Abstract
Two decades of cross-species neuroscience research on rapid action-stopping in the laboratory has motivated an underlying prefrontal–basal ganglia circuit. Here we provide an update of key studies of the past few years. We conclude that this basic neural circuit is on increasingly firm ground, and we move on to consider whether the action-stopping function implemented by this circuit applies beyond the simple laboratory stop signal task. We advance through a series of studies of increasing ‘real-worldness’, starting with laboratory tests of stopping of speech, gait and bodily functions, and then going beyond the laboratory to consider neural recordings and stimulation during moments of control presumably required in everyday activities such as walking and driving. We end by asking whether stopping research has clinical relevance, focusing on movement disorders such as stuttering, tics and freezing of gait. Overall, we conclude there are hints that the prefrontal–basal ganglia action-stopping circuit that is engaged by the basic stop signal task is recruited in myriad scenarios; however, truly proving this for real-world scenarios requires a new generation of studies that will need to overcome substantial technical and inferential challenges.
ToC blurb
Many studies implicate a prefrontal–basal ganglia circuit in the control of action-stopping. Here, Hannah and Aron provide an update of studies of this circuit, discuss its clinical relevance, and consider whether its action-stopping function applies in real-world scenarios, beyond the laboratory.
Introduction
‘Executive control’ refers to the way we use our goals to control our behaviour and thoughts. One type of executive function is the stopping of actions and thoughts, and it works alongside other functions such as working memory to help us respond appropriately under circumstances that involve novelty or the overriding of habits1,2. For example, holding the goal in mind of losing weight when on a diet might lead us to stop when reaching for an extra biscuit. The potential importance of stopping is emphasized in clinical disorders characterized by problems controlling thoughts or impulses to act (for example, intrusive thinking or motor tics). Although thought-stopping and action-stopping are highly relevant to healthy human behaviour, and may even rely on analogous brain systems3, action-stopping has received greater systematic investigation because it is easier to quantify and because we know more about the motor system. Action-stopping is therefore a stronger starting point for this Review, which broaches the question of whether our understanding of stopping, developed largely from highly-controlled laboratory-based experiments, generalizes to the real-world.
In the laboratory, action-stopping has been most commonly studied with the stop signal task4,5 (Fig. 1a). This requires people to rapidly stop an initiated response when a signal occurs. Although other forms of action-stopping exist, such as proactively braking or delaying impending actions (Box 1), as do other broader forms of behavioural control (for example, choosing not to act, or resisting interference from distraction), overall these are less understood and may even engage different processes or networks6-8, and so here we focus on the simpler case. A prominent model (hereafter referred to as the action-stopping model) proposes that action-stopping is achieved via a prefrontal cortex (PFC)–basal ganglia–thalamocortical ‘stopping’ network9 (hereafter referred to as the action-stopping network). We begin by reviewing this model, moving briefly over older results that have been covered many times10-12 and focusing more on recent human, and in some cases non-human animal, studies since the last major review9. We see that key claims of the model are well supported. We then ask whether the action-stopping model applies beyond simple laboratory-based tasks that mostly use manual responses. We then review recent work that starts to tackle whether action-stopping generalizes to more naturalistic settings and end with the application of the model to clinical movement disorders. Overall, these insights are intended to provide a roadmap for future research into real-world action control by highlighting the promises and challenges offered by the action-stopping model.
Fig. 1 ∣. Cortico-basal ganglia-thalamocortical networks for action control in the stop signal task.
a ∣ In the stop signal task, a ‘go’ signal (here, an arrow) requiring a response (a key press indicated by the direction of the arrow) is presented on every trial. In about 25% of trials, the go signal is followed shortly by a ‘stop’ signal (here, the arrow turning red), and on those trials, participants must attempt to stop the impending response. The delay between the go and stop cues varies from trial to trial and, if the delay between them is short, the participant is more likely to stop. By titrating the probability of successfully stopping at various delays and examining the response-time distribution, it is possible to estimate the latency of action-stopping, the stop signal reaction time (SSRT) 4,5. b ∣ The basal ganglia (BG) output nuclei (the globus pallidus interna (GPi) and substantia nigra pars reticulata (SNr)) provide tonic inhibition of the thalamic outputs that facilitate motor cortical areas. A classic view is that the direct pathway, which involves striatal projections to the GPi/SNr, supports actions by inhibiting the BG output, thus disinhibiting thalamic output to the cortex81. Meanwhile, the indirect pathway, which involves projections from the striatum to the GPi/SNr via the globus pallidus externa (GPe), may help to suppress actions by increasing inhibitory control over BG output (for a more nuanced discussion, see ref. 172). The hyperdirect pathway is also thought to be involved in inhibiting BG output173, offering a fast route for stopping because of the short-latency, monosynaptic connections between the cortex and subthalamic nucleus (STN)36, and the direct access of the STN to the GPi/SNr. c ∣ The prefrontal-BG-thalamocortical model of action-stopping9,10 proposes that, on detection of a stop signal, sensory information about the cue is fed forward to the prefrontal cortex, where the stop command is produced. Two prefrontal areas, the right inferior frontal cortex (rIFC) and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (particularly the pre-supplementary motor area (preSMA)) are thought send the stop command via the STN. Output from the STN excites the GPi (or SNr, for example with eye movements), which in turn inhibits thalamic excitatory drive to M1 and thus reduces the likelihood of movement. The striatum, acting via the GPe, has also been implicated in action-stopping, although its precise role is currently debated. Both the GPe and striatum have been left unshaded in the figure to illustrate the questions surrounding their inclusion in the network.
Box 1 ∣. Modes of action control.
Several different modes of action-stopping have been described. Each may be called upon in different situations and though some seem likely to depend on a shared neural architecture, others may engage different pathways.
[b1] Reactive stopping
Reactive stopping is triggered in response to an unpredictable cue, as in the standard stop signal task, and reflects emergency-like stopping. Although this form of stopping is most widely studied, in real life it is probably only called upon sometimes — for example, stopping oneself from stepping into the road when a car suddenly approaches. It is thought to rely on the action-stopping network (Fig. 1c).
[b1] Proactive control
Proactive control refers to preventing actions or slowing them down, and can occur in two ways. First, it can be set up in advance of an overt need to stop, and not implemented until that time. Laboratory tests of this scenario have shown that this form of proactive control recruits the pre-supplementary area (preSMA), striatum and pallidum, suggestive of indirect pathway recruitment74,177,178 (see also below). Second, it can act like a brake, recruited partially when you anticipate having to stop72,76,179. The subthalamic nucleus (STN)55,87,90 and a preSMA–striatal pathway72,77 have both been implicated in this form of proactive control72,77.
[b1] Selective stopping
Selective stopping in a behavioural sense refers to stopping one action without affecting other ongoing actions. This is often tested in tasks where participants initiate bilateral hand movements, but are then told to stop with one hand and continue with the other16,180. They can do so, but the response time of the continuing hand is delayed compared with typical responses. This implies the use of a global, STN-mediated64,67, mechanism. However, a mechanistically selective mechanism can be employed if one can proactively prepare to stop selectively177,178 (but see ref.86), and this may involve the prefrontal-striato-pallidal (indirect) pathway16,74.
[b1] Stop–change and switching
Stop–change and switching are when the stopping of one action is immediately followed by the initiation of another: a football player cancels a pass to one teammate when they see a defender nearby, and quickly passes to an unmarked teammate. Neuroimaging181,182, neurophysiology48,70,183 and brain stimulation183,184 studies suggest there is overlap in the systems involved in stop–change and switching with the action-stopping network, although this overlap probably depends on the type of switching task41,70.
The stop signal task provides an estimate of the behavioural latency of action-stopping, the stop signal reaction time (SSRT; the latency of stopping a cued action). As the time at which an uninitiated action plan is stopped cannot be directly observed, SSRT is derived on the basis of a mathematical model that uses reaction times and the probability of successful stopping5. Estimates of SSRT are therefore sensitive to violations of the model assumptions13, and occasional failures by individuals to trigger the stop process14. Notwithstanding w
Neural basis of action-stopping
Two decades of action-stopping research, building on a long history of work on cortico–basal ganglia circuits (Fig. 1b), has motivated a model of action-stopping that depends on PFC–basal ganglia circuits (Fig. 1c). Briefly, sensory information about a stop signal is transmitted to two key regions of the PFC — the right inferior frontal cortex (rIFC) and pre-supplementary motor area (preSMA) — which generate a stop command and forward it to the subthalamic nucleus (STN). The command is then sent via the basal ganglia output nuclei to the motor thalamus, with the effect of reducing drive to the motor cortical areas and thus reducing the likelihood of movement.
We now update the evidence for this model, brain region by brain region in the order in which they seem to be activated by the requirement to stop ‘reactively’ (‘reactive stopping’ in Box 1), noting in particular new data illustrating the timing of activity in each brain region that supports the presumed temporal flow through the network (Fig. 2; see ref. 15).
Fig. 2 ∣. Timing of events in the action-stopping network.
Top panel illustrates the approximate timing of events, and bottom panel shows a schematized version of changes in neural activity, with arrows representing the same key time points as in the main text. This example is for stopping a manual response in relation to a salient stop signal. Following a go signal, a putative go process (green) is initiated and proceeds over time. After some delay, typically about 200 ms, a stop signal is presented, prompting the initiation of a stop process (red) that races to completion with the go process. Physiologically, the generation of the stop command is reflected by activity in the right prefrontal cortex and frontal cortex (rPFC/rFC; particularly the right inferior frontal cortex and pre-supplementary area) within approximately 120 ms after the stop signal15,31. This is followed by activity in the basal ganglia (BG), starting with the subthalamic nucleus (STN), which receives hyperdirect input from the rPFC shortly after36,48. We presume activity in the BG precedes a global suppression of the primary motor cortex (M1) that occurs from about 140 ms after the stop signal15,82. In the meantime, muscle activity related to the go process may already have been initiated, but has not developed sufficiently to produce an overt response. A stop-related suppression of any ongoing muscle activity is evident in the electromyogram (EMG) within approximately 160 ms15,31,32,86. Assuming that this occurs soon enough — that is, that the stop process reaches completion before the go process — behavioural stopping as indicated by the stop signal reaction time (SSRT) occurs within about 220 ms. These timings impose new constraints on when activity in a brain region must occur by to be included in the network model. For example, recent work suggested that parietal cortex might functionally contribute to stopping174. However, the timing of its supposed role, which comes after the cancellation of muscle activity, argues against its inclusion in the action-stopping network and instead hints at a role in action-execution175 (for example, ensuring the timely release of actions). Note, however, that there remains a degree of uncertainty in the timing of the neurophysiological events described here. For example, some brain processes may take time to develop following the initial receipt of a signal from another brain region and may not be immediately detectable in currently available neurophysiological measures. There is also variability in how timings are reported, wherein, for example, beta-band burst timings are measured from the peak of activity, whereas EMG suppression is taken from its onset. Figure adapted from ref.15.
