Skip to main content
iScience logoLink to iScience
. 2022 Mar 14;25(4):104077. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2022.104077

Predictive attenuation of touch and tactile gating are distinct perceptual phenomena

Konstantina Kilteni 1,2,, H Henrik Ehrsson 1
PMCID: PMC8968059  PMID: 35372807

Summary

In recent decades, research on somatosensory perception has led to two important observations. First, self-generated touches that are predicted by voluntary movements become attenuated compared with externally generated touches of the same intensity (attenuation). Second, externally generated touches feel weaker and are more difficult to detect during movement than at rest (gating). At present, researchers often consider gating and attenuation the same suppression process; however, this assumption is unwarranted because, despite more than 40 years of research, no study has combined them in a single paradigm. We quantified how people perceive self-generated and externally generated touches during movement and rest. We show that whereas voluntary movement gates the precision of both self-generated and externally generated touch, the amplitude of self-generated touch is robustly attenuated compared with externally generated touch. Furthermore, attenuation and gating do not interact and are not correlated, and we conclude that they represent distinct perceptual phenomena.

Subject areas: Biological sciences, Neuroscience, Sensory neuroscience

Graphical abstract

graphic file with name fx1.jpg

Highlights

  • We tested the perception of self-generated and external touch during movement and rest

  • The intensity of self-generated touch is reduced during movement and rest (attenuation)

  • The precision of self-generated and external touch is reduced during movement (gating)

  • Attenuation and gating neither interact nor correlate, and are distinct phenomena


Biological sciences; Neuroscience; Sensory neuroscience

Introduction

Imagine you are at your doctor’s for a medical examination. Upon her request, you apply pressure with your index finger on your leg to indicate exactly where you feel the pain. The pressure you feel on your leg and the tip of your finger is feedback from your voluntary finger movement, and is called somatosensory reafference. Imagine that the doctor now applies pressure with her index finger on the same spot on your leg to reproduce and confirm your sensations. This pressure is generated by the doctor, not by you, and is called somatosensory exafference. Now imagine that the doctor asks you to first keep your leg relaxed and then flex and extend it while you or her continuously apply pressure on your leg. You therefore experience your (reafferent) or her (exafferent) touches on your leg while it is moving or resting. Distinguishing between these four conditions is fundamental for your sensorimotor control; your nervous system must know both the source of the touch and the state of your limb to appropriately use the sensory feedback. A cutaneous mechanoreceptor in your peripheral nervous system, however, is unable to distinguish whether a touch is reafferent or exafferent, and, thus, this distinction must be made centrally where tactile signals from the skin, sensory information from muscles and joints, and information from motor commands are available. How, then, does the central nervous system classify somatosensory signals during movement?

Several experimental studies in humans have shown that the brain attenuates somatosensory reafference (i.e., all somatosensory inputs generated by one’s own movement, including inputs originating directly from the moving body part and inputs from other passive body parts being touched) compared with exafference. In behavioral research, this process refers to participants perceiving self-generated strokes, forces, or taps as weaker than external equivalents of the same intensity (Asimakidou et al., 2022; Bays et al., 2005, 2006; Blakemore et al., 1999b; Kilteni et al., 2018, 2019, 2020, 2021; Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2017a, 2017b; Shergill et al., 2003; Walsh et al., 2011). This somatosensory attenuation is related to reduced activity in the secondary somatosensory cortex (Blakemore et al., 1998; Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2020; Shergill et al., 2013) and the cerebellum (Blakemore et al., 1998, 2001; Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2020) and increased connectivity between the two areas (Blakemore et al., 1999a; Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2020) during self-generated touches compared with externally generated touches. Somatosensory attenuation was observed in 98% or 315 of 322 people across a wide age range (Wolpe et al., 2016), and is considered one of the reasons why we cannot tickle ourselves (Blakemore et al., 2000b; Leavens and Bard, 2016; Weiskrantz et al., 1971).

Sensory attenuation is not exclusive to humans; similar strategies are used by other species across the animal kingdom (for reviews, see Brooks and Cullen, 2019; Crapse and Sommer, 2008; Cullen, 2004; Schneider and Mooney, 2018; Straka et al., 2018). For example, during self-chirping, the cricket’s central auditory processing is inhibited (both presynaptically and postsynaptically) in phase with the insect’s chirps to prevent desensitization of its auditory system while maintaining sensitivity to external sounds (Poulet and Hedwig, 2003, 2006). In mice, auditory cortical responses to self-generated sounds are attenuated, and this attenuation is present only for the tone frequencies the animal has associated with its locomotion and is absent when the same sounds are externally produced (Audette et al., 2021; Schneider et al., 2018). A weakly electric fish (Fukutomi and Carlson, 2020) is able to respond exclusively to externally generated electrical discharges by attenuating its predicted electrosensory reafference (Cullen, 2004; Sawtell, 2017). In primates, activity in the vestibular nucleus in response to vestibular reafference is attenuated during active head movements compared with passive head movements, allowing the animal to maintain its head and body posture and activate vestibular-related reflexes when appropriate (Brooks et al., 2015; Crapse and Sommer, 2008; Cullen, 2004, 2012; Roy, 2004).

At the same time, another branch of experimental research has shown that somatosensory sensitivity in response to externally generated stimuli is gated during and before a voluntary movement. In human psychophysical research, this phenomenon of movement-related tactile gating or tactile suppression manifests as an increase in the detection threshold (Angel and Malenka, 1982; Chapman et al., 1987; Fraser and Fiehler, 2018; Gertz et al., 2017; Post et al., 1994; Voudouris et al., 2019), a decrease in the detection rate (Angel and Malenka, 1982; Chapman et al., 1987; Chapman and Beauchamp, 2006; Colino and Binsted, 2016; Cybulska-Klosowicz et al., 2011; Post et al., 1994; Williams et al., 1998; Williams and Chapman, 2000), a decrease in the detection precision (Colino et al., 2014; Gertz et al., 2017; Voudouris et al., 2019; Voudouris and Fiehler, 2017), and a decrease in the subjective intensity of externally generated stimuli (Papakostopoulos et al., 1975; Post et al., 1994; Williams and Chapman, 2000) when the stimulated body part moves compared with when it is at rest. Several electrophysiological studies have shown that this gating reflects the inhibition of somatosensory evoked potentials during active movement compared with rest at subcortical and cortical sites along the somatosensory pathway (Chapman, 1994; Giblin, 1964; Lei et al., 2018; Papakostopoulos et al., 1975; Rushton et al., 1981; Starr and Cohen, 1985). Similar to somatosensory attenuation, tactile gating is a biologically preserved mechanism that is observed across different species (Azim and Seki, 2019). For example, responses recorded in the cat medial lemniscus evoked by nerve stimulation are suppressed before and during limb movements (Ghez and Lenzi, 1971). Similarly, the transmission of cutaneous afferent signals to the primary somatosensory cortex is suppressed in rats during movement compared with rest (Chapin and Woodward, 1981). In monkeys, the gating of cutaneous afferent input during active movement has been observed in both the primary somatosensory cortex (Jiang et al., 1990, 1991; Seki and Fetz, 2012) and the spinal cord (Seki et al., 2003; Seki and Fetz, 2012).

Somatosensory attenuation and tactile gating/suppression share two important conceptual similarities. First, they both refer to modulation, either in terms of magnitude or precision, of the perception of cutaneous stimuli during movement. Second, they have been assigned the same functional role (Chapman and Beauchamp, 2006): to reduce the flow of afferent information that can be predicted from the motor command and enable the detection of an external input that may be biologically important, such as touches caused by predators (Blakemore et al., 2000b; Brooks and Cullen, 2019; McNamee and Wolpert, 2019), or input that is task-relevant for the upcoming or ongoing movement (Chapman, 1994; Collins et al., 1998; Rushton et al., 1981).

Importantly, however, the two phenomena present one striking difference. Somatosensory attenuation relates to somatosensory reafference, that is, touches caused by our voluntary movement. In contrast, gating relates to somatosensory exafference, that is, external touches occurring during our voluntary movement. Nevertheless, somatosensory research often treats the two phenomena as a single generalized suppression strategy of the brain. For example, reviews and theoretical papers use the terms attenuation and gating (Juravle et al., 2017; Saradjian, 2015) or their literature (Brown et al., 2013) interchangeably. This intermix is also evident in experimental studies and more specifically in the design, measures, and interpretation of the findings. For example, some experiments have tried to relate neural responses triggered by externally generated touches (i.e., electrical stimulation) to the responses associated with self-generated touches (Boehme et al., 2019; Palmer et al., 2016), or they applied externally generated touches during a self-generated movement and interpreted them as self-generated touches (Limanowski et al., 2020). Other studies have applied externally generated touches (Fraser and Fiehler, 2018; Gertz et al., 2017) but used theories for the perception of self-generated touches to explain their findings.

If the two phenomena are indeed different, this false equivalence is detrimental to our understanding of human sensorimotor control. First, it prevents advances in our understanding at the computational level because researchers try to explain gating (e.g., Gertz et al., 2017) and attenuation (e.g., Voss et al., 2008) using the computational processes proposed for attenuation and gating, respectively. Similarly, at the neurobiological level, researchers intermix neural correlates of gating and attenuation (e.g., Boehme et al., 2019; Palmer et al., 2016) because they assume that they measure the same single phenomenon. Second, this confusion becomes particularly disadvantageous in clinical studies using gating and attenuation when interpreting findings of sensorimotor deficits in patients with schizophrenia (Blakemore et al., 2000a; Shergill et al., 2005, 2014), functional movement disorders (Parees et al., 2014), or Parkinson’s disease (Macerollo et al., 2019; Rushton et al., 1981; Wolpe et al., 2018).

Are these phenomena the same, or do they represent two fundamentally distinct processes? Does the brain treat all sensory stimuli similarly during movement, regardless of whether they are reafferent or exafferent? To the best of our knowledge, no previous study has attempted to simultaneously test attenuation and gating with the same stimulus and psychophysics task. Here, in a single experimental design, we investigated the perception of touches applied on the left hand while manipulating whether the left arm was in movement or resting (left limb state). We further manipulated whether the touches were reafferent, generated by the right hand, or exafferent, generated by an external source (origin of touch). We reasoned that if the two phenomena are the same, they should influence somatosensory perception in similar manners. Our results do not confirm this hypothesis: voluntary movement reduces the somatosensory precision of both sensory reafference and exafference (gating), whereas the perceived amplitude of sensory reafference is robustly attenuated compared with that of exafference (attenuation). Notably, the two phenomena do not correlate with each other or interact when present simultaneously. Thus, collectively, our results show that gating and attenuation are two separate processes that can be experimentally dissociated.

