Abstract
U.S. Latinos have disproportionately high rates of diet-related diseases which are associated with acculturation to the US. This negative shift in dietary quality is paradoxical in light of gains in income and education that would be expected to lead to better diet. We examined the extent to which the dietary acculturation paradox among Mexican Americans can be explained by segmented assimilation, a theory that considers how immigrants’ and their descendants’ trajectories of integration are influenced by a complex interplay of individual, social, and structural factors. First, we performed confirmatory cluster analysis to identify three assimilation segments (classic, underclass, and selective) based on education, income, and an acculturation proxy derived from language, nativity, and time in the U.S. among Mexican-origin participants (N=4,475) of the 2007–2016 National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES). These segments were then used as independent variables in linear regression models to estimate the relationship between cluster and dietary quality (assessed by the Health Eating Index (HEI)) and the interaction between cluster and gender, controlling for marital status. There were strong effects of cluster on dietary quality, consistent with hypotheses per segmented assimilation theory. The classic assimilation segment had the poorest diet, despite having higher income and education than the underclass segment. The selective segment had higher or similar dietary quality to the underclass segment. Consistent with expectations, this difference was driven by the relatively higher consumption of greens and beans and whole grains of those in the selective and underclass segments. Overall, women had better diets than men; however, the strongest contrast was in the underclass segment. This study advances understanding of dietary acculturation and potential disparities in diet-related health outcomes.
Keywords: acculturation, dietary acculturation paradox, Healthy Eating Index, Hispanic, obesity
1. INTRODUCTION
Latinos in the United States – the largest ethnic group in the country, numbering 60.6 million1 – have disproportionately high rates of diet-related conditions and diseases, including obesity and diabetes2,3. Risk for poor diet and obesity may be a function of economic and culturally-based dietary patterns: for example, Latinos have lower incomes and double the rates of food insecurity of non-Latino Whites4. In addition, studies of dietary patterns have found that while immigrants initially eat a diet considered healthful – rich in fruits and vegetables, high in fiber, and low in saturated fat, as they become more acculturated to mainstream US culture, they adopt a less healthful, Standard American Diet, characterized by fewer fruits and vegetables, less fiber, and more saturated fat5–10. Other studies have shown that increasing acculturation is associated with decreasing consumption of ethnic foods and increasing consumption of fats and sugars or other unhealthful nutrients11–13. Bicultural second- and third-generation Latinos are more likely to suffer from obesity and diet-related disease compared with their immigrant parents and grandparents14,15. Yet immigrant, first-generation Latinas are at higher risk of poverty than either second-generation Latinas or White non-Latina women. Thus, the negative shift in dietary quality is paradoxical in light of gains in income and education, which, among other populations, typically increase diet quality9,16–18. This paradoxical pattern is most consistently seen in Mexican American populations, who also suffer from higher rates of obesity and diabetes compared with other ethnic groups including other Latino subgroups19.
1.1. Dietary Acculturation Paradox
Dubbed the acculturation paradox, this phenomenon has been the subject of debate. On the one hand, numerous researchers have questioned the existence of such a paradox on the basis of inconsistent findings across health behaviors and health outcomes16,20–23. Evidence for the paradox has been documented in diet-related health outcomes as well as in mortality and birthing outcomes22,24–26. Yet some have noted methodological problems with epidemiological studies reporting such a so-called paradox, with arguments that the findings are attributable to poor measurement of the underlying construct of acculturation27. Another critique is that the mortality paradox observed among immigrants may be partially explained by the “salmon bias hypothesis,” suggesting that the longer-than-expected lifespans for immigrants to the U.S. is due to the immigrants representing the healthiest, fittest members of the sending countries24,28–30.
Nevertheless, recent studies employing diverse methods and samples have suggested that cultural understanding of food and dietary behaviors – and how these behaviors evolve through the process of acculturation, and throughout the life course – may help to explain the observed (paradoxical) epidemiological findings with respect to dietary behaviors. For example, in one study, second- and third-generation Mexican American women perceived “American” foods as generally more healthful than traditional Mexican foods31; this misperception could account at least in part for the negative dietary shift observed across immigrant generations. Other work suggests that structural factors that facilitate or impede integration into mainstream society are responsible for immigrants’ worse health32. In a qualitative study with Dominican immigrant women in New York City, for example, researchers found that dietary behaviors such as consuming non-traditional foods are due to modest material conditions or lack of access to traditional foods in their new environment33. These findings raise questions about what it means to integrate, as well as what dietary acculturation is, and how dietary acculturation might differ across Latino subgroups.
1.2. Segmented Assimilation
An approach to clarifying the confusing pattern of studies observing a dietary acculturation paradox may be segmented assimilation, a sociological framework that examines how immigrants’ and their descendants’ trajectories of integration are influenced by a complex interplay of individual, social, and structural factors. Segmented assimilation theory34 identifies three distinct patterns of integration: classic assimilation (the adoption of mainstream values and behaviors and rejection of original culture); underclass acculturation (poverty, low educational attainment, maintenance of original culture); and selective acculturation (retention of ethnic values along with economic and educational advancement). In the case of diet, income and education are negatively associated with dietary quality in the general population35–37. Because Latinos in the US are both more likely to live in poverty38 and to have low educational attainment39 compared with non-Latino whites, we would expect that they should have poorer dietary quality as well. Yet these patterns have been inconsistently observed, leading to the “dietary paradox”. But if we consider the distinct trajectories of cultural integration, we may observe more consistency in the relationship between dietary quality and acculturation. Those who “make it”—achieve a college education or a middle-class income by completely assimilating to mainstream U.S. culture—may also lose what could be considered the protective component of their culture of origin. In dietary terms, that would be indicated by the adoption of the poor-quality Standard American Diet10. But those who achieve a cultural balance (e.g., accepting some parts of U.S. mainstream culture while retaining cultural traditions and a strong sense of ethnic identity and pride) may be protected from adopting the Standard American Diet by retaining more of the dietary patterns from that culture of origin.
