Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Mar 24.
Published in final edited form as: Nat Neurosci. 2021 Jun 7;24(8):1176–1186. doi: 10.1038/s41593-021-00867-9

Baseline Brain Function in the Pre-Adolescents of the ABCD Study.

B Chaarani 1, S Hahn 1, N Allgaier 1, S Adise 1, MM Owens 1, AC Juliano 1, DK Yuan 1, H Loso 1, A Ivanciu 1, MD Albaugh 1, J Dumas 1, S Mackey 1, J Laurent 1, M Ivanova 1, DJ Hagler 2, MD Cornejo 3, S Hatton 2, A Agrawal 4, L Aguinaldo 2, L Ahonen 5, W Aklin 6, AP Anokhin 4, J Arroyo 7, S Avenevoli 8, D Babcock 9, K Bagot 10, FC Baker 11, MT Banich 12, DM Barch 4, H Bartsch 13, A Baskin-Sommers 14, JM Bjork 15, D Blachman-Demner 16, M Bloch 17, R Bogdan 4, SY Bookheimer 18, F Breslin 19, S Brown 2, FJ Calabro 5, V Calhoun 12,20, BJ Casey 14, L Chang 21, DB Clark 5, C Cloak 21, RT Constable 14, K Constable 6, R Corley 12, LB Cottler 22, S Coxe 23, R Dagher 24, AM Dale 2, M Dapretto 18, R Delcarmen-Wiggins 25, A S Dick 23, EK Do 15, NUF Dosenbach 4, GJ Dowling 6, S Edwards 21, TM Ernst 21, DA Fair 26, CC Fan 4, E Feczko 26, SW Feldstein-Ewing 26, P Florsheim 27, JJ Foxe 28, EG Freedman 28, NP Friedman 12, S Friedman-Hill 8, BF Fuemmeler 15, A Galvan 18, DG Gee 14, J Giedd 2, M Glantz 6, P Glaser 4, J Godino 2, M Gonzalez 29, R Gonzalez 23, S Grant 6, K M Gray 30, F Haist 2, MP Harms 4, S Hawes 23, AC Heath 2, S Heeringa 31, MM Heitzeg 31, R Hermosillo 26, MM Herting 32, JM Hettema 15, JK Hewitt 12, C Heyser 2, E Hoffman 6, K Howlett 6, RS Huber 33, MA Huestis 34, LW Hyde 31, WG Iacono 35, MA Infante 2, O Irfanoglu 36, A Isaiah 21, S Iyengar 37, J Jacobus 2, R James 15, B Jean-Francois 24, T Jernigan 2, NR Karcher 4, A Kaufman 17, B Kelley 38, B Kit 39, A Ksinan 15, J Kuperman 2, AR Laird 23, C Larson 27, K LeBlanc 6, C Lessov-Schlagger 4, N Lever 21, DA Lewis 5, K Lisdahl 27, AR Little 6, M Lopez 6, M Luciana 35, B Luna 5, PA Madden 4, HH Maes 15, C Makowski 2, AT Marshall 29, MJ Mason 40, J Matochik 7, BD McCandliss 41, E McGlade 33, I Montoya 6, G Morgan 17, A Morris 42, C Mulford 6, P Murray 7, BJ Nagel 26, MC Neale 15, G Neigh 15, A Nencka 43, A Noronha 7, SJ Nixon 22, CE Palmer 2, V Pariyadath 6, MP Paulus 19, WE Pelham 23, D Pfefferbaum 11, C Pierpaoli 44, A Prescot 33, D Prouty 11, LI Puttler 31, N Rajapaske 24, KM Rapuano 14, G Reeves 21, PF Renshaw 33, MC Riedel 23, P Rojas 23, M de la Rosa 23, MD Rosenberg 45, M J Ross 46, M Sanchez 23, C Schirda 5, D Schloesser 16, J Schulenberg 31, KJ Sher 47, C Sheth 33, PD Shilling 2, WK Simmons 19, ER Sowell 29, N Speer 12, M Spittel 16, L M Squeglia 30, C Sripada 31, J Steinberg 15, C Striley 22, MT Sutherland 23, J Tanabe 12, SF Tapert 2, W Thompson 2, R L Tomko 30, KA Uban 48, S Vrieze 35, NE Wade 2, R Watts 14, S Weiss 6, BA Wiens 22, OD Williams 23, A Wilbur 11, D Wing 2, D Wolff-Hughes 16, R Yang 2, DA Yurgelun-Todd 33, RA Zucker 31, A Potter 1, HP Garavan 1; ABCD Consortium
PMCID: PMC8947197  NIHMSID: NIHMS1727067  PMID: 34099922

Abstract

The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study is a 10-year longitudinal study of children recruited at ages 9 and 10. A battery of neuroimaging tasks are administered biennially to track neurodevelopment and identify individual differences in brain function. This study reports activation patterns from fMRI tasks completed at baseline, designed to measure cognitive impulse control with a Stop Signal task (n=5,547), reward anticipation and receipt with a Monetary Incentive Delay task (n=6,657), and working memory and emotion reactivity with an emotional N-back task (n=6,009). Further, we report the spatial reproducibility of activation patterns by assessing between-group vertex/voxelwise correlations of BOLD activation. Analyses reveal robust brain activations that are consistent with the published literature, vary across fMRI tasks/contrasts and slightly correlated with individual behavioral performance on the tasks. These results establish the pre-adolescent brain function baseline, guide interpretation of cross-sectional analyses, and will enable the investigation of longitudinal changes during adolescent development.

Introduction

The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study aims to characterize adolescent development and evaluate many influences that might shape developmental trajectories. While numerous factors are plausibly associated with neurodevelopment (e.g., nutrition, sleep, exercise, head injuries, substance use), we have a limited understanding of the magnitude of their effects, their interactions with one another, and the moderating influences of other risk or resilience factors. The longitudinal ABCD Study (www.ABCDstudy.org) aims to address these matters with an especially large, demographically diverse sample that is richly characterized with extensive phenotyping and genotyping. It realizes an open science model through which data are fully shared with the research community.

This manuscript describes the ABCD baseline assessment of brain function in 9- and 10-year-olds. As the bulk of the human neuroimaging literature has focused on adult functioning, less is known about brain function in childhood and, particularly, in pre-adolescent children. Neurodevelopment from ages 10 to 20 is, however, of particular interest as these ages are associated with notable brain, cognitive, and emotional maturation, as well as the emergence of many prevalent mental health disorders 1. Consequently, there is great interest in understanding the etiology and neurobiology of psychological processes thought to be risk factors for the development of mental and physical health challenges, including cognitive control, reward, working memory, and social/emotional function.

Cognitive control is often assessed using inhibitory tasks such as the Stop Signal Task (SST), in which a motor response must be countermanded 2. Inhibitory tasks including the SST are known to elicit activation in the dorsal anterior cingulate (dACC), inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), and insula 3. Studies of these processes in children and adolescents have typically found similar regional activation during inhibitory tasks 4. Consistent results have been found for fMRI and EEG studies showing that from childhood to adolescence there is an increase in brain activity in the dACC 5, which corresponds to improved inhibitory control 6. However, this pattern may not be consistent across the brain, as there have been mixed findings regarding whether activation in the dlPFC and IFG increases with age and improved inhibitory control 57, with more recent findings suggesting that activation in the prefrontal cortex may decrease throughout adolescence paralleling improved inhibitory control 4,8,9.

Functional neuroimaging investigations of working memory also demonstrate substantial concordance between adults and children in neural responses, as well as some notable differences 1012. A recent meta-analysis of verbal and visuospatial N-back tasks (in which one indicates if a currently presented stimulus is the same as a stimulus presented n items earlier in a serial stream of stimuli) found that adults and children show consistent patterns of activation in the dlPFC, posterior parietal cortex (PPC), supplementary motor area (SMA), and insula 10. However, similar to the SST, regional levels of activation during the N-back have been shown to differ between children and adults, mirroring improvements in working memory ability 1315. In the largest study to date to examine change in functional activation during the N-back across adolescence (N = 951), Satterthwaite et al. 13 found increases in activation in the dlPFC, SMA, and PPC from age 8 to 22. Interestingly, they also found decreased activation in the default mode network (DMN), suggesting improved segregation of the cognitive control and DMN regions with advancing age and improving performance on the task. One study using a subset from the ABCD baseline assessment found that frontoparietal activity during the 2-Back (relative to 0-Back) relates to working memory performance measured out of the scanner using the list sort task, suggesting that brain activity during the N-Back functions as a general index of working memory ability 16.