Results

Participants rested their left hands palm up with their index fingers on a molded support, and their right hands were placed palm down on top of a set of sponges (Figures 1A–1D). In all conditions, they performed a force-discrimination task (Bays et al., 2006; Kilteni et al., 2019, 2020; Shergill et al., 2005): in each trial, a motor delivered two taps (the test tap and the comparison tap) on the pulp of their left index finger, and they were asked to verbally indicate which tap felt stronger (Figures 1E–1H). Whereas the test tap had a fixed intensity (2 N), the intensity of the comparison tap randomly changed in every trial (1, 1.5, 1.75, 2, 2.25, 2.5, or 3 N). An auditory “go” signal indicated the trial onset and the onset of the response period.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Experimental conditions

Two factors were manipulated in the experiment, resulting in four experimental conditions: whether the left arm was in rest (A and B) or in movement while receiving the test tap (C and D) (state of the left limb) and whether the test tap was externally triggered by the motor (A and C) or self-triggered by the participants’ right hand (B and D) (origin of touch). In all four conditions (E–H), the participants received the two taps (test and comparison tap) on the pulp of their left index fingers from the motor (black stars), and were required to verbally indicate which was stronger: the first or the second tap. In the attenuation and attenuation&gating conditions, the participants self-triggered the test tap on their left index finger by moving their right arm to tap a sensor with their right index finger (blue stars). (I) In the gating and attenuation&gating conditions (C and D), the participants extended their left arm from a starting position to an ending position, sliding the experimental setup along two drawer runners. During this movement, participants experienced the test tap. A motion sensor recorded the position of the platform in time. (J) Example position traces recorded by the motion sensor for the movements of one participant during the attenuation&gating condition. (K) Fitted logistic models of the responses of one example participant to the four experimental conditions.

In all conditions, our experimental manipulation exclusively concerned the test tap; the comparison tap was always externally triggered and delivered on the relaxed left arm, therefore serving as a reference stimulus. In a factorial design, we controlled for whether the left arm moved (Figures 1C and 1D) or remained relaxed (Figures 1A and 1B) during the test tap and whether the test tap was produced by the right hand (self-generated) (Figures 1B and 1D) or not (externally generated) (Figures 1A and 1C). This design resulted in four experimental conditions, the order of which was fully counterbalanced (Figures 1A–1D).

In the baseline condition (Figure 1A), participants did not move their limbs but passively received the test and the comparison taps on the left index finger. This control condition was used to assess the participants’ somatosensory perception in the absence of any movement (Bays et al., 2005; Kilteni et al., 2019, 2020). In the attenuation condition (Figure 1B), participants actively tapped a force sensor placed on top of their left index finger with their right one. The tap of their right index finger on the force sensor triggered the test tap on their left index finger. This classic condition was used to assess the perception of a self-generated tap on a passive limb (Bays et al., 2005; Kilteni et al., 2019, 2020). In the gating condition (Figure 1C), participants were asked to continuously move their left arm forward, sliding the experimental setup with the motor and the force sensors between a start and an end position (distance 25 cm) at a comfortable velocity of approximately 20 cm/s (Figures 1I, 1J, S1, and S2). During this movement, participants received the test tap on their left index finger. This condition represents gating because it assesses the perception of an externally generated tap on a moving limb (Cybulska-Klosowicz et al., 2011; Williams et al., 1998; Williams and Chapman, 2000, 2002). Finally, in the attenuation&gating condition (Figure 1D), the participants performed the same movement with their left arm, but they were further asked to actively tap with their right index finger on the force sensor that triggered the test tap on their left index finger during the movement. The force sensor was attached to the experimental setup and moved together with the left hand. This condition combines the gating and attenuation phenomena since it is used to assess the perception of a self-generated tap on a moving limb.

The participant’s responses to each condition were fitted with a generalized linear model (Figures 1K and S3). For all participants and all conditions, the fitted model was better than a null/restricted model with no predictors, according to McFadden’s R squared measure. Two parameters of interest were extracted: the point of subjective equality (PSE), which represents the intensity at which the test tap felt as strong as the comparison tap (p = 0.5), and the just noticeable difference (JND), which reflects the participants’ sensitivity (precision) in force discrimination. A lower PSE in an experimental condition indicates that the test tap felt weaker in that condition. A higher JND in an experimental condition indicates that the discrimination sensitivity was lower in that condition (i.e., a larger difference in the force intensities needed to be detected). PSE and JND correspond to different qualities of sensory judgments – accuracy and precision, respectively – and can be independent (Mapp and Ono, 2006). We hypothesized that the two phenomena are different and, thus, they affect the PSE and JND differently. Specifically, we expected a decrease in the perceived magnitude (lower PSE) for conditions with sensory reafference (attenuation and attenuation&gating conditions), with no effects on the sensory precision of the participants (JND) when they received reafference on a still limb (attenuation condition). That is, a self-generated touch will feel weaker than an externally generated touch, but no effect on somatosensory precision will be observed. In contrast, we predicted a decrease in the somatosensory precision (higher JND) of both sensory reafference and exafference for conditions where the limb that receives the touches moves (gating and attenuation&gating conditions). That is, the precision with which a touch (either self-generated or externally generated) is perceived is lower on a moving limb because of the additional kinaesthetic, proprioceptive, tactile signals from the movement. A small, if any, effect on the PSE was expected for the gating condition. We hypothesized that the attenuation phenomenon mainly affects the PSE and not the JND, and the gating phenomenon mainly affects the JND and not the PSE. Our hypotheses were supported by the data (Figures 2A–2G).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Experimental results

(A and B) Bar graphs showing the PSEs and JNDs (mean ± SEM) for each condition. A lower PSE value indicates a lower perceived magnitude, whereas a higher JND value indicates lower somatosensory sensory precision. Reafferent touches produced by the movement of the right arm (attenuation and attenuation&gating conditions) were associated with a significant decrease in the PSEs, whereas the movement of the left arm that receives the touches (gating and attenuation&gating) results in a significant increase in the JNDs. A small decrease in PSE was also observed in the gating and attenuation&gating conditions compared with the baseline and attenuation conditions, respectively.

(C) Group psychometric functions for each condition generated using the mean PSE and the mean JND across participants. The leftward shift of the curves for the attenuation and attenuation&gating conditions illustrates that somatosensory reafference is perceived as weaker than exafference. The flattening of the curves for the gating and attenuation&gating conditions illustrates the worsening of somatosensory precision of both the reafference and exafference on a moving limb.

(D and E) Line plots illustrating the decreases in PSEs when experiencing reafferent touches compared with exafferent touches when the left arm is still (D) and when the left arm moves (E).

(F and G) Line plots illustrating the increases in JNDs when receiving touches on a moving limb compared with rest when touches are exafferent (F) and reafferent (G).

We performed repeated-measures ANOVA on the PSEs with the origin of the touch (reafference vs exafference) and the state of the left limb (movement vs rest) as factors. This analysis revealed a significant main effect of the origin of the touch (F(1, 23) = 36.10, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.611), a significant main effect of the left limb state (F(1, 23) = 13.91, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.377) and a nonsignificant interaction (F(1, 23) = 0.26, p = 0.615, ηp2 = 0.011) (Figure 2A). A Bayesian repeated measures ANOVA further concluded against the interaction term by supporting the model without the interaction term (M1) compared to the full factorial model (M2) (BFM1 /BFM2 = 3.631). The attenuation condition produced a significant decrease in the PSE compared with the baseline condition (n = 24, t(23) = −5.908, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = −1.206, CI95 = [−0.332, −0.160], BF01 < 0.0003) (Figure 2D). This replicates previous attenuation findings, indicating that a self-generated tap on a passive limb feels weaker than a tap of the same intensity but of an external origin (Bays et al., 2005; Kilteni et al., 2019, 2020; Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2017a, 2017b, 2020; Shergill et al., 2003; Wolpe et al., 2016). Similarly, the PSE in the attenuation&gating condition was significantly lower than that in the gating condition (n = 24, t(23) = −5.032, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = −1.027, CI95 = [−0.377, −0.157], BF01 = 0.002) (Figure 2E), extending the previous conclusion to when the receiving limb is moving. Together, these two contrasts show that reafferent (self-generated) touches feel weaker than exafferent touches, both when the receiving hand is in movement and when at rest (Text S1).

The gating and attenuation&gating conditions also resulted in a significant decrease in the PSE compared with that in the baseline condition (n = 24, t(23) = −2.409, p = 0.024, Cohen’SD = −0.492, CI95 = [−0.141, −0.011]) and the attenuation condition (n = 24, V = 55, p = 0.005, rrb = −0.633, CI95 = [−0.161, −0.022]), respectively. However, these decreases were quite modest (≅ 30% of the weakening produced by the attenuation condition) and supported only by anecdotal evidence from Bayesian statistics (BF01 = 0.433 and 0.738, respectively). Together, these contrasts suggest that exafferent touches may feel slightly weaker on a moving limb than on a passive limb, consistent with previous findings regarding tactile gating (Papakostopoulos et al., 1975; Post et al., 1994; Williams and Chapman, 2000). Nevertheless, compared with tactile reafference, the perceived magnitude of tactile exafference is not substantially decreased.

When testing for the effects of the conditions on the somatosensory precision of the participants (JND), a significant main effect of the state of the left limb was observed (F(1, 23) = 17.1, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.426), but neither a significant main effect of the origin of touch (F(1, 23) = 0.52, p = 0.478, ηp2 = 0.022) nor a significant interaction (F(1, 23) = 0.06, p = 0.809, ηp2 = 0.003) was identified (Figure 2B). Similar to PSEs, the absence of an interaction for the JNDs was further supported by Bayesian repeated-measures ANOVA, which provided evidence against the interaction term (BFM1 /BFM2 = 3.522). The attenuation condition did not result in any change in the JND compared with that in the baseline condition (n = 24, t(23) = 0.331, p = 0.744, Cohen’s d = 0.068, CI95 = [−0.034, 0.047]), and this result was substantially confirmed by Bayesian analysis (BF01 = 4.432). Similarly, no significant differences in the JND were detected between the gating and the attenuation&gating conditions (n = 24, t(23) = 0.72, p = 0.481, Cohen’s d = 0.146, CI95 = [-0.024, 0.05]), which was again confirmed by the Bayesian analysis (BF01 = 3.691). Together, these two contrasts indicate that receiving sensory reafference per se is not followed by worsening of sensory precision on the receiving limb.