Although there is a large literature on segmented assimilation in the context of educational, economic, and political trajectories, relatively few studies to date have used the theory to examine health-related outcomes. One study using data from the National Latino and Asian American Survey found partial support for the pattern of segmented assimilation and this was associated with obesity among Latinos40. Another study found that children of Mexican immigrants demonstrate more dietary assimilation outside the home (i.e., schools and restaurants), but the relative healthfulness of the food depended on the location41. Recently, researchers seeking to move beyond culture-based explanations for the high rates of obesity among Latinos compared with white Americans argued that acculturation and socioeconomic status operate as dual streams of influence on the risk of obesity42. Using a conceptual framework that includes both socioeconomic status and an acculturation index, the researchers found that higher socioeconomic status was negatively associated with weight gain while acculturation was positively associated with weight gain. Further, they found that gender is an important modifier of acculturation effects, such that acculturation was a greater risk for obesity among men. Although this study informs a more complex understanding of acculturation and diet-related outcomes, the mechanisms through which acculturation influenced weight gain were not tested. Thus, the specific dietary changes that might reflect dietary acculturation differentially across assimilation groups remain unclear.
Other studies have not explicitly examined segmented assimilation but have tested or found evidence that can be considered preliminary support for components of the theory. For example, one study in South Texas found evidence that less-acculturated Mexican Americans had healthier diets compared with more-acculturated (bilingual and English-monolingual) Mexican Americans; this pattern was true across two proxy indicators of acculturation, language preference and generation43. In a study with Hispanic/Latino youth44, researchers distinguished between those with a bicultural orientation – equally preferring US and culture of origin – were considered “integrated”; those with a high US and low Latino orientation, “assimilated”; and those with a low US and high Latino orientation, “separated”. While there was no consistent pattern between dietary quality and acculturative category, less-acculturated youth – as defined by two distinct proxies, generation and language preference – had better dietary quality than more-acculturated youth44. That study controlled for family income but did not examine how income might moderate the effects of acculturation on diet, or how income (as a proxy for the greater construct of socioeconomic incorporation) might factor into assimilation trajectory.
Similarly, in a study conducted in a US-Mexico border city, female Mexican migrants with low socioeconomic position were more likely to adopt the low-quality Standard American Diet than those with higher levels of income and education45. The women with low socioeconomic position migrated having already developed poor dietary habits as a function of their social position and associated access to healthier foods and ways of eating both in Mexico and in the United States45. Such findings would reject the “salmon bias” hypothesis for the acculturation paradox. In another study examining Latinos’ use of nutrition labels using population-based data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, results suggested some evidence for segmented assimilation: Having a low income had a negative effect on English-speaking Latinos’ nutrition label use, whereas less-acculturated (Spanish-speaking) Latinos’ label use did not significantly decrease (from the relatively high use rate of about 80%) with poverty46. This pattern of effects was replicated with dietary quality as the outcome46. These results suggest that having low income is detrimental for those with behavioral acculturation (i.e., acquisition of English language), but less significant for those who do not acculturate (i.e., retain native language).
Together, these studies are consistent with the notion that the segmented assimilation framework can shed light on the seemingly paradoxical findings in the relationship between acculturation and dietary behaviors. Missing from the literature, however, is an explicit test of the theory in the context of dietary quality. Thus, we sought to understand the relationships between socioeconomic, acculturation factors, and dietary quality.
1.3. Present Study
The present study examined the extent to which the dietary acculturation paradox among Mexican Americans can be explained by segmented assimilation. Consistent with Florez and Abraído-Lanza’s40 approach to examine segmented assimilation and obesity, we used cluster analysis to characterize three clusters of participants in terms of acculturation and socioeconomic status as predicted by segmented assimilation theory: classic assimilation (adoption of mainstream values and rejection of original culture, indicated by English dominance and relatively high levels of income and education); selective acculturation (retention of ethnic values along with economic and educational advancement, indicated by Spanish and English bilingualism and high levels of income and education); and underclass acculturation (retention of ethnic values and behaviors and lack of socioeconomic advancement, indicated by relatively high poverty, low educational attainment, and Spanish-language preference). Critically, segmented assimilation theory speaks to both inter- and intra-generational trajectories. That is, individuals who arrive to the United States from other countries may adapt to U.S. customs and culture over their lifetimes, and examination of the changes in their behaviors and would be consider their individual assimilation trajectory. Yet studying individual trajectories is impractical for most researchers, as it would require multiple data collection efforts over individuals’ lifetimes. Thus, assimilation research typically examines groups of individuals and compares changes in group-level attributes47. Consistent with that approach, in this study, we aggregate individual level data to create clusters of individuals who have similar patterns of assimilation. We hypothesized that those following the classic assimilation path would have the worst diet – this is the group who, in moving away from the presumed healthier dietary customs of their origin cultures, adopt the Standard American Diet. Those in the selective acculturation group were expected to have the best diets, as they benefit from the protective effects of high incomes, education, as well as maintenance of some cultural behaviors including adherence to traditional Mexican diets high in the foods recommended to achieve nutrient adequacy (i.e., greens, beans, whole grains). Those who appear to have the least integration to U.S. society –the underclass pattern – were expected to have good diets, somewhat protected by their adherence to traditional Mexican diets, but with some residual negative effects of poverty that are not completely ameliorated by low acculturation.
A secondary goal was to explore gender differences in cluster effects on dietary quality. Research suggests that Latinas are more likely to consumer a healthier diet than Latinos. The positive effects of marriage and caregiving responsibilities on women’s dietary behaviors may be explained in part by cultural norms, for example, having to eat less in order to ensure their family members have enough, or not having access to animal protein to the same extent as men—behaviors that nutritionists define as healthier, even if they are experienced as deprivations48. However, there is also evidence suggesting that the family context – specifically, the presence of a male figure in the household – is positively associated with weight gain behaviors48. However, men in the selective acculturation cluster may be more inclined to adopt less traditional gender-constrained roles in the household and therefore take a more active role in the family diet. Yet existing work has examined only main effects of gender, making it unclear whether the combined effects of assimilation path would show a similar or varying pattern with gender. Thus, we hypothesized that gender would moderate the effects of acculturation on diet such that gender differences would be more pronounced in the classic assimilation and underclass clusters and less pronounced in the selective. Specifically, we expected that the negative effects of classic and underclass assimilation on dietary quality would be more pronounced for men than for women.
2. METHODS
2.1. Sample Design and Data Collection
We used data from five National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) waves between 2007 and 2016. NHANES is a nationally representative study of the civilian, non-institutionalized U.S. population conducted by the National Center for Health Statistics. Given the diversity of cultures subsumed within the panethnic labels “Hispanic” and “Latino”49, the centrality of foods and dietary patterns in cultural identity-making31, and evidence of increased dietary health risks among Mexican-origin Latinos49, we limited the sample to Mexican Americans. The final sample thus consisted of self-identified Mexican origin adults, ages 20–80 (N=4475; see Table 1). The complex, multistage, probability sampling scheme and estimation procedures are described in CDC publications50,51; analyses were adjusted using the provided weights and strata.