Importantly, by varying the stimuli, psychological tasks can also implicitly probe social and emotional processes. A recent meta-analysis of affective working memory tasks in adolescents and adults found that there was little effect on working memory performance from using affective stimuli and that the differences that were observed were more concentrated among older adults (Schweizer et al. 17; 165 studies, N=7,433). However, this analysis did find differences in brain activation associated with affective working memory stimuli (33 fMRI studies, n=683), with affective stimuli more likely to elicit activation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), amygdala, temporal cortex, and occipital cortex. While this analysis did not examine emotional faces specifically, studies of emotional faces were included, likely explaining the observed temporal/occipital activations. In other studies, working memory tasks utilizing facial stimuli have been found to elicit differential patterns of activation in the temporal/occipital cortices, specifically in the fusiform face area (FFA) and the occipital face area (OFA) 18,19. A recent large study by Fuhrman et al. 20 (N = 661) found that the ability to distinguish faces is still developing until age 16, confirming prior smaller studies 21. Scherf et al. 22 found that adolescents aged 11–14 showed similar but attenuated patterns of face-related activation compared to adults. In one of the largest studies of adolescents to date (N=1,100), Tahmasebi et al. 23 found similar regions activated in 13–15-yearolds by facial stimuli including the FFA, OFA, and STS. However, this study included neither younger children nor older adult comparison groups. While the primary regions elicited by facial recognition tasks are likely similar throughout development, there is evidence that activation in core facial recognition regions (i.e., FFA, OFA, and superior temporal sulcus) increases in response to these tasks throughout development starting at age 7–8 24. Additionally, work by Kadosh et al. found that complementary regions supporting the primary facial recognition regions change throughout development (N=42) 25. When considering young children (aged 5–8), Scherf et al. 26 found that children did not demonstrate these characteristic patterns of activation to faces at all, lacking activation in the FFA, OFA, and STS (N=30), although their activation to place stimuli in the parahippocampal area was similar to that of adults. Indeed, several other studies concluded that the FFA is not consistently activated in children under the age of 8 (for review, see Scherf et al. 22).

With regard to reward-related processes such as reward anticipation and receipt, many studies have suggested similar task-related fMRI activation in children and adolescents aged 12–17 as in adults, with a network of reward anticipation regions including the ventral and dorsal striatum, the insula, SMA, premotor cortex, thalamus, and amygdala and a network of reward receipt regions including the ventral striatum (VS), amygdala, vmPFC, and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) 2729.

Overall, investigations into these key neurocognitive processes suggest qualitatively similar patterns of activation in children and adults, with some inconsistencies across studies, likely due, in part, to small samples of convenience. Indeed, low reproducibility in psychological and clinical neuroimaging studies due to small sample sizes is now acknowledged to be a critical concern in the field 3032. While Thirion et al. 33 suggested that 20 or more participants are required for reliable task-based fMRI inferences, Turner et al. 34 recently pointed out using the large fMRI dataset of the Human Connectome Project (HCP) 35 that such recommendations are outdated. Indeed, Turner et al. report that even datasets with 100 or more participants can produce results that do not replicate, suggesting that larger sample sizes are necessary for task-based fMRI 34.

Here, we report cortical and subcortical analyses of the ABCD task fMRI battery assessing response inhibition, working memory, and reward processing, assayed via the SST, Emotional N-back (EN-back) and MID tasks (Figure 1), respectively, at the study’s first acquisition time point. We focus on: a) the patterns and magnitude of brain activity as predicted by prior research in adolescent samples; b) the reproducibility of activation patterns, including an assessment of group-level reproducibility as a function of sample size; and (c) the relationships between activation magnitudes and individual differences in task abilities during response inhibition and working memory. We hypothesized that the three fMRI tasks would show robust patterns of activation consistent with those identified in prior studies in children mentioned in the literature review above, including cortico-striatal activations associated with motor response inhibition 3,4, frontoparietal activation associated with working memory performance 10,36, and dopaminergically rich subcortical regions associated with reward processes 2729. Additionally, we hypothesized that patterns of associations between task performance and task activation would mirror activation patterns, as has been found in prior literature. Further, we hypothesized that reproducible group-level activation maps would likely differ among tasks (being biggest on the block design primary EN-Back contrasts) and likely requiring more participants to reproduce effects than are included in typical neuroimaging studies.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Schematics of the Stop Signal Task (a), the Emotional N-back task (b) and the Monetary Incentive Delay Task (c) (from Casey et al., 2018). ITI = Inter-trial interval; RT = Reaction time; SSD = Stop signal delay; SS = Stop signal.

The analyses that are reported were designed to provide a largely descriptive account of the patterns of activation present in the ABCD sample. For instance, we report all analyses in effect sizes and do not threshold by statistical significance. The goal of the present paper is to provide a baseline reference (task activation magnitudes, sensitivity to individual differences in performance, reproducibility) for researchers using the ABCD task fMRI data to address their questions of interest regarding adolescent brain development.

Results

After applying the exclusion criteria (see Table 1 and Methods for details), the resulting sample sizes and demographics among the fMRI tasks were as follows: SST (N=5,547, mean age=9.96±0.63, 49.82% male), EN-back (N=6,009, mean age=9.96±0.63, 50.77% male), and MID (N=6,657, mean age=9.95±0.63, 50.43% males). Table 2 presents the demographic composition of both samples who were included and who were excluded from the current study (see Methods for details).

Table 1.

Inclusion criteria in the task-fMRI analyses and number of participants remaining after each step of exclusions. Covariates include age, sex, education, puberty, race, family, and scanner ID. MID: Monetary incentive Delay; SST: Stop Signal Task; MRI: Magnetic Resonance Imaging; FD: Framewise Displacement; QC: Quality Control.

Criteria\Task EN-Back SST MID
Total number scanned participants 10,189 10,294 10,385
Two runs that passed MRI quality control 8,981 9,035 9,140
Data available excluding Philips scans 8,163 8,140 8,201
Mutual vertex and voxel data availability 7,969 7,288 7,427
Motion censoring (mean FD < 0.9 mm) 7,680 7,000 7,239
Degrees of Freedom across runs > 200 7,680 7,000 7,225
Beta-weights outlier detection 6,666 6,995 7,214
Passed behavioral performance QC 6,085 5,116 6,753
No missing covariates 6,009 5,547 6,657

Table 2:

Demographics for all participants included in and excluded from analyses.

Measure EN-back (N=6,009) Sample included in paper EN-back (N= 4,353) Sample not included in paper Statistics Age = t-test between participants in paper vs. excluded from paper All other variables = Pearson chi-square SST (N=5,547) Sample included in paper SST (N=4,815) Sample not included in paper Statistics Age = t-test between participants in paper vs. excluded from paper All other variables = Pearson chi-square MID (N=6,657) Sample included in paper MID (N=3,705) Sample not included in paper Statistics Age = t-test between participants in paper vs. excluded from paper All other variables = Pearson chi-square
Sex
Male
Female
No answer

3,051
2,958

2,378
1,974
1
X2 (1, N=10,361) = 15.14 p<.001
Cramer’s V = -.04

2,767
2,780

2,666
2,149
X2 (1, N=10,362) =
30.63, p<.001
Cramer’s V = -.05

3,357
3,300

2,073
1,632
X2 (1, N=10,362) =
28.98, p<.001
Cramer’s V = -.05
Age (M±SD)
Nine
Ten
9.96 ± 0.63
2,929
3,080
9.84 ± 0.61
2,529
1,824

t(9,520.93)= 10.31, p<.001
 9.96 ± 0.63
2,731
2,816
9.86 ± 0.62
2,713
2,102

t(10,361) = 298.92, p<.001
 9.95 ± 0.63
3,335
3,322
9.85 ± 0.61
2,123
1,582

t(7,814.1) = 8.14, p<.001
Race/Ethnicity
White
Black
Hispanic/Latino
Asian
Other
No answer

 3,428 685
1,170
121
605

1,884
926
 920 93
513
17

X2 (4, N=10,345) = 262.28, p<.001
Cramer’s V = .16

 3,172 657
1,069
114
535

 2,148 957
1,023
100
585
2

X2 (4, N=10,360) = 206.05, p<.001
Cramer’s V = .14

 3,680 826
1,347
129
675

1,632
785
 743 85
443
17

X2 (4, N=10,345) = 185.56, p<.001
Cramer’s V = .13
Highest Household Ed.
< Highschool Diploma
Highschool Diploma/GED
Some College
Bachelors
Post-Graduate Degree
No answer

192
445
1,493
1,650
2,221
8

288
545
1,293
1,003
1,215
9

X2 (4, N=10,345) = 236.65, p<.001
Cramer’s V = .15

182
437
1,359
1,517
2,044
8

298
553
1,429
1,136
1,393
6
X2 (4, N=10,348) = 170.76, p<.001
Cramer’s V = .13

244
553
1,671
1,795
2,386
8

236
437
 1,115 858
1,050
9

X2 (4, N=10,345) = 143.87, p<.001
Cramer’s V = .12
 Household Income / Year
<50K
50K-100K
>100K
No answer
Participant did not know

1,395
1620
2,584
229
181

1,475
1,084
1,344
205
245
X2 (2, N=9,502) =
203.70, p<.001
Cramer’s V = .15

1,285
1,485
2,382
224
171

1,585
1,220
1,546
209
255
X2 (2, N=9,503) =
168.93, p<.001
Cramer’s V = .13

1,613
1,774
2,780
250
240

 1,257 930
1,148
189
181
X2 (2, N=9,502) =
155.41, p<.001
Cramer’s V = .13
Handedness
Right
Left
Ambidextrous