Right inferior frontal cortex.
It was proposed the ventral pars opercularis of the rIFC initiates the stop command19, implying it should be active early during the stop process. Indeed, recent findings confirm the early involvement of the rIFC.
Imaging studies first identified rIFC as a potential mediator of action-stopping18,20-23. Causal evidence came from patients with prefrontal damage24 (pointing particularly to the pars opercularis), and then from offline transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) studies showing that disrupting the rIFC prolonged SSRT25,26. An electrocorticography (ECoG) study revealed increased oscillatory power in the beta band (13–30 Hz) in rIFC in the time period between the stop signal and SSRT27, but lacked the temporal resolution to say more about its role in initiating the stop command. Functional imaging18 and imaging of white-matter tracts18,28 have suggested that regions of rIFC and preSMA that are activated following a stop signal are connected with each other and to the STN, thus pointing to a possible hyperdirect pathway.
These findings have since been buttressed by another ECoG study showing an increase in gamma-band activity in the rIFC after the stop signal29 (earliest in the ventral pars opercularis (also see ref. 30) relative to more anterior IFC regions and the anterior insula), and three recent studies combining electroencephalography (EEG), TMS and electromyography (EMG)15,31,32. The latter three studies revealed four main findings. First, bursts of beta-band activity were observed in right-lateralized frontal electrodes on successful stop trials within approximately 120 ms after the stop signal15,31,32. Second, the TMS revealed that these bursts were soon followed by a broad suppression of the motor system (a putative signature of basal ganglia recruitment, see below)15. Third, there was subsequently a cancellation of motor output, manifesting as a decline in muscle activity15,31,32. Fourth, behavioral stopping occurred at about 220 ms after the stop signal (Fig. 2)
Two of these studies involved TMS-induced disruption targeting rIFC. One showed that the timing of beta-band activity correlated with that of action-stopping at the single-trial level, and that online TMS disruption around the time of this beta activity prolonged the latency of action-stopping31. The other showed that offline disruption again targeting rIFC slowed stopping speed and reduced rIFC beta-band activity32.
A criticism of the three EEG studies15,31,32 is that the poor spatial resolution of EEG precludes knowing whether beta-band activity truly emanated from the rIFC33. However, evidence from a combined functional MRI (fMRI)–magnetoencephalography (MEG) study, offering better spatial resolution, replicated the timely increase in beta that was specific to right, not left, IFC34. Together, these studies begin to address previous concerns about the specificity of rIFC activation (including its laterality) and its involvement in action-stopping per se35. For example, both the time-locking of beta-band activity to action-stopping31, rather than to the stop signal, and the elevation of beta specifically after a stop signal34, rather than any infrequent or unexpected signal, are more consistent with a role for rIFC in stopping than simply monitoring or attentional capture.
A recent study in individuals with Parkinson disease provided high temporal-resolution evidence of a functional rIFC–STN hyperdirect pathway for action-stopping36. First, stimulating the STN (specifically the ventral part, as implicated in the action-stopping model37, see below) evoked short-latency potentials (occurring about 2 ms after stimulation) in ECoG electrodes over the rIFC, consistent with a monosynaptic connection36. Second, event-related potentials in the rIFC associated with stopping preceded those in the STN, consistent with the temporal primacy of the rIFC. Last, individuals with greater functional correlation between these regions stopped faster than did individuals with less co-activity between these regions.
Overall, these studies are broadly consistent with the idea that the rIFC plays a key role early in the stop process and that effects on putative basal ganglia and muscle are later in time.
Pre-supplementary motor area.
The preSMA was also thought to have a critical role in implementing the stop command, and recent findings support that. However, its functional role in relation to rIFC is still unclear.
The right preSMA is activated in fMRI studies of action-stopping18,20,38,39. ECoG indicated that successful stopping is associated with increased beta-band and gamma-band (30–90 Hz) oscillatory power before the SSRT in preSMA27. There is also some causal evidence of preSMA involvement in stopping from individuals with large lesions40,41, and from TMS studies39,42,43. PreSMA has been implicated in various sensorimotor functions44, including conflict resolution (as reviewed in ref.44). With respect to stopping, one possibility is that monitoring-related activity in preSMA identifies the need to stop, and that the preSMA forwards this via the frontal aslant tract to the rIFC18,45, which implements the stop process10. This predicts that preSMA activity precedes that of rIFC during action-stopping. Alternatively, preSMA could exert its influence directly on the STN via hyperdirect projections18,45-48, and act synergistically with any influence of rIFC on the STN. MEG studies have provided conflicting results concerning the temporal primacy of preSMA activity versus rIFC activity34,49.
A recent EEG study showed that fronto-central beta-band bursts during action-stopping50, presumably corresponding to preSMA27, were followed shortly by beta-band bursting in sensorimotor areas, a putative signature of inhibitory activity. The temporal relationship fits with the presumed flow of the stop command from prefrontal to motor areas. However, a non-human primate EEG study questioned the functional relevance of fronto-central beta-band bursting, on the grounds that it occurs too infrequently to explain stopping and was more sensitive to stopping failures than stopping success51.
New rodent data have provided causal evidence for a direct influence of the dorsomedial PFC (dmPFC, which in humans encompasses the preSMA) over the STN52. Tracing was used to identify subpopulations of intermixed neurons in rodent dmPFC with different projection targets, including the STN. Next, optogenetic activation and inactivation of the STN-projecting neurons in the dmPFC during a go/no-go task, as well as the STN directly, reduced and increased impulsive responding, respectively. Furthermore, stimulation of dmPFC neurons projecting to lateral hypothalamus showed opposite behavioural effects in the go/no-go task to stimulation of dmPFC–STN projections, providing a clear dissociation in the function of dmPFC targets.
Overall, it seems there is a functional hyperdirect pathway from preSMA or dmPFC to the STN that is activated in sufficient time to affect stopping; however, future research still needs to delineate the relative roles of the preSMA and rIFC in what seems to be a preSMA–rIFC–STN network.
Subthalamic nucleus.
The ventral STN was proposed to receive the stop command from the PFC and relay it via basal ganglia output neurons to the thalamus. Recent findings provide support for a hyperdirect pathway from the PFC to the ventral STN.
The STN has been implicated in interrupting53, delaying and ‘braking’54-56, and outright stopping of actions that have been prepared or already initiated18,20,48,57. Several studies have shown that deep brain stimulation of the STN in individuals with Parkinson disease changes SSRT58-61. Neurophysiological recordings in such individuals have suggested that STN activity increases about 100 ms or so before behavioural stopping36,62,63 (Fig. 2). Relatedly, beta-band power increases for stopping trials in local field potentials recorded from the STN of individuals with Parkinson disease64,65. Trials in which beta-band activity is greater are also ones in which there is a greater global motor system suppression64, whereby, when a participant stops one effector from moving, there is a reduction in the excitability of task-irrelevant muscle representations measured with TMS over primary motor cortex15,66. Stopping via the STN might therefore reflect an emergency-like system that briefly interrupts all ongoing behaviour, which may be more efficient than identifying and stopping all active motor representations individually. These global effects may reflect the putatively broad output from STN to the globus pallidus interna (GPi)9,67. However, the evidence for this idea is still weak, as tracing studies imply that STN–GPi projections are topographically and somatotopically organized68,69.
The action-stopping model was recently updated, based on monkey tract-tracing data, to suggest that the ventral part of the STN specifically would be important for stopping because it receives input from lateral PFC, including the rIFC37. The prediction was validated by a monkey recording study70 and the above-mentioned study in people with Parkinson disease36. There is also now good causal evidence of STN involvement in stopping: optogenetic studies of the rodent STN53 and the dmPFC–STN connection52 (mentioned above) show that they are important for interrupting ongoing and planned behaviours.
An interesting feature of STN activity in action-stopping is the increase of beta-band power. This is also observed in rIFC and preSMA, which has prompted the speculation that beta-band oscillations could be a property of the stopping network. One suggestion is that the bursts reflect brief periods of top-down communication from PFC to the STN that instantiates the stopping command37.
Potential role for the striatum.
The striatum has also been implicated in stopping, but whether it should be included in the standard action-stopping network is still unclear.
fMRI studies of stopping often show striatal activation18,20,39,71,72. Furthermore, disruption of preSMA and rIFC with offline TMS extends SSRT, with changes in SSRT correlated with changes in the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) activation in the striatum39,73. However, inconsistent with a striatal role in standard action-stopping, SSRT is normal in individuals with pre-manifest Huntington disease who have reductions in striatal volume compared with healthy controls74,75.
Striatal BOLD activity seems to increase when one is told that the likelihood of needing to stop is high, irrespective of whether one is eventually required to stop72,76. This could indicate that the striatum is more involved proactively delaying or ‘braking’ actions to avoid impulsive responding (‘proactive control’ in Box 1). Chemogenetic silencing of dmPFC–striatal neurons in rodents supports this view77. Neural recordings from the rodent globus pallidus externa (GPe), downstream of the striatum, during a stopping task are also consistent with such a role78. When exerting proactive control, the trajectory of population-level activity in the GPe, reflecting collective dynamics of many neurons over time, began further away from the trajectory followed during normal movement initiation78.
Alternatively, the striatum has been suggested to be important when preparing to stop via a more selective mechanism than the global one recruited during reactive stopping74 (‘selective stopping’ in Box 1). Cell-type-specific recording experiments in rodents have shown that direct and indirect projections from the striatum are both more active during movement than during immobility79,80, the coactivation fitting with the idea that the indirect pathway acts on different basal ganglia outputs to selectively suppress actions that compete with the desired one81.
The precise role of the striatum in simple action-stopping remains unclear, but could be resolved using high-temporal-resolution neurophysiological recordings from the striatum and downstream GPe (that is, the indirect pathway) to clarify the conditions under which there is a functional activation of the striato-pallidal pathway.
Motor cortex and spinal motor neurons.
The last cortical region where stopping commands presumably intervene to prevent movement is the primary motor cortex (M1).
TMS over M1 showed that corticospinal excitability is suppressed via a withdrawal of drive to, or an active physiological inhibition (GABAergic) of, agonist-muscle M1 within approximately 140 ms after a stop signal17,82. As noted already, this motor system suppression is detectable in task-irrelevant muscles66,83, and may be mediated by the STN64.
