG, … K, W. (2015). The cognitive and behavioral phenotype of the 16p11.2 deletion in a clinically ascertained population. Biological Psychiatry, 77(9), 785–793. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.04.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanson E, Nasir RH, Fong A, Lian A, Hundley R, Shen Y, … Miller DT (2010). Cognitive and behavioral characterization of 16p11.2 deletion syndrome. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 31(8), 649–657. 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181ea50ed [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hippolyte L, Maillard AM, Rodriguez-Herreros B, Pain A, Martin-Brevet S, Ferrari C, … Metspalu A (2016). The number of genomic copies at the 16p11. 2 locus modulates language, verbal memory, and inhibition. Biological Psychiatry, 80(2), 129–139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hus V, Gotham K, & Lord C (2014). Standardizing ADOS domain scores: Separating severity of social affect and restricted and repetitive behaviors. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(10), 2400–2412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim SH, Junker D, & Lord C (2014). Observation of spontaneous expressive language (OSEL): A new measure for spontaneous and expressive language of children with autism spectrum disorders and other communication disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(12), 3230–3244. 10.1007/s10803-014-2180-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim SH, Paul R, Tager-Flusberg H, Lord C, Volkmar FR, Paul R, … Pelphrey KA (2014). Language and communication in autism. In Hand-book of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (4th ed. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc; Retrieved from 10.1002/9781118911389.hautc10/abstract [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar RA, & Christian SL (2009). Genetics of autism spectrum disorders. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports, 9(3), 188–197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar RA, Marshall CR, Badner JA, Babatz TD, Mukamel Z, Aldinger KA, … Christian SL (2009). Association and mutation analyses of 16p11. 2 autism candidate genes. PLoS One, 4(2), e4582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laws G, & Bishop DVM (2003). A comparison of language abilities in adolescents with down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research; Rockville, 46(6), 1324–1339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le Couteur A, Lord C, & Rutter M (2003). The autism diagnostic interview-revised (ADI-R), Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
- Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore P, Risi S, Gotham K, & Bishop S (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: ADOS-2, Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
- Marshall CR, Noor A, Vincent JB, Lionel AC, Feuk L, Skaug J, … Scherer SW (2008). Structural variation of chromosomes in autism spectrum disorder. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 82(2), 477–488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy SE, Makarov V, Kirov G, Addington AM, McClellan J, Yoon S, … Sebat J (2009). Microduplications of 16p11.2 are associated with schizophrenia. Nature Genetics, 41(11), 1223–1227. 10.1038/ng.474 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller JF (1999). Profiles of language development in children with down syndrome. In Improving the communication of people with down syndrome (pp. 11–39). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Pub. [Google Scholar]
- Mullen EM (1995). Mullen scales of early learning, Circle Pines, MN: AGS. [Google Scholar]
- Pinborough-Zimmerman J, Satterfield R, Miller J, Bilder D, Hossain S, McMahon W (2007). Communication Disorders: Prevalence and Comorbid Intellectual Disability, Autism, and Emotional/Behavioral Disorders. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 16(4), 359–367. 10.1044/1058-0360(2007/039). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Philofsky A, Hepburn SL, Hayes A, Hagerman R, & Rogers SJ (2004). Linguistic and cognitive functioning and autism symptoms in young children with fragile X syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retardation: AJMR, 109(3), 208–218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice ML, Warren SF, & Betz SK (2005). Language symptoms of developmental language disorders: An overview of autism, down syndrome, fragile X, specific language impairment, and Williams syndrome. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 26(1), 7–27. 10.1017/S0142716405050034 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sanders SJ, Ercan-Sencicek AG, Hus V, Luo R, Murtha MT, Moreno-De-Luca D, … State MW (2011). Multiple recurrent de novo CNVs, including duplications of the 7q11. 23 Williams syndrome region, are strongly associated with autism. Neuron, 70(5), 863–885. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sebat J, Lakshmi B, Malhotra D, Troge J, Lese-Martin C, Walsh T, … Wigler M (2007). Strong association of de novo copy number mutations with autism. Science, 316(5823), 445–449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaffer D, Fisher P, Lucas CP, Dulcan MK, & Schwab-Stone ME (2000). NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): Description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(1), 28–38. 10.1097/00004583-200001000-00014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shinawi M, Liu P, Kang S-HL, Shen J, Belmont JW, Scott DA, … Lupski JR (2010). Recurrent reciprocal 16p11.2 rearrangements associated with global developmental delay, behavioural problems, dysmorphism, epilepsy, and abnormal head size. Journal of Medical Genetics, 47(5), 332–341. 10.1136/jmg.2009.073015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shriberg LD, Ballard KJ, Tomblin JB, Duffy JR, Odell KH, & Williams CA (2006). Speech, prosody, and voice characteristics of a mother and daughter with a 7;13 translocation affecting FOXP2. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 49(3), 500–525. 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/038) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sparrow SS, Balla DA, & Cicchetti DV (2005). Vineland-II adaptive behavior scales, Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Tager-Flusberg H, Rogers S, Cooper J, Landa R, Lord C, Paul R, … Yoder P (2009). Defining spoken language benchmarks and selecting measures of expressive language development for young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(3), 643–652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vernes SC, Newbury DF, Abrahams BS, Winchester L, Nicod J, Groszer M, … Fisher SE (2008). A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders. New England Journal of Medicine, 359(22), 2337–2345. 10.1056/NEJMoa0802828 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh KM, & Bracken MB (2011). Copy number variation in the dosage-sensitive 16p11.2 interval accounts for only a small proportion of autism incidence: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Genetics in Medicine: Official Journal of the American College of Medical Genetics, 13(5), 377–384. 10.1097/GIM.0b013e3182076c0c [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss LA, Shen Y, Korn JM, Arking DE, Miller DT, Fossdal R, … Daly MJ (2008). Association between microdeletion and microduplication at 16p11.2 and autism. New England Journal of Medicine, 358(7), 667–675. 10.1056/NEJMoa075974 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zufferey F, Sherr EH, Beckmann ND, Hanson E, Maillard AM, Hippolyte L, … Roberts TP (2012). A 600 kb deletion syndrome at 16p11.2 leads to energy imbalance and neuropsychiatric disorders. Journal of Medical Genetics, 49(10), 660–668. 10.1136/jmedgenet-2012-101203 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.