4,849
418
742

3,401
325
627
X2 (2, N=10,362) = 11.08, p=.004
Cramer’s V = .03

4,497
377
673

3,753
366
696
X2 (2, N=10,362) = 16.01, p<.001
Cramer’s V = .04

5,379
470
808

2,871
273
561
X2 (2, N=10,362) = 19.85, p<.001
Cramer’s V = .04

Individual behavior performance measures and task-fMRI beta-weights

Performance on the SST was in the anticipated range (mean [SD] SSRT = 302.6 [67.1]; Go RT = 529.9 [77.4]), with a rate of correct inhibitions of 51.4% (0.06). The distributions for EN-back D’ were as expected, with children performing better (p<0.001) on the 0-back task (D’=2.51 [0.9]) than the 2-back task (D’=2.0 [0.3]). D’ mean [SD] from the postscan recognition memory test were: 0.94 [0.63] for happy faces, 0.92 [0.63] for fearful faces, 0.85 [0.61] for neutral faces and 1.34 [0.82] for places. The distributions of SSRT and D’ behavioral performance measures are shown in Figure 2. Paralleling the individual differences in performance, task activations in relevant task ROIs also show large interindividual variation: Figure 2 shows the distribution of average local maxima beta-weights within task-specific regions known to be involved in SST (Figure 2-d,e), EN-back (Figure 2-f), and MID (Figure 2-g) tasks. An ANCOVA assessing the correlation between D’ values from the post-scan recognition memory test and EN-back beta-weights in bilateral dlPFC showed significant associations across contrasts and conditions, yet with small correlation coefficients, with the most important association observed between the happy faces condition and the 2-back vs. 0-back contrast (p=0.0001 and r=0.17).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

The distribution of behavioral performance measures and beta weights in the sample. Top quadrants: The distribution of Stop Signal Reaction Time (SSRT) (a) and 0-back and 2-back and D-prime (b,c) behavioral performance measures in the sample. Bottom quadrants: The distribution of average local maxima beta weights in the sample within task-specific regions commonly known to be involved in SST (d,e), EN-back (f) and MID (g) tasks. Sample Sizes: SST (N=5,547), EN-back (N=6,009), and MID (N=6,657).

Task activation Cohen’s d

Cortical and subcortical Cohen’s d maps of contrasts for each task are represented in composite figures 3-a,b, 4-a,b, 5a,b and 6-a,b. In addition, static and 3D dynamic views of both thresholded and unthresholded Cohen’s d maps are available online (download required to display the dynamic maps) at https://drive.google.com/drive/folders/1VPnY8SS68JYis-AI-mt8_BJqc4r6GD5D. Task-fMRI activation maps generated from the ABCD dataset are provided in “Supplementary Data 1” for the research community to use as taskfMRI activation templates in children, and are available online as manuscript files and can be downloaded as highresolution mgz images.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

SST’s activation maps, performance correlation maps and group-level spatial consistency at cortical and subcortical levels. Cohen’s d maps are thresholded at ≥ 0.2 in magnitude to only display small, medium, and large effect sizes. No thresholding is applied to the correlation maps. N=5,547.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

EN-back’s working memory activation maps, performance correlation maps and group-level spatial consistency at cortical and subcortical levels. Cohen’s d maps are thresholded at ≥ 0.2 in magnitude to only display small, medium, and large effect sizes. No thresholding is applied to the correlation maps. N=6,009.

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

EN-back’s emotional activation maps, performance correlation maps and group-level spatial consistency at cortical and subcortical levels. Cohen’s d maps are thresholded at ≥ 0.2 in magnitude to only display small, medium, and large effect sizes. No thresholding is applied to the correlation maps. N=6,009.

Figure 6.

Figure 6.

MID’s activation maps and group-level spatial consistency at cortical and subcortical levels. Cohen’s d maps are thresholded at ≥ 0.2 in magnitude to only display small, medium, and large effect sizes. N=6,657.

As shown in Figure 3, the SST showed robust activation for both correct Stop vs correct Go and incorrect Stop vs correct Go conditions in multiple fronto-parietal, temporal, insular and occipital regions of the cortex. Key nodes of the response inhibition circuitry, such as the IFG, dACC, pre-SMA and, subcortically, putamen, and caudate were activated. Deactivations were observed in left postcentral somato-sensorimotor cortex (presumably reflecting the motor response that is present on Go trials and absent on successful inhibitions) and in DMN centers including precuneus and vmPFC.

As shown in Figure 4, the 0-back versus fixation and 2-back versus fixation contrasts of the EN-back task produced widespread robust activation in bilateral regions including parts of the superior, middle and inferior frontal gyri, inferior parietal lobule, dACC/SMA, the precentral gyrus, and the occipital pole. Substantial deactivations were also observed within a number of bilateral regions, including the pre-central and post-central gyri, superior parietal lobule, lingual gyrus, precuneus/PCC, the rostral ACC/vmPFC, and the posterior insula. The 2 back versus 0 back subtraction showed more circumscribed activity in the middle and superior frontal gyri, inferior parietal gyrus, precuneus, and dACC, as well as focal deactivations in the vmPFC/rostral ACC, PCC, precentral gyrus, and posterior insula. As expected, the Faces versus Places contrast revealed dissociable activations: face stimuli produced elevated activation in the FFA, OFA and bilateral amygdalae, while place stimuli robustly activated large portions of occipital cortex in addition to hippocampal and parahippocampal regions. The negative and positive faces contrasted to neutral faces (Figure 5) showed reduced activations compared to the other contrasts, with the most notable effects being activation in the amygdalae for negative vs. neutral faces, and deactivation in bilateral putamen for positive vs. neutral faces.

As shown in Figure 6, anticipation of potential wins and losses on the MID task produced largely similar patterns of activation in parts of the ACC, precentral gyrus, inferior parietal lobule, frontal and occipital gyri, as well as in bilateral anterior insula (AI) and extensive subcortical regions. The feedback contrasts showed that negative outcomes (failing to win a reward and failing to avoid a loss) produced robust activity in AI as well as in temporal and temporo-parietal regions. Subcortically, a post hoc ROI-level analysis showed that the anticipation of rewards produced more activation than did the anticipation of losses including robust ventromedial striatal activity (Cohen’s d in ventromedial striatum was significantly different, p<10−4, between the two contrasts), while the putamen showed an opposite pattern for feedback with more activation when avoiding a loss than winning a reward (Cohen’s d in putamen was significantly different, p<0.0003).

We also calculated the absolute maximum Pearson’s correlation coefficient for cortical and subcortical activation measures for a single voxel and vertex per contrast and the variables age, puberty, education, and scanner site. Overall, the vertex/voxel wise univariate analyses did not explain more than 1% of the variance associated with task activation (see Tables S1 & S2 and supplementary notes 1 & 2 for more details). We also assessed the relationship between cortical thickness and functional brain activation in activated cortical regions for each fMRI task, and found that cortical thickness did not explain more than 0.5% of the variance associated with BOLD activation in one single activated regions (see Figure S1 and supplementary note 3 for more details). In addition, we computed the correlation of BOLD activation across the three fMRI tasks and found very small associations among them (see Figure S2 and supplementary note 4 for more details). Finally, to determine if framewise displacement (FD) and the number of censored frames contaminates the activation estimates of each task, we computed the Pearson correlation coefficient between the Cohen’s d maps as produced with our original set of covariates and Cohen’s d maps computed when degrees of freedom (DoF) and FD, calculated per participant, were added as covariates. Activation maps were highly correlated after adding either DoF or FD (see Table S3, Figure S3 and supplementary note 5 for more details).

Between-group task spatial reproducibility

Cortical and subcortical Cohen’s d correlation coefficients are shown for each task and contrast in Figures 3-e, 4-e, 5-c, and 6-c as a function of sample size. In addition, an HTML dynamic tool to display the correlation coefficients as a function of sample size is available for download in “Supplementary Data 2”, with a usage demo provided in Figure S4.

The cognitive, working memory contrasts of the EN-back task showed the strongest between-group spatial reproducibility, requiring a sample size of n=18 for cortical and n=56 for subcortical maps on average to reach a correlation coefficient of 0.8 or more across contrasts. Meanwhile, the emotional contrasts reached a maximum correlation coefficient of 0.75 with n=2500, except for cortical maps of the negative vs. neutral faces contrast requiring n=1285 to reach a correlation coefficient of 0.8 or more (Fig 4-e, 5-c). Group-level spatial reproducibility for the primary SST contrasts required a sample size of n=32 for cortical and n=80 for subcortical maps on average to reach a correlation coefficient of 0.8 or more across contrasts (Fig 3-e). Finally, the MID task required a sample size of n=112 for cortical and n=143 for subcortical maps on average to reach a correlation coefficient of 0.8 or more across the primary contrasts (Fig 6-c). Meanwhile, the secondary contrasts (anticipation of large vs. small rewards and anticipation of large vs. small losses) required a sample size of n=775 for cortical and n=1027 on average for subcortical maps to reach a correlation coefficient of 0.8 or more (Fig 6-c).

Individual Differences

Cortical and subcortical performance correlation maps of SST and EN-back contrasts are shown in Figures 3-c,d and 4c,d. In addition, static and 3D dynamic views of correlation coefficient maps are available online at the following address (download required to display the interactive maps): https://drive.google.com/drive/folders/1VPnY8SS68JYis-AImt8_BJqc4r6GD5D).