Table 1:
Sample demographics of Mexican American participants in the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES), 2007–2016, adjusted for sampling cluster and strata.
N | Mean or Percent | SE | |
---|---|---|---|
| |||
Female | 2775 | 47.85 | 0.73 |
Age, mean years | 5366 | 40.77 | 0.43 |
Married | 2967 | 53.82 | 1.46 |
Poverty Income Ratio | 4673 | 1.84 | 0.05 |
Educational Attainment | |||
< 9th grade | 1833 | 28.18 | 1.00 |
9–11th grade | 1075 | 21.47 | 0.79 |
High school graduate or equivalent | 1029 | 20.80 | 0.78 |
Some college | 1011 | 21.04 | 0.97 |
College graduate | 408 | 8.51 | 0.61 |
Country of Birth | |||
US | 3199 | 58.12 | 1.94 |
Mexico | 2154 | 41.88 | 1.94 |
Language Spoken at Home | |||
Spanish only | 2267 | 39.86 | 1.42 |
Mostly Spanish | 817 | 15.58 | 0.98 |
Equal Spanish/English | 765 | 14.18 | 0.84 |
Mostly English | 742 | 14.66 | 1.20 |
English only | 767 | 15.73 | 1.17 |
Assimilation Cluster | |||
Underclass | 2465 | 55.08 | |
Selective | 969 | 21.65 | |
Classic | 1041 | 23.26 | |
Healthy Eating Index | |||
Total Score | 3465 | 51.42 | 0.53 |
Adequacy Subscore | 3465 | 29.31 | 0.30 |
Moderation Subscore | 3465 | 22.11 | 0.25 |
2.2. Measures
The methods and measures are described in greater detail by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention52.
2.2.1. Segmented Assimilation Cluster Components
The following measures of acculturation, income, and education were used in a confirmatory cluster analysis, where cluster assignment was then used as an independent variable in models examining dietary quality.
Acculturation.
To construct the measure of acculturation we first created a measure of the individual’s immigration status based on the NHANES variable dmdyrsus, which is an ordinal variable indicating how many years a foreign-born individual has lived in the US. This variable ranges from 1, indicating that the individual has lived in the US less than 1 year, to 9, indicating that the individual has lived in the US for 50 or more years. We adjusted this variable for age by dividing by age and multiplying by 100 to obtain a scalar variable that functions as a proxy to the percentage of an individual’s life that they have lived in the US. For example, the lowest observed value of the scalar variable (1.25) was for an 80-year-old individual who had lived in the US for less than 1 year (dmdyrsus = 1). The highest value observed (28.57) was a 21-year-old individual who had lived in the US for at least 20 years (dmdyrsus = 6). This scalar variable was then discretized as 0 (a value less than or equal to 7; 29.16% of sample), 1 (a value greater than 7 and less than or equal to 12; 23.06% of sample), or 2 (a value greater than 12; 25.79% of sample). Individuals born in the US (NHANES variable dmdborn4 = 1) were given the value 3 (21.99% of sample). This final ordinal variable, ranging from 0 to 3, was then added to the NHANES variable acd040, which is a 5-category ordinal variable ranging from 1 to 5 indicating increasing use of English at home (value 1 for those speaking only Spanish at home and 5 for those speaking only English). The final acculturation variable ranged in value from 1 to 8, where 1 indicates low acculturation (speaking only Spanish and living a small proportion of one’s life in the US) and where 8 indicates high acculturation (speaking only English and living a large proportion of one’s life in the US). No other language-based proxy measure is available in NHANES.
Income.
We used the Poverty to Income Ratio (PIR), an index for the ratio of family income to poverty that ranges from 0 to 5. This measure is based on the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services’ (HHS) poverty guidelines, which are issued each year and determine financial eligibility for federal assistance programs including Head Start, Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program (SNAP), Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children (WIC), and the National School Lunch Program. A value of 1.3 or lower determines eligibility for SNAP. PIR was calculated by dividing family income by the poverty level specified by HHS guidelines, adjusted for family size, as well as the appropriate year and state.
Education.
We used the highest grade or level of education completed by adults 20 years and older. The five response categories were: less than 9th grade education, 9–11th grade education (included some 12th grade and no diploma), High school graduate/GED, some college or associates (AA) degree, and college graduate or higher.
2.2.2. Cluster Analysis and Assignment
We performed confirmatory cluster analysis on the variables acculturation, education, and income, as described above. In accord with segmented assimilation theory (SAT), we sought to confirm 3 clusters that were consistent with conceptual framework proposed by SAT in their education, income, and acculturation values. We used a k-means cluster procedure, which involved a disjoint cluster analysis based on Euclidean distances, with least squares estimation. Once each individual was assigned to a cluster, we used the cluster assignment as an independent variable in the linear models described below in section 2.3.
2.2.3. Participant Demographics
Gender.
We used a binary variable where 1 indicates male and 2 indicates female.
Marital Status.
We controlled for marital status, dichotomized as married (1) or non-married (0), because prior research has shown that marriage is negatively associated with diet48.
2.2.4. Dietary Quality
The Healthy Eating Index (HEI)-2010 is an overall measure of dietary quality, specifically assessing adherence to the Dietary Guidelines for Americans53. The HEI incorporates foods and nutrients that should be consumed to ensure a nutrient-adequate diet, and those that should be limited or consumed in moderation for chronic disease prevention. Dietary intake data were taken from two 24-hour dietary recalls. Dietary quality was assessed using an average of the two dietary recalls for each individual to calculate HEI scores.
HEI Total Score.
The HEI score is calculated as a summary of 12 components, 9 of which assess adequacy of the diet, including 1) total fruit; 2) whole fruit; 3) total vegetables; 4) greens and beans; 5) whole grains; 6) dairy; 7) total protein foods; 8) seafood and plant proteins; and 9) fatty acids. The remaining 3 components – refined grains, sodium, and empty calories (i.e., energy from solid fats, alcohol, and added sugars (SOFAAS)) – assess dietary components that are recommended to be consumed in moderation. Higher scores reflect better diet quality for all components and for the total because lower intakes are scored higher for the moderation components. The scores of the 12 components are summed to yield a total score with a maximum value of 100.
HEI and Adequacy and Moderation Components.