The timing of the motor system suppression fits with changes in neural activity in primate premotor cortex and M184,85. Human studies have shown that changes in motor cortical excitability (and presumably output) during stopping are transmitted very quickly to the muscles15,31,86. Successful stopping of manual responses, in which a participant initiates a response but cancels it before it runs to completion, is often associated with a sharp decline in muscle activity within approximately 160 ms after the stop signal15,31,86, and approximately 20 ms after the global motor system suppression15. It remains unclear whether stop commands are always transmitted via M1 or can also target the spinal cord directly.
A different perspective on the basal ganglia
Recently, a variation of the ‘classic’ action-stopping model was proposed87, with a key difference in how the stop process is implemented by the basal ganglia. The classic model proposes that the STN implements the stop via the GPi and substantia nigra par reticulata (GPi/SNr) (Figs 1,2). The new model, motivated by recordings from rodent basal ganglia88,89, instead proposes that the STN–GPi/SNr pathway serves as a ‘pause’ to delay actions and that a subsequent, pallido-striatal-mediated ‘stop’ process cancels them87. This suggestion is supported by observations that STN activity peaks too soon (within about 15 ms), is too transient and is not specific enough to stopping trials to fully explain action-stopping88. In a follow-up study, a specific population of pallidal cells called arkypallidal cells were identified, defined by the fact that they solely innervate the striatum89. Arkypallidal cells fired after STN neurons and selectively in response to stop signals, rather than to both go and stop signals. The timing of their activity was consistent with precipitating the sudden decline in striatal ‘go’ activity. Together, these findings provide further evidence of the role of the basal ganglia in action-stopping and specifically implicate an arkypallido-striatal pathway.
The above ‘pause-then-cancel’ model could unify differing accounts of STN and striatal involvement in stopping, and is consistent with the idea of the STN acting to delay actions in situations of response conflict54,55,90 or surprise53,91,92 (see ‘Unexpected events’). The fast but brief pause may buy time for deliberation on what (not) to do. This could allow actions deemed inappropriate to be cancelled selectively, and actions deemed appropriate to resume after only a short delay87.
However, the pause-then-cancel model of action-stopping is less complete than the classic model. For example, we do not know which brain regions drive the rodent arkypallidal cells during stopping87 and, to date, there are no comparable data from arkypallidal cells in humans or non-human primates. Furthermore, human and non-human primate data concerning the temporal flow of activity across brain regions (Fig. 2) are inconsistent with a pause-then-cancel model. First, activity in the PFC15,27,31,36,50 and STN63,65,70 on stop trials occurs substantially later than the STN response in rodents88,89. Second, the interval between STN activity and the first behavioural sign of stopping (that is, cancellation of muscle activity) seems very short in humans and non-human primates, unlike in rodents, leaving little room to incorporate a pause. Presently, the classic action-stopping model best explains the human and non-human primate data, and therefore we focus this model hereafter.
Beyond the stop signal task
Most studies have examined the stopping of simple, discrete actions such as button presses, within the context of stop signal tasks (level 1 in Table 1). However, action-stopping is probably relevant to various human activities, from speaking to walking. Little is known about how action control emerges in these diverse settings, but we suppose that it could sometimes engage the action-stopping network. We now review studies that have begun to examine the stopping of more naturalistic actions, though often still within the context of stereotyped laboratory experiments (levels 1,2 in Table 1).
Table 1 ∣.
Degrees of real-world applicability in stopping experiments
Level | Stopping context |
Environment | Action |
---|---|---|---|
1 | Stop signal task (for example, involving explicit stop cues, such as an arrow turning red) | Highly controlled laboratory setting Repeated trials Stopping can be somewhat anticipated |
Simple (for example, involving a single joint or minimal coordination) Contrived (for example, a key press) Discrete Cued |
2 | Variants of stopping tasks (for example, with stop cues that are more naturalistic, such as an obstacle in the participant’s path) | Highly controlled laboratory setting Repeated trials Stopping can be somewhat anticipated |
Complex (for example, multi-joint or multi-limb, or complex coordination) Somewhat naturalistic (for example, speaking or stepping) Discrete Cued |
3 | Virtual reality or simulated (for example, with stop cues that are again more natural, such as a virtual car braking suddenly in front of the participant while ‘driving’) | Less constrained laboratory setting Less-repetitious trials or events Stopping cannot be easily anticipated |
Complex Naturalistic (for example, walking, driving or semi-structured conversation) Discrete or continuous Self-paced |
4 | Real-world (for example, with stop cues that are entirely natural, resulting from changes in environment around the participant) | Unconstrained, real-world setting Non-repetitive, truly novel trials and events Stopping is entirely unpredictable |
Complex Naturalistic Discrete or continuous Self-paced |
Speech.
Effective verbal communication relies on fine motor control, including the ability to interrupt speech at any time. Speech presents an interesting case because whereas rIFC has been implicated in the stopping of manual and saccadic actions, the homologous left IFC (lIFC; also known as Broca’s area) has been strongly implicated in the programming of speech93,94. Speech production also requires timely control across facial, throat and respiratory muscles. Control can be directed to any or all of them to influence what sounds are made.
The fact that stopping latencies for speech (for example, when producing a syllable) and manual responses correlate with one another across participants indirectly points to a common neural mechanism95. Successful stopping of speech activates the rIFC and preSMA95, and is associated with similar PFC 96 and STN beta-band activity97 as stopping manual responses. Moreover, stopping speech produces a global suppression of the motor system83 that correlates with elevated STN beta-band activity64. Possible causal evidence for the involvement of the stopping network has been provided in the form of cessation of speech and manual actions during intraoperative stimulation of the preSMA and the rIFC and lIFC98-100. Although stimulation of IFC in either hemisphere can disrupt speech, it might do so for different reasons: depending on the mode of stimulation and task requirements, rIFC stimulation could activate the stopping network (‘stop’), whereas lIFC stimulation could disrupt the programming of speech (‘go’).
Stopping of speech and stopping of manual actions seem to rely on similar neural substrates, although further work could be done to demonstrate that this holds true for production of words and sentences, not just of letters or syllables, and for natural communicative contexts.
Gait and balance.
Successful gait and balance depend on effective coordination between limb and trunk muscles. The stopping of gait is interesting because it does not entail a complete cessation of all leg or postural muscle activity, but instead often requires a corrective response, such as stepping to maintain balance or to avoid an obstacle. In other words, it often requires switching to another action (Box 1), rather than simply cancelling the current one, and therefore poses new challenges to the study of action-stopping (such as knowing when and whether a stop occurs during a seemingly continuous action).
Gait and postural control are achieved via the integration of descending outputs from multiple brain systems (including the cortex and the mesencephalic locomotor region) at the level of the spinal cord101. Cortico-basal ganglia circuits are thought to have an important role in gait control, particularly when challenging environments demand greater voluntary control101. An open question is whether, for example, stopping to avoid unexpected obstacles while walking relies on similar PFC-basal ganglia circuits as those recruited during manual stopping (perhaps with the inclusion of outputs from the STN and GPi/SNr directly to the mesencephalic locomotor region101).
The involvement of the basal ganglia in gait is well established. Recent animal experiments involving cell-specific and circuit-specific optogenetic stimulation and calcium imaging have confirmed that activity in the striatum102, SNr103, STN and GPe regulate locomotion104. In addition, individuals with Parkinson disease often experience problems with gait105 (see ‘Freezing of gait’), and recent local field potential recordings from the STN of such individuals have implicated this region in the regulation of stepping106. Future work could directly examine whether the STN is implicated in the stopping of gait.
Several studies now point to the recruitment of global stopping or brake-like mechanisms when gait or balance are disturbed. In one study, participants made voluntary steps to visually presented targets on the floor107. In 25% of trials, shortly after participants had initiated a step, a ‘jump’ of the target to a different location required them to step to the new target instead. The initial reaction to the target jump was a brief reduction in the body’s forward acceleration. This ‘braking’ effect occurred within about 250 ms after the target jump, similar to SSRT of manual responses, and regardless of whether doing so was helpful (that is, regardless of whether the target jump required a decrease or increase in step length). Similar nonspecific braking effects have been observed in leg muscle activity108, and would presumably be present in postural (trunk) muscles too, as these are thought to generate the initial momentum of the body when stepping107. Another study noted a global motor system suppression when participants attempted to prevent a corrective step after a postural perturbation109. The same group found that participants with a shorter manual SSRT showed a greater reduction of leg muscle activity when stopping a corrective step110, implying that action-stopping measured in simple laboratory tasks is a generalizable capability. Last, recent research in rodents using optogenetics and calcium imaging implicated a dmPFC–STN pathway as being important for visually directed stopping of locomotion111.
Together, these initial studies suggest potential for overlap in the neural systems for stopping manual actions and gait or postural actions.
Automatic bodily functions.
Breathing and blinking are typically ‘automatic’ actions that can temporarily be brought under voluntary control (for example, when holding breath under water). The suppression of blinking112,113, coughing114,115, breathing116 and the impulse to remove oneself from a noxious stimulus117, all activate nodes of the stopping network, including the preSMA, rIFC, striatum and pallidum. Notably, the preSMA and striatum are also more active during deliberately slow, paced breathing118, which could be interpreted as evidence for a ‘braking’ role of these regions in the control of breathing.
An open question with these studies is whether the brain activations reflect an actively suppressive mechanism or another process related to the discomfort, difficulty or attentional demands associated with the task. Some evidence corroborates the motor suppression account. First, the ‘urge’ to act seems to grow with continued suppression113,115, implicating an inhibitory mechanism in preventing expression of the action. Second, we recently showed that suppressive processes, in the form of GABAergic inhibition and beta-band oscillations, were present in sensorimotor cortex while suppressing an action that would relieve pain119. Third, despite a lack of causal evidence in humans, slowing and complete cessation of breathing have been found following stimulation of lateral PFC in primates120.
A notable absentee in the fMRI literature on control of automatic bodily functions is the STN, perhaps because experiment set-ups have not been well suited to monitor activity in such a small structure57. One might also ask why an STN-mediated system for reactive stopping would be recruited when there is no external signal calling for an immediate stop. This is pertinent because some have proposed a distinction between externally and internally triggered stopping117,121. However, in many cases, the distinction may be blurred, because individuals might use internal sensations, such as a tickly throat, as a proxy external signal to trigger stopping. Furthermore, neuroimaging indicates that both forms of stopping recruit a largely overlapping network of prefrontal structures117,121.
Overall, neuroimaging data seem to highlight a potential overlap in the control of manual actions and automatic bodily functions, but various caveats and a lack of causal evidence prompt further study.
Tonic control.