The correlation analyses between the SST beta-weights and SSRT revealed that activation in individual vertices/voxels explained up to 2% of the individual performance differences in response inhibition. Whereas correlations were largely negative for the correct Stop contrast (faster SSRT accompanied by greater stop-related activation), a more varied pattern of positive and negative correlations was observed for the incorrect Stop contrast.

The performance correlation maps of the EN-back task largely recapitulated the task activation maps insofar as the correlations tended to be the largest where task activation was strongest. Activation in individual vertices/voxels explained up to 2.2% of individual performance differences in working memory as measured by the EN-back D’ accuracy metric.

In addition, we assessed performance correlation between EN-back and SST and found significant negative correlations, albeit with small coefficients of determination, between SSRT and D’ for 0-back and 2-back (see Figure S2 and supplementary note 4 for more details).

Finally, the spatial reproducibility of these brain-performance correlation maps is shown in Figures 3-e and 4-e. The reproducibility of these maps is notably smaller than the group activation maps with up to 2,500 participants being insufficient in most cases to reach an asymptote of 0.8.

Discussion

In this work, we reported fMRI activation patterns for SST, EN-back, and MID tasks from the baseline assessment of the ABCD Study cohort. Further, we reported both the group-level spatial reproducibility of activation patterns as a function of sample size and the sensitivity of the activation maps to individual differences in behavioral task performance.

Overall, the task-activation patterns observed in this study are consistent with the extant literature on adolescents and adults. The SST activation patterns replicate previous task-fMRI SST data and meta-analyses in adults, adolescents, and children, insofar as it robustly activated regions known to play an important role in inhibitory control, such as the insula, superior/middle/inferior frontal gyrus, dACC/SMA, dlPFC, PPC, thalamus, and basal ganglia (Rae et al.37; N=331; Hung et al., 38; N=1447; Swick et al.39; N=440). The correct and incorrect Stop maps were largely similar, likely indicating that the response inhibition circuitry is engaged even when the attempt to inhibit fails, which previous EEG data links to motor inhibition circuitry being activated, albeit too slowly, on commission errors 40. The incorrect Stop maps do show greater activations than correct Stops in left postcentral somato-sensorimotor gyrus, the anterior insula, and the dACC (mean Cohen’s d significantly different at p<0.001), reflecting amplified right-handed motor and salience network activations when committing a commission error.

The regions activated by the 0-back versus fixation, 2-back versus fixation, and 2-back versus 0-back conditions of the EN-back task included the dlPFC, PPC, SMA, and anterior insula. This activation pattern has been consistently observed across different N-back stimulus types and contrasts 41 as well as in an analysis, using the largest sample to date (N=1064), of the EN-back task in the HCP 18,36. The activation patterns are consistent with other tasks of working memory and executive functioning 42, and may constitute a unified cognitive control network 43. Additionally, consistent with the deactivations we report, the EN-back has been shown to reliably deactivate the DMN 18,44 relative to both resting and active baselines. This task was also shown to provide a useful probe of the intrinsic anticorrelation that has been proposed between cognitive control and DMN regions 45. In line with our findings, a recent paper investigating the associations between working memory, cognitive abilities, and functional MRI activation in data from over 4,000 9–10-year-olds enrolled in the ABCD study 16, working memory function was significantly related to 2-back vs. 0-back (i.e., high vs. low memory load) activation in regions of frontal and parietal cortex including bilateral intraparietal sulci, dorsal premotor cortex/frontal eye fields, dlPFC, AI, dACC extending into the pre-SMA, and precuneus. The results also revealed that working memory was not significantly associated with emotion-related activation during the EN-back task, inhibitory control-related activation during the SST, or reward-related activation during the MID task. The faces versus places contrast yielded stimulus-specific activation in amygdala, hippocampus, precuneus, and in different regions of visual cortex including FFA and OFA, consistent with evidence linking face viewing to the FFA and OFA 22,26 and place processing to the precuneus and hippocampus 46,47. The contrasts identifying differential responses to emotional faces, when compared with neutral faces, showed smaller Cohen’s d effect-sizes, but have specific value by assaying social/emotional and memory processes (e.g., heightened amygdalar response to negative faces) secondary to the primary focus on working memory.

The regions activated during reward anticipation in the MID task included the striatum, dACC, AI, and parietal and occipital gyri, which is consistent with large studies (N>830) in children and adults, providing evidence that these regions are robustly associated with the reward anticipation condition of the MID 27,28,48. Similarly, the feedback contrasts showing activations in PCC, vmPFC, and VS are also consistent with this literature. However, in the large study by Cao et al. 28 using the IMAGEN dataset, only vmPFC was activated by reward outcomes. When Silverman et al. 27 directly contrasted positive and negative valence events, they found positive events to be associated with greater activation in VS, PCC, subcallosal gyrus, and lateral occipital cortex. However, that study did not distinguish between anticipation and outcome phases. Subcortically, the present results showed higher activation in ventromedial striatum for reward anticipation than loss anticipation. In addition, there was higher activation when avoiding a loss (negative reinforcement) than winning a reward in the ventrolateral striatum. These distinct reinforcement-related effects are consistent with a recent Activation Likelihood Estimation meta-analysis of the MID task in 1,271 adults across 50 studies, which reported greater activation during reward anticipation than for reward outcome in the VS, insula, and SMA; this meta-analysis also found greater activation for reward outcome compared to reward anticipation in the vmPFC and PCC 29. Comparing activation during the anticipation phase for large and small reward, we observe a noticeable gradation in response, with a markedly lower activation across subcortical regions, dorsomedial prefrontal, cingulate, and right inferior frontal cortex when anticipating smaller rewards. In contrast, we observe much less gradation of response during anticipation of loss. Though broader activation in subcortex, and right inferior frontal cortex during anticipation of large (as opposed to small) loss is apparent, the gradation is less pronounced than for reward anticipation. These observations are consistent with a more general aversion to loss, in contrast to a more value-dependent neural response in anticipation of reward.

Group-level task spatial reproducibility

The group-level spatial reproducibility plots demonstrated that group activation patterns for the primary contrasts of interest tend to be highly consistent across individuals for the SST, EN-back, and MID tasks, highlighting the robust processes of interest involved in response inhibition, working memory, and reward processing. The highest spatial reproducibility was observed in the EN-back task, particularly in working memory and faces vs. places contrasts; this may reflect both the robustness of the cognitive processes it engages as well as task design features. Specifically, the ENback task employs a block-design, as opposed to the event-related designs of the SST and MID tasks and does not require as fine-grained a decomposition of the time-series data like that required, for example, by the more numerous regressors (i.e., more conditions) of the MID task. Moreover, another factor influencing reproducibility is the number of trials per condition for any given participant, which is lower for the MID and emotional EN-back contrasts, where lower spatial reproducibility coefficients are observed.

It is important to note that these analyses describe the spatial reproducibility of task-fMRI with a focus on group-level data in which we varied sample size. Thus, these results speak to the ability of tasks to generate the same activation patterns across separate groups of participants. They should not be interpreted as findings of test-retest reliability, which concerns whether individual differences in task activation are similar across different scanning sessions. Test-retest reliability is also an essential task design feature for a longitudinal study 49. It cannot be assessed with just the single ABCD baseline assessment and the subsequent biennial assessments introduce potential confounding associated with developmental changes and perhaps even practice effects. However, this is a matter that can potentially be addressed with subsequent data releases. For example, there is variation in both the ages at which the baseline assessments were obtained (from 9 to 10) and variation in the intervals between the baseline and second assessments (scheduled for two years but this can vary between one and three years) which, combined, may make it possible to estimate age and practice effects separately, and consequently assess task test-retest reliability.

The present paper’s group reproducibility findings may help inform the neuroscientific community on which tasks/contrasts from the ABCD dataset provide the most consistent maps for mean group statistics which can, in turn, inform investigations comparing groups hypothesized to show activation differences.

Task sensitivity to individual differences in performance

The range in task performance and in ROI-level activation shown in Figure 2 suggest that the tasks are suitable for exploring inter-individual differences. The SST performance analyses showed that brain activations when successfully inhibiting were inversely correlated with SSRT but that these vertex/voxelwise relationships were modest, explaining a maximum of 2% (r=−0.14) (lateral thalamic voxels) of the individual behavioral performance differences. It is noteworthy that more varied correlation patterns were observed for activity during incorrect Stops, with better inhibitors (i.e., faster SSRT) showing greater activation in parts of the insula and the dlPFC. The areas showing these positive correlations with SSRT were those that likely reflect error-related processes (i.e., more activation for incorrect Stops than correct Stops), suggesting a greater interoceptive and cognitive control response to errors in better inhibitors. Boehler et al. 50 previously reported the left AI to be the sole region showing a strong relationship between brain activity during stopping and SSRT (r=−0.69 and −0.58, depending on how SSRT was measured), albeit with a sample size of just 15 participants. In the ENback task, performance analyses showed that brain activations were positively correlated with D’ measures, explaining a maximum of 2.2% (r=0.15) of the individual behavioral performance differences (the strongest associations were observed in medial thalamic voxels).