In addition to the overall HEI score, we examined adequacy and moderation components. Specifically, we were interested in whether differences in overall dietary quality were more attributable to lower consumption of healthier foods – those whose consumption is encouraged, including fruit, vegetables, whole grains, dairy, protein, and fatty acids – or to higher consumption of less-healthy foods – those whose consumption is discouraged, including refined grains, sodium, and empty calories (solid fats, alcohol, and added sugars). The adequacy score was calculated by summing the nine individual adequacy component scores (maximum 60), and the moderation score was calculated by summing the three moderation component scores (maximum 40). Further details and examples of the subcomponents can be found at the National Cancer Institute HEI website54.
2.3. Statistical Analysis
All statistical analyses were performed using SAS® version 9.4 for Windows®. The SAS macro provided by the National Cancer Institute was used to create the HEI scores from the dietary recall data55. The FASTCLUS procedure was used for the confirmatory cluster analysis. The Wilcoxon rank sum test, ANOVA, and chi square tests were used to examine differences between the clusters for ordinal, continuous, and categorical variables, respectively. The SURVEYREG procedure was used to fit linear models to test for cluster effects and their interaction with gender on dietary quality as measured by the HEI scores, while controlling for marital status, using the strata and weights provided in the NHANES data set to adjust for the complex sampling design. Normality was assessed using histograms, as well as diagnostic plots from the linear regressions and ANOVA models. A p-value of less than 0.05 was considered significant in all analyses. Multiple comparisons within a linear model were adjusted using the Tukey-Kramer adjustment.
3. RESULTS
The identified clusters conformed well to segmented assimilation theory in terms of average education level, income, and acculturation score, and there were significant differences in total HEI, the adequacy component, and the moderation component across clusters (see Table 2). Women had better diets than men overall, and gender by segment interactions were significant for the total HEI and the adequacy scores but not for the moderation scores. Marital status had significant effects in all models.
Table 2:
Cluster Variables and Demographics. Data are reported as medians and IQRs, unless otherwise noted as percent (%).
Underclass (N=2465) |
Selective (N=969) |
Classic (N=1041) |
p-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
Education 1 | 1.0 (1.0) | 4.0 (2.0) | 3.0 (2.0) | <.001a |
Poverty to Income Ratio 2 | 1.1 (0.9) | 3.7 (2.4) | 1.3 (1.3) | <.001b |
Acculturation | 2.0 (2.0) | 5.0 (1.0) | 7.0 (3.0) | <.001a |
Female % | 50.1 | 51.2 | 54.8 | .041c |
Married % | 43.4 | 37.4 | 53.2 | <.001c |
Wilcoxon rank sum test
ANOVA
Chi square test
Education was a 5-category variable, where 1 is less than ninth grade, 3 is high school or GED or equivalent, and 4 two-year college degree.
Poverty to Income Ratio: Ratio of family income to poverty level. 1.1 means 10% above poverty level. 3.7 is 370% of poverty level.
Acculturation ranges from 1–8 and is a combination of country of birth, time in the US, and language spoken.
3.1. Cluster Analysis
Cluster analysis yielded three clusters that were well-separated and well-defined. The clusters conformed to segmented assimilation theory, consisting of a classic assimilation pattern, an underclass pattern, and a selective pattern as hypothesized. As expected, the selective segment was characterized by having higher education and income levels compared with the classic segment. Table 2 shows the medians and interquartile range for the 3 cluster variables and percentages for gender and marital status for each cluster.
3.2. Associations between clusters and diet quality
Healthy Eating Index (HEI) Total
When considering only the main effects, the classic segment had significantly lower diet quality (mean=49.1 (standard error=0.37)) than both the selective (52.8 (0.39), p<.001) and the underclass (52.5 (0.36), p<.001) segments, but selective and underclass segments were similar (p=.85). In general, women eat better diets than men (53.4 (0.31) vs 49.5 (0.5931), p<.001) Data are shown in Appendix.
After adjusting for sex, marital status, and the interactions between sex and cluster, the effect of assimilation pattern on dietary quality differed by sex (interaction p-value=.002) (Tables 3, 4). Underclass females had better diet (55.2 (0.42)) than underclass males (49.9 (0.40), p<.001), as did women in the classic segment (51.5 (0.47) vs 47.0 (0.48), p<.001). In the selective assimilation segment, women and men had similar diets (53.9 (0.52) vs 51.7 (0.59), p=0.07). Men in the classic assimilation segment had the lowest HEI scores (47.04 (0.48)) while underclass women had the highest (55.20 (.42), p-value<.001). Being married was associated with better diet quality (52.2 (0.34) vs 50.8 (0.27), p<.001).
Table 3:
Diet quality as assessed by Healthy Eating Index (HEI), overall and by gender and assimilation cluster adjusted means (standard errors) and p-values for comparison between genders within assimilation cluster
Overall |
Women |
Men |
P-value | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | HEI Mean (SE) | N | HEI Mean (SE) | N | HEI Mean (SE) | ||
|
|
|
|||||
Selective | 969 | 52.8 (0.39) | 496 | 53.9 (0.52) | 473 | 51.7 (.59) | 0.07 |
Classic | 1041 | 49.1 (0.37) | 570 | 51.3 (0.48) | 471 | 47.0 (0.48) | <.001 |
Underclass | 2465 | 52.5 (0.36) | 1235 | 55.2 (0.42) | 1230 | 49.9 (0.40) | <.001 |
Overall | 4475 | 52.3 (0.20) | 2301 | 53.4 (0.31) | 2174 | 49.5 (0.31) | <.001 |
All pairwise comparisons with p-values are shown in the appendix.