Stopping tasks focus on the control of brief ‘urges’ or ‘drives’ to act but, as we have just seen, some can persist and grow over time. These range from benign urges to scratch an itch, to pathological ones associated with motor tics or compulsive skin-picking122. How do we deal with protracted urges? Some urges wax and wane, so one possibility is that action-stopping is applied briefly and repeatedly to stop them whenever they cross some threshold. For constant urges, however, the stopping mechanism could be used tonically for periods of seconds or more. Indeed, the above-mentioned work on suppressing pain-relieving actions suggested that the motor inhibitory processes persisted for several seconds119.
A clear example of tonic inhibitory control over actions comes from a study of the Kohnstamm phenomenon123. The Kohnstamm involves brain regions implicated in voluntary control, including M1124,125, and although it feels involuntary126, it can be brought under voluntary control without recruiting antagonist muscles 124,126. This implies that a stop process is recruited to suppress ongoing motor output. The mechanisms of such control remain unexplored. However, attempts to stop one arm from rising following the induction of Kohnstamm movements in both arms sometimes results in a brief interruption of both limbs, suggesting that a global stopping mechanism (that is, with a somatotopically broad focus) akin to that in the stop signal task, is initially recruited126.
Studying the neurophysiological correlates of Kohnstamm suppression could reveal the mechanisms underlying tonic control of involuntary actions in health and disease (see ‘Tics’).
Unexpected events.
Unexpected events result from changes in our environment (such as a door slamming) or our own actions (for example, mispronouncing a word). They have a surprising quality that interrupts ongoing action and thought. A recent theory suggested that unexpected-event-induced interruptions recruit a similar network to that recruited by outright action-stopping9. For example, an earlier study showed that unexpected sounds elicited a global motor system suppression around 150 ms (similar to stop signals; Fig. 2) and slowed motor responding127.
Recent studies have built on this idea. One showed that unexpected sounds presented after a ‘no go’ signal not only improved stopping success, pointing to an overlapping process, but were also associated with enhanced activity of the action-stopping network, as indexed by EEG and TMS-induced EMG signals128. Meanwhile, unexpected events in a rodent study disrupted licking, just as optogenetic activation of the STN did53. Importantly, optogenetic inactivation of the STN blunted this effect of unexpected events, providing strong evidence for a causal role of the STN in surprise-induced interruptions.
So far, a picture emerges whereby motor interruptions prompted by unexpected events recruit an analogous system to that used in voluntarily stopping actions, albeit by pausing rather than stopping outright. This suggests that the system may be recruited automatically or voluntarily to partially or fully interrupt actions, although this idea awaits further verification. The emerging picture also indirectly speaks to a long-running debate about the specificity of rIFC activation19,35,129,130. Previous neuroimaging had shown that infrequent cues that do not require stopping still recruited rIFC, questioning whether rIFC activation in stopping tasks reflected a stop-process131. In light of the unexpected-events literature, it is plausible that those infrequent cues recruited motor inhibitory processes triggered by an rIFC–STN pathway, which would explain the slowing of responses.
From laboratory to real world
Despite being preliminary in most cases, the evidence above suggests that the scope of the action-stopping model could extend beyond the standard task. Yet those examples still rely on contrived experimental paradigms where the same stopping demands are encountered repeatedly. Examining real-world stopping has recently been made possible by technological advances in wearable MEG132 and near-infrared spectroscopy133. In addition, new devices being used in individuals being monitored or treated for various disorders can provide stable, long-term intracranial neural recordings and controlled brain stimulation, and have been integrated with wireless scalp EEG and gaze or motion tracking for concurrent neurophysiological and behavioural evaluation during free movement134.
A challenge as we move towards real-world scenarios is knowing in any given moment whether behavioural stopping has occurred. It might be tempting to rely on indirect markers, such as right prefrontal beta-band activity. However, this runs the risk of reverse inference135: that is, assuming that similar neural correlates observed in one context (such as walking) reflect the same mental processes identified in another (such as the stop signal task). One way to minimize this risk would be to adopt a programmatic approach (Fig. 3). For example, following on from the above-mentioned gait and stepping studies (level 2 in Table 1 and Fig. 3), one could progress to evaluating stopping and its neural correlates during unconstrained gait in a virtual reality setting, where naturalistic events in the visual scene might cause someone to stop (level 3 in Fig. 3). The question then is whether those moments rely on the same neural substrates as stopping in the simpler laboratory tasks. If so, event-related brain stimulation could be used to disrupt prefrontal or basal ganglia processing and provide causal evidence for their involvement, for example by delivering it around the time of a door closing in front of you. This virtual world investigation would then motivate fully real-world studies of gait as people walk around their neighbourhoods (level 4 in Fig. 3).
Fig. 3 ∣. From the laboratory to the real-world: gait.
a ∣ A research programme might begin by studying stopping in the laboratory with the stop signal task (level 1 in Table 1), using the primary effectors involved in the high-level behaviour of interes — in this case, the legs. Neurophysiological techniques can be used to look for the neural correlates of stopping, such as beta-band activity in the right prefrontal cortex measured usingelectroencephalography (EEG) and global motor system suppression measured using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and electromyography (EMG; here, on the task-unrelated hand muscle), b ∣ One could then progress to more complex and naturalistic actions, but still within a highly constrained laboratory setting (level 2 in Table 1). In this example, the participant is instructed to perform simple stepping actions. On some trials, an obstacle suddenly appears, requiring the participant to prevent the step. Here, EEG and TMS could be used alongside EMG, force plates and motion-capture imaging that provide behavioural readouts of action-stopping, c ∣ The next step might involve free-moving and naturalistic gait while individuals explore virtual environments. Wearable EEG, in this case an intracranial device implanted in a patient for recording and stimulating the brain, could be combined with motion tracking134. (level 3 in Table 1). Numerous situations could conceivably involve action-stopping: an open manhole, another person crossing one’s path or a door closing. Sudden body or limb decelerations time-locked to the events in the visual scene would provide indices of behavioural stopping, such as the latency of stopping, and would provide a time window in which to look for neural correlates of stopping. d ∣ Finally, one might approach real-world studies, as people explore their normal environments. Here, wearable EEG in patients could be combined with gaze-tracking134 and smartphone-based accelerometry176 to examine similar instances in which the stopping network might be recruited (level 4 in Table 1).
A similar approach could be used to study driving, beginning with simple manual stop signal tasks (level 1) and progressing to reaching-and-grasping tasks (level 2). One could then envisage individuals with wearable MEG, scalp EEG or patients with intracranial EEG, sitting in driving simulators with gaze-tracking and measurements of steering, acceleration and braking enabling the quantification of behaviour (level 3). Here, seeing a car in your blind spot as you begin changing lanes might cause you to interrupt the planned turning of the steering wheel. Similar events could then be studied in real cars using wearable EEG combined with gaze and motion tracking (level 4).
Even with this programmatic approach, there remains an important issue that may affect the extension of the stopping literature to the real world. Although the standard stop signal task purports to measure reactive stopping, it still involves some anticipatory slowing of responses on trials where no stop signal is presented — response times are longer than in a choice reaction time task136 and increase with the likelihood of needing to stop137. Yet, how common is anticipatory slowing in the real world? When driving, you might anticipate having to reduce pressure on the accelerator as you approach traffic lights; but, the need to stop or brake cannot always be predicted. Although unexpected-event paradigms have been developed as a response to this criticism, the events are still encountered many times in a study. An alternative might be to study one-off events138, but these are not amenable to studying the neural correlates of behaviour, which are unreliable at the single-trial level. It may also be difficult to dissociate direct sensory effects on behavioural interruption (for example, see ref. 139), akin to a ‘startle’, from recruitment of the stopping network. A challenge, therefore, is to understand whether, and to what extent, the mechanisms of stopping differ when the need to do so is anticipatable versus completely unexpected.
Movement disorders
We now turn to whether variation in the functioning of the action-stopping network underlies clinical problems. Dysfunctional PFC-basal ganglia networks have been proposed to contribute to the symptoms experienced by patients with movement and psychiatric disorders11,12,122. Although these issues have been reviewed extensively11,12,122 , the focus has either been on psychiatric symptoms (such as impulsivity) where the theoretical and empirical link between real-world behaviour and action-stopping is often poorly defined or weak140, or on various motor symptoms that do not all fit within the action-stopping framework. Here, we focus specifically on movement disorder symptoms that have a more direct conceptual link with action-stopping.
Stuttering.
Stuttering is characterized by periods of silence, sound prolongations and sound or syllable repetitions. Although there is no single cause thought to underlie stuttering, these symptoms might sometimes reflect an aberrant stopping mechanism. Such a mechanism could prematurely interrupt an ongoing motor programme or inhibit successive motor programmes that relate to the next syllable141.
The fact that stopping of manual responses is impaired in people who stutter142-144 suggests there may be a dysfunction in the action-stopping network at a higher level in the brain than the motor output. Recent meta-analysis of fMRI studies has confirmed an earlier suggestion145 of a right-lateralized hyperactivity, including in the right preSMA and rIFC, during speech in people who stutter compared with those who do not, and in dysfluent versus fluent speech146. Stuttering severity was also recently shown to correlate with structural connectivity in the right frontal aslant tract between the hyperactive preSMA–SMA and the rIFC141. Although this right-hemisphere hyperactivity could be explained in several ways — for example, as a compensatory adaptation to the left hemisphere-induced dysfluency147,148 — it could indeed reflect an aberrant action-stopping system. Future studies combining detailed psychophysical evaluations of dysfluent speech (for example, characterizing the precise timing of speech interruptions) with examination of the neural correlates of stopping using high-temporal-resolution methods could clarify the putative role of the stopping network in stuttering. One prediction is that periods of stuttering are accompanied by a global motor system suppression and elevated beta-band power in the right PFC. Another prediction is that disruption of the hyperactive rIFC or preSMA with TMS during dysfluent speech might temporarily restore fluent speech.
Tics.
Tics are sudden, involuntary movements or vocalizations that are contextually inappropriate. Tic generation is thought to be due to activation of a dysfunctional cortico-striatal pathway149,150. This results in the release of stereotyped movements that would normally be prevented by tonic inhibitory output from the GPi/SNr. Tics therefore seem more a problem with action-initiation than action-stopping, per se. Accordingly, a meta-analysis showed that the stopping of voluntary actions in people with tics, at least those without comorbid disorders such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, is comparable to that in people without tics151.
Although tics may not arise from a problem with action-stopping, stopping may be relevant for how tics are controlled. Indeed, a defining characteristic of tics is that they can often be temporarily suppressed by volition152. Moreover, new research shows that the ability to suppress tics early in childhood is negatively correlated with tic severity in the future153. Understanding tic suppression is therefore clinically relevant154.