The relatively small correlations between brain activation and performance on these two cognitive tasks spotlight a fundamental challenge that motivates the ABCD Study. Namely, the size and scope of the ABCD Study provides an opportunity for a deep exploration of the mechanisms linking brain function to individual differences in numerous phenotypic measures and in individual developmental trajectories. Future research avenues include expanding beyond vertex/voxel-wise associations to incorporate multivariate approaches, and including other brain metrics such as task connectivity, intrinsic connectivity, brain structure, and anatomical connectivity. Improved assessments of behavior (e.g., computational modeling of task performance) and brain (e.g., incorporating individual differences in brain shape and function localization) may improve our ability to detect brain-behavior associations. Distal factors such as genetics or in utero exposures and current factors such as exercise, sleep, education and other intellectual pursuits may all contribute to the magnitude and patterns of brain-behavior associations. Consequently, incorporating heterogeneity across participants (e.g., biotyping) and exploiting the longitudinal aspects of the study wherein within-participant changes in behavior can be associated with within-participant neuro-developmental changes may prove especially sensitive approaches for linking brain to behavior.

The large sample size of the ABCD Study enables researchers to move beyond group-level phenomena towards understanding inter-individual differences and will, as the children age and repeat assessments, elucidate intra-individual differences in brain function. Obtaining sensitive measurements of brain function will enable researchers to track changes in function reliably through development, assess how brain function co-develops with brain structure, and identify what factors (genetic, environmental) affect brain development. Importantly, robust and reliable measures at the pre-adolescent stage will enable researchers to assess if future outcomes of interest (e.g., mental health problems, substance use, academic excellence, resilience) can be predicted by baseline brain function, thereby informing etiological mechanisms. In addition, through the parallel assessment of behavior and any lagged changes that might be observed in relationship to brain function, researchers can identify plausible causal influences of those behaviors on brain development, and vice versa.

Conclusions

The present results demonstrate robust fMRI activation patterns in tasks that engage inhibitory control, working memory, and reward processing. They establish a well-characterized baseline from which to follow the children in the ABCD Study throughout adolescent development. Overall, the task-activation patterns observed in this report are consistent with prior studies and underscore the value of the ABCD study as a scientific resource for tracking changes in brain function during adolescence and into early adulthood. In addition to enabling cross-sectional analyses of inter-individual and group differences, these activation patterns offer the potential for examining baseline predictors of future development and behavior and for quantifying changes in brain function that may arise from the numerous influences expected to affect development and behavior.

Methods

Sample

The ABCD sample was largely recruited through public, private, and charter elementary schools. ABCD adopted a population neuroscience approach to recruitment 30,51 by employing epidemiologically informed procedures to ensure demographic variation in its sample that would mirror the variation in the US population of 9- and 10-year-olds 52. A probability sampling of schools was conducted within the defined catchment areas of the study’s nationally distributed set of 21 recruitment sites. All children in each sampled school were invited to participate following classroom-based presentations, distribution of study materials, and telephone screening for eligibility. Exclusions included common MRI contraindications (such as cardiac pacemakers and defibrillators, internal pacing wires, cochlear and metallic implants and Swan-Ganz catheters), inability to understand or speak English fluently, uncorrected vision, hearing or sensorimotor impairments, a history of major neurological disorders, gestational age <28 weeks, birth weight <1,200 grams, birth complications that resulted in hospitalization for more than one month, current diagnosis of schizophrenia, moderate or severe autism spectrum disorder, a history of traumatic brain injury, or unwillingness to complete assessments. The ABCD sample also includes 2105 monozygotic and dizygotic twins. Consent (parents) and assent (children) were obtained from all participants and the ABCD study was approved by the appropriate institutional review boards. Data collection and analyses were not performed blind to the conditions of the experiments. The ABCD Study’s anonymized and curated data, including all assessment domains, is released annually to the research community. The ABCD study is a single cohort, observational and longitudinal design that has not randomization of participants to groups. Information on how to access ABCD data through the NIMH Data Archive (NDA) is available on the ABCD study data sharing webpage: https://abcdstudy.org/scientists_data_sharing.html. Further information on research design is available in the Life Sciences Reporting Summary linked to this article.

Inclusion criteria for the current study were predetermined by the ABCD DAIRC53. In brief, participants were included if they had 1) two fMRI runs per task, 2) cortical vertex and subcortical voxel data available at the time of analysis, 3) hemispheric mean beta-weights within two standard deviations of the sample mean for each task, 4) at least 200 degrees of freedom over the two scan runs, 5) had mean FD < 0.9 mm for both runs, 6) met task-specific performance criteria (described in “Behavioral task-performance”), and 7) had complete information for covariates of interest (age, sex, scanner serial number, race, puberty54) and highest parent education (see Table 1 for details on datapoints that were excluded from analyses, their rationale, and the number of participants remaining after each step of exclusions). This resulted in varying sample sizes and demographics among the fMRI tasks, which were as follows: SST (N=5,024, mean age=9.96±0.63, 49.82% male), EN-back (N=6,009, mean age=9.96±0.63, 50.77% male), and MID (N=6,657, mean age=9.95±0.63, 50.43% males). No statistical methods were used to pre-determine sample sizes, but our sample sizes exceed those reported in previous publications 18,28,35. Given these large sample sizes, data distribution was assumed to be normal, though this was not formally tested (see Figure 2 for details on data distributions).

Task fMRI data for 1512 participants obtained on Philips scanners were also excluded from this paper due to incorrect post-processing. Corrected data will be available in the ABCD Data Release 3.0. An official statement providing more details is available on the ABCD study website (https://abcdstudy.org/scientists/data-sharing/). An R script is available at https://github.com/ABCD-STUDY/fMRI-cleanup to remove Philips fMRI data from tabulated data. Table 2 presents the demographic composition of both samples who were included and who were excluded from the current study. Although the differences between these two samples are statistically significant, which is not surprising with over 10,000 participants in the analyses, the effect sizes were small (Cramer’s V ≤.16) and, most importantly, the fMRI samples showed considerable demographic diversity (and in this regard are very similar to the full sample). In addition, propensity weighting scores are available in the ABCD Data Analysis and Exploration Portal (DEAP), for researchers who wish to adjust sample estimates to population-level demographics.

fMRI Tasks

The ABCD Study’s fMRI behavioral tasks include the SST, EN-Back, and MID tasks. These tasks were selected to probe inhibitory control, emotion processing and working memory, and reward processing 53. Participants practiced the three tasks prior to scanning to ensure that they understood the instructions and were familiar with the response collection device. While the fMRI tasks were always collected last as part of the fixed order of the scanning session, the order in which the fMRI tasks occurred was randomized across subjects, as well as the ordering of the event-related fMRI task’s trials. There were 12 trial-order variations (pseudorandomized) of the MID and SST tasks. Siblings were given the same order of scans and trial-order version of the MID and SST tasks to minimize within-family variability. For further details, see Casey et. al 53. The ABCD imaging protocol was designed to extend the benefits of high temporal and spatial resolution of imaging protocols of the HCP 55 with the multiple scanner systems of participating sites 56.

Stop Signal Task (SST)

The SST 57 presented leftward and rightward facing arrows in serial order (“Go” stimuli). Participants indicated the direction of the arrows using a two-button response box (left and right buttons). Participants were instructed to respond as quickly and accurately as possible but were told not to respond on trials in which a left or right arrow was followed by an arrow pointing upward (the “Stop” signal).

The SST had an event-related design with two runs. Each had 180 trials, of which 30 were “Stop” trials, yielding a total of 60 “Stop” trials and 300 “Go” trials. Each trial lasted 1sec. The time between the “Go” and “Stop” signals (the StopSignal Delay; SSD) varied dynamically based on a participant’s success on the prior trial so as to achieve a 50% success rate (starting at 50 msec, the SSD increased by 50 msec if the participant successfully stopped on the previous trial, and decreased by 50 msec if he/she responded; see Figure 1-a).

Emotional N-back Task

The EN-back task was a modified version of a traditional N-back task 18,58 using a block design that added elements of facial and emotional processing. This task was designed so that through fMRI contrast subtraction it would be possible to investigate working memory, facial recognition, and emotional processes independently or to investigate the interaction between working memory, faces, and emotion. The current analysis focused on contrasts that isolated working memory and facial recognition/emotion; these contrasts were shown to be effective in eliciting neural responses consistent with standard working memory, facial recognition, and emotion in adults in the task’s original usage in the HCP 18. Participants saw a series of stimuli and indicated whether each one was the same or different than the stimulus N items earlier (i.e., “N back”). The EN-back task had two conditions: a 2-back as the active condition and a 0-back as the baseline condition, which included similar visuo-motor demands but lower working memory load. In the 0-back condition, participants indicated if each stimulus matched a single target presented at the beginning of the block, thereby obviating the need to maintain and update a two-item working memory load throughout the task. Responses on the 2back and 0-back were input on a two-button keypad, with one button indicating the stimulus was a match and the other indicating no match (see Figure 1-b).