In contrast, moving the limb while receiving an external touch (gating condition) resulted in a significant increase in the JND compared with that in the baseline condition (n = 24, t(23) = 3.134, p = 0.005, Cohen’s d = 0.640, CI95 = [0.019, 0.093], BF01 = 0.108) (Figure 2F). This result was further confirmed by a significant increase in JND in the attenuation&gating condition compared with the attenuation condition (n = 24, t(23) = 2.984, p = 0.007, Cohen’s d = 0.609, CI95 = [0.019, 0.106], BF01 = 0.146) (Figure 2G). Together, these two differences indicate that voluntary movement per se decreases the precision with which reafferent and exafferent stimuli are perceived on the moving limb (Text S1).

Our results indicate that predicting the sensory consequences of a voluntary movement leads to a decrease in the perceived magnitude of sensory reafference (PSE) without a concomitant worsening of somatosensory precision (JND). In contrast, voluntary movement leads to a decrease in somatosensory precision (JND) for both sensory reafference and exafference. These effects are observed in the group psychometric fits (Figure 2C). We tested whether we could better predict the participants’ performance in the attenuation&gating condition when using the PSE from the attenuation condition and the JND from the gating condition rather than the PSE from the gating condition and the JND from the attenuation condition, to further illustrate that somatosensory attenuation affects the amplitude (PSE), whereas tactile gating affects the precision (JND) and not vice versa. Indeed, the first model was significantly better: n = 24, V = 39, p < 0.001, rrb = −0.74, CI95 = [−68.980, −12.736], BF01 = 0.443 (Figures S4, 3A, and 3B).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Model predictions and scatterplots for PSEs and JNDs

(A) Average responses of participants in the attenuation&gating condition (yellow) and average predicted responses using the parameters from the attenuation and gating conditions (blue). The responses depicted in dark blue indicate the PSE of the attenuation condition and the JND from the gating condition, whereas the responses depicted in light blue represent the PSE of the gating condition and the JND of the attenuation condition. The error bars and ribbons represent 95% confidence intervals.

(B) For each participant, we estimated the Bayesian information criterion (BIC) of the fitted logistic model in the attenuation&gating condition and the two models with fixed parameters from the attenuation and gating conditions. The model using the PSE of the attenuation condition and the JND of the gating condition was a significantly better model than that using the PSE of the gating condition and the JND of the attenuation condition. The upper right panel presents the same data after excluding one participant corresponding to the outlier for illustration purposes. The exclusion of the outlier did not change the statistical results.

(C) The participants’ PSEs in the attenuation condition were significantly correlated with those in the attenuation&gating condition.

(D) The participants’ JNDs in the gating condition were significantly correlated with those in the attenuation&gating condition. No significant correlations between PSEs and JNDs were observed.

In the abovementioned ANOVAs, no significant interactions were observed between the two factors (the origin of touch and the state of the limb), neither for the PSEs nor for the JNDs, according to both frequentist and Bayesian analyses. Therefore, the effect of the left limb state was not influenced by the effect of the origin of touch, neither for the PSE nor for the JND. Instead, the two effects were summed when simultaneously present, and no superadditive effects were detected (interactions), indicating that each factor had its own independent effect on the responses. We performed a correlation analysis of the PSEs and JNDs to further test whether the effects produced by each phenomenon related to the others at all. No significant correlations were detected between any of the PSEs and any of the JNDs (all p-values > 0.225, BF01 = [1.971, 3.950]). The only significant correlation found for PSEs was between the PSE in the attenuation condition and the PSE in the attenuation&gating condition (t(22) = 4.89, r = 0.722, p < 0.001, CI95 = [0.449, 0.871], BF01 = 0.002) (Figure 3C). That is, the weaker the participants perceived the magnitude of their self-generated touch during rest, the weaker the magnitude of their self-generated touch during movement felt. As the PSE values decreased significantly in these two conditions and these decreases correlated with each other, this result provides further support that their common experimental denominator, i.e., the reafferent nature of the touch, was responsible for the decrease in the PSE and the subsequent attenuation phenomenon. In contrast, the JND in the gating condition was significantly correlated only with the JND in the attenuation&gating condition (t(22) = 3.47, r = 0.595, p = 0.008, CI95 = [0.252, 0.805], BF01 = 0.047) (Figure 3D). This specific correlation suggests that the worse the somatosensory precision of an external touch when participants moved their receiving hand, the worse is the somatosensory precision for a self-generated touch during the same movement of the receiving hand. As the JNDs significantly increased only in these two conditions and these increases correlated with each other, this result provides evidence that their common experimental denominator, i.e., the movement of the left limb that receives the touches, was responsible for the increase in the JND.

A common finding in tactile gating studies is that the gating effects, both behavioral and electrophysiological, get stronger with higher movement velocities (Angel and Malenka, 1982; Cybulska-Klosowicz et al., 2011; Gertz et al., 2017; Rauch et al., 1985; Williams et al., 1998); the faster the limb movements, the worse is the perception of the moving limb (see also He et al., 2021 for findings in macaques). Therefore, one could hypothesize that any differences observed between the gating and attenuation&gating conditions might be due to differences in the velocity of the participants’ movements. Since no significant JND differences were observed between these two conditions (Figure 2), which was further supported by Bayesian analysis, this concern can be excluded. However, one can argue that the PSE in the attenuation&gating condition was lower than the PSE in the attenuation condition because the participants moved faster in the attenuation&gating condition and not because of the reafferent nature of the touch. This concern can also be excluded since we observed that the participants moved slightly slower rather than faster in the attenuation&gating condition (20.3 ± 0.003 cm/s) than in the gating condition (23.3 ± 0.003 cm/s): (peak trial velocity; t(23) = −4.062, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = −0.829, CI95 = [−0.004, −0.001], BF01 = 0.015). Although the total distances the participants ran with their left arm were comparable, participants in the attenuation&gating condition moved slower because they had to coordinate both their arms to tap the sensor that the left arm moves with the right hand (Figure 1D). This difference was further confirmed when examining the peak velocities during the test tap between the two conditions (gating: 22.2 ± 0.003 cm/s; attenuation&gating: 19 ± 0.003 cm/s) (Figures S5, S6, and S7). Therefore, these kinematic differences do not explain the significant decrease in the PSEs in the attenuation&gating condition compared with the gating condition, but actually underscore the importance of reafference in somatosensory attenuation.

Discussion

The present study contrasted the conditions of attenuation and gating in a single experimental paradigm to investigate the relationship between the two phenomena on the same limb. Therefore, we independently manipulated the origin of the touch (reafference vs exafference) and the state of the receiving limb (movement vs rest), studying all four possible combinations of these levels. We replicated the classic phenomenon of predictive attenuation of touch (Bays et al., 2005, 2006; Blakemore et al., 1999b; Kilteni et al., 2018, 2019, 2020; Shergill et al., 2003; Walsh et al., 2011; Wolpe et al., 2016) by showing that somatosensory reafference feels weaker than somatosensory exafference. Importantly, however, this decrease in the perceived amplitude (PSE) was not followed by a concomitant worsening of somatosensory precision (JND). That is, participants had the same discrimination capacity (JND) for both reafferent and exafferent touches applied on their passive limb, a conclusion supported also by Bayesian statistics. Nevertheless, when the limb that received the touches moves, this voluntary movement per se leads to a decrease in somatosensory precision (JND) for both reafferent and exafferent touches, replicating the classic tactile gating phenomenon (Angel and Malenka, 1982; Chapman et al., 1987; Chapman and Beauchamp, 2006; Cybulska-Klosowicz et al., 2011; Fraser and Fiehler, 2018; Gertz et al., 2017; Post et al., 1994; Williams et al., 1998). The two effects did not correlate and interact but were summed when present together.

We must emphasize that in all experimental conditions, we measured the participants’ somatosensory perception on the same limb (i.e., the left limb). Although the participants moved their right hand to generate the tap on their left index finger in the attenuation and attenuation&gating conditions, our outcome measure was the perception of the (self-generated) tap on their left index finger. This approach is a particular strength of the current experimental design because it allows to independently induce, and separately study, the two phenomena on the same limb.

The main conclusion of the present study is that the predictive attenuation of touch and tactile gating are two distinct perceptual phenomena. Our findings can help conciliate several previous observations on gating and attenuation that have been studied in isolation in recent decades. First, attenuation is observed not only on the active limb (Walsh et al., 2011) but also on a passive limb (the left hand in the present study) as long as the contact between the body parts is predicted by voluntary movement (Bays et al., 2005, 2006; Kilteni et al., 2018, 2019, 2020; Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2017a, 2017b, 2020; Shergill et al., 2003; Wolpe et al., 2016). In contrast, abundant evidence has shown that external touches applied to the limb contralateral to the limb that moves are not gated (Chapman et al., 1987; Cohen and Starr, 1987; Colino et al., 2014; Papakostopoulos et al., 1975; Pertovaara et al., 1992; Rushton et al., 1981). Second, a touch that results from a passive movement (Kilteni et al., 2020), or touches that are simultaneously presented in both hands (double touch) (Bays et al., 2005), is not attenuated. In contrast, gating effects have been repeatedly documented for passive movements, both electrophysiologically (Rushton et al., 1981) and behaviorally (Chapman et al., 1987; Chapman and Beauchamp, 2006; Williams and Chapman, 2002). Third, self-generated tactile signals are attenuated as long as they are presented at the timing predicted by the action (Bays et al., 2005; Blakemore et al., 1999b; Kilteni et al., 2019); even a 100-ms delay between the movement and its tactile feedback substantially reduces the attenuation of the latter. In contrast, externally generated stimuli are gated with less temporal sensitivity; for example, gating is observed for stimuli presented at several (unpredicted) times during movement (Rushton et al., 1981), at movement onset (Colino and Binsted, 2016), and importantly, even hundreds of milliseconds before movement onset (Chapman and Beauchamp, 2006; Colino et al., 2014; Colino and Binsted, 2016; Williams et al., 1998). Fourth, although gating shows no specificity for the type of motor activity and manifests both during isotonic (Angel and Malenka, 1982; Chapman et al., 1987; Papakostopoulos et al., 1975; Post et al., 1994) and isometric (Pertovaara et al., 1992; Post et al., 1994) contractions (see also Suresh et al., 2021 for findings in macaques), attenuation is motor-command-specific; a consistent but arbitrary and unnatural mapping between the motor command and the touch, for example, moving a joystick with one hand to produce touch on the other, does not produce attenuation (Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2017a; Parees et al., 2014; Shergill et al., 2003). These results are not contradictory based on our findings; instead, they refer to different perceptual phenomena.