Table 4:
Selected components of the Healthy Eating Index (HEI) Adequacy and Moderation Sub-Scale, by assimilation cluster and gender, adjusted means (standard errors)
Men | Women | P-value | |
---|---|---|---|
| |||
Adequacy Sub-Scale | 27.58 (0.28) | 30.81 (0.22) | <.001 |
Classic | 25.6 (0.42) | 29.3 (0.37) | <.001 |
Selective | 29.2 (0.52) | 31.3 (0.27) | .007 |
Underclass | 27.9 (0.23) | 31.9 (0.27) | <.001 |
Greens and Beans | |||
Classic | 1.51 (0.07) | 1.84 (0.07) | <.001 |
Selective | 1.87 (0.07) | 2.2 (0.07) | <.001 |
Underclass | 2.20 (0.05) | 2.23 (0.05) | 0.99 |
Interaction | 0.002 | ||
Whole Grain | |||
Classic | 1.92 (0.08) | 2.56 (0.09) | <.001 |
Selective | 2.29 (0.08) | 2.45 (0.09) | 0.45 |
Underclass | 1.13 (0.04) | 2.05 (0.06) | <.001 |
Interaction | <.001 | ||
| |||
Moderation Sub-Scale | 21.95 (0.21) | 22.62 (0.18) | 0.013 |
Classic | 21.4 (0.34) | 22.0 (0.33) | 0.784 |
Selective | 22.5 (0.38) | 22.6 (0.30) | >.99 |
Underclass | 22.0 (0.24) | 23.3 (0.22) | <.001 |
Solid fats, alcohol, and added sugars (SOFAAS) | 13.27 (0.17) | 13.83 (0.12) | <.001 |
Classic | 12.5 (0.20) | 13.11 (0.19) | 0.11 |
Selective | 13.46 (0.30) | 13.40 (0.17) | 1.000 |
Underclass | 13.87 (0.25) | 15.00 (0.21) | <.001 |
Interaction | 0.006 |
3.2.1. HEI Adequacy and Moderation Components
3.2.1.1. Adequacy Scores
For the unadjusted effects of cluster for the adequacy component, the selective cluster had the highest mean adequacy score (30.3 (0.35)), but was similar (p-value=.41) to underclass (29.9 (0.20)), with both being significantly better than classic (27.4 (0.35), p-values both <.001)). Results shown in the Appendix.
In the adjusted model accounting for interaction effects, consistent with overall HEI results and as hypothesized, there were significant gender by cluster interactions (p-value=.0087, Table 4), with the highest mean adequacy scores observed in the underclass female group and the worst mean scores in the classic male group (p-value < .001). Females scored higher than males in all clusters (all p-values < .01). In the selective cluster, males and females were not significantly different from their counterparts in underclass cluster (p-values = .11 and .75, respectively).
3.2.1.2. Moderation Scores
In unadjusted analyses, the Underclass (22.6 (0.21)) and Selective clusters (22.5 (0.24)) had similar moderation scores (p-value=.98), and both had better dietary quality than the classic (21.7 (0.25)), p-values =.024, .032, respectively). In the adjusted model, gender did not modify the association between cluster and moderation score (p-value=.07). Marital status did not affect moderation scores (p=0.86). Full results shown in the Appendix.
3.2.2. HEI Adequacy and Moderation Sub-Scores
3.2.2.1. Adequacy Sub-scores
To better understand what might be driving the observed effects in the adequacy component, we examined greens and beans and whole grains adequacy sub-scores (Table 4). We selected these components specifically because they account for significant portion of what is considered the traditional healthful Mexican diet56. The strongest moderation effects of sex on assimilation cluster appeared to occur for the greens and beans and whole grain sub-scores (p-values .014 and <.001, respectively). Men and women differed significantly in classic and selective clusters but were similar in the underclass for greens and beans. For whole grain, in the selective cluster men and women were similar while differing significantly in the classic and underclass.
3.2.2.2. Moderation Sub-Scores
To test what might be driving the differences in the moderation score between the clusters, we further examined the SOFAAS component of the moderation sub-score (Table 4). There were significant differences between all clusters in dietary intake of SOFAAS. The underclass cluster had the highest score of 14.43 (0.21), with the selective next (13.43 (0.19)), and the classic having the lowest (12.80 (0.15)). Women had a higher average score than men (13.83 (0.12) vs 13.27 (0.17), p-value<.001) and married had higher scores than single (13.87 (0.15) vs 13.24 (0.13), p-value<.001). Gender significantly moderated the effect of cluster (p-value = .006), with underclass women having significantly higher SOFAAS score than men (15.00 (0.21) vs 13.87 (0.25), p-value <.001), with the classic and selective segments showing no significant gender differences (deltas=0.61 and −0.052; p-values = 0.12 and >.99, respectively). Complete results are shown in the Appendix.
4. DISCUSSION
The main study findings provide support for the hypothesized assimilation clusters and a segmented assimilation approach to understand the paradoxical literature on dietary quality and acculturation among Mexican Americans. As hypothesized, Mexican Americans who follow the classic assimilation path, gaining education and income by shedding their ethnic identity, had the worst diets. In contrast, both groups who retained aspects of their ethnic identity – the selective and underclass assimilation clusters – had better quality diets. In addition to the total HEI and the adequacy and moderation sub-scores reported in Section 3.2, we also examined specific sub-components and found a few cases that help to explain our results (Appendix). For example, those in the classic assimilation cluster have generally worse dietary quality due to their higher consumption of empty calories and lower consumption of greens and beans, fatty acids, and whole fruits. This pattern is consistent with expectations that those following the classic path to assimilation reject traditional dietary patterns and adopt the Standard American Diet. These results are consistent with prior studies of dietary behavior or diet-related outcomes and components of segmented assimilation 40,43–45,57, which in general have demonstrated that having low income is detrimental for those with behavioral (i.e., English-language acquisition) acculturation, while for those who do not acculturate, the strength of their culture (i.e., the retention of their native language) is a more powerful—and protective—force than economics. By formally identifying and testing distinct assimilation clusters as per segmented assimilation theory, this study advances understanding of how adaptations to new cultures necessarily implicates structural and social mechanisms and are not simply individual behavioral choices. In this way, results for this study of dietary behaviors are also consistent with studies employing segmented assimilation in other health contexts18,58,59.
We found strong gender effects on dietary quality: Latinas in the US, without regard to acculturation status, are more likely to consume a healthier diet than Latinos, perhaps because women in general tend to assume responsibility for caregiving and feeding in the family. Moreover, consistent with our hypotheses, we found a strong effect modification by gender, such that men in the underclass segment had the lowest quality diet of all groups, while the dietary quality of women and men in the selective cluster was not significantly different (HEI-2010 53.9 versus 51.7, p=.07).