The notion of ‘suppressing’ a tic might only be a metaphor; however, it could also actually reflect an action-stopping mechanism that prevents aberrant motor drive155. This fits with the observation that suppressing a tic does not usually abolish the urge to tic156. Evaluating whether tic suppression does actually recruit an action-stopping system requires neuroscience studies. Although imaging has implicated some regions of PFC (including the rIFC, lIFC, preSMA and dorsolateral PFC), along with the striatum and pallidum, in tic suppression112,157,158, activation of these regions is not specific enough to a particular process to be sure. Causal brain stimulation studies may therefore be useful here.
A notable TMS study showed global motor system suppression during bouts of sustained, voluntary tic suppression compared with when participants were allowed to tic freely155. This result is compatible with action-stopping, although it has also been shown that, when patients voluntarily suppress tics, they can perform simple motor tasks with other effectors159. Overall, there is a hint that action-stopping is relevant in tic control, but it is not definitive, and there are open questions about how global or selective the action-stopping mechanism used to control them might be.
Freezing of gait.
Parkinson disease is characterized by various motor symptoms including absent or slowed movements, tremor, rigidity and gait disturbances. Although many of these can potentially be broadly attributed to excitation–inhibition imbalance in basal ganglia circuits122, the mechanisms underlying the motor symptoms are likely to be manifold. One instance where the stopping network may be applicable is in the freezing of gait — a temporary and involuntary slowing or stopping105. The triggers include turning, approaching narrow passages, distraction and cognitive load or stress160. A recent theory proposes that all these triggers engender a form of conflict (sensory-motor, affective or cognitive)161. Normally, conflict, such as that between response options, is proposed to trigger hyperdirect inputs from the PFC to the STN to ‘apply the brakes’ and aid in conflict resolution. However, in Parkinson disease, dysfunction of this pathway is proposed to lead to freezing.
Preliminary evidence supports this idea. An EEG study with participants with Parkinson disease found that, compared with normal walking, freezing episodes were associated with elevated theta-band activity over fronto-central electrode162, a common signature of response conflict37. Another showed that (fronto-central) cortico-STN coupling in the theta or alpha band was reduced during freezing163, consistent with dysfunction of the hyperdirect pathway. An innovative fMRI study using a virtual reality stepping paradigm, with visual cues designed to induce freezing, provided complementary evidence: freezing-like behaviour, in the form of prolonged stepping latencies when approaching doorways, was associated with impaired preSMA–STN connectivity164.
A recent review of many neurophysiological studies suggested that there are dissociable PFC-basal ganglia networks for processing conflict (that is, serving to pause action by raising the threshold of evidence needed for a decision) versus for action-stopping, and that these are characterized by low-frequency oscillations and beta-band activity, respectively37. The above studies on freezing of gait pointed to low-frequency oscillations, consistent with freezing of gait being attributable to disruptions in conflict processing. However, a recent study found increases in both STN low-frequency and beta-band oscillatory power during freezing behaviour165. Thus, it seems possible that the conflict system initially slows gait and the stopping system suspends it entirely during freezing episodes.
Experimental paradigms that use virtual reality to prompt different responses could be used to test these ideas and to test whether freezing is associated with other neurophysiological correlates of stopping network activity, such as the global motor system suppression. 166
Conclusions
Research in different species over the past few years supports key claims of the originally described action-stopping model, and goes further. The rIFC (especially the ventral pars opercularis) is crucial for stopping, and is functionally and structurally connected to the ventral STN, apparently by a hyperdirect pathway; activity of rIFC precedes that in the STN, and the putative signature of STN function is global motor system suppression that precedes the cancellation of muscle activity, which in turn precedes the SSRT. Although several open questions about this model remain (Box 2), we feel the neural basis is well-enough established to turn now to the important question of ecological validity.
Box 2 ∣. Neural bases of action-stopping: open questions and controversies.
A body of literature supports the idea of a prefrontal cortex-basal ganglia-thalamocortical network being crucial for action-stopping. Yet this support is not without challenge and there remain a number of open questions about the precise anatomy, physiology and function of the network.
Is action-stopping a unitary process enacted solely via the hyperdirect pathway, or does it entail a hyperdirect pathway-mediated ‘pause’ followed by a striatally mediated ‘stop’?
Are the global effects of reactive stopping on the motor system attributable to the subthalamic nucleus (STN), and does this reflect an anatomical system with truly broad STN-to-pallidum connectivity?
Recent computational modelling suggests that a failure to stop can occur because the process was triggered too late or else because it was not triggered at all (‘trigger failures’)14. This implies that behavioural stopping can be dissociated into different processes — the trigger for control, and the actual brake — and is relevant to the ongoing debate about different functions of different prefrontal brain regions during stopping19,35,129,130. Does this distinction map onto right inferior frontal junction for attentional capture and triggering of the control process, and the pars opercularis for the control?
Is there a mechanistic link between beta-band oscillations in the stopping network and action-stopping, or are these oscillations epiphenomenal? Although evidence is accumulating to support a possible role for beta-band oscillations in action-stopping15,31,50,64,119, some work has questioned its functional relevance36,51.
Does prefrontal beta-band activity locate predominantly to the right inferior frontal cortex or pre-supplementary motor area? Electrocorticography has implicated both regions27. However, most EEG and MEG studies have emphasized the importance of right prefrontal cortex15,31,32,34, with only few neurophysiological studies focusing on medial prefrontal cortex50,51.
How does right prefrontal beta-band activity stand-up against other neurophysiological correlates of stopping? The P300 event-related potential detected over frontocentral electrodes has been proposed as a sensitive marker of the stop process185,186. However, questions have been raised about whether its onset latency (more than 225–250 ms) is too long for it to contribute to stopping15 and whether it is specific enough to stopping15,187. This contrasts with right prefrontal beta-band activity, which increases specifically on stop trials and occurs before the cancellation of muscle activity15,31,32.
Here, we have considered behaviours at increasing levels of real-worldness. First, we summarized laboratory research on speech, gait, tonic control, automatic bodily functions and unexpected events. In each case, there are varying levels of evidence showing that similar brain regions are activated, or neurophysiological signatures elicited. However, given the problem of reverse inference, future work should seek causal evidence that these regions are important for stopping in each context. Second, we considered possible studies, beyond the laboratory, that could take advantage of technical advances in the recording and perturbation of neural activity in freely moving participants. These studies would put the generalizability of the action-stopping network to a serious test: asking if the recruitment of a PFC–STN hyperdirect pathway really does occur outside of repetitive trials of simple, discrete and prescribed actions.
We highlighted specific instances where the stopping network could be involved in the generation of, or compensation for, motor symptoms of movement disorders. Current evidence is strongest for the freezing of gait in Parkinson disease, in which neuroimaging indicates the importance of preSMA–STN function, although there remains a good case for further exploring the role of the stopping network in stuttering and tic control (Box 3). Although we have not explored the role of the action-stopping network in psychiatric disorders in this article, some recent results point to the engagement of a stopping system in preventing unwanted thought intrusions167,168. This suggests a common process for stopping action and thought, consistent with a long-standing model3,169. The potential extension of the action-stopping network to blocking long-term memory retrieval169, current working memory91 and, more generally, in thought control, is intriguing and would benefit from more study. It has high relevance for understanding intrusive thinking170, particularly in clinical disorders.
Box 3 ∣. Questions about the wider applicability of the action-stopping network.
The putative action-stopping network was developed on the basis of neuroscience studies performed in the laboratory using simple, often manual, stopping tasks and is assumed to generalise broadly to other actions and behavioural contexts. The questions here may guide research aimed at testing this assumption.
Is the reactive stopping network recruited to stop naturalistic actions such as walking or speaking, and can we demonstrate this in real-world settings (such as when having a conversation)?
Does the behaviourally selective stopping of complex, multi-component actions (such as stopping throwing a ball while continuing to run) involve a selective or global mechanism? How does one toggle the switch between selective and global stopping?
How is the stop process of dynamic, inertial movements coordinated across agonist and antagonist muscles about a joint to allow concomitant suppression or activation that presumably acts to brake limb motion188?
Is the stopping network recruited to exert tonic control over persistent movement ‘urges’ or ‘drives’, or is it recruited phasically and repeatedly whenever such drives or urges reach some critical threshold?
Does the voluntary suppression of motor tics rely on the suppression of motor output via the action-stopping network?
Does stuttering reflect the inappropriate recruitment of the stopping network that interrupts speech, or a compensatory response to aid in the regulation of already dysfluent speech?
Does freezing of gait reflect the inappropriate recruitment of the stopping network triggered by internally or externally triggered conflict?
We focused on the rapid action-stopping network. There are, however, other modes of inhibitory control that may engage slightly different pathways (Box 1). Fully discussing those other modes with regard to naturalistic behaviour is beyond the scope of this Review; our aim was to focus on the simple action-stopping mode as the best-developed case. We see hints that this action-stopping network is engaged in myriad other contexts, however.
A final point about ecological validity concerns the relation between SSRT and other behavioural metrics. SSRT, like many measures of executive function140, relates only weakly to self-reported measures of real-world self-control140. How can we reconcile this with the proposal that the rapid action-stopping system might underpin many moments of everyday life? We suggest that the speed of stopping may not be as important as whether and when stopping is triggered. Indeed, recent work using a new mathematical model of behaviour during the stop signal task estimated the frequency of failures to trigger the stop process, and found this to be better correlated with measures of behavioural impulsivity (such as delay aversion) than SSRT171. However, when real-world stopping demands speed (for example, for fast corrections of gait or balance), we would expect to see a closer relationship between behavioural stopping latency or success and SSRT110.
The simple stop signal paradigm was launched by Logan and Cowan in 19845 on the basis that it was a model for the stopping of action and thought. Nearly 40 years later, with considerable information about the brain regions, circuits and neurophysiological signatures of action-stopping, the time has come to assess whether the action-stopping model is mostly an artifact of the laboratory setting or a core function of everyday life.
Acknowledgements
The authors gratefully acknowledge funding support from the US National Institutes of Health (NS106822 and DA026452) and comments on the manuscript from J. Rothwell.
Glossary definitions
- Offline transcranial magnetic stimulation (Offline TMS).
Non-invasive brain stimulation eliciting long-lasting after-effects. Typically delivered before a participant undergoes behavioural testing or neuroimaging.
- Electrocorticography (ECoG).
Invasive electrophysiological technique that uses electrodes placed directly on the exposed surface of the brain to record electrical activity from the cerebral cortex.
- Hyperdirect pathway
Pathway connecting cortical areas directly to the subthalamic nucleus, bypassing the striatum.
- Electromyography (EMG).
Electrophysiological technique that uses electrodes placed on the skin over a muscle to record its electrical activity.