The EN-back consisted of two runs, each containing eight blocks of trials and four 15 sec periods containing just a fixation cross. Blocks contained 10 trials lasting 2.5 sec each and were preceded by a 2.5 sec instruction screen indicating the condition for the upcoming block. Of the 10 trials in each block, 2 were targets, 2–3 were non-target lures, and the remainder were non-lures (i.e., stimuli only presented once). There are 160 trials in total with 96 unique stimuli of 4 different stimulus types (24 unique stimuli per type). Three-quarters of the stimuli types were human faces, demonstrating happy, fearful, or neutral facial expressions, with facial expression stimulus type held constant within each block. The faces used were all adult faces, which was considered ideal given previous research suggesting that children demonstrate similar, but stronger neural responses to adult faces relative to child faces 59,60. Faces were racially diverse and derived from two pre-existing collections: the NimStim emotional stimulus set 61 and the Racially Diverse Affective Expressions (RADIATE) set of stimuli 62. Additionally, images of places were used as a fourth stimulus type. The place stimuli were taken from prior visual perception studies 63,64. For the working memory component, the main contrast is a block design analysis contrasting 2-back and 0-back (8 blocks each). Finally, a post-scan recognition memory test was performed 18,53 to measure memory processes associated with hippocampal functioning. The task included 48 old stimuli presented during the EN-back task and 48 new stimuli, with equal numbers of each stimulus type in the old and new stimulus sets (12 each of happy, fearful, and neutral facial expressions as well as places in each set). A total of 96 pictures were presented during the recognition memory test. Participants were asked to rate each picture as either “Old” or “New”. Each picture was presented for 2s followed immediately by a 1s presentation of a fixation cross. The task assessed memory for stimuli presented during the EN-back and took approximately 5–10 min.

Monetary Incentive Delay (MID) Task

The MID task included both anticipation and receipt of reward and loss 65,66. Participants attempted to win or avoid losing money by quickly responding to cued stimuli using a response box in their dominant hand. This task is entirely focused on response time rather than response choice; hence there was only one response option on this task. For “win” trials, participants could “win” or “not win” $5.00 or $.20 depending on whether they responded in the time allotted. For “lose” trials, they could either “not lose” or “lose” the same amounts by responding within the time frame. In “neutral” trials participants completed the same action but with no money available to be won or lost (see Figure 1-c).

The MID had an event-related design. The specific sequence of each trial was as follows: participants saw a cue denoting the trial type (2s), with “win” trials shown in a pink circle, “lose” trials shown in a yellow square, and “neutral” trials shown in a blue triangle. Then, participants viewed a fixation cross of jittered duration (1.5–4 sec), followed by a signal to respond, denoted by a black shape that corresponded to the trial type. The duration in which participants were able to respond (i.e., duration of the response signal) varied between trials (0.15–0.5 sec). The time allowed to respond at the beginning of the task was determined by the participant’s performance during a practice session prior to scanning and, during scanning, was adjusted after every third incentivized trial based on the overall accuracy rate of the previous six trials in order to produce a 60% accuracy rate across the task. If the participant’s accuracy fell below the target accuracy level, the duration of the target was lengthened. If the participant’s accuracy was above the target accuracy level, the target duration was shortened. Immediately after responding, participants received written feedback (e.g., “You won $5”) which was presented for 2s minus the duration of the response target. Each run consisted of 50 contiguous trials (10 per trial type) presented in pseudorandom order and lasted 5:42. Participants were compensated based on their performance on the task with mean earnings being $20 and maximum possible earnings being $60.

fMRI acquisition and preprocessing

High spatial and temporal resolution simultaneous multi-slice/multiband EPI task-based fMRI scans, with fast integrated distortion correction, were acquired to examine functional activity. For Siemens and GE 3 tesla scanners, the scanning parameters were: matrix of 90 × 90, 60 slices, FOV= 216 × 216, TE/TR (msec) = 800/30, flip angle= 52 degrees, and resolution (mm) = 2.4 × 2.4 × 2.4. The fMRI acquisitions (2.4 mm isotropic, TR = 800 ms) used multiband EPI with slice acceleration factor 6. The order of the three fMRI tasks was randomized across participants. The full details of the imaging acquisition protocol were previously described in Casey et al. 53. The ABCD Data Analysis, Informatics, & Resource Center (DAIRC) performed centralized initial quality control and processing of the fMRI data. All MRI assessments were reviewed by a neuroradiologist for incidental findings. Using a combination of automated and manual methods, the fMRI datasets were quality controlled for problems such as acquisition protocol compliance, imaging artifacts, motion or file corruption. Processing steps subsequent to fMRI preprocessing include the removal of initial frames to ensure equilibration of the T1w signal and normalization of voxel time series by dividing by the mean across time of each voxel. The fMRI preprocessing pipeline started with a within-volume head motion estimation and correction by computing rigid body transformations between the first time point and each subsequent one. Scans were further processed for image distortions resulting from B0 field inhomogeneity, within voxel field gradients and gradient nonlinearities. 2.4 mm isotropic resampling was performed in order to align fMRI volumes across each participant, and a registration matrix computed with the T1w image. Estimates of task-related activation strength were computed at the individual subject level using a AFNI’s 3dDeconvolve (Cox, 1996), which implemented a general linear model (GLM) applied to each voxel’s time-series with additional nuisance regressors and motion estimates. Hemodynamic response functions are modelled with two parameters using a gamma variate basis function plus its temporal derivative (using AFNI’s ‘SPMG’ option within 3dDeconvolve). Fast oscillatory signals within the motion estimates related to respiration, between 0.31 to .043 Hz, were temporally filtered with an infinite impulse response filter. Framewise displacement (FD) was then calculated from the filtered motion estimates, and frames with FD >0.9 mm were censored. Preprocessed time courses were sampled onto the cortical surface for each individual subject, and then registered to the standard FreeSurfer surface atlas (fsaverage). After projecting to the surface, the data were smoothed along the cortical surface (5 mm). Voxels containing cortical gray matter were projected onto the surface by sampling values 1 mm from the gray/white boundary, into cortical gray matter, at each vertex (using FreeSurfer’s mri_vol2surf with “white” surface, “-projdist 1” option, and default “nearest” interpolation). Average beta coefficients and standard errors were then computed for each of the two runs of each task and for each participant, weighted by the nominal degrees of freedom (number of frames remaining after motion censoring minus number of model parameters). Data used in the current study were derived from the data included in the ABCD data release 2.0.1, and included GLM beta coefficients and standard errors of the mean (SEM; calculated from the ratio of the beta and t statistic) calculated for each voxel and vertex. The full details of the task fMRI quality control and processing pipelines were previously described 53,56.

Task models included stimulus timing for each condition and linear contrasts of conditions 56. For MID and SST analyses, events were modelled as instantaneous. The EN-back was programmed as a block design. The SST model included regressors for successful Go trials (“Correct Go”), failed Go trials (“Incorrect Go”), successful Stop trials (“Correct Stop”), and failed Stop trials (“Incorrect Stop”), creating contrasts of interest correct Stop vs correct Go and incorrect Stop vs correct Go. The EN-back model included separate regressors for the 0-back faces, 2-back faces, 0-back places, and 2-back places conditions; the contrasts of interest were 0-back vs. Fixation, 2-back vs. Fixation, 2-back vs. 0-back, Faces vs. Places, negative vs neutral faces and positive vs neutral faces. The MID model contained separate regressors for the different anticipation periods (large and small rewards or losses and no incentive (“neutral”) trials) and large and small win and loss feedback. MID computed contrasts of interest were large reward versus neutral anticipation, small reward versus neutral anticipation, large loss versus neutral anticipation, small loss versus neutral anticipation, reward positive versus negative feedback, loss positive versus negative feedback.

Behavioral task-performance

Poor performance on the SST leading to exclusion was determined by: fewer than 150 Go trials, less than 60% correct on Go trials, incorrect Go trials greater than 30%, late Go trials (across correct and incorrect trials) greater than 30%, no response on Go trials greater than 30%, fewer than 30 Stop trials, and Stop trial accuracy lower than 20% or greater than 80%. The SST used an adaptive algorithm to achieve a 50% success rate. To accomplish this, the onset between the Go and Stop signal was varied based on individual performance. The adaptive algorithm allowed for calculation of the Stop Signal Reaction Time (SSRT, the time required to inhibit the motor response 57), which was used as the performance variable in analyses assessing individual differences in response inhibition ability. The SSRT was computed by subtracting the median stop signal delay of all stop trials from the nth percentile Go reaction time, where n represents the percentage of successful inhibitions (for details on the theoretical underpinnings for this estimation, see Logan and Cowan 67). Participants with SSRT < 50 msec were excluded from the analysis.

For the EN-back, D’ was computed for both the 2-back and 0-back conditions by calculating each participant’s hit rate, the proportion of targets for which the participant correctly indicated a match, and false alarm rate, the proportion of nontargets for which the participant incorrectly indicated a match or did not respond. The hit and false alarm rates were then z-transformed. D’ was calculated as the z-transformed hit rate minus the z-transformed false alarm rate. D’ for the postscan recognition memory test was also calculated for each participant in the EN-back fMRI sample as the z-transformed hit rate minus the z-transformed false alarm rate. Children were excluded from the analyses if D’ was less than 0.