This study thus advances theoretical understanding of mechanisms to explain changes in dietary behaviors in Mexican Americans, specifically providing evidence for segmented assimilation as a mechanism to partially explain prior paradoxical research results. But perhaps the greatest concern highlighted by our findings is that despite significant differences in dietary quality observed across assimilation and gender subgroups (HEI 47.0–55.2), no group achieved what is objectively considered a “healthy diet”: A diet meeting the Healthy People 2020 goals would require HEI of 74; while a score of 100 is the definition of achieving the Daily Guidelines for Americans60. And even the subgroup with the healthiest diets, Mexican American women in the Underclass segment, had worse dietary quality than the mean HEI for American adults (HEI 55.2 versus 58.3). Krebs-Smith and colleagues61 recommend “grading” HEI scores as a way of interpreting overall dietary quality; using their rubric, neither the general American population nor Mexican Americans specifically would earn a passing grade (>59). Even so, using Kirkpatrick and colleagues’62 guidelines to compare subgroup dietary quality scores, we observed meaningfully significant differences across Mexican American assimilation clusters, and between most clusters and the general American population mean. There is thus opportunity to decrease both disparities in dietary quality and to improve overall dietary quality in Americans across ethnicities.
4.1. Strengths and Limitations
A strength of this study is the use of one model for examining how the intersection of multiple sources of inequality contribute to health disparities32,63,64. Nonetheless, our conclusions are limited by the nature of the measures available. For example, consistent with prior studies including those relying on NHANES data, in this study we assumed that acculturation could be approximated by a set of proxy indicators. Additionally, the measure of income fails to account for geographic location, despite wide variation in cost-of-living. This particular limitation may have resulted in an underestimation of observed effects, since Latinos are concentrated in higher-cost-of-living regions65. While our study’s outcome measure, dietary quality, was assessed using the gold standard in measurement, the HEI based on 24-hour dietary recall, additional measures of dietary behavior available NHANES may further illuminate the complex relationships between acculturation and dietary quality. For example, future research might consider acculturation group differences in consumption of meals prepared outside of the home. As with all cross-sectional studies non-response bias is a potential problem and cause and effect cannot be inferred; hence associations may be spurious or confounded. The cross-sectional nature of this study also precludes the study of individual assimilation trajectories; studying individuals over time would allow for deeper and more nuanced understandings of the nature of assimilation and the ways in which these observed patterns influence dietary behaviors. Our focus in this study was the Mexican American population, for theoretical and practical reasons: We sought to constrain the cultural and dietary variability subsumed in the panethnic label “Latino” in order to test a theoretical model of segmented assimilation. Practically, Mexican Americans are the largest Latino subgroup in the United States, accounting for two-thirds of the Latino population, the “acculturation paradox” is most consistently seen in this group, and they consistently have the highest rates of obesity, diabetes, and other diet-related disease19. Nonetheless, it is critical to point out that Mexican Americans may not illustrate patterns for other Latino subgroups, and as such, future studies should examine the extent to which the segmented assimilation framework may explain observed disparities and paradoxical patterns in other Latino subgroups. Finally, we note that while some recent data suggest that diet quality is not improving for Mexican Americans and that income disparities in diet quality are worsening66, the notion of the “dietary acculturation paradox” may soon cease to be relevant: As Mexico and other Latin American countries experience the global nutrition transition67, risk of poor diet and related health outcomes affect those countries as much as U.S. Latinos68,69.
4.2. Conclusions
This study advances understanding of the complexities of dietary acculturation, shedding some light on the paradoxical findings in the literature on diet and diet-related outcomes among immigrants and their descendants. We find support for the sociological framework of segmented assimilation and call on future researchers to consider ways of integrating more complex measurements of acculturation into health behavior and outcomes research.
Future work may benefit from differentiating between more or less modifiable potential drivers of food choice and how these may differ across subgroups of Latinos (or other immigrant groups). For example, barriers such as misperceptions surrounding the healthfulness of foods with the Standard American Diet versus issues related to access to and cost of cultural ingredients may pose barriers that are specific to one segment more than another. This information would provide important information for future interventions designed to improve diet quality70. For example, Latinos who can be characterized along the underclass pattern may lack access to culturally relevant foods in their neighborhoods. On the other hand, they may be able to overcome physical access barriers but find the foods unaffordable. Both of these barriers to healthier diets are structural and thus implicate policy solutions.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
The project was supported by the National Institutes of Health through the following grants: P30DK092924 and K01CA190659 (ASR), UL1TR001860 (MDW) and R01CA159447 (LSM). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. The authors wish to thank Dr. Joanne Arsenault for helpful feedback on early versions of this manuscript, and the anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful comments that improved the final manuscript.
References:
- 1.U.S. Census Bureau. Annual Estimates of the Resident Population by Sex, Age, Race, and Hispanic Origin for the United States and States: April 1, 2010 to July 1, 2019 (NC-EST2019-SR11H).; 2020.
- 2.Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Kit BK, et al. Prevalence of childhood and adult obesity in the United States, 2011–2012. JAMA. 2014;311(8):806. doi: 10.1001/jama.2014.732 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ogden CL, Fakhouri TH, Carroll MD, et al. Prevalence of Obesity Among Adults, by Household Income and Education — United States, 2011–2014. MMWR Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 2017;66(50):1369–1373. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm6650a1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Rabbitt M, Smith MD, Coleman-Jensen A. Food Security among Hispanic Adults in the United States, 2011–2014.; 2016.