- Online TMS
Non-invasive brain stimulation eliciting short-lived after-effects. Typically delivered whilst a participant undergoes behavioural testing or neuroimaging.
- Conflict resolution
In the motor domain, this refers to the process of resolving competition between competing action plans.
- Go/no-go task
A paradigm where the participant is required to perform speeded responses to a Go cue and to withhold a response following a No-Go cue.
- Global motor system suppression
Suppression of motor system excitability detected in task-irrelevant muscle representations when stopping with another effector. Relies on TMS and EMG methods.
- Pre-manifest Huntington disease
Refers to the pre-symptomatic phase of the disease in an individual carrying the genetic mutation causing it.
- Agonist muscle
The muscle(s) which, when activated, is primarily responsible for causing movement about a joint.
- Gait
Refers to the normal pattern of limb movements underpinning locomotion.
- Kohnstamm phenomenon
A long-lasting (10–60 s) involuntary muscle contraction that develops after a sustained, voluntary isometric contraction: after pushing your arm against a wall for a long period, you experience your arm rising.
- Antagonist muscle
The muscle(s) which, when activated, oppose the movement caused by the agonist muscle(s) about a joint.
Footnotes
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
References
- 1.Diamond A Executive Functions. Annual Review of Psychology 64, 135–168 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Miyake A et al. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cognitive Psychology (2000) doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Guo Y, Schmitz TW, Mur M, Ferreira CS & Anderson MC A supramodal role of the basal ganglia in memory and motor inhibition: Meta-analytic evidence. Neuropsychologia 108, 117–134 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Verbruggen F et al. A consensus guide to capturing the ability to inhibit actions and impulsive behaviors in the stop-signal task. eLife 8, (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Logan GD & Cowan WB On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control. Psychological Review 91, 295–327 (1984). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Sebastian A et al. Disentangling common and specific neural subprocesses of response inhibition. Neuroimage (2013) doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Sebastian A et al. Differential effects of age on subcomponents of response inhibition. Neurobiology of Aging (2013) doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Raud L, Westerhausen R, Dooley N & Huster RJ Differences in unity: The go/no-go and stop signal tasks rely on different mechanisms. NeuroImage 210, 116582 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Wessel JR & Aron AR On the Globality of Motor Suppression: Unexpected Events and Their Influence on Behavior and Cognition. Neuron vol. 93 259–280 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Aron AR From Reactive to Proactive and Selective Control: Developing a Richer Model for Stopping Inappropriate Responses. Biological Psychiatry 69, e55–e68 (2011). An earlier influential review of the literature on different modes of action control and their putative neural bases.
- 11.Chambers CD, Garavan H & Bellgrove MA Insights into the neural basis of response inhibition from cognitive and clinical neuroscience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews vol. 33 631–646 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Bari A & Robbins TW Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control. Progress in Neurobiology 108, 44–79 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bissett PG, Jones HM, Poldrack RA & Logan GD Severe violations of independence in response inhibition tasks. Science Advances 7, eabf4355 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Matzke D, Love J & Heathcote A A Bayesian approach for estimating the probability of trigger failures in the stop-signal paradigm. Behavior Research Methods 49, 267–281 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Jana S, Hannah R, Muralidharan V & Aron AR Temporal cascade of frontal, motor and muscle processes underlying human action-stopping. eLife 9, (2020). A multimodal neurophysiological study motivating the temporal ‘flow’ model of processing throughout the stopping network in humans.
- 16.Smittenaar P, Guitart-Masip M, Lutti A & Dolan RJ Preparing for selective inhibition within frontostriatal loops. Journal of Neuroscience 33, 18087–18097 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.van den Wildenberg WPM, Burle B, Vidal F, van der Molen MW & Ridderinkhof KR Mechanisms and Dynamics of Cortical Motor Inhibition in the Stop-signal Paradigm : A TMS Study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 225–239 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Aron AR, Behrens TE, Smith S, Frank MJ & Poldrack RA Triangulating a Cognitive Control Network Using Diffusion-Weighted Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) and Functional MRI. Journal of Neuroscience 27, 3743–3752 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Aron AR, Robbins TW & Poldrack RA Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: One decade on. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 18, 177–185 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Aron AR & Poldrack RA Cortical and subcortical contributions to Stop signal response inhibition: role of the subthalamic nucleus. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 26, 2424–33 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Konishi S, Nakajima K, Uchida I, Sekihara K & Miyashita Y No-go dominant brain activity in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. European Journal of Neuroscience (1998) doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00167.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Konishi S et al. Common inhibitory mechanism in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by event-related functional MRI. Brain (1999) doi: 10.1093/brain/122.5.981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Garavan H, Ross TJ & Stein EA Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: An event-related functional MRI study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 96, 8301–8306 (1999). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Aron AR, Fletcher PC, Bullmore ET, Sahakian BJ & Robbins TW Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nature Neuroscience 6, 115–116 (2003). A classic study of patients with brain lesions showing that rIFC is crucial for action-stopping.
- 25.Chambers CD et al. Executive “brake failure” following deactivation of human frontal lobe. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18, 444–455 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Verbruggen F, Aron AR, Stevens MA & Chambers CD Theta burst stimulation dissociates attention and action updating in human inferior frontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107, 13966–13971 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Swann NC et al. Roles for the pre-supplementary motor area and the right inferior frontal gyrus in stopping action: Electrophysiological responses and functional and structural connectivity. Neuroimage 59, 2860–2870 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Madsen KS et al. Response inhibition is associated with white matter microstructure in children. Neuropsychologia 48, 854–862 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Bartoli E, Aron AR & Tandon N Topography and timing of activity in right inferior frontal cortex and anterior insula for stopping movement. Human Brain Mapping 39, 189–203 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Suda A et al. Functional Organization for Response Inhibition in the Right Inferior Frontal Cortex of Individual Fluman Brains. Cerebral cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991) 30, 6325–6335 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hannah R, Muralidharan V, Sundby KK & Aron AR Temporally-precise disruption of prefrontal cortex informed by the timing of beta bursts impairs human action-stopping. Neuroimage 117222 (2020) doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Sundby KK, Jana S & Aron AR Double blind disruption of right inferior frontal cortex with TMS reduces right frontal beta power for action-stopping. Journal of Neurophysiology jn.00459.2020 (2020) doi: 10.1152/jn.00459.2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Swann N et al. Intracranial EEG reveals a time- and frequency-specific role for the right inferior frontal gyrus and primary motor cortex in stopping initiated responses. Journal of Neuroscience 29, 12675–12685 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Schaum M et al. Right inferior frontal gyrus implements motor inhibitory control via beta-band oscillations in humans. eLife 10, 1–28 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Swick D & Chatham CH Ten years of inhibition revisited. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8, 115–116 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Chen W et al. Prefrontal-Subthalamic Hyperdirect Pathway Modulates Movement Inhibition in Humans. Neuron 106, 579–588.e3 (2020). A study in individuals with Parkinson disease supporting the existence of a functional connection between the rIFC and STN that is relevant to stopping.
- 37.Aron AR, Herz DM, Brown P, Forstmann BU & Zaghloul K Frontosubthalamic Circuits for Control of Action and Cognition. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 36, 11489–11495 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Rae CL, Hughes LE, Weaver C, Anderson MC & Rowe JB Selection and stopping in voluntary action: A meta-analysis and combined fMRI study. Neuroimage 86, 381–391 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Watanabe T et al. Effects of rTMS of pre-supplementary motor area on fronto basal ganglia network activity during stop-signal task. Journal of Neuroscience 35, 4813–4823 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Floden D & Stuss DT Inhibitory control is slowed in patients with right superior medial frontal damage. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18, 1843–1849 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Roberts RE & Husain M A dissociation between stopping and switching actions following a lesion of the pre-supplementary motor area. Cortex 63, 184–195 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Kohl S et al. Cortical Paired Associative Stimulation Influences Response Inhibition: Cortico-cortical and Cortico-subcortical Networks. Biological Psychiatry 85, 355–363 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Cai W, George JS, Verbruggen F, Chambers CD & Aron AR The role of the right presupplementary motor area in stopping action: Two studies with event-related transcranial magnetic stimulation. Journal of Neurophysiology 108, 380–389 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Nachev P, Kennard C & Husain M Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas. Nature Reviews Neuroscience vol. 9 856–869 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Thiebaut de Schotten M, Dell’Acqua F, Valabregue R & Catani M Monkey to human comparative anatomy of the frontal lobe association tracts. Cortex 48, 82–96 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Rae CL, Hughes LE, Anderson MC & Rowe JB The prefrontal cortex achieves inhibitory control by facilitating subcortical motor pathway connectivity. Journal of Neuroscience 35, 786–794 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Inase M, Tokuno H, Nambu A, Akazawa T & Takada M Corticostriatal and corticosubthalamic input zones from the presupplementary motor area in the macaque monkey: Comparison with the input zones from the supplementary motor area. Brain Research 833, 191–201 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Isoda M & Hikosaka O Role for subthalamic nucleus neurons in switching from automatic to controlled eye movement. Journal of Neuroscience 28, 7209–7218 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Allen C, Singh KD, Verbruggen F & Chambers CD Evidence for parallel activation of the pre-supplementary motor area and inferior frontal cortex during response inhibition: A combined MEG and TMS study. Royal Society Open Science 5, (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Wessel JR β-Bursts Reveal the Trial-to-Trial Dynamics of Movement Initiation and Cancellation. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 40, 411–423 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Errington SP, Woodman GF & Schall JD Dissociation of Medial Frontal β-Bursts and Executive Control. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 40, 9272–9282 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Li B, Nguyen TP, Ma C & Dan Y Inhibition of impulsive action by projection-defined prefrontal pyramidal neurons. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 117, 202000523 (2020). A rodent optogenetic study confirming the importance of a dmPFC-STN pathway for action control.