The MID task used an adaptive algorithm to maintain accuracy at 60%. To be included in the analysis, across the two runs, children had to have at least four events for each trial type, including positive and negative feedback.

Participant inclusion criteria

Inclusion criteria for the tasks were predetermined by the ABCD DAIRC 53. In brief, participants were included if they had 1) two fMRI runs per task, 2) cortical vertex and subcortical voxel data available at the time of analysis, 3) hemispheric mean beta-weights within two standard deviations of the sample mean for each task, 4) at least 200 degrees of freedom over the two scan runs, 5) had mean framewise displacement < 0.9 mm for both runs, 6) met task-specific performance criteria (described above), and 7) had complete information for covariates of interest (age, sex, scanner serial number, race, puberty (Peterson et al. 54) and highest parent education). The quality control and inclusion criteria as well as the number of participants remaining after each step of exclusions are detailed in Table 1.

Statistical Analyses

Task activation maps

The Permutation Analysis of Linear Models (PALM)’s GLM (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/PALM/ was used to generate cortical and subcortical task-specific functional activation maps, contrasting the fMRI beta weights against zero, with age (months), sex, scanner serial number, race, puberty and highest parent education included as nuisance covariates. Scanner serial number and ethnicity were entered as dummy coded variables. All covariates were demeaned. The calculations accommodated the non-independence of the participants by incorporating information on sibling status into the exchangeability blocks of the permutation analyses. Cohen’s d effect sizes were computed for each voxel/vertex as the mean of the residualized betas of the contrast divided by the standard deviation of the residualized betas:

Cohen'sd=Mean(residualizedbetas)Standarddeviation(residualizedbetas)

Thus, a Cohen’s d effect size of 1 indicates that the mean beta weight differs from zero by one standard deviation. A threshold of d ≥ 0.2 was applied to the task activation maps in figures 3,4,5 and 6.

Participants with one or two siblings raise the issue of having dependent and independent participants in the analyses, and implies that the data are not homoscedastic, i.e., all observations do not share the same variance as there are three variance groups determined by family information (0, 1, or 2 siblings). To account for family dependence and adjust the Cohen’s d values accordingly, we computed first, for each task, a t statistic map using permutation analyses (N=100,000) with the same covariates mentioned above. Next, we repeated the same permutation analysis after adding sibling status as a dummy-coded variable (each family received a unique value, shared by the siblings of that family) implemented with PALM’s exchangeability blocks structure 68, consisting of two columns, with each column indicating a deeper level of dependence (i.e. a unique dummy coding system where indices on one level indicate how the unique sub-indices of the next level should be shuffled). This restricts the shuffling to only occur among the observations that share the same family index, i.e., within block only. In this kind of permutation, variances are estimated for each block, and the AspinWelch v statistics that are robust to heteroscedasticity are computed, instead of t statistics, for each voxel/vertex. Finally, cortical and subcortical Cohen’s d maps are weighted by the t statistic/v statistic ratio to generate another set of Cohen’s d maps adjusted for family information. Only the latter maps are reported in the results.

To get an estimate of inter-individual variation in activation maps, beta-weights were extracted from relevant task-specific ROIs known to show robust task-specific activation. These ROIs included bilateral IFG for the SST, bilateral dlPFC for the EN-back task and bilateral AI for the MID task. To create the IFG ROI, we combined the pars orbitalis, triangularis and opercularis parcels from the aparc2009 FreeSurfer atlas 69. To create the dlPFC ROI, we combined the bilateral middle frontal gyrus and the inferior frontal sulcus parcels from the same atlas.

Performance correlations.

To assess the sensitivity of activation patterns to individual differences in behavioral performance, vertex and voxelwise whole brain correlation analyses were calculated with PALM, with performance measures included as the independent variables in the design matrices and the same covariates as used above. In addition, beta-weights were extracted from relevant ROIs known to show robust task-specific activation. These ROIs included bilateral inferior frontal and cingulate gyri for the SST and bilateral dlPFC for the EN-back task. For the SST contrasts (correct Stop vs. correct Go and incorrect Stop vs. correct Go), Pearson’s correlation coefficients were computed between beta-weights and SSRT measures. For the 0-back versus fixation and 2-back versus fixation contrasts of the EN-back task, whole-brain Pearson’s correlation coefficients were computed between beta-weights and D’ measures derived from the 0-back and 2-back conditions, respectively. For the MID task, as its individualized adaptive algorithm does not yield a suitable performance measure directly assessing sensitivity to reward,

we went beyond the task performance measures (used with the SST and EN-back tasks) to search for correlates. We describe the relationship between MID activation and several measures assessing sensitivity to reward in Supplementary note 6.

Group-level spatial reproducibility

To assess the spatial reproducibility of group activation maps and performance correlation maps (for SST and EN-back), we calculated the vertexwise/voxelwise correlation between a “gold standard” map and independent samples of varying sizes. First, we split the participants into two equally sized independent groups, stratified by sex and scanner. Separately for each of the two groups we calculated residualized beta weights according to a linear regression model fit on the following demeaned variables: age, sex, scanner serial number, race, puberty score and highest parent education. One group was designated the “gold standard” from which we calculated a single group activation map by computing the Cohen’s d measure of effect size from all its participants, in addition to Pearson’s correlation coefficients between betaweights and performance measures. In the second group, we sampled random subsets, from n=2 to n=2,500 with 2,000 repetitions at each size and generated a Cohen’s d activation map. Each subset was sampled from the entire second group.

We calculated vertex/voxelwise Pearson’s correlation coefficients between each of these generated activation maps and the independent “gold standard” activation map. Average correlations by sample size were generated by computing the mean correlation over the 2,000 repetitions at each size. These calculations were applied to both cortical vertices and subcortical voxels. Python 3 was used to carry out this analysis. The Python 3 codes are available in the “Supplementary Software ” linked to the article.

Code availability

The Python codes used to compute reproducibility curves undertaken as part of this study and which generate the figures are openly available in ‘Supplementary Data’ and at https://github.com/sahahn/ABCD_Consortium_Analysis. The following additional software packages used for this study are freely and openly available: PALM (v.alpha116): https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/PALM/

Data Availability

The ABCD Study anonymized data including all assessment domains is released annually to the research community. Information on how to access ABCD data through the NIMH Data Archive (NDA) is available on the ABCD study data sharing webpage: https://abcdstudy.org/scientists_data_sharing.html. Instructions on how to create an NDA study are available at https://nda.nih.gov/training/modules/study.html). The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time.

The ABCD data used in this report came from 10.15154/1520620. DOIs can be found at https://dx.doi.org/10.15154/1520620. The ABCD data used in this report also came from the fast-track data release. The raw data are available at https://nda.nih.gov/edit_collection.html?id=2573. Activation maps and spatial reproducibility data are available in ‘Supplementary Data 1’ and ‘Supplementary Data 2’, respectively.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material

Acknowledgements

Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive DevelopmentSM (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10,000 children age 9–10 and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study® is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041048, U01DA050989, U01DA051016, U01DA041022, U01DA051018, U01DA051037, U01DA050987, U01DA041174, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041028, U01DA041134, U01DA050988, U01DA051039, U01DA041156, U01DA041025, U01DA041120, U01DA051038, U01DA041148, U01DA041093, U01DA041089, U24DA041123, U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/consortium_members/. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in analysis or writing of this report. Most ABCD research sites rely on a central Institutional Review Board at the University of California, San Diego for the ethical review and approval of the research protocol, with a few sites obtaining local IRB approval. The views expressed in this manuscript are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the official views of the National Institutes of Health, the Department of Health and Human Services, the United States federal government, or ABCD consortium investigators.

Computations were performed on the Vermont Advanced Computing Core supported in part by NSF award No. OAC1827314.