Based on this dissociation between gating and attenuation, we propose that findings from earlier studies using unimanual movements might reflect the combination of attenuation and gating effects because they have not isolated the two phenomena. For example, during a unimanual movement toward a target, Voudouris and Fiehler (2021) showed smaller suppression effects at the timepoint of the maximal speed but increased suppression effects close to the time of contact (approximately 0–50 ms before contact) when the hand decelerated. Similarly, Fraser and Fiehler (2018) showed greater suppression during the later phases of reaching, when the limb decelerated to approach the target (approximately at 140–180 ms before contact). In principle, these effects contradict the findings of Cybulska-Klosowicz et al., who showed that gating effects are greater during fast speeds than during slower speeds of elbow extension (a non-reaching movement) (Cybulska-Klosowicz et al., 2011). According to Cybulska-Klosowicz et al., Voudouris and Fiehler (2021) and Fraser and Fiehler (2018) should have observed greater gating effects during peak velocity than during later stages. However, predictions of the somatosensory consequences of the movement (i.e., touching the target) during the later stage of reaching movements exist, and, thus, one should observe somatosensory attenuation effects as well. Indeed, previous studies have shown significant attenuation effects at times very close to the time of contact (e.g., +/−150 ms around the button press) (Bays et al., 2005; Kilteni et al., 2019). Based on our findings that attenuation and gating are distinct perceptual phenomena, we speculate that the findings of Voudouris and Fiehler (2021) and Fraser and Fiehler (2018) during the later stages of reaching movements reflect a combination of reduced gating effects because the speed of the movement decreases and increased attenuation effects due to the predicted consequences.

Concerning this point, active tactile exploration, such as when we move our hand to explore the shape or texture of an object (Ryan et al., 2021), includes self-generated touch (contact between our finger and the object) experienced on the same limb that performs the voluntary movement. In contrast to the common view that active touch enhances performance during exploration, several studies have shown that texture perception with active touch is not superior to texture perception with passive touch (e.g., Heller, 1989; Lederman, 1981; Simões-Franklin et al., 2011; Verrillo et al., 1999). These findings led to the proposal that the relative motion between the hand and the object matters and not whether the subject actively moves the hand against the object/texture or the object/texture is moved against the passive hand (Lederman, 1981). Having established that active touch is not superior to passive touch, the question that immediately arises is: Why is active touch not inferior to passive touch, given the gating and attenuation effects present during voluntary movement? Importantly, studies have shown that when exploring textures in an unconstrained manner, we perform highly stereotyped exploratory movements (Lederman and Klatzky, 1987) with slow mean scanning speeds (e.g., 52–120 mm/s; Callier et al., 2015) that are not sufficiently fast to elicit significant gating effects (Cybulska-Klosowicz et al., 2011). Similarly, we apply weaker forces when asked to explore the roughness/slipperiness of the texture than its hardness (Callier et al., 2015), and evidence has shown that attenuation is smaller for weaker forces than for stronger forces (Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2020). We therefore propose that during tactile exploration (e.g., braille reading), we optimize the speed and pressure of our scanning movements to minimize tactile gating and attenuation effects and maximize the quality of the extracted information during tactile search (Cybulska-Klosowicz et al., 2011).

Our study is the first to behaviorally dissociate the predictive attenuation of touch and tactile gating. A previous study (Bays and Wolpert, 2008) reported that the perceived intensity of an electrical stimulus applied to the tip of the relaxed left index finger does not change if this finger receives a self-produced force generated by the right index finger. However, in that study, the perception of touch was tested on a resting limb only, which is already known not to elicit gating effects (Chapman et al., 1987; Cohen and Starr, 1987; Colino et al., 2014; Papakostopoulos et al., 1975; Pertovaara et al., 1992; Rushton et al., 1981). Therefore, the two phenomena were not disentangled because they were not tested on the same limb, as in the current study. Similarly, another study (Palmer et al., 2016) measured electrophysiological responses (somatosensory evoked potentials) to external electrical stimulation on the right and the left wrist while the participants produced a self-generated force with their right index finger on their left index finger. The authors did not measure the perception of external touches during movement (gating) but only the perception of self-generated touches during rest (attenuation) and thus were unable to dissociate the two phenomena behaviorally. Moreover, the authors observed the modulation of the late electrophysiological responses depending on whether the participants produced a self-generated force, and suggested that the attenuation and gating might share the same mechanism of reduced sensory precision. However, as shown in the present study, sensory precision is reduced during gating and not during attenuation, suggesting that a common mechanism governing both phenomena is unlikely.

Motor control relies on integrating afferent sensory information with efferent motor signals (Shadmehr et al., 2008). Distinguishing between gating and attenuation is fundamentally important for motor control theories because it may indicate a different integration or weighting mechanism of the motor and sensory information, depending on the context. During voluntary motor control, these basic processes most likely coexist and cooperate, but our study suggests that they are distinct processes. Interestingly, when the two phenomena were co-present on the same limb (attenuation&gating condition), the participants’ performance showed no interaction effects but simple additive effects of the attenuation and gating condition. Although this condition involved a bimanual movement (i.e., participants moved both hands) and one could hypothesize the need for increased attentional demands or the presence of interhemispheric inhibition mechanisms with respect to the other conditions, the two phenomena did not interact but simply added together.

One well-established computational framework inspired by engineering approaches posits that the brain produces motor commands through an inverse model (Kawato, 1999) or controller (Todorov, 2004). A copy of the motor command, termed “efference copy”, is used by a forward model to predict the expected sensory feedback of the movement, which is then combined with the actual sensory input to estimate the state of the body (Kawato, 1999; McNamee and Wolpert, 2019; Todorov, 2004; Wolpert and Ghahramani, 2000). With respect to the attenuation of sensory reafference, it has been proposed that the prediction signal of the forward model is used to “cancel” the sensory reafference (Bays and Wolpert, 2007, 2008; Blakemore et al., 2000b; McNamee and Wolpert, 2019; Wolpert and Flanagan, 2001). In other words, central motor processes play a more important role in somatosensory attenuation than actual sensory feedback. Support for this comes from studies showing that conditions that present highly predictable touches but in the absence of movement do not yield attenuation (Bays et al., 2005; Kilteni et al., 2020) (see also (Bäß et al., 2008; Diedrichsen et al., 2003; Klaffehn et al., 2019) for similar conclusions). The dependence of attenuation on action prediction was further shown when participants attenuated the touches applied to one hand that were predicted by their other hand’s movement, even when the two hands unexpectedly failed to make contact (Bays et al., 2006). Providing further confirmation, neuroimaging studies on somatosensory attenuation consistently report activation of the cerebellum (Blakemore et al., 1998, 2001; Blakemore et al., 1999a; Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2020; Shergill et al., 2013), a structure that is associated with motor prediction (Miall and Wolpert, 1996; Shadmehr et al., 2008, 2010; Therrien and Bastian, 2019; Wolpert et al., 1998). Our findings of a reduced perceived magnitude of somatosensory reafference compared with exafference (i.e., lower PSEs in the attenuation and attenuation&gating conditions compared with the baseline and gating conditions, respectively) support this forward model mechanism for the attenuation of self-generated input.

In contrast, this computational account that relies on action prediction, efference copy, and internal forward models is not applicable to tactile gating (tactile suppression) since gated touches can be of an exafferent nature and occur at any (unpredictable) time during movement, even before movement onset (Chapman, 1994; Williams et al., 1998; Williams and Chapman, 2000, 2002). In other words, no information is available that the brain can use to predict exafferent touches with the forward model because no causal relationship exists between the motor command and sensory input. This observation is consistent with the proposal that peripheral afferent signals from muscle spindles and joint afferents play the major role in gating (Rushton et al., 1981; Williams et al., 1998) and that gating effects have also been observed during passive movement, without significant differences in active movements (Chapman et al., 1987; Williams and Chapman, 2002) (but see (He et al., 2021)). Then, if the efference copy is not the basis for gating, how are the gating effects computationally explained?

The alternative computational framework of active inference has been proposed to explain somatosensory attenuation (Brown et al., 2013); however, this framework was based on an assumed equivalence between attenuation and gating. The active inference approach refutes the necessity of an efference copy and emphasizes the importance of a generative model and reflex arcs in the place of forward and inverse models and controllers (Adams et al., 2013; Friston, 2010). According to the active inference account, the brain predicts the sensory input that would be expected from a specific action, and the body moves to fulfill these sensory predictions. Motor commands are thus conceptually replaced by proprioceptive predictions, and action occurs as a way to minimize the proprioceptive prediction errors when the movement has not yet been executed (Pickering and Clark, 2014). A major role is assigned to the precision (i.e., reliability) that weights these sensory prediction errors depending on the context and can be manipulated through attention allocation. Within this computational architecture, attenuation of somatosensory input is viewed as a reduction in the precision of somatosensory evidence during movement to allow the expression of proprioceptive predictions that trigger the movement (Brown et al., 2013). In other words, the agent attends away from all somatosensory inputs to execute the movement. However, this proposal does not address the attenuation of sensory reafference (self-generated touch) with respect to exafference (externally generated touch) since the agent should theoretically attend away from all somatosensory inputs during the movement, regardless of their source. In contrast, we observed that self-generated touch on a moving limb (attenuation&gating condition) is perceived as weaker than externally generated touch on a moving limb (gating condition). Moreover, the active inference proposal refers to the limb that moves, and thus, the framework might not explain the effects observed on a passive limb. Indeed, it is puzzling why increasing the precision of the proprioceptive prediction errors on the hand that is to move (right hand) would reduce the precision of somatosensory evidence on the contralateral limb that is not meant to move (left hand) and where there are no proprioceptive predictions. In contrast, the active inference account may sufficiently explain the tactile gating effect, i.e., the reduction in the precision of somatosensory input on the moving limb during movement. Our findings of a reduced precision of somatosensory input on the moving limb (i.e., higher JNDs in the gating and attenuation&gating conditions compared with the baseline and attenuation conditions, respectively) are compatible with this active inference mechanism for the gating of sensory input during movement.

Other mechanisms that have been proposed to explain tactile gating are divided attention between the motor and the perceptual task (Williams et al., 1998), and a backward masking or postdictive mechanism (Voss et al., 2008) that is not necessarily dependent on motor signals. According to this postdictive masking mechanism, when moving a limb, the sensation from the muscles, joints, and skin of the moving limb masks the externally generated touches that are applied on this limb. These sensations could potentially affect the perception of earlier stimuli in a postdictive manner (Williams and Chapman, 2002), which might explain the gating effects observed for external touches applied even before the movement onset (Chapman and Beauchamp, 2006). Our gating findings of a reduced precision of somatosensory input on the moving limb are compatible with this backward masking mechanism. Furthermore, in the context of a passive movement (i.e., in the absence of motor commands), the sensations from the muscles, joints, and skin of the passively moving limb might mask the externally generated touches on the same limb, and this mechanism might account for the absence of gating differences between active and passive movements (Chapman et al., 1987; Williams and Chapman, 2002).