- 53.Fife KH et al. Causal role for the subthalamic nucleus in interrupting behavior. eLife 6, (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Cavanagh JF et al. Subthalamic nucleus stimulation reverses mediofrontal influence over decision threshold. Nature Neuroscience 14, 1462–1467 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Herz DM et al. Mechanisms Underlying Decision-Making as Revealed by Deep-Brain Stimulation in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease. Current Biology 28, 1169–1178.e6 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Wiecki T. v. & Frank MJ A computational model of inhibitory control in frontal cortex and basal ganglia. Psychological Review 120, 329–355 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.de Hollander G, Keuken MC, van der Zwaag W, Forstmann BU & Trampel R Comparing functional MRI protocols for small, iron-rich basal ganglia nuclei such as the subthalamic nucleus at 7 T and 3 T. Human Brain Mapping 38, 3226–3248 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Swann N et al. Deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus alters the cortical profile of response inhibition in the beta frequency band: A scalp EEG study in parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neuroscience 31, 5721–5729 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Ray NJ et al. The role of the subthalamic nucleus in response inhibition: Evidence from deep brain stimulation for Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 47, 2828–2834 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Mirabella G et al. Deep brain stimulation of subthalamic nuclei affects arm response inhibition in Parkinson’s patients. Cerebral Cortex 22, 1124–1132 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.van den Wildenberg WPM et al. Stimulation of the subthalamic region facilitates the selection and inhibition of motor responses in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18, 626–636 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Bastin J et al. Inhibitory control and error monitoring by human subthalamic neurons. Translational Psychiatry 4, e439–e439 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Benis D et al. Response inhibition rapidly increases single-neuron responses in the subthalamic nucleus of patients with Parkinson’s disease. Cortex 84, 111–123 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Wessel JR et al. Stop-related subthalamic beta activity indexes global motor suppression in Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders 31, 1846–1853 (2016). A study of individuals with Parkinson disease supporting the idea that the global motor system suppression is related to basal ganglia (STN) output.
- 65.Benis D et al. Subthalamic nucleus activity dissociates proactive and reactive inhibition in patients with Parkinson’s disease. NeuroImage 91, 273–281 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Badry R et al. Suppression of human cortico-motoneuronal excitability during the Stop-signal task. Clinical Neurophysiology 120, 1717–1723 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Hazrati LN & Parent A Convergence of subthalamic and striatal efferents at pallidal level in primates: an anterograde double-labeling study with biocytin and PHA-L. Brain Research 569, 336–340 (1992). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Miyachi S et al. Somatotopically arranged inputs from putamen and subthalamic nucleus to primary motor cortex. Neuroscience Research 56, 300–308 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Kelly RM & Strick PL Macro-architecture of basal ganglia loops with the cerebral cortex: Use of rabies virus to reveal multisynaptic circuits. Progress in Brain Research vol. 143 447–459 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Pasquereau B & Turner RS A selective role for ventromedial subthalamic nucleus in inhibitory control. eLife 6, (2017). A non-human primate study supporting the idea that the ventral portion of STN is specifically involved in action-stopping, consistent with this region receiving inputs from lateral PFC.
- 71.Ye Z et al. Predicting beneficial effects of atomoxetine and citalopram on response inhibition in Parkinson’s disease with clinical and neuroimaging measures. Human Brain Mapping 37, 1026–1037 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Zandbelt BB, Bloemendaal M, Neggers SFW, Kahn RS & Vink M Expectations and violations: Delineating the neural network of proactive inhibitory control. Human Brain Mapping 34, 2015–2024 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Zandbelt BB, Bloemendaal M, Hoogendam JM, Kahn RS & Vink M Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation and Functional MRI Reveal Cortical and Subcortical Interactions during Stop-signal Response Inhibition. (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Majid DSA, Cai W, Corey-Bloom J & Aron AR Proactive selective response suppression is implemented via the basal ganglia. Journal of Neuroscience 33, 13259–13269 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Rao JA et al. Disruption of response inhibition circuits in prodromal Huntington disease. Cortex 58, 72–85 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Jahfari S, Stinear CM, Claffey M, Verbruggen F & Aron AR Responding with restraint: What are the neurocognitive mechanisms? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22, 1479–1492 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Terra H et al. Prefrontal Cortical Projection Neurons Targeting Dorsomedial Striatum Control Behavioral Inhibition. Current Biology 30, 4188–4200.e5 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Gu B-M, Schmidt R & Berke JD Globus pallidus dynamics reveal covert strategies for behavioral inhibition. eLife 9, 1–19 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Klaus A et al. The Spatiotemporal Organization of the Striatum Encodes Action Space. Neuron 95, 1171–1180.e7 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Jin X, Tecuapetla F & Costa RM Basal ganglia subcircuits distinctively encode the parsing and concatenation of action sequences. Nature Neuroscience 17, 423–430 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Mink JW The basal ganglia: Focused selection and inhibition of competing motor programs. Progress in Neurobiology 50, 381–425 (1996). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Coxon JP, Stinear CM & Byblow WD Intracortical Inhibition During Volitional Inhibition of Prepared Action. J Neurophysiol 95, 3371–3383 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Cai W, Oldenkamp CL & Aron AR Stopping speech suppresses the task-irrelevant hand. Brain and Language 120, 412–415 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Pani P et al. Visual salience of the stop signal affects the neuronal dynamics of controlled inhibition. Scientific Reports 8, 1–13 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Jerjian SJ, Sahani M & Kraskov A Movement initiation and grasp representation in premotor and primary motor cortex mirror neurons. eLife 9, 1–26 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Raud L & Huster RJ The Temporal Dynamics of Response Inhibition and their Modulation by Cognitive Control. Brain Topography 30, 486–501 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Schmidt R & Berke JD A Pause-then-Cancel model of stopping: evidence from basal ganglia neurophysiology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences (2017) doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Schmidt R, Leventhal DK, Mallet N, Chen F & Berke JD Canceling actions involves a race between basal ganglia pathways. Nature Neuroscience 16, 1118–1124 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Mallet N et al. Arkypallidal Cells Send a Stop Signal to Striatum. Neuron 89, 308–316 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Frank MJ, Samanta J, Moustafa AA & Sherman SJ Hold your horses: Impulsivity, deep brain stimulation, and medication in Parkinsonism. Science 318, 1309–1312 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Heston J et al. Activation of subthalamic nucleus stop circuit disrupts cognitive performance. eNeuro (2020) doi: 10.1523/ENEUR0.0159-20.2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Wessel JR et al. Surprise disrupts cognition via a fronto-basal ganglia suppressive mechanism. Nature Communications 7, 1–10 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Ardila A, Bernal B & Rosselli M How Localized are Language Brain Areas? A Review of Brodmann Areas Involvement in Oral Language. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology vol. 31 112–122 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Stewart L, Walsh V, Frith U & Rothwell JC TMS Produces Two Dissociable Types of Speech Disruption. Neuroimage 13, 472–478 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Xue G, Aron AR & Poldrack RA Common neural substrates for inhibition of spoken and manual responses. Cerebral Cortex 18, 1923–1932 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Wagner J, Wessel JR, Ghahremani A & Aron AR Establishing a Right Frontal Beta Signature for Stopping Action in Scalp EEG: Implications for Testing Inhibitory Control in Other Task Contexts. Journal of cognitive neuroscience 30, 107–118 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Ghahremani A et al. Stopping and slowing manual and spoken responses: Similar oscillatory signatures recorded from the subthalamic nucleus. Brain and Language 176, 1–10 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Kinoshita M et al. Role of fronto-striatal tract and frontal aslant tract in movement and speech: an axonal mapping study. Brain Structure and Function 220, 3399–3412 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Lüders H et al. Localization of Cortical Function: New Information from Extraoperative Monitoring of Patients with Epilepsy. Epilepsia 29, S56–S65 (1988). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Filevich E, Kühn S & Haggard P Negative motor phenomena in cortical stimulation: Implications for inhibitory control of human action. Cortex vol. 48 1251–1261 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Takakusaki K Functional Neuroanatomy for Posture and Gait Control. Journal of Movement Disorders 10, 1–17 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Gritton HJ et al. Unique contributions of parvalbumin and cholinergic interneurons in organizing striatal networks during movement. Nature Neuroscience 22, 586–597 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Rizzi G & Tan KR Synergistic Nigral Output Pathways Shape Movement. Cell Reports 27, 2184–2198.e4 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Pamukcu A et al. Parvalbumin + and Npas1 + Pallidal Neurons Have Distinct Circuit Topology and Function Parvalbumin + and Npas1 + Pallidal Neurons Have Distinct Circuit Topology and 3 Function 4 5. (2020) doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0361-20.2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Ebersbach G, Moreau C, Gandor F, Defebvre L & Devos D Clinical syndromes: Parkinsonian gait. Movement Disorders 28, 1552–1559 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Fischer P et al. Alternating modulation of subthalamic nucleus beta oscillations during stepping. Journal of Neuroscience 38, 5111–5121 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Bancroft MJ & Day BL The Throw-and-Catch Model of Human Gait: Evidence from Coupling of Pre-Step Postural Activity and Step Location. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10, (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Potocanac Z, Pijnappels M, Verschueren S, van Dieën J & Duysens J Two-stage muscle activity responses in decisions about leg movement adjustments during trip recovery. Journal of Neurophysiology 115, 143–156 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109. Goode C, Cole DM & Bolton DAE Staying upright by shutting down? Evidence for global suppression of the motor system when recovering balance. Gait and Posture 70, 260–263 (2019). An innovative study in humans attempting to study the neural basis of action stopping in a more naturalistic gait-and-balance context.