Footnotes

Competing Interests Statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

References

  • 1.Membride H Mental health: early intervention and prevention in children and young people. Br. J. Nurs. Mark Allen Publ. 25, 552–554, 556–557 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Verbruggen F et al. A consensus guide to capturing the ability to inhibit actions and impulsive behaviors in the stop-signal task. eLife 8, (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Zhang R, Geng X & Lee TMC Large-scale functional neural network correlates of response inhibition: an fMRI meta-analysis. Brain Struct. Funct. 222, 3973–3990 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Dwyer DB et al. Large-scale brain network dynamics supporting adolescent cognitive control. J. Neurosci. 34, 14096–14107 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Luna B, Marek S, Larsen B, Tervo-Clemmens B & Chahal R An Integrative Model of the Maturation of Cognitive Control. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 38, 151–170 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Nk F & J K Developmental changes in performance monitoring: how electrophysiological data can enhance our understanding of error and feedback processing in childhood and adolescence. Behav. Brain Res. 263, 122–132 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Segalowitz SJ, Santesso DL & Jetha MK Electrophysiological changes during adolescence: a review. Brain Cogn. 72, 86–100 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Alahyane N, Brien DC, Coe BC, Stroman PW & Munoz DP Developmental improvements in voluntary control of behavior: Effect of preparation in the fronto-parietal network? NeuroImage 98, 103–117 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ordaz SJ, Foran W, Velanova K & Luna B Longitudinal Growth Curves of Brain Function Underlying Inhibitory Control through Adolescence. J. Neurosci. 33, 18109–18124 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Yaple ZA, Stevens WD & Arsalidou M Meta-analyses of the n-back working memory task: fMRI evidence of age-related changes in prefrontal cortex involvement across the adult lifespan. NeuroImage 196, 16–31 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Rottschy C et al. Modelling neural correlates of working memory: a coordinate-based meta-analysis. NeuroImage 60, 830–846 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Yaple Z & Arsalidou M N-back Working Memory Task: Meta-analysis of Normative fMRI Studies With Children. Child Dev. 89, 2010–2022 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Satterthwaite TD et al. Functional Maturation of the Executive System during Adolescence. J. Neurosci. 33, 16249–16261 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Thomason ME et al. Development of spatial and verbal working memory capacity in the human brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 316–332 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.O’Hare ED, Lu LH, Houston SM, Bookheimer SY & Sowell ER Neurodevelopmental Changes in Verbal Working Memory Load-Dependency: An fMRI Investigation. NeuroImage 42, 1678–1685 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Rosenberg MD et al. Behavioral and neural signatures of working memory in childhood. bioRxiv 659409 (2019) doi: 10.1101/659409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Schweizer S et al. The Impact of Affective Information on Working Memory: A Pair of Meta-Analytic Reviews of Behavioral and Neuroimaging Evidence. Psychol. Bull. 145, 566–609 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Barch DM et al. Function in the human connectome: task-fMRI and individual differences in behavior. NeuroImage 80, 169–189 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Gauthier I et al. The fusiform ‘face area’ is part of a network that processes faces at the individual level. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 495–504 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Fuhrmann D et al. Perception and recognition of faces in adolescence. Sci. Rep. 6, (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Cohen Kadosh K Differing Processing Abilities for Specific Face Properties in Mid-Childhood and Adulthood. Front. Psychol. 2, (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Scherf KS, Behrmann M & Dahl RE Facing changes and changing faces in adolescence: a new model for investigating adolescent-specific interactions between pubertal, brain and behavioral development. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2, 199–219 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Tahmasebi AM et al. Creating probabilistic maps of the face network in the adolescent brain: a multicentre functional MRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 938–957 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kadosh KC What can emerging cortical face networks tell us about mature brain organisation? Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 1, 246–255 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Cohen Kadosh K, Johnson MH, Henson RNA, Dick F & Blakemore S-J Differential face-network adaptation in children, adolescents and adults. NeuroImage 69, 11–20 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Scherf KS, Behrmann M, Humphreys K & Luna B Visual category-selectivity for faces, places and objects emerges along different developmental trajectories. Dev. Sci. 10, F15–30 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Silverman MH, Jedd K & Luciana M Neural networks involved in adolescent reward processing: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage 122, 427–439 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Cao Z et al. Mapping adolescent reward anticipation, receipt, and prediction error during the monetary incentive delay task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 262–283 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Oldham S et al. The anticipation and outcome phases of reward and loss processing: A neuroimaging metaanalysis of the monetary incentive delay task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 3398–3418 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Falk EB et al. What is a representative brain? Neuroscience meets population science. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 110, 17615–17622 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Baker M 1,500 scientists lift the lid on reproducibility. Nature 533, 452–454 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Song X, Panych LP, Chou Y-H & Chen N-K A Study of Long-Term fMRI Reproducibility Using Data-Driven Analysis Methods. Int. J. Imaging Syst. Technol. 24, 339–349 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Thirion B et al. Analysis of a large fMRI cohort: Statistical and methodological issues for group analyses. NeuroImage 35, 105–120 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Turner BO, Paul EJ, Miller MB & Barbey AK Small sample sizes reduce the replicability of task-based fMRI studies. Commun. Biol. 1, 1–10 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Van Essen DC et al. The WU-Minn Human Connectome Project: an overview. NeuroImage 80, 62–79 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Owens MM, Duda B, Sweet LH & MacKillop J Distinct functional and structural neural underpinnings of working memory. NeuroImage 174, 463–471 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Rae CL, Hughes LE, Weaver C, Anderson MC & Rowe JB Selection and stopping in voluntary action: a meta-analysis and combined fMRI study. NeuroImage 86, 381–391 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Hung Y, Gaillard SL, Yarmak P & Arsalidou M Dissociations of cognitive inhibition, response inhibition, and emotional interference: Voxelwise ALE meta-analyses of fMRI studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 4065–4082 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Swick D, Ashley V & Turken U Are the neural correlates of stopping and not going identical? Quantitative meta-analysis of two response inhibition tasks. NeuroImage 56, 1655–1665 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Garavan H, Ross TJ, Murphy K, Roche R.a. P. & Stein EA Dissociable executive functions in the dynamic control of behavior: inhibition, error detection, and correction. NeuroImage 17, 1820–1829 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Owen AM, McMillan KM, Laird AR & Bullmore E N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 46–59 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Wager TD & Smith EE Neuroimaging studies of working memory: a meta-analysis. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 3, 255–274 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Niendam TA et al. Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 12, 241–268 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR & Schacter DL The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 1–38 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Fox MD et al. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 102, 9673–9678 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Baird AA et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of facial affect recognition in children and adolescents. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 38, 195–199 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Hare TA et al. Biological substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in adolescence during an emotional go-nogo task. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 927–934 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Heitzeg MM et al. Effect of GABRA2 genotype on development of incentive-motivation circuitry in a sample enriched for alcoholism risk. Neuropsychopharmacol. Off. Publ. Am. Coll. Neuropsychopharmacol. 39, 3077–3086 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Elliott ML et al. What Is the Test-Retest Reliability of Common Task-Functional MRI Measures? New Empirical Evidence and a Meta-Analysis: Psychol. Sci. (2020) doi: 10.1177/0956797620916786. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Boehler CN, Appelbaum LG, Krebs RM, Hopf J-M & Woldorff MG The influence of different Stopsignal response time estimation procedures on behavior-behavior and brain-behavior correlations. Behav. Brain Res. 229, 123–130 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Paus T Population Neuroscience. (Springer-Verlag, 2013). doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-36450-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Garavan H et al. Recruiting the ABCD sample: Design considerations and procedures. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci 32, 16–22 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Casey BJ et al. The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study: Imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci 32, 43–54 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Petersen AC, Crockett L, Richards M & Boxer A A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. J. Youth Adolesc. 17, 117–133 (1988). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Glasser MF et al. A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature 536, 171–178 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Hagler DJ et al. Image processing and analysis methods for the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study. NeuroImage 202, 116091 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Logan GD, Schachar RJ & Tannock R Impulsivity and Inhibitory Control. Psychol. Sci. 8, 60–64 (1997). [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Cohen AO, xxx, dsadas & ddd, sss. The impact of emotional cues on short-term and long-term memory during adolescence. Proc. Soc. Neurosci. San Diego CA 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Hoehl S, Brauer J, Brasse G, Striano T & Friederici AD Children’s processing of emotions expressed by peers and adults: an fMRI study. Soc. Neurosci. 5, 543–559 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Marusak HA, Carré JM & Thomason ME The stimuli drive the response: an fMRI study of youth processing adult or child emotional face stimuli. NeuroImage 83, 679–689 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Tottenham N et al. The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Res. 168, 242–249 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Conley MI et al. The racially diverse affective expression (RADIATE) face stimulus set. Psychiatry Res. 270, 1059–1067 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Kanwisher N Neural events and perceptual awareness. Cognition 79, 89–113 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Park S & Chun MM Different roles of the parahippocampal place area (PPA) and retrosplenial cortex (RSC) in panoramic scene perception. NeuroImage 47, 1747–1756 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Knutson B, Westdorp A, Kaiser E & Hommer D FMRI Visualization of Brain Activity during a Monetary Incentive Delay Task. NeuroImage 12, 20–27 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Yau W-YW et al. Nucleus Accumbens Response to Incentive Stimuli Anticipation in Children of Alcoholics: Relationships with Precursive Behavioral Risk and Lifetime Alcohol Use. J. Neurosci. 32, 2544–2551 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Logan GD & Cowan WB On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control. Psychol. Rev. 91, 295 (1984). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Winkler AM, Webster MA, Vidaurre D, Nichols TE & Smith SM Multi-level block permutation. NeuroImage 123, 253–268 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Destrieux C, Fischl B, Dale A & Halgren E Automatic parcellation of human cortical gyri and sulci using standard anatomical nomenclature. NeuroImage 53, 1–15 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Material

Data Availability Statement

The ABCD Study anonymized data including all assessment domains is released annually to the research community. Information on how to access ABCD data through the NIMH Data Archive (NDA) is available on the ABCD study data sharing webpage: https://abcdstudy.org/scientists_data_sharing.html. Instructions on how to create an NDA study are available at https://nda.nih.gov/training/modules/study.html). The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time.

The ABCD data used in this report came from 10.15154/1520620. DOIs can be found at https://dx.doi.org/10.15154/1520620. The ABCD data used in this report also came from the fast-track data release. The raw data are available at https://nda.nih.gov/edit_collection.html?id=2573. Activation maps and spatial reproducibility data are available in ‘Supplementary Data 1’ and ‘Supplementary Data 2’, respectively.

RESOURCES