- 5.Satia JA. Dietary acculturation and the nutrition transition: an overview. Applied Physiology, Nutrition, and Metabolism. 2010;35:219–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.New C, Xiao L, Ma J. Acculturation and overweight-related attitudes and behavior among obese hispanic adults in the United States. Obesity. 2013;21(11):2396–2404. doi: 10.1002/oby.20146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Satia-Abouta J, Patterson RE, Neuhouser ML, Elder JP. Dietary acculturation: Applications to nutrition research and dietetics. Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 2002;102(8):1105–1118. doi: 10.1016/S0002-8223(02)90247-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Batis C, Hernandez-Barrera L, Barquera S, Rivera JA, Popkin BM. Food acculturation drives dietary differences among Mexicans, Mexican Americans, and Non-Hispanic Whites. The Journal of nutrition. 2011;141(10):1898–1906. doi: 10.3945/jn.111.141473 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Guendelman S, Abrams B. Dietary intake among Mexican-American women: generational differences and a comparison with white non-Hispanic women. American Journal of Public Health. 1995;85(1):20–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Cordain L, Eaton SB, Sebastian A, et al. Origins and evolution of the Western diet: health implications for the 21st century. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2005;81(2):341–354. doi: 10.1093/ajcn.81.2.341 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Mazur RE, Marquis GS, Jensen HH. Diet and food insufficiency among Hispanic youths: Acculturation and socioeconomic factors in the third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2003;78(6):1120–1127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Winkleby MA, Albright CL, Howardpitney B, Lin J, Fortmann SP. Hispanic/White differences in dietary fat intake among low educated adults and children. Preventive Medicine. 1994;23(4):465–473. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1994.1064 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Balcazar H, Castro FG, Krull JL. Cancer risk reduction in Mexican American women: The role of acculturation, education, and health risk factors. Health Education & Behavior. 1995;22(1):61–84. doi: 10.1177/109019819502200107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Sundquist J, Winkleby M. Country of birth, acculturation status and abdominal obesity in a national sample of Mexican-American women and men. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2000;29(3):470–477. doi: 10.1093/ije/29.3.470 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Bermudez OLI, Falcon LM, Tucker KL. Intake and food sources of macronutrients among older Hispanic adults: Association with ethnicity, acculturation, and length of residence in the United States. Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 2000;100(6):665–673. doi: 10.1016/S0002-8223(00)00195-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Abraído-Lanza AF, Chao MT, Flórez KR. Do healthy behaviors decline with greater acculturation? Implications for the Latino mortality paradox. Social Science & Medicine. 2005;61(6):1243–1255. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.01.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Dixon LB, Sundquist J, Winkleby M. Differences in energy, nutrient, and food intakes in a US sample of Mexican-American women and men: findings from the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 1988–1994. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2000;152(6):548–557. doi: 10.1093/AJE/152.6.548 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Castro FG, Marsiglia FF, Kulis S, Kellison JG. Lifetime segmented assimilation trajectories and health outcomes in Latino and other community residents. American journal of public health. 2010;100(4):669–676. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.167999 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Min J, Goodale H, Xue H, Brey R, Wang Y. Racial-Ethnic Disparities in Obesity and Biological, Behavioral, and Sociocultural Influences in the United States: A Systematic Review. Advances in Nutrition. 2021;12(4):1137–1148. doi: 10.1093/advances/nmaa162 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Horevitz E, Organista KC. The Mexican Health Paradox: Expanding the Explanatory Power of the Acculturation Construct. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences. 2012;35(1):3–34. doi: 10.1177/0739986312460370 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gallo LC, Penedo FJ, Espinosa de los Monteros K, Arguelles W. Resiliency in the face of disadvantage: do Hispanic cultural characteristics protect health outcomes? Journal of personality. 2009;77(6):1707–1746. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00598.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Castro FG. Emerging Hispanic health paradoxes. American Journal of Public Health. 2013;103(9):1541. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301529 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Velasco-Mondragon E, Jimenez A, Palladino-Davis AG, Davis D, Escamilla-Cejudo JA. Hispanic health in the USA: a scoping review of the literature. Public Health Reviews. 2016;37(1):31. doi: 10.1186/s40985-016-0043-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ruiz JM, Steffen P, Smith TB. Hispanic mortality paradox: a systematic review and meta-analysis of the longitudinal literature. American Journal of Public Health. 2013;103(3):e52–60. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.301103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Pinheiro PS, Williams M, Miller E a, Easterday S, Moonie S, Trapido EJ. Cancer survival among Latinos and the Hispanic Paradox. Cancer causes & control : CCC. 2011;22(4):553–561. doi: 10.1007/s10552-011-9727-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Boen CE, Hummer RA. Longer—but Harder—Lives?: The Hispanic Health Paradox and the Social Determinants of Racial, Ethnic, and Immigrant–Native Health Disparities from Midlife through Late Life. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2019;60(4):434–452. doi: 10.1177/0022146519884538 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Mattei J, McClain AC, Falcón LM, Noel SE, Tucker KL. Dietary Acculturation among Puerto Rican Adults Varies by Acculturation Construct and Dietary Measure. Journal of Nutrition. 2018;148(11):1804–1813. doi: 10.1093/jn/nxy174 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Abraído-Lanza AF, Dohrenwend BP, Ng-Mak DS, Turner JB. The Latino mortality paradox: a test of the “salmon bias” and healthy migrant hypotheses. American Journal of Public Health. 1999;89(10):1543–1548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Fenelon A, Chinn JJ, Anderson RN. A comprehensive analysis of the mortality experience of hispanic subgroups in the United States: Variation by age, country of origin, and nativity. SSM - Population Health. 2017;3:245–254. doi: 10.1016/J.SSMPH.2017.01.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Borrell LN, Lancet E a. Race/ethnicity and all-cause mortality in US adults: revisiting the Hispanic paradox. American Journal of Public Health. 2012;102(5):836–843. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2011.300345 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Ramírez AS, Golash-Boza T, Unger JB, Baezconde-Garbanati L. Questioning the Dietary Acculturation Paradox: A Mixed-Methods Study of the Relationship between Food and Ethnic Identity in a Group of Mexican-American Women. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. 2018;118(3):431–439. doi: 10.1016/J.JAND.2017.10.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Viruell-Fuentes EA, Miranda PY, Abdulrahim S. More than culture: Structural racism, intersectionality theory, and immigrant health. Social Science & Medicine. 2012;75(12):2099–2106. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.12.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Weisberg-Shapiro P, Devine CM. “Because we missed the way that we eat at the middle of the day:” Dietary acculturation and food routines among Dominican women. Appetite. 2015;95:293–302. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.07.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Portes A, Fernández-Kelly P, Haller W. The adaptation of the immigrant second generation in America: A theoretical overview and recent evidence. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies. 2009;35(7):1077–1104. doi: 10.1080/13691830903006127 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Drewnowski A, Specter SE. Poverty and obesity: The role of energy density and energy costs. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. Published online 2004. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/79.1.6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Monsivais P, Aggarwal A, Drewnowski A. Are socio-economic disparities in diet quality explained by diet cost? Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. Published online 2012. doi: 10.1136/jech.2010.122333 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Kirkpatrick SI, Dodd KW, Reedy J, Krebs-Smith SM. Income and Race/Ethnicity Are Associated with Adherence to Food-Based Dietary Guidance among US Adults and Children. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. 2012;112(5):624–635.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2011.11.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.U.S. Census Bureau. Historical. Historical Poverty Tables: People and Families--1959 to 2019. Published 2020. Accessed January 25, 2021. https://www.census.gov/data/tables/time-series/demo/income-poverty/historical-poverty-people.html