- 110.Rydalch G, Bell HB, Ruddy KL & Bolton DAE Stop-signal reaction time correlates with a compensatory balance response. Gait & Posture 71, 273–278 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Adam E, Johns T & Sur M Cortico-subthalamic projections send brief stop signals to halt visually-guided locomotion. (2020) doi: 10.1101/2020.02.05.936443. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 112.van der Salm SMA et al. Distinctive tics suppression network in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome distinguished from suppression of natural urges using multimodal imaging. NeuroImage: Clinical 20, 783–792 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Berman BD, Horovitz SG, Morel B & Hallett M Neural correlates of blink suppression and the buildup of a natural bodily urge. NeuroImage 59, 1441–1450 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Mazzone SB, Cole LJ, Ando A, Egan GF & Farrell MJ Investigation of the neural control of cough and cough suppression in humans using functional brain imaging. Journal of Neuroscience 31, 2948–2958 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Ando A et al. Neural correlates of cough hypersensitivity in humans: evidence for central sensitisation and dysfunctional inhibitory control. Thorax 71, 323–329 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.McKay LC, Adams L, Frackowiak RSJ & Corfield DR A bilateral cortico-bulbar network associated with breath holding in humans, determined by functional magnetic resonance imaging. NeuroImage 40, 1824–1832 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Lynn MT, Demanet J, Krebs RM, van Dessel P & Brass M Voluntary inhibition of pain avoidance behavior: an fMRI study. Brain Structure and Function 221, 1309–1320 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Critchley HD et al. Slow Breathing and Hypoxic Challenge: Cardiorespiratory Consequences and Their Central Neural Substrates. (2015) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0127082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Sundby KK, Wagner J & Aron AR The functional role of response suppression during an urge to relieve pain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 31, 1404–1421 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Smith WK the Representation of Respiratory Movements in the Cerebral Cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 1, 55–68 (1938). [Google Scholar]
- 121.Schel MA et al. Neural correlates of intentional and stimulus-driven inhibition: a comparison. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8, 27 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Jahanshahi M & Rothwell JC Inhibitory dysfunction contributes to some of the motor and non-motor symptoms of movement disorders and psychiatric disorders. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences vol. 372 20160198 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.de Havas J, Gomi H & Haggard P Experimental investigations of control principles of involuntary movement: a comprehensive review of the Kohnstamm phenomenon. Experimental Brain Research 235, 1953–1997 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Ghosh A, Rothwell J & Haggard P Using voluntary motor commands to inhibit involuntary arm movements. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 281, (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Parkinson A, McDonagh M & Vidyasagar R Brain activation in an involuntary human action. Brain Research 1304, 57–65 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.de Havas J, Ghosh A, Gomi H & Haggard P Voluntary motor commands reveal awareness and control of involuntary movement. Cognition 155, 155–167 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Wessel JR & Aron AR Unexpected events induce motor slowing via a brain mechanism for action-stopping with global suppressive effects. Journal of Neuroscience 33, 18481–18491 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Dutra IC, Waller DA & Wessel JR Perceptual surprise improves action stopping by nonselectively suppressing motor activity via a neural mechanism for motor inhibition. Journal of Neuroscience 38, 1482–1492 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Hampshire A & Sharp DJ Contrasting network and modular perspectives on inhibitory control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 19, 445–452 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Aron AR, Robbins TW & Poldrack RA Right inferior frontal cortex: addressing the rebuttals. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8, (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Sharp DJ et al. Distinct frontal systems for response inhibition, attentional capture, and error processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107, 6106–6111 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Boto E et al. Moving magnetoencephalography towards real-world applications with a wearable system. Nature 555, 657–661 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Lacerenza M et al. Wearable and wireless time-domain near-infrared spectroscopy system for brain and muscle hemodynamic monitoring. Biomedical Optics Express (2020) doi: 10.1364/boe.403327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Topalovic U et al. Wireless Programmable Recording and Stimulation of Deep Brain Activity in Freely Moving Humans. Neuron 108, 322–334.e9 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135. Poldrack RA Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10, 59–63 (2006). A demonstration of novel technology for wearable devices for recording and perturbing brain activity, and its integration with other behavioural recording devices.
- 136.Verbruggen F & Logan GD Proactive Adjustments of Response Strategies in the Stop-Signal Paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 35, 835–854 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Lee W-T & Kang M-S Electrophysiological Evidence for Distinct Proactive Control Mechanisms in a Stop-Signal Task: An Individual Differences Approach. Frontiers in Psychology 11, 1105 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Horstmann G The surprise-attention link: a review. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1339, 106–115 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Liang F et al. Sensory Cortical Control of a Visually Induced Arrest Behavior via Corticotectal Projections. Neuron 86, 755–767 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Eisenberg IW et al. Uncovering the structure of self-regulation through data-driven ontology discovery. Nature Communications 10, 1–13 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Neef NE et al. Structural connectivity of right frontal hyperactive areas scales with stuttering severity. Brain 141, 191–204 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142. Eggers K, de Nil LF & van den Bergh BRH Inhibitory control in childhood stuttering. Journal of Fluency Disorders 38, 1–13 (2013). A neuroimaging study highlighting the relationship between connectivity in right prefrontal regions of the stopping network and stuttering severity.
- 143.Markett S et al. Impaired motor inhibition in adults who stutter – evidence from speech-free stop-signal reaction time tasks. Neuropsychologia 91, 444–450 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Treleaven SB & Coalson GA Manual response inhibition and quality of life in adults who stutter. Journal of Communication Disorders 88, 106053 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Fox PT et al. A PET study of the neural systems of stuttering. Nature 382, 158–162 (1996). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Belyk M, Kraft SJ & Brown S Stuttering as a trait or state - an ALE meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. European Journal of Neuroscience 41, 275–284 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Kell CA et al. How the brain repairs stuttering. Brain 132, 2747–2760 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Preibisch C et al. Evidence for compensation for stuttering by the right frontal operculum. NeuroImage 20, 1356–1364 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Mink JW Basal ganglia dysfunction inTourette’s syndrome: A new hypothesis. Pediatric Neurology vol. 25 190–198 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Kataoka Y et al. Decreased number of parvalbumin and cholinergic interneurons in the striatum of individuals with tourette syndrome. Journal of Comparative Neurology 518, 277–291 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Morand-Beaulieu S et al. The puzzling question of inhibitory control in Tourette syndrome: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehaviora! Reviews vol. 80 240–262 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Koller WC & Biary NM Volitional control of involuntary movements. Movement Disorders 4, 153–156 (1989). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Kim S et al. Tic Suppression in Children With Recent-Onset Tics Predicts 1-Year Tic Outcome. Journal of Child Neurology 34, 757–764 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Fründt O, Woods D & Ganos C Behavioral therapy for Tourette syndrome and chronic tic disorders. Neurology: Clinical Practice vol. 7 148–156 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Ganos C et al. Motor cortical excitability during voluntary inhibition of involuntary tic movements. Movement Disorders 33, 1804–1809 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156. Brandt VC et al. Temporal relationship between premonitory urges and tics in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Cortex 77, 24–37 (2016). A study in individuals with tics, demonstrating that tonic voluntary suppression of tics is associated with a global motor system suppression akin to that seen when stopping voluntary actions.
- 157.Ganos C et al. The neural correlates of tic inhibition in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Neuropsychologia 65, 297–301 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Peterson BS et al. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of tic suppression in tourette syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry 55, 326–333 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Zapparoli L, Macerollo A, Joyce EM, Martino D & Kilner JM Voluntary tic suppression and the normalization of motor cortical beta power in Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome: an EEG study. European Journal of Neuroscience ejn. 14548 (2019) doi: 10.1111/ejn.14548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Ehgoetz Martens KA et al. The functional network signature of heterogeneity in freezing of gait. Brain 141, 1145–1160 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Lewis SJG & Shine JM The Next Step: A Common Neural Mechanism for Freezing of Gait. Neuroscientist vol. 22 72–82 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Shine JM et al. Abnormal patterns of theta frequency oscillations during the temporal evolution of freezing of gait in parkinson’s disease. Clinical Neurophysiology 125, 569–576 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163.Pozzi NG et al. Freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease reflects a sudden derangement of locomotor network dynamics. Brain 142, 2037–2050 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 164. Matar E et al. Identifying the neural correlates of doorway freezing in Parkinson’s disease. Human Brain Mapping 40, 2055–2064 (2019). A study in individuals with Parkinson disease demonstrating that connectivity in a putative PFC–STN hyperdirect pathway is involved in the freezing of gait.
- 165.Chen CC et al. Subthalamic nucleus oscillations correlate with vulnerability to freezing of gait in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiology of Disease 132, 104605 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Rubinstein TC, Giladi N & Hausdorff JM The power of cueing to circumvent dopamine deficits: A review of physical therapy treatment of gait disturbances in Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders vol. 17 1148–1160 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167.Castiglione A & Aron AR Unwanted Memory Intrusions Recruit Broad Motor Suppression. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1–10 (2020) doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Castiglione A, Wagner J, Anderson M & Aron AR Preventing a Thought from Coming to Mind Elicits Increased Right Frontal Beta Just as Stopping Action Does. Cerebral Cortex 29, 2160–2172 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169. Anderson MC & Green C Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control. Nature 410, 366–369 (2001). A neurophysiological study showing that stopping an unwanted thought intrusion from coming to mind is associated with a similar prefrontal beta-band signature as stopping an action.
- 170.Visser RM et al. Neuropsychological mechanisms of intrusive thinking. Intrusive thinking across neuropsychiatric disorders: from molecules to free will. vol. 30 (2020). [Google Scholar]
- 171.Skippen P et al. Reliability of triggering inhibitory process is a better predictor of impulsivity than SSRT. Acta Psychologica 192, 104–117 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 172.Klaus A, Alves da Silva J & Costa RM What, If, and When to Move: Basal Ganglia Circuits and Self-Paced Action Initiation. Annual Review of Neuroscience 42, 459–483 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 173.Nambu A, Tokuno H & Takada M Functional significance of the cortico-subthalamo-pallidal “hyperdirect” pathway. Neuroscience Research 43, 111–117 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 174.Osada T et al. An Essential Role of the Intraparietal Sulcus in Response Inhibition Predicted by Parcellation-Based Network. The Journal of Neuroscience 39, 2509–2521 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 175.Hannah R & Jana S Disentangling the role of posterior parietal cortex in response inhibition. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 39, 6814–6816 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 176.Silsupadol P, Teja K & Lugade V Reliability and validity of a smartphone-based assessment of gait parameters across walking speed and smartphone locations: Body, bag, belt, hand, and pocket. Gait and Posture 58, 516–522 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 177.Cai W, Oldenkamp CL & Aron AR A proactive mechanism for selective suppression of response tendencies. Journal of Neuroscience 31, 5965–5969 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 178.Majid DSA, Cai W, George JS, Verbruggen F & Aron AR Transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals dissociable mechanisms for global versus selective corticomotor suppression underlying the stopping of action. Cerebral Cortex 22, 363–371 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 179.Jahfari S et al. How preparation changes the need for top-down control of the basal ganglia when inhibiting premature actions. Journal of Neuroscience 32, 10870–10878 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 180.Aron AR & Verbruggen F Stop the presses: Dissociating a selective from a global mechanism for stopping: Research article. Psychological Science 19, 1146–1153 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 181.Mars RB, Piekema C, Coles MGH, Hulstijn W & Toni I On the programming and reprogramming of actions. Cerebral Cortex 17, 2972–2979 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 182.Boecker M et al. When response inhibition is followed by response reengagement: An event-related fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping 32, 94–106 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 183.Isoda M & Hikosaka O Switching from automatic to controlled action by monkey medial frontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience 10, 240–248 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 184.Neubert F-X, Mars RB, Buch ER, Olivier E & Rushworth MFS Cortical and subcortical interactions during action reprogramming and their related white matter pathways. 107, 13240–13245 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 185.Greenhouse I & Wessel JR EEG signatures associated with stopping are sensitive to preparation. Psychophysiology 50, 900–908 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 186.Wessel JR & Aron AR It’s not too late: The onset of the frontocentral P3 indexes successful response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Psychophysiology 52, 472–480 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 187.Huster RJ, Messel MS, Thunberg C & Raud L The P300 as marker of inhibitory control – Fact or fiction? Cortex 132, 334–348 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 188.Atsma J, Maij F, Gu C, Medendorp WP & Cornell BD Active Braking of Whole-Arm Reaching Movements Provides Single-Trial Neuromuscular Measures of Movement Cancellation. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 38, 4367–4382 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]