Regarding the neural mechanisms underlying the two phenomena, it has been shown that somatosensory attenuation results in reduced activity in the secondary somatosensory cortex (Blakemore et al., 1998; Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2020; Shergill et al., 2013) and the cerebellum (Blakemore et al., 1998, 2001; Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2020) and increased functional connectivity between the two areas (Blakemore et al., 1999a; Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2020). Accordingly, the cerebellum predicts the sensory consequences of the action based on the efference copy and cancels somatosensory activity (Blakemore et al., 1998; Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2020). In contrast, sensory gating studies in primates have shown suppression effects at very early stations along the somatosensory pathway, including the spinal cord (Seki et al., 2003), the cuneate nucleus (He et al., 2021), and the thalamus (Fahy et al., 1993). In support of our conclusions, Chakrabarti and Schwarz (2018) observed suppression effects on rats at the level of the brainstem (at the first synaptic level), where motor predictions are unlikely available, and proposed that sensory attenuation and sensory gating must be distinct phenomena (Chakrabarti and Schwarz, 2018).

In another study, Ishiyama et al. (2019) compared the neural responses of rats when they groomed themselves (as a model for self-touch) with those when the experimenter touched (or tickled) the rats (externally generated touch). The authors observed substantially inhibited somatosensory responses during self-touch compared with externally generated touch and tickling. Interestingly, the inhibition of responses during self-touch was observed in the somatosensory cortex in a widespread and global manner. The inhibition was not restricted to the somatotopically organized zones that were the target of afferent inputs from the body parts stimulated by self-touch, and similar widespread inhibitory responses were also observed for externally generated touches applied at the same time as the grooming action. Based on these findings, the authors proposed a global inhibition suppression model that does not distinguish between self- and externally generated touches. Based on our findings, we propose that these “global inhibition” effects might actually reflect sensory gating effects and not the attenuation of self-generated touches, as animals actively move their bodies when grooming themselves. Consequently, suppression effects are expected for both self-generated and externally generated touches because the animal is motorically active. However, since the authors did not record motor responses in any of the experimental conditions, the extent to which the reported effects are owing to self-touch or movement is not clear.

Our attenuation and attenuation&gating conditions differed from the rest with respect to cutaneous stimulation on both hands. Therefore, one could ask if the attenuation effects we observe in these two conditions, and not in the gating or baseline conditions, may not be due to action prediction, but simply to the simultaneous tactile stimulation on the pulps of both index fingers (“double touch”). However, this suggestion, recently made by Thomas et al. (2021), is not supported by earlier experiments on double touch. Specifically, previous studies investigating interferences in the detection of a tactile stimulus by the concurrent somatosensory stimulation of another skin location (for a review, see Tamè et al., 2016) showed elevated thresholds for stimuli delivered within the same finger or hand (Sherrick, 1964) and smaller effects, if present at all, for stimuli applied on different hands (Gescheider et al., 1970; Laskin and Spencer, 1979). Crucially, any bimanual interferences disappear if the double touch stimuli are applied on homologous fingers (e.g., right and left index fingers) or if the two hands differ in their posture (e.g., one hand is palm up and the other is palm down) (Tamè et al., 2011), which is precisely the hands’ configuration employed in the current attenuation and attenuation&gating conditions, and in all previous attenuation studies (Asimakidou et al., 2022; Bays et al., 2006; Kilteni et al., 2018, 2019, 2020, 2021; Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2017a, 2017b, 2020; Lalouni et al., 2020; Shergill et al., 2003, 2005, 2013), one should add. Therefore, bimanual tactile interferences are unlikely to explain somatosensory attenuation effects.

Furthermore, previous studies have shown that double touch is not sufficient to produce attenuation. Specifically, Bays et al. delivered simultaneous double tactile stimulation on the participants’ right and left index finger in the absence of any movement and showed that the touch on the left index finger was perceived similarly strong during double touch and single touch (i.e., when the right index finger was not stimulated). Only when the participants generated the touch themselves on their left index finger, the attenuation effects were present (Bays et al., 2005). Similarly, Kilteni et al. showed that simultaneous double tactile stimulation on both index fingers as the result of the passive movement of the right hand toward the left one (Kilteni et al., 2020) does not produce somatosensory attenuation effects. Moreover, if somatosensory attenuation were caused by double touch, we should expect to observe the full attenuation effect whenever double touch stimuli are delivered. In contrast, when participants perform the force-matching task or the force discrimination task (as in the present study) and thus receive bimanual tactile stimulation, but a distance or a spatial mismatch is introduced between their hands/fingers, the attenuation is substantially reduced (Bays, 2008; Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2020; 2017b) or not present at all (Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2017a; Knoetsch and Zimmermann, 2021). This reduction or abolishment of attenuation by the hands’ distance is not found during double touch in the absence of action (Rahman and Yau, 2019), suggesting that potential differences in spatial attention associated with distance manipulations do not drive these effects. Together, these earlier studies speak against the double touch being a sufficient condition to elicit somatosensory attenuation.

In addition, several previous studies have shown that double touch is not necessary for somatosensory attenuation. For example, attenuation is also observed during unimanual movements (Walsh et al., 2011), or when tactile stimulation is only provided to the passive left index finger during the force-matching task and the participants are only imagining the action of the right finger (using kinesthetic-motor imagery) (Kilteni et al., 2018). Critically, Bays et al. (2006) used a very similar setup to Thomas et al. to explicitly examine whether the double touch produces attenuation, and observed attenuation even when the right index finger moved to touch the left index finger but unexpectedly missed the contact (Bays et al., 2006). Collectively, the above observations strongly suggest that bimanual stimulation (double touch) cannot explain the attenuation phenomenon and that action prediction attenuates the predicted touch.

Our results also contradict Thomas et al.’s proposal that attenuation is due to non-predictive generalized gating processes (Thomas et al., 2021). Our study clearly shows that attenuation and gating are different phenomena; if sensory attenuation was the same as tactile gating, all stimuli applied to the moving limb would be attenuated. In contrast, we show that sensory reafference is robustly attenuated compared with sensory exafference, both in passive and moving limbs. Similar results were reported in another study (Bays and Wolpert, 2008), where participants attenuated only their reafferent touches and not exafferent touches presented on the same limb simultaneously. Therefore, our results do not support the equivalence of attenuation and gating phenomena proposed by Thomas et al. (2021).

Finally, Thomas et al. (2021) showed that participants perceived expected touches as stronger than unexpected touches on the fingers of their left hand when these touches were triggered by moving the right index finger on the air (no touch on the right index finger). This finding led them to propose that action leads to an enhancement of the predicted touch rather than attenuation. However, these results must be interpreted with caution for the reasons described below. First, one must include a baseline condition where the participant does not move (baseline condition in the present study) to argue that a perceived sensation is enhanced or attenuated. Consequently, lower values than those in the baseline are considered attenuation, and higher values are considered enhancement. Thomas et al. did not consistently include such a baseline condition in their experiments, and, therefore, it cannot be concluded whether there is an enhancement or reduced attenuation in one condition than in another. Second, Thomas et al. provided participants with an arbitrary mapping between the movement of one hand and sensory feedback on the other hand (e.g., lifting the right index finger delivers a touch on the left middle finger). Arbitrary mappings between movements and touch are known not to elicit somatosensory attenuation (Bays and Wolpert, 2008; Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2017a, 2017b, 2020; Parthasharathy et al., 2020). Thus, the finding that non-naturalistic conditions do not allow the formation of predictions and the attenuation of the produced touch is not surprising. Third, the study of Bays et al. (2006) that used a similar setup as that of Thomas et al. and a separate control condition that Thomas did not include, showed attenuation and no enhancement of the predicted touch. Finally, a key argument of Thomas et al. is that expectations should amplify our self-generated sensations to make our experiences more accurate in the presence of sensory noise. As we described above, several experiments have shown that active touch does not lead to better performance than passive touch and that we move our digits in a way to maximize the information we can extract when exploring the tactile world around us. It is important to acknowledge that the enhanced perception of self-generated touch suggested by Thomas et al. (2021) would not be a more precise perception, as participants would be experiencing the stimulus as more intense than it truly is. This type of perceptual bias would be inaccurate, similar to the attenuated perception that we register in the current study.

We conclude that the human brain uses two different basic processes to suppress reafferent and exafferent information during movement and rest. This separation of attenuation and gating may explain why we tense our muscles when being tickled by others to decrease our sensitivity to external tickles, although we cannot tickle ourselves because we attenuate our self-tickles.

Limitations of the study

The purpose of the present study was to test whether the attenuation and gating phenomena are the same or different perceptual processes, and our experiment was designed to address this particular question. To this end, we tested the perception of touches delivered to the left hand during movements of the participants’ hands that were somewhat spatially restricted (i.e., fixed starting and end position) and within a specific velocity range. Although we see no reason why the current findings would not generalize to other effectors and types of movements, future studies should establish this. Moreover, having presented evidence that the two phenomena are perceptually distinct, it could be of particular interest to study their temporal relationship with the movement kinematics. Especially in the case of somatosensory attenuation, it would be interesting to characterize the relationship between the kinematics of the right hand (not recorded in the present study) with the somatosensory perception on the left hand during the different phases of the movement to assess how predictive attenuation builds up right before the expected time point of the self-touch in line with the predictive nature of this process.

STAR★Methods

Key resources table

REAGENT or RESOURCE SOURCE IDENTIFIER
Software and algorithms

R Studio Version 1.4.1717 R Studio Team (2021) https://www.rstudio.com/
JASP Version 0.13.1 JASP Team (2020) https://jasp-stats.org/

Other

Motor Maxon Group https://www.maxongroup.com/
Force sensor Honeywell Inc https://buildings.honeywell.com/us/en
Motion tracking device Polhemus Fastrak https://polhemus.com/motion-tracking/all-trackers/fastrak
Drawer runners IKEA https://www.ikea.com/us/en/p/besta-drawer-runner-soft-closing-40348715/

Resource availability

Lead contact

Further information and requests for resources should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the lead contact, Konstantina Kilteni (konstantina.kilteni@ki.se).

Materials availability

Detailed description of the materials (motor, sensors, drawer runner) are listed in the STAR Methods file. Any further information is available from the lead contact upon request.