- 39.U.S. Census Bureau. Educational Attainment in the United States: 2019. Published 2020. Accessed January 25, 2021. https://www.census.gov/content/census/en/data/tables/2019/demo/educational-attainment/cps-detailed-tables.html
- 40.Flórez KR, Abraído-Lanza AF. Segmented Assimilation. Family & Community Health. 2017;40(2):132–138. doi: 10.1097/FCH.0000000000000143 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Dondero M, Van Hook JL, Frisco ML, Martin MA. Dietary Assimilation among Mexican Children in Immigrant Households: Code-switching and Healthy Eating across Social Institutions. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. Published online November 1, 2018:002214651880999. doi: 10.1177/0022146518809995 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Frisco ML, Martin MA, Van Hook JL. Socioeconomic status and acculturation: Why Mexican Americans are heavier than Mexican immgrants and whites. In: Frank R, ed. Immigration and Health: Advances in Medical Sociology. Emerald Publishing Limited; 2019:71–96. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Reininger B, Lee M, Jennings R, Evans A, Vidoni M. Healthy eating patterns associated with acculturation, sex and BMI among Mexican Americans. Public Health Nutrition. 2017;20(7):1267–1278. doi: 10.1017/S1368980016003311 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Arandia G, Sotres-Alvarez D, Siega-Riz AM, et al. Associations between acculturation, ethnic identity, and diet quality among U.S. Hispanic/Latino Youth: Findings from the HCHS/SOL Youth Study. Appetite. 2018;129:25–36. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.06.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Bojorquez I, Rentería D, Unikel C. Trajectories of dietary change and the social context of migration: a qualitative study. Appetite. 2014;81:93–101. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.06.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Wilson MD, Ramírez AS, Arsenault JE, Miller LMS. Nutrition Label Use and Its Association With Dietary Quality Among Latinos: The Roles of Poverty and Acculturation. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior. 2018;50(9):876–887. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2018.05.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Portes A, Zhou M. The New Second Generation: Segmented Assimilation and Its Variants. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science. 1993;530(74–96). Accessed March 5, 2018. 10.1177/0002716293530001006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Weisberg-Shapiro P, Devine C. “Men like to Eat More Rice and Beans and Things like That”: The Influence of Childhood Experience and Life Course Events on Dietary Acculturation. Ecology of Food and Nutrition. 2019;58(5):413–429. doi: 10.1080/03670244.2019.1606805 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Tucker KL. Dietary Patterns in Latinx Groups. The Journal of Nutrition. Published online July 28, 2021. doi: 10.1093/jn/nxab225 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Johnson CL, Dohrmann SM, Burt VL, Mohadjer LK. National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Sample Design, 2011–2014. Vital and Health Statistics. 2014;2(162). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Curtin LR, Mohadjer LK, Dohrmann SM. National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey: Sample design, 2007–2010. In: Vital and Health Statistics Reports. Vol 2. National Center for Health Statistics, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2013. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Centers for Disease Control & Prevention. National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 2009–2010 Data Documentation, Codebook, and Frequencies.
- 53.Guenther PM, Kirkpatrick SI, Reedy J, et al. The Healthy Eating Index-2010 is a valid and reliable measure of diet quality according to the 2010 Dietary Guidelines for Americans. The Journal of Nutrition. 2014;144(3):399–407. doi: 10.3945/jn.113.183079 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.National Institutes of Health, National Cancer Institute, Division of Cancer Control and Population Science, Epidemiology and Genomics Research Program.
- 55.National Center for Health Statistics. National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Healthy Eating Index SAS Macro. Published online 2010.
- 56.Secretaría de Salud. La dieta de la milpa. Published 2020. Accessed January 25, 2021. https://www.gob.mx/salud/acciones-y-programas/la-dieta-de-la-milpa-259188
- 57.Akresh IR. Dietary assimilation and health among Hispanic immigrants to the United States. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2007;48(4):404–417. doi: 10.1177/002214650704800405 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Finch BK, Lim N, Perez W, Do DP. Toward a Population Health Model of Segmented Assimilation: The Case of Low Birth Weight in Los Angeles. Sociological Perspectives. 2007;50(3):445–468. doi: 10.1525/sop.2007.50.3.445 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Meyer OL, Liu X (Lucia), Tancredi D, Ramirez AS, Schulz R, Hinton L. Acculturation level and caregiver outcomes from a randomized intervention trial to enhance caregivers’ health: evidence from REACH II. Aging and Mental Health. 2018;22(6):730–737. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2017.1317330 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Wilson MM, Reedy J, Krebs-Smith SM. American Diet Quality: Where It Is, Where It Is Heading, and What It Could Be. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. 2016;116(2):302–310.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2015.09.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Krebs-Smith SM, Pannucci TRE, Subar AF, et al. Update of the Healthy Eating Index: HEI-2015. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. 2018;118(9):1591–1602. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2018.05.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Kirkpatrick SI, Reedy J, Krebs-Smith SM, et al. Applications of the Healthy Eating Index for Surveillance, Epidemiology, and Intervention Research: Considerations and Caveats. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. 2018;118(9):1603–1621. doi: 10.1016/J.JAND.2018.05.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Abraído-Lanza AF, Echeverría SE, Flórez KR. Latino Immigrants, Acculturation, and Health: Promising New Directions in Research. Annual Review of Public Health. 2016;37(1):219–236. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-032315-021545 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Evans CR. Modeling the intersectionality of processes in the social production of health inequalities. Social Science and Medicine. 2019;226:249–253. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.01.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Stepler R, Lopez MH. U.S. Latino Population Growth and Dispersion Has Slowed Since Onset of the Great Recession; 2016.
- 66.Rehm CD, Peñalvo JL, Afshin A, Mozaffarian D. Dietary intake among US adults, 1999–2012. JAMA. 2016;315(23):2542–2553. doi: 10.1001/jama.2016.7491 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Popkin BM, Gordon-Larsen P. The nutrition transition: worldwide obesity dynamics and their determinants. International journal of obesity and related metabolic disorders : journal of the International Association for the Study of Obesity 2004;28 Suppl 3(S3):S2–9. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Barquera S, Schillinger D, Aguilar-Salinas CA, et al. Collaborative research and actions on both sides of the US-Mexico border to counteract type 2 diabetes in people of Mexican origin. Globalization and Health. 2018;14(1):84. doi: 10.1186/s12992-018-0390-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Popkin BM, Reardon T. Obesity and the food system transformation in Latin America. Obesity Reviews. 2018;19(8):1028–1064. doi: 10.1111/obr.12694 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Payán DD, Díaz Rios LK, Ramírez AS, de Trinidad Young M-E. Structural Barriers Influencing Food Insecurity, Malnutrition, and Health Among Latinas During and After COVID-19: Considerations and Recommendations. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. 2021;121(5). doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2021.01.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.