Experimental model and subject details

After providing written informed consent, twenty-four participants (12 women and 12 men, 22 right-handed, 1 ambidextrous, and 1 left-handed) aged 21–40 years participated. Handedness was assessed using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971). The sample size was set to twenty-four (24) before data collection commenced based on our previous studies using the same methods (Kilteni et al., 2019, 2020), while ensuring a counterbalanced order of conditions. Three participants were excluded because of technical issues with the kinematic recordings and replaced by three new participants to reach the target sample size. The Swedish Ethical Review Authority (https://etikprovningsmyndigheten.se/) approved the study (no. 2016/445-31/2, amendment 2019–04536). All participants provided written informed consent.

Method details

General procedure

Participants sat comfortably on a chair with their arms placed on a table. Their left hands rested palm up, with their index fingers placed on a molded support. The right arms rested palm down on top of a set of sponges. In each trial, a motor (Maxon EC Motor EC 90 flat; Switzerland) delivered two taps (the test tap and the comparison tap) on the pulp of their left index finger through a cylindrical probe (25 mm height) with a flat aluminum surface (20 mm diameter) attached to a lever on the motor. A force sensor (FSG15N1A, Honeywell Inc.; diameter, 5 mm; minimum resolution, 0.01 N; response time, 1 ms; measurement range, 0–15 N) within the probe recorded the forces applied on the left index finger. Following the presentation of the two taps, participants were required to verbally indicate which tap felt stronger: the first or the second. A second identical force sensor within an identical cylindrical probe was placed on top of, but not in contact with, the probe of the left index finger (Figure 1).

A wooden surface was placed under the motor and the sensors. This surface was placed on top of two commercially available drawer runners (IKEA, https://www.ikea.com/us/en/p/besta-drawer-runner-soft-closing-40348715/). One side of the runners was attached to the table with Velcro, and the other side was attached to the bottom side of the wooden surface. With this configuration, the surface, with the motor, the sensors and the participants’ hands, could be moved forward and backward.

In the gating and attenuation&gating conditions (Figures 1C and 1D), participants were asked to extend their elbow upon an auditory ‘go’ cue. The extension of the elbow moved the platform forward on the table (Figure 1F). A piece of green tape on the table (Figure 1I) indicated the start position of the platform, while a piece of red tape indicated the end position. The participants were asked to move the platform from the start position to the end position (distance = 25 cm). During the movement of the left arm, the participants received the test tap on their left index finger. Before the condition started, we emphasized to the subjects that their task was to pay attention to the force that they would receive during the movement rather than covering exactly the distance between the lines. Moreover, the participants were trained to perform the movement in approximately 1,000–1,500 ms after the ‘go’ cue and then stop. In the gating condition, the test tap was applied 800 ms after the ‘go’ cue to ensure that it was delivered during the movement. Similarly, in the attenuation&gating condition, the participants triggered the test tap during the movement. The comparison tap was applied 800–1,500 ms after the test tap to ensure that the participants had stopped moving. Once the participants responded, they returned the platform to the starting position.

In all conditions, the comparison tap was delivered on the left index finger with a random delay of 800–1,500 ms from the test tap. We opted to present the test tap before the comparison tap (fixed order design), consistent with previous studies (Bays et al., 2005; Kilteni et al., 2019, 2020, 2021), to maintain the delay between the two taps constant across conditions and remove any effect of the comparison tap on the tap participants had to perform with their right index finger. For example, if the participants first received a 3 N comparison tap, they might press stronger to generate the subsequent test tap. A fixed order design might introduce a temporal bias to the participants (e.g., participants perceive the second tap as stronger), but any of these biases cancel each other in the comparisons between conditions.

In the attenuation and attenuation&gating conditions (Figures 1B and 1D), the tap of the participants’ right index fingers on the force sensor triggered the test tap on their left index finger with an intrinsic delay of ≈ 36 ms. In these two conditions, participants were asked to tap, neither too weakly nor too strongly, with their right index finger, “as if tapping the screen of their smartphone”. This instruction was provided to ensure that the relationship between the force they applied with their right index finger on the force sensor and the force they received on their left index finger by the motor (2 N) remained constant throughout the experiment, thereby establishing perceived causality (Bays and Wolpert, 2008).

A motion tracking sensor (6DOF Polhemus Fastrak, USA, weight = 9.1 g, dimensions = 2.29 × 2.82 × 1.52 cm) was placed on top of the platform to record the motion of the platform due to the movement of the participants’ left arm. The sensor recorded the x, y and z positions at a sampling rate of approximately 120 Hz.

Each condition included 70 trials. The test tap was set to 2 N, while the intensity of the comparison tap was systematically varied among seven different force levels (1, 1.5, 1.75, 2, 2.25, 2.5 or 3 N). Each tap lasted for 100 ms. In every trial, participants verbally indicated which tap on their left index finger felt stronger: the first (test) or the second (comparison). Participants were told not to try to balance their responses (50% first and 50% second), and they were further instructed to make their best guess if the intensity of the two taps felt similar.

In addition, participants were administered white noise through a pair of headphones to preclude any sounds created by the motor to serve as a cue for the task. The loudness of the white noise was adjusted such that participants could clearly hear the auditory cues of the trial. In all conditions, the view of the pulp of the left index finger was occluded. Participants were asked to fixate on a cross placed on a wall 2 m opposite them, but they were allowed to look at the force sensor to guide the movement of the right index finger when needed (Figures 1B and 1D). No feedback was provided to the participants about their responses.

Force discrimination analysis

In each condition, the participants’ responses were fitted with a generalized linear model using a logit link function (Equation 1):

p=eβ0+β1x1+eβ0+β1x (Equation 1)

We extracted two parameters of interest: the PSE (PSE=β0β1), which represents the intensity at which the test tap felt as strong as the comparison tap (p = 0.5) and quantifies the perceived intensity, and the JND JND=log(3)β1, which reflects the participants’ discrimination capacity. Before fitting the responses, the values of the applied comparison taps were binned to the closest value with respect to their theoretical values (1, 1.5, 1.75, 2, 2.25, 2.5 or 3 N).

Kinematic analysis

Both position and velocity data were smoothed with a moving average filter in MATLAB 2018a. Velocity was calculated as the first derivative of position. We calculated the minimum and the maximum position of the platform during the entire trial duration to calculate the distance participants moved in every trial under each condition. The peak trial velocity was defined as the peak velocity of the entire trial. The peak tap velocity was defined as the peak velocity during the period that the test tap was applied.

Rejection of trials

After data collection, one hundred seventy-three (173) of 6720 trials (2.57%) were rejected. First, in thirty-four trials (34), the intensity of the test tap (2 N) was not applied accurately (test tap <1.85 N or test tap >2.15 N), and the responses were missing in sixteen (16) trials. Second, we rejected one hundred seventeen (117) trials from the gating and attenuation&gating conditions because participants either did not move their left arm (or moved it too slowly) during the test tap (mean velocity <10 cm/s) or they moved it during the comparison tap (mean velocity >5 cm/s). The thresholds were based on a previous study (Cybulska-Klosowicz et al., 2011) showing no gating effects for velocities smaller than 5 cm/s. The analysis was therefore performed with 6547 trials.

Quantification and statistical analysis

Statistical analysis

We used R (Core Team, 2018) and JASP (JASP and JASP Team, 2019) to analyze our data. The data normality was assessed using the Shapiro–Wilk test. Depending on the data normality, we then performed planned comparisons using either a paired t-test or a Wilcoxon signed-rank test. We report 95% confidence intervals (CI95) for each statistical test. Effect sizes are reported as the partial eta-squared (ηp2) values for the ANOVAs, Cohen’s d for t-tests or the matched rank biserial correlation rrb for the Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. In addition, a Bayesian factor analysis using default Cauchy priors with a scale of 0.707 was performed for all statistical tests to provide information about the level of support for the null hypothesis compared to the alternative hypothesis (BF01) based on the data. We interpret a factor between 1/3 and 3 as “anecdotal evidence” (Quintana and Williams, 2018), indicating that support for either the preferred or null hypotheses is insufficient. Finally, correlations were determined by calculating the Pearson’s coefficient r because the data were normally distributed. All tests were two-tailed.

Corrections for multiple comparisons

Since our PSE and JND comparisons were planned, we did not apply corrections for multiple comparisons. However, all results remained exactly the same when applying corrections for the false discovery rate (FDR) (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). In the correlation analyses, we corrected for multiple comparisons (FDR) since, although we expected correlations between the PSEs and between the JNDs, we had no a priori hypotheses for correlations between PSEs and JNDs.

Acknowledgments

We thank M.H., M.C., and L.M. for their feedback on an earlier version of the manuscript. K.K. was supported by the Swedish Research Council (VR Starting Grant 2019-01909 granted to K.K.). H.H.E. and experimental costs were supported by the Swedish Research Council, Torsten Söderbergs Stiftelse, and Göran Gustafssons Stiftelse. We thank Kanaka Raghavan for assistance with the illustrations.

Author contributions

K.K. and H.H.E. conceived and designed the experiment. K.K. collected the data and conducted the statistical analysis. K.K. and H.H.E. wrote the manuscript.

Declaration of interests

The authors have no competing financial interests to declare.

Inclusion and diversity

We worked to ensure gender balance in the recruitment of human subjects.

Published: April 15, 2022

Footnotes

Supplemental information can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.isci.2022.104077.

Supplemental information

Document S1. Figures S1–S7 and Text S1
mmc1.pdf (2.5MB, pdf)

Data and code availability

The data are not available because we have no ethical permit to share them publicly. Instead, we visualized all individual data and their distributions in our main and supplementary figures for maximizing transparency. Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contact upon request.

References

  1. Adams R.a., Shipp S., Friston K.J. Predictions not commands: active inference in the motor system. Brain Struct. Funct. 2013;218:611–643. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0475-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Angel R.W., Malenka R.C. Velocity-dependent suppression of cutaneous sensitivity during movement. Exp. Neurol. 1982;77:266–274. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(82)90244-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Asimakidou E., Job X., Kilteni K. The positive dimension of schizotypy is associated with a reduced attenuation and precision of self-generated touch. bioRxiv. 2022 doi: 10.1101/2022.01.22.476743. Preprint at. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Audette N.J., Zhou W., Schneider D.M. Temporally precise movement-based predictions in the mouse auditory cortex. bioRxiv. 2021 doi: 10.1101/2021.12.13.472457. Preprint at. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Azim E., Seki K. Gain control in the sensorimotor system. Curr. Opin. Physiol. 2019;8:177–187. doi: 10.1016/j.cophys.2019.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bäß P., Jacobsen T., Schröger E., Bäss P., Jacobsen T S.E. Suppression of the auditory N1 event-related potential component with unpredictable self-initiated tones: evidence for internal forward models with dynamic stimulation. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2008;70:137–143. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bays P.M., Flanagan J.R., Wolpert D.M. Attenuation of self-generated tactile sensations is predictive, not postdictive. PLoS Biol. 2006;4:281–284. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bays P.M., Wolpert D.M. Computational principles of sensorimotor control that minimize uncertainty and variability. J. Physiol. 2007;578:387–396. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.120121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bays P.M., Wolpert D.M. In: Sensorimotor Foundations of Higher Cognition. Haggard E.P., Rosetti Y., Kawato M., editors. Oxford University Press; 2008. Predictive attenuation in the perception of touch; pp. 339–358. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bays P.M., Wolpert D.M., Flanagan J.R. Perception of the consequences of self-action is temporally tuned and event driven. Curr. Biol. 2005;15:1125–1128. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.05.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Benjamini Y., Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B. 1995 doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Blakemore S.-J., Smith J., Steel R., Johnstone E.C., Frith C.D. The perception of self-produced sensory stimuli in patients with auditory hallucinations and passivity experiences: evidence for a breakdown in self-monitoring. Psychol. Med. 2000;30:1131–1139. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799002676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Blakemore S.-J., Wolpert D., Frith C. Why canʼt you tickle yourself? Neuroreport. 2000;11:R11–R16. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200008030-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Blakemore S.-J., Wolpert D.M., Frith C.D. Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation. Nat. Neurosci. 1998;1:635–640. doi: 10.1038/2870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Blakemore S.-J.J., Frith C.D., Wolpert D.M. The cerebellum is involved in predicting the sensory consequences of action. Neuroreport. 2001;12:1879–1884. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200107030-00023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Blakemore S.J., Wolpert D.M., Frith C.D. The cerebellum contributes to somatosensory cortical activity during self-produced tactile stimulation. Neuroimage. 1999;10:448–459. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Blakemore S.J., Frith C.D., Wolpert D.M. Spatio-temporal prediction modulates the perception of self-produced stimuli. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1999;11:551–559. doi: 10.1162/089892999563607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Boehme R., Hauser S., Gerling G.J., Heilig M., Olausson H. Distinction of self-produced touch and social touch at cortical and spinal cord levels. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2019;116:2290–2299. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1816278116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Brooks J.X., Carriot J., Cullen K.E. Learning to expect the unexpected: rapid updating in primate cerebellum during voluntary self-motion. Nat. Neurosci. 2015;18:1310–1317. doi: 10.1038/nn.4077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Brooks J.X., Cullen K.E. Predictive sensing: the role of motor signals in sensory processing. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging. 2019;4:842–850. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Brown H., Adams R.a., Parees I., Edwards M., Friston K. Active inference, sensory attenuation and illusions. Cogn. Process. 2013;14:411–427. doi: 10.1007/s10339-013-0571-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Callier T., Saal H.P., Davis-Berg E.C., Bensmaia S.J. Kinematics of unconstrained tactile texture exploration. J. Neurophysiol. 2015;113:3013–3020. doi: 10.1152/jn.00703.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Chakrabarti S., Schwarz C. Cortical modulation of sensory flow during active touch in the rat whisker system. Nat. Commun. 2018;9:1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-06200-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Chapin J.K., Woodward D.J. Modulation of sensory responsiveness of single somatosensory cortical cells during movement and arousal behaviors. Exp. Neurol. 1981;72:164–178. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(81)90135-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Chapman C.E. Active versus passive touch: factors influencing the transmission of somatosensory signals to primary sornatosensory cortex. Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 1994;72:558–570. doi: 10.1139/y94-080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Chapman C.E., Beauchamp E. Differential controls over tactile detection in humans by motor commands and peripheral reafference. J. Neurophysiol. 2006;96:1664–1675. doi: 10.1152/jn.00214.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Chapman C.E., Bushnell M.C., Miron D., Duncan G.H., Lund J.P. Sensory perception during movement in man. Exp. Brain Res. 1987;68:516–524. doi: 10.1007/BF00249795. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Cohen L.G., Starr A. Localization, timing and specificity of gating of somatosensory evoked potentials during active movement in man. Brain. 1987 doi: 10.1093/brain/110.2.451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Colino F.L., Binsted G. Time course of tactile gating in a reach-to-grasp and lift task. J. Mot. Behav. 2016;48:390–400. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2015.1113917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Colino F.L., Buckingham G., Cheng D.T., van Donkelaar P., Binsted G. Tactile gating in a reaching and grasping task. Physiol. Rep. 2014;2:1–11. doi: 10.1002/phy2.267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Collins D.F., Cameron T., Gillard D.M., Prochazka A. Muscular sense is attenuated when humans move. J. Physiol. 1998;508:635–643. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1998.00635.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Core Team R. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R. Found. Stat. Comput. 2018 doi: 10.1007/978-3-540-74686-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Crapse T.B., Sommer M.A. Corollary discharge across the animal kingdom. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;9:587–600. doi: 10.1038/nrn2457. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Cullen K.E. Sensory signals during active versus passive movement. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2004;14:698–706. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Cullen K.E. The vestibular system: multimodal integration and encoding of self-motion for motor control. Trends Neurosci. 2012;35:185–196. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Cybulska-Klosowicz A., Meftah E.M., Raby M., Lemieux M.L., Chapman C.E. A critical speed for gating of tactile detection during voluntary movement. Exp. Brain Res. 2011;210:291–301. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2632-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Diedrichsen J., Verstynen T., Hon A., Lehman S.L., Ivry R.B. Anticipatory adjustments in the unloading task: is an efference copy necessary for learning? Exp. Brain Res. 2003;148:272–276. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1318-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Fahy F.L., Riches I.P., Brown M.W. Exp. Brain Res. 1993;9:457–472. doi: 10.1007/BF00234113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Fraser L.E., Fiehler K. Predicted reach consequences drive time course of tactile suppression. Behav. Brain Res. 2018;350:54–64. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Friston K. The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010 doi: 10.1038/nrn2787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Fukutomi M., Carlson B.A. A history of corollary discharge: contributions of mormyrid weakly electric fish. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2020;14 doi: 10.3389/fnint.2020.00042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gertz H., Voudouris D., Fiehler K. Reach-relevant somatosensory signals modulate tactile suppression. J. Neurophysiol. 2017;117:2262–2268. doi: 10.1152/jn.00052.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gescheider G.A., Herman D.D., Phillips J.N. Criterion shifts in the measurement of tactile masking. Percept. Psychophys. 1970;8:433–436. doi: 10.3758/BF03207041. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Ghez C., Lenzi G.L. Modulation of sensory transmission in cat lemniscal system during voluntary movement. Pflügers Arch. Eur. J. Physiol. 1971;323:273–278. doi: 10.1007/BF00586390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Giblin D.R. Somatosensory evoked potentials IN healthy subjects and IN patients with lesions OF the nervous system. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1964;112:93–142. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1964.tb26744.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. He Q., Versteeg C.S., Suresh A.K., Miller L.E., Bensmaia S.J. Modulation of cutaneous responses in the cuneate nucleus of macaques during active movement. bioRxiv. 2021 doi: 10.1101/2021.11.15.468735. Preprint at. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Heller M.A. Texture perception in sighted and blind observers. Percept. Psychophys. 1989;45:49–54. doi: 10.3758/BF03208032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Ishiyama S., Kaufmann L.V., Brecht M. Behavioral and cortical correlates of self-suppression, anticipation, and ambivalence in rat tickling. Curr. Biol. 2019;29:3153–3164.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.07.085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. JASP and JASP Team . Computer software; 2019. JASP. [Google Scholar]
  50. Jiang W., Chapman C.E., Lamarre Y. Modulation of the cutaneous responsiveness of neurones in the primary somatosensory cortex during conditioned arm movements in the monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 1991;84:342–354. doi: 10.1007/BF00231455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Jiang W., Lamarre Y., Chapman C.E. Modulation of cutaneous cortical evoked potentials during isometric and isotonic contractions in the monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 1990;84:342–354. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90010-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Juravle G., Binsted G., Spence C. Tactile suppression in goal-directed movement. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2017;24:1060–1076. doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1203-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kawato M. Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1999;9:718–727. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(99)00028-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kilteni K., Andersson B.J., Houborg C., Ehrsson H.H. Motor imagery involves predicting the sensory consequences of the imagined movement. Nat. Commun. 2018;9:1617. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-03989-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kilteni K., Ehrsson H.H. Sensorimotor predictions and tool use: hand-held tools attenuate self-touch. Cognition. 2017;165:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kilteni K., Ehrsson H.H. Body ownership determines the attenuation of self-generated tactile sensations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2017;114:8426–8431. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1703347114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Kilteni K., Ehrsson H.H. Functional connectivity between the cerebellum and somatosensory areas implements the attenuation of self-generated touch. J. Neurosci. 2020;40:894–906. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1732-19.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kilteni K., Engeler P., Boberg I., Maurex L., Ehrsson H.H. No evidence for somatosensory attenuation during action observation of self-touch. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2021;54:6422–6444. doi: 10.1111/ejn.15436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kilteni K., Engeler P., Ehrsson H.H. Efference copy is necessary for the attenuation of self-generated touch. iScience. 2020;23:100843. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2020.100843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Kilteni K., Houborg C., Ehrsson H.H. Rapid learning and unlearning of predicted sensory delays in self-generated touch. Elife. 2019;8:1–17. doi: 10.7554/eLife.42888. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Klaffehn A.L., Baess P., Kunde W., Pfister R. Sensory attenuation prevails when controlling for temporal predictability of self- and externally generated tones. Neuropsychologia. 2019;132:107145. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Knoetsch F., Zimmermann E. The spatial specificity of sensory attenuation for self-touch. Conscious. Cogn. 2021;92:103135. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2021.103135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Lalouni M., Fust J., Vadenmark-Lundqvist V., Ehrsson H.H., Kilteni K., Jensen K. Predicting pain: differential pain thresholds during self-induced, externally induced, and imagined self-induced pressure pain. Pain. 2020 doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Laskin S.E., Spencer W.A. Cutaneous masking. I. Psychophysical observations on interactions of multipoint stimuli in man. J. Neurophysiol. 1979;42:1048–1060. doi: 10.1152/jn.1979.42.4.1048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Leavens D.A., Bard K.A. Tickling. Curr. Biol. 2016;26:R91–R93. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.06.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Lederman S.J. The perception of surface roughness by active and passive touch. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 1981;18:253–255. doi: 10.3758/BF03333619. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  67. Lederman S.J., Klatzky R.L. Hand movements: a window into haptic object recognition. Cogn. Psychol. 1987;19:342–368. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(87)90008-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Lei Y., Ozdemir R.A., Perez M.A. Gating of sensory input at subcortical and cortical levels during grasping in humans. J. Neurosci. 2018;38:7237–7247. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0545-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Limanowski J., Lopes P., Keck J., Baudisch P., Friston K., Blankenburg F. Action-dependent processing of touch in the human parietal operculum and posterior insula. Cereb. Cortex. 2020;30:607–617. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Macerollo A., Limousin P., Korlipara P., Foltynie T., Edwards M.J., Kilner J. Dopaminergic modulation of sensory attenuation in Parkinson’s disease: is there an underlying modulation of beta power? Front. Neurol. 2019;10:1–14. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.01001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Mapp A.P., Ono H. York University; Toronto, Ontario: 2006. Precision and Accuracy with Three Psychophysical Methods (English version) [Web application]http://www.yorku.ca/psycho/index.html [Google Scholar]
  72. McNamee D., Wolpert D.M. Internal models in biological control. Annu. Rev. Control Robot. Auton. Syst. 2019;2:339–364. doi: 10.1146/annurev-control-060117-105206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Miall R.C., Wolpert D.M. Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Networks. 1996;9:1265–1279. doi: 10.1016/S0893-6080(96)00035-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Oldfield R.C.R.C. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Palmer C.E., Davare M., Kilner J.M. Physiological and perceptual sensory attenuation have different underlying neurophysiological correlates. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:10803–10812. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1694-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Papakostopoulos D., Cooper R., Crow H.J. Inhibition of cortical evoked potentials and sensation by self-initiated movement in man. Nature. 1975;258:321–324. doi: 10.1038/258321a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Parees I., Brown H., Nuruki a., Adams R.a., Davare M., Bhatia K.P., Friston K., Edwards M.J. Loss of sensory attenuation in patients with functional (psychogenic) movement disorders. Brain. 2014;137:2916–2921. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Parthasharathy M., Mantini D., Orban De Xivry J.-J. Increased upper-limb sensory attenuation with age. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.09.17.301739. Preprint at. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Pertovaara A., Kemppainen P., Leppänen H. Lowered cutaneous sensitivity to nonpainful electrical stimulation during isometric exercise in humans. Exp. Brain Res. 1992;89:447–452. doi: 10.1007/BF00228261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Pickering M.J., Clark A. Getting ahead: forward models and their place in cognitive architecture. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014;18:451–456. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Post L.J., Zompa I.C., Chapman C.E. Perception of vibrotactile stimuli during motor activity in human subjects. Exp. Brain Res. 1994;100:107–120. doi: 10.1007/BF00227283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Poulet J.F.A., Hedwig B. Corollary discharge inhibition of ascending auditory neurons in the stridulating cricket. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:4717–4725. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-11-04717.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Poulet J.F.A.A., Hedwig B. The cellular basis of a corollary discharge. Science. 2006;311:518–522. doi: 10.1126/science.1120847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Quintana D.S., Williams D.R. Bayesian alternatives for common null-hypothesis significance tests in psychiatry: a non-technical guide using JASP. BMC Psychiatry. 2018;18:1–8. doi: 10.1186/s12888-018-1761-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Rahman M.S., Yau J.M. Somatosensory interactions reveal feature-dependent computations. J. Neurophysiol. 2019;122:5–21. doi: 10.1152/jn.00168.2019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Rauch R., Angel R.W., Boylls C.C. Velocity-dependent suppression of somatosensory evoked potentials during movement. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Evoked Potentials. 1985;62:421–425. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(85)90051-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Roy J.E. Dissociating self-generated from passively applied head motion: neural mechanisms in the vestibular nuclei. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:2102–2111. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3988-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Rushton D.N., Roghwell J.C., Craggs M.D. Gating of somatosensory evoked potentials during different kinds of movement in man. Brain. 1981;104:465–491. doi: 10.1093/brain/104.3.465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Ryan C.P., Bettelani G.C., Ciotti S., Parise C., Moscatelli A., Bianchi M. The interaction between motion and texture in the sense of touch. J. Neurophysiol. 2021;126:1375–1390. doi: 10.1152/jn.00583.2020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Saradjian A.H. Sensory modulation of movement, posture and locomotion. Neurophysiol. Clin. 2015;45:255–267. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2015.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Sawtell N.B. Neural mechanisms for predicting the sensory consequences of behavior: insights from electrosensory systems. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 2017;79:381–399. doi: 10.1146/annurev-physiol-021115-105003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Schneider D.M., Mooney R. How movement modulates hearing. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2018;41:553–572. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Schneider D.M., Sundararajan J., Mooney R. A cortical filter that learns to suppress the acoustic consequences of movement. Nature. 2018;561:391–395. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0520-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Seki K., Fetz E.E. Gating of sensory input at spinal and cortical levels during preparation and execution of voluntary movement. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:890–902. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4958-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Seki K., Perlmutter S.I., Fetz E.E. Sensory input to primate spinal cord is presynaptically inhibited during voluntary movement. Nat. Neurosci. 2003;6:1309–1316. doi: 10.1038/nn1154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Shadmehr R., Krakauer J.W., Neuroanatomy A.C., Motor F.O.R. A computational neuroanatomy for motor control. Exp. Brain Res. 2008;185:359–381. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1280-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Shadmehr R., Smith M.A., Krakauer J.W. Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2010;33:89–108. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Shergill S.S., Bays P.M., Frith C.D., Wolpert D.M. Two eyes for an eye: the neuroscience of force escalation. Science. 2003;301:187. doi: 10.1126/science.1085327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Shergill S.S., Samson G., Bays P.M., Frith C.D., Wolpert D.M. Evidence for sensory prediction deficits in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2005;162:2384–2386. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.12.2384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Shergill S.S., White T.P., Joyce D.W., Bays P.M., Wolpert D.M., Frith C.D. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of impaired sensory prediction in schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71:28. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.2974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Shergill S.S., White T.P., Joyce D.W., Bays P.M., Wolpert D.M., Frith C.D. Modulation of somatosensory processing by action. Neuroimage. 2013;70:356–362. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Sherrick C.E. Effects of double simultaneous stimulation of the skin. Am. J. Psychol. 1964;77:42–53. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Simões-Franklin C., Whitaker T.A., Newell F.N. Active and passive touch differentially activate somatosensory cortex in texture perception. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2011;32:1067–1080. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Starr A., Cohen L.G. “Gating” of somatosensory evoked potentials begins before the onset of voluntary movement in man. Brain Res. 1985;348:183–186. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(85)90377-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Straka H., Simmers J., Chagnaud B.P. A new perspective on predictive motor signaling. Curr. Biol. 2018;28:R232–R243. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.01.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Suresh A.K., Greenspon C.M., He Q., Rosenow J.M., Miller L.E., Bensmaia S.J. Sensory computations in the cuneate nucleus of macaques. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 2021;118 doi: 10.1073/pnas.2115772118. e2115772118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Tamè L., Braun C., Holmes N.P., Farnè A., Pavani F. Bilateral representations of touch in the primary somatosensory cortex. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2016;33:48–66. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2016.1159547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Tamè L., Farnè A., Pavani F. Spatial coding of touch at the fingers: insights from double simultaneous stimulation within and between hands. Neurosci. Lett. 2011;487:78–82. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.09.078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Therrien A.S., Bastian A.J. The cerebellum as a movement sensor. Neurosci. Lett. 2019;688:0–1. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.06.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Thomas E.R., Yon D., de Lange F.P., Press C. Action enhances predicted touch. Psychol. Sci. 2021;33:48–59. doi: 10.1177/09567976211017505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Todorov E. Optimality principles in sensorimotor control. Nat. Neurosci. 2004;7:907–915. doi: 10.1038/nn1309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Verrillo R.T., Bolanowski S.J., McGlone F.P. Subjective magnitude of tactile roughness. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 1999;16:352–360. doi: 10.1080/08990229970401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Voss M., Ingram J.N., Wolpert D.M., Haggard P. Mere expectation to move causes attenuation of sensory signals. PLoS One. 2008;3:2–6. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002866. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Voudouris D., Broda M.D., Fiehler K. Anticipatory grasping control modulates somatosensory perception. J. Vis. 2019;19:1–10. doi: 10.1167/19.5.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Voudouris D., Fiehler K. Enhancement and suppression of tactile signals during reaching. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2017;43:1238–1248. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Voudouris D., Fiehler K. Dynamic temporal modulation of somatosensory processing during reaching. Sci. Rep. 2021;11:1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-81156-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Walsh L.D., Taylor J.L., Gandevia S.C. Overestimation of force during matching of externally generated forces. J. Physiol. 2011;589:547–557. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.198689. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Weiskrantz L., Elliott J., Darlington C. Preliminary observations on tickling oneself. Nature. 1971;230:598–599. doi: 10.1038/230598a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Williams S.R., Chapman C.E. Time course and magnitude of movement-related gating of tactile detection in humans. II. Effects of stimulus intensity. J. Neurophysiol. 2000;84:863–875. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.2.863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Williams S.R., Chapman C.E. Time course and magnitude of movement-related gating of tactile detection in humans. III. Effect of motor tasks. J. Neurophysiol. 2002;88:1968–1979. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.4.1968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Williams S.R., Shenasa J., Chapman C.E. Time course and magnitude of movement-related gating of tactile detection in humans. I. Importance of stimulus location. J. Neurophysiol. 1998;79:947–963. doi: 10.1152/jn.00527.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Wolpe N., Ingram J.N., Tsvetanov K.A., Geerligs L., Kievit R.A., Henson R.N., Wolpert D.M., Rowe J.B. Ageing increases reliance on sensorimotor prediction through structural and functional differences in frontostriatal circuits. Nat. Commun. 2016;7:13034. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Wolpe N., Zhang J., Nombela C., Ingram J.N., Wolpert D.M., Rowe J.B. Sensory attenuation in Parkinson’s disease is related to disease severity and dopamine dose. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:15643. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-33678-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Wolpert D.M., Miall RC K.M., Wolpert D.M., Miall R.C., Kawato M., Wolpert D.M., Miall RC K.M. Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1998;2:338–347. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01221-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Wolpert D.M., Flanagan J.R. Motor prediction. Curr. Biol. 2001;11:R729–R732. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00432-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Wolpert D.M., Ghahramani Z. Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nat. Neurosci. 2000;3:1212–1217. doi: 10.1038/81497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Document S1. Figures S1–S7 and Text S1
mmc1.pdf (2.5MB, pdf)

Data Availability Statement

The data are not available because we have no ethical permit to share them publicly. Instead, we visualized all individual data and their distributions in our main and supplementary figures for maximizing transparency. Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contact upon request.


Articles from iScience are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES