Skip to main content
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences logoLink to Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
. 2022 Apr 4;377(1851):20210148. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2021.0148

Variation between species, populations, groups and individuals in the fitness consequences of out-group conflict

Amy Morris-Drake 1,, Patrick Kennedy 1,, Ines Braga Goncalves 1,, Andrew N Radford 1,†,
PMCID: PMC8977661  PMID: 35369741

Abstract

Out-group conflict is rife in the natural world, occurring from primates to ants. Traditionally, research on this aspect of sociality has focused on the interactions between groups and their conspecific rivals, investigating contest function and characteristics, which group members participate and what determines who wins. In recent years, however, there has been increasing interest in the consequences of out-group conflict. In this review, we first set the scene by outlining the fitness consequences that can arise immediately to contest participants, as well as a broader range of delayed, cumulative and third-party effects of out-group conflict on survival and reproductive success. For the majority of the review, we then focus on variation in these fitness consequences of out-group conflict, describing known examples both between species and between populations, groups and individuals of the same species. Throughout, we suggest possible reasons for the variation, provide examples from a diverse array of taxa, and suggest what is needed to advance this burgeoning area of social evolution.

This article is part of the theme issue ‘Intergroup conflict across taxa’.

Keywords: conspecific rivals, group living, mortality, out-group conflict, reproductive consequences, social conflict

1. Fitness consequences of out-group conflict

In social species across the animal kingdom, groups regularly interact with conspecific outsiders. These interactions can be peaceful or simply involve information exchange [1,2], but often there is conflict if the outsiders are seeking valuable resources such as food, sleeping sites, territory space, matings and breeding positions [35]. Threats may come from single individuals, same-sex coalitions or rival groups; we use ‘out-group’ conflict to refer to that with any conspecific outsider(s), and ‘intergroup’ conflict to refer to that with other groups specifically. Traditionally, work on out-group conflict has focused on contest behaviour; for instance, variation in the contributions of different group members, reasons that interactions escalate from signal exchanges to physical fights, and factors influencing who wins [2,3,610]. Indeed, a recent systematic review of the topic found that 91% of 394 papers included at least some investigation of contest characteristics [11]. However, there is now also increasing research quantifying behavioural [see 4] and, most importantly, fitness [see 12] consequences of out-group conflict.

The most obvious fitness consequences are those that can arise immediately to participants in a physical contest. For instance, there can be loss of life, extra-group matings, transfer of females between groups and replacement of breeders of both sexes [7,1317]. However, out-group conflict also generates a much broader range of delayed, third-party and cumulative consequences for survival and reproductive success (reviewed in [12]). For example, a contest could have delayed fitness consequences for participants: individuals injured in fights [1820] may subsequently have a greater mortality rate and reduced breeding performance [21,22]. Individual contests can also lead to later knock-on consequences for non-participating group members. For instance, a breeding vacancy created by contest-related mortality of the incumbent can be filled by another group member [23]. An outsider taking over a breeding position can generate reproductive opportunities for unrelated opposite-sex individuals [24], but can also cause feticide, infanticide and eviction [2528]. Moreover, group-size changes can affect mortality risk from predation and starvation, competitiveness in future out-group encounters, offspring survival and the likelihood of group extinction [2,20,2932].

Beyond contests, some fitness consequences can also arise from interactions with secondary cues of outsiders: for example, close encounters with both rivals or their faecal deposits can lead to disease and parasite transmission [3335]. More broadly, the general landscape of out-group threat, not just contests, can affect space use, defensive actions (e.g. patrolling and scent-marking), movement, resting, vigilance, and intragroup affiliation and aggression [4,19,32,3640]. The result could be use of more risky areas, greater energy expenditure, reduced foraging time and lessened parental care [36,41,42]. Finally, there can be cumulative effects of multiple contests or the build-up of outsider pressure over time. Groups may lose part or all their territory to rivals, reducing access to resources crucial for survival and both current and future reproductive success [36,43,44]. Furthermore, the cumulative effect of out-group threat probably generates chronic stress [45]. Chronic stress is associated with reduced body condition, increased susceptibility to disease and predation, and lessened investment and success in reproduction for adults [4650]. There can also be transgenerational effects for offspring [49,51], potentially through maternal effects [52] or conflict-induced decreases in the quality of parental care [41,53].

The increasing quantification of fitness consequences that can result from out-group conflict makes it timely to consider variation both between and within species. Determining the causes of such variation is important for a full understanding of sociality because out-group conflict is hypothesized to be a powerful selective force in the evolution of, for example, social structure, within-group dynamics, territoriality, cooperation and cognition [5458]. Interspecific variation in fitness consequences might arise owing to differences in, for instance, dispersal patterns, the composition of groups and inclusive fitness benefits. Intraspecific variation might occur at multiple scales: between populations (e.g. those with different densities or availability of resources), groups in the same population (e.g. those that differ in the number of neighbours or in size) and individuals in the same group (e.g. those of different sex, dominance status or health). More broadly, inter- and intraspecific variation can have far-reaching implications for population dynamics, community structure and ecosystem functioning, including in response to environmental change [59,60]. In this prospective review, we document known variation in the fitness consequences of out-group conflict at a species, population, group and individual level (figure 1). While rather more research has investigated potential reasons for variation between groups and individuals than between species and populations, we suggest possible explanations at all four levels. Throughout, we provide illustrative examples from a wide range of taxa and consider what is needed moving forward to develop our understanding of this widespread, but somewhat neglected, aspect of sociality.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Variation in fitness consequences arising from out-group conflict can occur both between species and between populations, groups and individuals of the same species. Silhouette images from http://phylopic.org. Coloured images from original drawings by Martin Aveling.

2. Variation between species

There exists considerable interspecific variation in the likelihood of mortality for those participating in out-group interactions. In some species, many interactions with neighbouring groups are peaceful (e.g. bonobos, Pan paniscus [1]), even sometimes involving a paradoxical lack of discrimination and blurring of identity between in-group and out-group individuals (e.g. unicolonial ants [61]). In species where out-group contests do commonly occur, there is still variation in likely mortality: in some, such as green woodhoopoes, Phoeniculus purpureus, contests essentially never escalate to violence, being decided by vocal and visual displays [2]; in others, such as pied babblers, Turdoides bicolor, contests can sometimes (ca 10% of occurrences) involve physical fighting (e.g. pecking and leg grappling) which can result in injuries but rarely death [62]; while in yet others, including chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, banded mongooses, Mungos mungo and some dampwood termites (e.g. Zootermopsis nevadensis), contests are often lethal [51,63,64]. For example, intergroup aggression accounted for 17% of adult deaths in one well-studied chimpanzee population [65] and 10% of total adult mortality in banded mongooses [17]. Some of the interspecific variation in mortality is driven by the cause of out-group contests: in chimpanzees, banded mongooses and greater anis, Crotophaga major, for instance, coordinated raids are made into rival territories in a targeted attempt to kill adults or offspring [6668], which increases the likelihood of fatalities. Such coalitionary killing through raiding has been suggested to arise when there is a major imbalance in power (e.g. in species where big differences in group size occur), which is especially true in fission–fusion societies ([69,70], but see [71]). As a direct contrast, groups of vervet monkeys, Chlorocebus pygerythrus, prefer to intrude on neighbouring territories when the owners are not in the area, thus avoiding direct conflict and minimizing the mortality risk [72]. The likelihood that contests escalate is also dependent on the benefits of securing the disputed resource and the costs of violence (the risk of injury or death). For instance, access to out-group females may be more valuable for male chimpanzees than bonobos, since the operational sex ratio is more heavily male-skewed (and hence there are fewer within-group mating opportunities) in the former [73]. Not only are there interspecific differences in overall contest-related mortality rates but in who dies in battle: there are sex and dominance-related differences between species that, to at least some extent, mirror differences in contest participation (see Individual-level variation).

Species also differ in the extent to which out-group conflict leads to reproductive opportunities. In some, most opportunities to become a new reproductive arise when the existing dominant dies or leaves, so successors are typically groupmates (e.g. Polistes wasps [74]). In others, a large proportion of replacements are owing to enforced takeovers, and so successors are outsiders (e.g. geladas, Theropithecus gelada [75]). This distinction is not absolute as there are, for example, species where vacancies are filled by out-group individuals: in some stingless bees, for instance, foreign queens regularly take over queenless colonies (e.g. Melipona scutellaris [76]); and in acorn woodpeckers, Melanerpes formicivorus, rival coalitions of outsiders battle to fill vacant breeding positions [77]. Enforced takeovers might be expected to be more likely in species where same-sex coalitions combine their efforts to usurp current breeders, as is the case in geladas, pied babblers and African lions, Panthera leo [15,75,78]. Which sex tends to be replaced by outsiders also differs between species. Female-biased dispersal predominates in birds; for example, in pied babblers and Arabian babblers, Argya squamiceps, females are more likely than males to disperse and aggressively takeover breeding positions [79,80]. By contrast, male-biased dispersal predominates in mammals; for instance, takeovers in meerkats, Suricata suricatta, are almost always by males [81,82]. There are exceptions to this general taxonomic difference, however, with mating system, relatedness patterns and competition for limiting resources all important in explaining interspecific variation [83]. Reproductive opportunities also arise through sneaky extra-group matings during intergroup interactions (IGIs) [17,84]. The scope for such matings may depend on factors as varied as the amount of vegetation cover and the level of group cohesion during contests, but two may be especially important: how much access there is to unrelated opposite-sex individuals within the group, and the extent to which mate-guarding can be circumvented. These latter two factors are illustrated in a comparison of meerkats (only ca 3% of pups are fathered by out-group males [85]) and banded mongooses (18% of pups are sired by out-group males [86]). Breeding female meerkats typically have access to an unrelated in-group male, and so have little incentive to pursue out-group matings, and meerkat groups are despotic, meaning that most females do not breed and thus mate-guarding is relatively easy for the dominant male. By contrast, banded mongoose females are more likely to be related to the males in their own group and groups are more egalitarian, so multiple females come into oestrus simultaneously and mate-guarding them all is thus challenging.

Out-group conflict may be a major avenue of parasite and disease transmission in some species—for example, rabies transmission by biting in out-group confrontations in red foxes, Vulpes vulpes [87]—but not in others, such as birds where contests are predominantly decided through displays [6]. Infection can arise not only from physical contact with rival groups or rovers but from inspection of scent-marks (e.g. tuberculosis in meerkats and banded mongooses, respectively [33,35]). The risk of infection might therefore be expected to be higher in mammals, where inspection of scent-marks is common, relative to birds. As with the transmission of non-pathogenic microbes [88], pathogen transmission can occur between groups via three main pathways: territory and resource overlap, out-group aggression and temporary between-group movements and dispersal events. Species that have overlapping territories or resource use—neighbouring groups of dwarf mongooses, Helogale parvula, sometimes use the same sleeping burrow on consecutive nights, for instance [89]—probably come across secondary cues of rivals, or indeed parasites left from prior occupancy, more often than those who do not share physical spaces. This risk can be minimized if groups avoid areas within their home ranges that have recently been used by neighbouring groups, as is the case in mountain gorillas, Gorilla beringei beringei [90]. Out-group conflict and parasite transmission could be further connected by positive feedbacks: if groups are weakened by parasites, they may be targeted by rivals exploiting the imbalance of power, who then contract the parasite in turn (e.g. the spread of Varroa mites in honeybees, Apis mellifera [91]).

The influence of out-group conflict on within-group social behaviour [4] and probable associated fitness consequences—receipt of affiliation or aggression may lead to short-term hygiene and mating benefits [2,92,93] and cumulative, long-term changes in an individual's status and hence reproductive prospects [94]—seems to differ between species. In some, there are changes to behaviour during contests [10,95], whereas in others the changes occur in the aftermath of an IGI [37,38,96,97]. There are also differences in whether affiliative and/or aggressive behaviour is affected, as well as the direction of the behavioural change. For example, increases in post-IGI affiliation are seen in green woodhoopoes, dwarf mongooses and mountain gorillas [37,38,96], but a decrease was documented in banded mongooses and Javan gibbons, Hylobates moloch [98,99]. At least some of the interspecific variation in within-group behavioural interactions during contests is probably generated by differences in kinship structure, patterns of philopatry and the shareability of resources [100], as well as the extent to which the inclusive fitness interests of group members are aligned in competing against a rival group. In principle, members of fission–fusion societies may have less alignment of interests than cooperative breeders; and in advanced eusocial groups, there is near complete alignment of interest [101]. In species where individuals have stronger temptations to defect (e.g. leave the group, seek extra-group matings or fail to contribute to the contest), threats or rewards may be more necessary [10,16,95]. Increases in within-group affiliation in the aftermath of out-group interactions are often interpreted from a functional perspective to increase ‘social cohesion’ [38,96,102,103], although that general term may obscure a diversity of phenomena: for example, honest signalling of willingness to collaborate rather than to compete; sharing of information about threat levels; pre-emptively coordinating the group for future contests; rewarding participation; strengthening dyadic bonds or coalitions through the exchange of services; and reconfirming or testing dominance hierarchies. Species vary in the importance of the different forms of ‘social cohesion’; for instance, signalling cooperativeness is only needed in those without an intrinsic common interest in cooperation.

Finally, the cumulative effect of out-group conflict can potentially have consequences for reproductive success but, again, there is variation in those documented for the limited number of species studied. For example, while there is no effect of neighbour pressure on reproductive success in Tasmanian native hens, Tribonyx mortierii [104], it is negatively correlated with chimpanzee inter-birth intervals, and neighbour pressure during pregnancy is also associated with lower offspring survival post-birth [49]. By contrast, intergroup conflict in crested macaques, Macaca nigra, is associated with high foetal survival and no changes to infant survival [105]; similarly, banded mongoose intergroup conflict is associated with higher foetal survival, possibly owing to reduced intragroup sexual conflict in response to infanticide [51]. Some caution is needed when interpreting these results as they arise (understandably) from long-term correlative datasets; a recent experimental test with the tractable cichlid fish species Neolamprologus pulcher found a negative effect of chronic out-group conflict on reproductive success [106]. One possible driver of interspecific variation in reproductive consequences concerns the food on which different species are reliant. If foraging is focused on scarce, locally clumped, monopolizable resources (e.g. ripe fruit for chimpanzees), losing groups probably face higher nutritional stress and, hence, potentially lower foetal survival and quality. By contrast, where species rely more on dispersed, abundant, small food items (e.g. the invertebrate prey of banded mongooses), the loss of intergroup contests might have less or no impact on early reproduction. Out-group pressure might manifest in reproductive consequences through chronic stress [107,108] and/or disruption to within-group social relationships [109], and there may also be lasting instability following breeding takeovers, but much future work is needed to unpick these possibilities.

While it is increasingly clear that species can differ vastly in the fitness consequences of out-group conflict, current explanations (including our own) are often post hoc suggestions relating to idiosyncratic life-history or ecological differences. There is a strong need for theoretical modelling to generate testable predictions; this approach has demonstrably aided our understanding of other aspects of out-group conflict, such as the factors influencing individual participation in contests and the determinants of group success [110112]. Phylogenetically controlled meta-analyses can then be used to test those predictions and to uncover underlying correlates of interspecific variation (e.g. [113]). However, to be robust, such meta-analyses need data from a much greater number of species; currently, relatively few detailed studies have quantified out-group impacts on, for instance, disease transmission, parental care and reproductive output. Ideally, clear metrics of out-group conflict would be established and consistently used in studies; these might be individual measures of, for example, the landscape of out-group pressure, frequency of out-group interactions and contest intensity, or some combined index [49,58]. Often, relevant data need to be accumulated over many generations and/or years, which is one of several reasons why long-term studies tracking known individuals are so valuable [114].

3. Variation within species

Variation in fitness consequences arising from out-group conflict can occur at several levels within the same species: between populations, groups and individuals. At each level, much of the variation arises from differences in the frequency, intensity and outcome of interactions between conspecific rivals (table 1). For instance, an increased frequency of out-group interactions will, all else being equal, result in more time and energy invested in conflict, a greater risk of injury or disease transmission, and longer-lasting, cumulative consequences for survival and reproductive success. Interactions that are resolved after initial visual or vocal signalling probably carry fewer costs than those that escalate to physical fighting; and interactions that last longer not only carry greater costs in terms of time and energy invested but might result in subsequent reductions in contributions to cooperative behaviours such as offspring care [41,99]. Losing a contest probably results in more negative consequences than winning, though winning does not necessarily preclude costs [18], and there can be considerable variation in the type and magnitude of the consequences, as well as which individuals suffer these. Elucidating reasons for variation in out-group interaction frequency, intensity and outcome are therefore crucial, as well as establishing more nuanced differences in the fitness consequences.

Table 1.

Potential differences at the population, group and individual level that could explain intraspecific variation in the frequency and intensity of out-group interactions and involvement in them, as well as factors that affect the type and magnitude of the consequences in the aftermath of a contest (its outcome).

populations groups individuals
frequency population density; territory spacing; pathogen pressure; seasonality; anthropogenic effects number of neighbours; presence of valuable resources; breeding vacancies; changing group size personal threat; personal costs; reward and punishment; involvement of kin or strongly bonded groupmates
intensity inter- and intragroup relatedness; breeding system relative resource-holding potential; rival identity; resource value asymmetry; interaction location personal threat; personal costs; reward and punishment; involvement of kin or strongly bonded groupmates
outcome resource availability; disease risk; predation risk; habitat degradation winning versus losing; rival identity resource lost; change in group members

(a) . Population-level variation

Different populations of the same species may vary in the likelihood of out-group conflict, and therefore in the magnitude of consequences, for a range of reasons. First, demographic factors can amplify or diminish the value of competing with outsiders. For instance, in the Ngorongoro Crater, Tanzania, which has a high population density of African lions, females are more likely to respond to experimentally simulated territory intrusions than do lionesses in the Serengeti, Tanzania, which has a lower population density [115]. Similarly, trematode colonies within snails may invest in a bigger ‘standing army’ in areas with higher prevalence of competitors [116]. Diana monkey, Cercopithecus diana, groups in a forest with high population density (where intergroup competition is high) exhibit greater aggression towards neighbours cf. strangers (‘nasty neighbour’ behaviour), while groups in a low-density forest exhibit lower aggression towards neighbours cf. unfamiliar rivals (‘dear-enemy’ behaviour) [117]. Second, spatial factors can drive variation in the frequency of out-group interactions. For instance, green woodhoopoe groups inhabiting linear riverine valleys in South Africa face challenges at only a maximum of two, relatively narrow, territory margins [2], while those from a Kenyan population that inhabits open woodland are often surrounded by neighbours on all sides and thus experience threats from multiple directions [118]. Chacma baboons, Papio ursinus, restricted to movement up and down desert canyons are less likely to detect intrusions than troops densely packed on open swamps [119]. Third, latitudinal gradients in pathogen pressure may drive differences in antagonism towards out-groups [120]. Where pathogen prevalence is higher, infection can be reduced by avoiding contact with other groups, which could influence IGI rates. As a final general example, anthropogenic effects can intensify animal out-group competition through habitat loss or the introduction of novel resources. For instance, in banded mongoose populations living near human habitation, groups may coalesce at food-waste sites, increasing the likelihood of intergroup encounters [121].

The frequency of IGIs in a population may also vary seasonally, especially if key contested resources (e.g. water, food and mating opportunities) are more or less plentiful at different times of the year, with implications for territory size, group overlap and mortality [122,123]. For example, some studies find that IGIs are more frequent in the breeding season [51,124], while others find the opposite pattern [125]. Seasonal differences might be because territories are only defended for part of the year, when the relevant resources (e.g. nesting or mating sites) are required [126]. In those species that do defend year-round territories, seasonal variation in defensive activities and IGIs is commonly argued to be adaptive: increases in the breeding season may, for instance, be owing to the benefits of increased defence or information gathering about potential competitors or mates at that time [127,128]. Moreover, there could be season-specific behaviours that increase the likelihood of IGIs: for example, banded mongoose groups are most likely to be involved in a contest when females are in oestrus compared to any other period [51] because those individuals initiate intergroup contests to sneak matings with out-group males [17]. However, seasonal differences in the behaviour of permanent territory-holders might also be the consequence of variation in food availability, which is typically lower in the non-breeding season [129]. In pied babblers, for instance, there are fewer IGIs during the non-breeding season compared to the breeding season, but this is in part owing to differences in food availability; supplementary feeding in the non-breeding season when foraging success was lower led to an increase in responsiveness to the simulated intrusion of a rival group [124]. In general, population differences in diet and food availability are likely to influence key aspects of sociality, including group formation and structure, as well as the occurrence of IGIs [55,130132].

Populations may vary not only in the frequency of out-group conflict but also in both the type and magnitude of consequences arising from it. For instance, there are population differences in the occurrence of lethal violence in chimpanzees, at least partially owing to variation in the extent to which additional group members lend their support once an IGI has started [133]. Moreover, whereas male chimpanzees from the population in Taï National Park, Côte d'Ivoire kidnap females without being violent towards these ‘prisoners’, males in Gombe National Park, Tanzania subject females to severe aggression which is probably lethal in some cases [73,133]. It is theoretically possible that demographic differences between populations may also impact the indirect fitness consequences of conflict, by altering intra- and intergroup relatedness. Moreover, losing an out-group contest (or sustaining an injury from contest participation) may be more costly in a population with limited or fluctuating resources, high disease risk, higher predation or habitat degradation, compared to in populations inhabiting less-challenging locations.

Moving forward, we need to understand what drives between-population variation in both levels of out-group conflict and the associated fitness consequences; most previous studies have focused on one or the other (and usually the former). An unusually detailed picture of both aspects has been worked out in red fire ants, Solenopsis invicta [134,135]. Populations are either monogyne (where each colony has only a single queen) or polygyne (where each colony has multiple queens). Monogyne populations experience intense intercolony conflict, but polygyne populations show an absence of intercolony conflict (supercoloniality) and frequently share workers. This variation can be traced directly to a ‘social chromosome’ supergene (a cluster of tightly linked loci): BB homozygotes at the Gp-9 locus are monogyne, whereas Bb heterozygotes are polygyne (with the b allele acting as a greenbeard allele whose workers reject any BB homozygote queens). The variation in intercolony conflict also has demonstrable fitness consequences: compared to polygyne populations, colonies in monogyne populations defend territories against other colonies, which greatly reduces intercolony contact and thus lowers the transmission levels of a severe queen-killing pathogen. Even in this excellent example, with strong evidence for the proximate mechanism identified, more still needs to be learned: for instance, what factors sustain the polymorphism and its functional significance.

(b) . Group-level variation

Within a population, each group experiences a particular level of outsider pressure over a given timeframe, leading to variation in the consequences of out-group conflict. For instance, groups probably have different numbers of neighbours and differ in their spatial positioning relative to others [136,137]. In general, a group with more neighbours and/or a more central territory has a greater likelihood of an IGI arising than a group with fewer neighbours or that is located on the edge of a population, although this may be complicated by dear-enemy effects [5]. Other factors that could lead to intrapopulation variation in the rate of IGIs include whether: a territory contains a particularly valuable resource, such as a fruiting tree or a female in oestrus, that might attract rival groups or roving males, respectively [14,138]; a group has infants, which rivals might target [139]; there is a breeding vacancy available in a group, which might attract multiple outsiders [77]; or a neighbouring group is changing in size [51]. In banded mongooses, for instance, groups that were growing had more IGIs than groups that were shrinking, possibly owing to the need to forage over larger areas and, therefore, expand beyond their existing territorial boundaries [51]. However, it is also possible that groups shrinking in size could exert high levels of pressure if they are looking to increase their group size via kidnapping, as seen in pied babblers [140]. An increased frequency of IGIs not only enhances the likelihood of short-term consequences but also cumulative effects on reproductive output [49].

The consequences of out-group conflict can also vary for the same group between contests in which it is involved depending on, for example, the intensity (i.e. duration and level of escalation) of each one. For many species, longer and escalated contests are more likely when the resource-holding potential (RHP) of the interacting parties is similar, as it may take more time to assess the competitive ability of the rival and require physical aggression to reach an outcome [2,141,142]. In black howler monkeys, Alouatta pigra, for example, groups that contain an equal number of males have longer IGIs than those where there is an asymmetry [141]. However, in some species this effect of RHP similarity is not necessarily apparent: in tufted capuchins, Sapajus apella, for instance, neither males nor females showed a decreased probability of approach when the numerical odds strongly favoured the opposing group [143]. Here, resource context appears more important [143]; as another example of this type of variation, grey-cheeked mangabey, Lophocebus albigena, groups that had recently arrived at a location were more likely to approach playback of rivals than those who had been there longer, with site residency probably indicating the degree of short-term, local resource exploitation [144]. Rival identity is also known to affect the intensity of IGIs: for instance, mountain gorillas exhibit greater tolerance towards groups containing familiar or related individuals [142,145]; in pied babblers, conflicts with kin groups are shorter than those with non-kin rivals [62]. Moreover, for species that exhibit dear-enemy or nasty-neighbour relationships, interactions with groups representing the greater threat might be expected to be more intense [5]. Lastly, contest location is a key factor that can affect its intensity. Contests at the core of a territory, which is generally considered more valuable owing to plentiful resources [146,147], can be more intense than those in peripheral areas, as seen in mountain gorillas, black and white colobus, Colobus guereza and blue monkeys, Cercopithecus mitis [130,145,148].

The fitness consequences of out-group contests will also differ depending on the outcome, with losing having negative effects both immediately through loss of the disputed resource [44,149] and in the aftermath of the interaction. For instance, behavioural or space-use changes are often more extreme after losing [36,96,150]. In addition, the consequences of losing could vary depending on the identity of the rival. For example, in green woodhoopoes, strangers are seeking to take over the whole territory while neighbours only invade temporarily [151], making a loss to the former of greater lasting consequence. While winning a contest can, by contrast, translate into positive consequences through the acquisition of the disputed resource [64,139], a recent study of acacia ants, Crematogaster mimosa, showed that winning groups can also incur important costs. Colonies that won physical contests still suffered a reduced workforce (owing to mortality during fighting), which resulted in compromised defence against predators and neighbouring conspecifics in the future [18]. It is often differences in RHP that determine the outcome of intergroup contests. For some species, especially those in which most group members contribute to contests, asymmetries in total group size are a key deciding factor [6,20,148]. In other cases, where only a subset of a group contributes (e.g. just males), group composition can be a better predictor of outcome than group size [152,153]. In grey wolves, Canis lupus, for example, groups are more likely to win if they have a greater number of older individuals or adult males participating, even if they are the smaller group [154]; in wedge-capped capuchins, Cebus olivaceus, and tufted capuchins, relative male group size is the most important predictor of IGI outcome [155,156]; whereas in black and white colobus, groups with fewer but larger adult males are more likely to win IGIs [152]. These differences in RHP between groups, sometimes modified by additional factors such as contest location [136,157], therefore, contribute to group-level variation in fitness consequences.

By contrast to species- and population-level variation in the fitness consequences of out-group conflict, more studies have explicitly investigated reasons for group-level variation. This is probably owing to the relative ease of collating data from different groups in the same population as opposed to at a broader scale, especially in terms of using consistent definitions and methods. Much of the focus on group-level variation has been on differences in contest intensity and outcome; moving forward, we still need to consider in greater depth what influences outsider pressure level (see [49] for calculation of a multifaceted outside pressure index). For instance, while a group dominance hierarchy exists among neighbours in some species, including African lions, grey wolves, Verreaux's sifaka, Propithecus verreauxi and black and white colobus [44,154,158,159], we are only starting to understand how this may impact the level of out-group pressure experienced and its influence on variation in fitness consequences across groups [125]. In addition, it is becoming increasingly apparent that nuanced relationships can exist between neighbouring groups owing to other factors such as relatedness and familiarity [145]; it is important to capture these within the ‘intergroup dominance hypothesis' [160]. Information on the relative strengths of different groups potentially also extends beyond near neighbours. For example, in acorn woodpeckers, individuals from territories up to 3 km away travel to witness power struggles taking place for breeding vacancies before returning to their own territory; such spectators are trading off the gathering of social information against the risk to their home territory created by their absence [77]. Finally, there are members of the population without territories that should be incorporated into this broader consideration of outside pressure levels: there are floating individuals [161], roving males travelling between groups during the breeding season [14] or splinter groups trying to establish a territory [162]. Taking this overarching view and studying the complex network of out-group conflict will help to shed light on how the pressure exerted by outsiders varies between groups and how that translates into differences in fitness consequences.

(c) . Individual-level variation

Differences in out-group contest participation can lead to variation in fitness consequences among group members, both immediately and with a delay. Intuitively, the more involved an individual is in a physical contest, the greater the risk to health and life, as evidenced in banded mongooses and red fire ants [17,163]. This risk level will depend, to at least some extent, on the intensity of a given interaction (see above). Intriguingly, there can be variation in which group members instigate interactions with rivals—for instance, females in vervet monkeys [10] and grey-cheeked mangabeys [164], while males in chimpanzees [73]—but the instigators do not necessarily suffer the greatest costs [17]. Variation in participation may also have knock-on impacts for within-group behaviour [4]. For instance, during prolonged contests between neighbouring groups, female vervet monkeys are affiliative towards participating male groupmates, as reward for current contribution and plausibly to foster continued participation, and aggressively punish uncooperative males [10]. Similarly, there is evidence from a variety of species that individuals participating more in an out-group contest receive increased affiliation in the aftermath [96,102]. These changes in within-group interactions may have short-term benefits with respect to, for instance, hygiene and stress levels [2,93]. Longer term benefits may arise from improved social network position and the strength of social bonds with groupmates, which are known to influence, for example, reproductive success and life expectancy [94,165]. Contest participation might also result in immediate reproductive benefits if, for example, males are rewarded with matings by females [92,166].

Individual variation in out-group contest participation occurs both between species and contests. At a species level (see also Variation Between Species), there are social invertebrates that have evolved specialized warrior castes (e.g. turtle ants, Cephalotes rohweri [167]; trematode spp. [116]); these individuals overwhelmingly deal with conspecific competition and thus bear the costs. In vertebrates, sex and dominance most commonly affect participation levels. All group members might contribute to some extent but to different degrees [41,84]; there are species in which one or other sex predominantly contribute—males in species as varied as Tasmanian native hens [104], grey wolves [168], tufted capuchins [132] and bonnet macaques, Macaca radiata [169], while greater female participation occurs in blue monkeys [170]. Dominant individuals often contribute more than subordinate groupmates, either because they have priority access to the disputed resources [171,172] or have more at stake [173], but subordinates may contribute more when they suffer higher costs than dominants from a lost contest (e.g. owing to heightened food competition with immigrants [6]). Variation in participation can also differ from contest to contest depending on a variety of factors. These include intruder identity and thus the threat presented (see Population-level variation and Group-level variation), as well as a range of individual factors related to reproduction, health status or the behaviour of groupmates. For instance, pregnancy and the presence of dependent young have been shown to decrease female participation levels in vervet monkeys and common marmosets, Callithrix jacchus [84,171], but to increase them in mountain gorillas and Verreaux's sifakas [172,174], and to have no effect in black howler monkeys [175]. Good body condition may reduce the costs of fighting [176], while poor health status may impact territoriality—for instance, space use in wood mice, Apodemus sylvaticus [177], travelling distances in grey wolves [178] and territory size in Tasmanian devils, Sarcophilus harrisii [179]—and thus alter the likelihood and quality of participation in out-group encounters. Finally, the involvement of kin and/or groupmates to whom an individual is strongly bonded can potentially enhance the likelihood of contest participation, as seen in chimpanzees [180].

Regardless of participation, the outcome of out-group conflicts can have distinct consequences for different group members. Contests that result in the partial loss of territory or access to shelter may affect all individuals similarly [3,36]. By contrast, the loss of access to particularly valuable but limited food resources probably impacts females and dependent young more than other group members, given the importance of food resources for female reproductive success and offspring growth [181], while the loss of a mating opportunity to a rover affects the cuckolded individual the most [14,182]. Arguably, the most extreme inter-individual differences in fitness consequences arise following breeder replacements by outsiders, as evidenced by African lions following a pride takeover by out-group males. Usurped males lose future reproductive success through the loss of breeder status, and may lose current reproductive success through infanticide and eviction or killing of older offspring by the incoming males; replaced males may also suffer severe injury or even death during contests [26,78]. In addition to the almost inevitable loss of dependent young, reproductively active females may experience temporary infertility following takeovers [78]. For other female group members, however, there may be an increase in reproductive success through access to unrelated males as daughters rarely mate with members of their father's coalition [183], while the new breeder benefits the most, having acquired multiple females with whom to breed [78].

Several aspects of individual variation in participation and in the subsequent fitness consequences of out-group conflict remain to be explored in detail. First, although the disparity of outcomes to different group members from a single interaction has been highlighted in several systems (e.g. lions, vervet monkeys, banded mongooses; see above), less consideration has been given to how single individuals may both gain and lose from a single interaction with rivals. For instance, individuals may gain or maintain territory but lose a partner or be injured; time spent interacting with rivals may have secondary impacts on the individual's offspring owing to a reduced level of care or increased vulnerability to predators [41,184]. Second, while changes to within-group interactions in the aftermath of out-group conflict have been proposed to induce greater group cohesion, there has been limited consideration of how these behavioural changes may link to future contest participation; increases in within-group affiliation and aggression during contests with rivals can affect participation levels [9,10,95], but we know little about the influence on contests arising days or weeks later. Third, a little-considered source of variation is the presence of conspecifics not directly involved in the conflict (i.e. third parties). Audience effects are known to influence contest behaviour in dyadic interactions between single individuals [185,186] but eavesdropping and its consequences in relation to out-group conflict have received little research attention [187]. The presence of other group members as an audience might be important: for instance, male white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus, responded more strongly to playback of an out-group male when tested in the presence of a male groupmate than when tested alone [188], although this might be owing to the potential support available rather than an audience effect per se. Moreover, because many group-living species defend territories whose borders are shared with multiple neighbour groups [25,90], and individuals from further afield may come to spectate on contests between rival coalitions [77], outsider audiences are a potentially important source of variation in out-group behaviour and thus its consequences.

4. Conclusion

Out-group conflict is probably a potent evolutionary force in species across the animal kingdom [5458], so quantifying the fitness consequences and determining the reasons for interspecific and intraspecific variation can greatly enhance our understanding of sociality. Our aim with this review has, therefore, been twofold. First, to showcase some, at least, of what is already known about the extensive variation in fitness consequences between species, populations, groups and individuals. Second, because currently we are often only speculating about the drivers of those differences, to stimulate further empirical and theoretical research by pinpointing some outstanding questions at each level of variation. Moving forwards, effort should be made on standardizing metrics relating to out-group conflict that can be used across taxa, to facilitate direct comparisons between studies. Tests of variation between species and populations, in particular, will probably benefit from collaboration among different research groups; certainly, the sharing of (unpublished) data accumulated gradually from long-term studies will allow otherwise impossible comparative analyses. We advocate that, where possible, future studies combine investigation of both out-group and intragroup conflict, as fitness consequences are probably a result of their interaction but not necessarily in a simple or easily predicted way [49,58]. Moreover, we see value in cross-fertilization between work on humans and non-human animals; research on other organisms can provide valuable insight into our own evolution as the management and impacts of intergroup conflict are a core component of human history ([189,190], but see [191]). Finally, there is a need to consider how environmental change influences out-group conflict and its consequences—climate warming has been suggested to increase the likelihood of conflict, for example [192]—especially given the unprecedented rate at which human activities are altering both terrestrial and aquatic landscapes. As research into the fitness consequences of out-group conflict continues to gain momentum, there will be an increasing appreciation of its importance in shaping social evolution.

Acknowledgements

We thank Susanne Schindler, Julie Kern, Harry Suter and Ben Cobb for useful discussions about out-group conflict, and Martin Aveling for the beautiful artwork in figure 1.

Data accessibility

This article has no additional data.

Authors' contributions

A.M.: conceptualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; P.K.: conceptualization, writing–original draft, writing—review and editing; I.B.G.: conceptualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; A.N.R.: conceptualization, funding acquisition, supervision, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing. All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.

Competing interests

We declare that we have no competing interests.

Funding

All authors were supported by a European Research Council Consolidator Grant (project no. 682253) awarded to A.N.R.

References

  • 1.Furuichi T. 2011. Female contributions to the peaceful nature of bonobo society. Evol. Anthropol. 20, 131-142. ( 10.1002/evan.20308) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Radford AN, Du Plessis MA. 2004. Territorial vocal rallying in the green woodhoopoe: factors affecting contest length and outcome. Anim. Behav. 68, 803-810. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.01.010) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kitchen DM, Beehner JC. 2007. Factors affecting individual participation in group-level aggression among non-human primates. Behaviour 144, 1551-1581. ( 10.1163/156853907782512074) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Radford AN, Majolo B, Aureli F. 2016. Within-group behavioural consequences of between-group conflict: a prospective review. Proc. R. Soc. B 283, 20161567. ( 10.1098/rspb.2016.1567) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Christensen C, Radford AN. 2018. Dear enemies or nasty neighbors? Causes and consequences of variation in the responses of group-living species to territorial intrusions. Behav. Ecol. 29, 1004-1013. ( 10.1093/beheco/ary010) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Radford AN. 2003. Territorial vocal rallying in the green woodhoopoe: influence of rival group size and composition. Anim. Behav. 66, 1035-1044. ( 10.1006/anbe.2003.2292) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Batchelor TP, Briffa M. 2011. Fight tactics in wood ants: individuals in smaller groups fight harder but die faster. Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 3243-3250. ( 10.1098/rspb.2011.0062) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Green PA, Briffa M, Cant MA. 2020. Assessment during intergroup contests. Trends Ecol. Evol. 36, 139-150. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2020.09.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Arseneau-Robar TJM, Muller E, Taucher AL, van Schaik CP, Bshary R, Willems EP. 2018. Male monkeys use punishment and coercion to de-escalate costly intergroup fights. Proc. R. Soc. B 285, 20172323. ( 10.1098/rspb.2017.2323) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Arseneau-Robar TJM, Taucher AL, Muller E, van Schaik C, Bshary R, Willems EP. 2016. Female monkeys use both the carrot and the stick to promote male participation in intergroup fights. Proc. R. Soc. B 283, 20161817. ( 10.1098/rspb.2016.1817) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gordon TAC, Harding HR, Radford AN. In preparation. Intraspecific intergroup interactions: causes, contests and consequences.
  • 12.Braga Goncalves I, Morris-Drake A, Kennedy P, Radford AN. In press. Fitness consequences of outgroup conflict. eLife. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 13.Wrangham RW, Wilson ML, Muller MN. 2006. Comparative rates of violence in chimpanzees and humans. Primates 47, 14-26. ( 10.1007/s10329-005-0140-1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Young AJ, Spong G, Clutton-Brock T. 2007. Subordinate male meerkats prospect for extra-group paternity: alternative reproductive tactics in a cooperative mammal. Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 1603-1609. ( 10.1098/rspb.2007.0316) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ridley AR. 2012. Invading together: the benefits of coalition dispersal in a cooperative bird. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 66, 77-83. ( 10.1007/s00265-011-1255-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Breuer T, Robbins AM, Robbins MM. 2016. Sexual coercion and courtship by male western gorillas. Primates 57, 29-38. ( 10.1007/s10329-015-0496-9) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Johnstone RA, Cant MA, Cram D, Thompson FJ. 2020. Exploitative leaders incite intergroup warfare in a social mammal. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 117, 29 759-29 766. ( 10.1073/pnas.2003745117) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Rudolph KP, McEntee JP. 2016. Spoils of war and peace: enemy adoption and queen-right colony fusion follow costly intraspecific conflict in acacia ants. Behav. Ecol. 27, 793-802. ( 10.1093/beheco/arv219) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Jordan NR, Buse C, Wilson AM, Golabek KA, Apps PJ, Lowe JC, Van der Weyde LK, McNutt JW. 2017. Dynamics of direct inter-pack encounters in endangered African wild dogs. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 71, 115. ( 10.1007/s00265-017-2338-9) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Dyble M, Houslay TM, Manser MB, Clutton-Brock T. 2019. Intergroup aggression in meerkats. Proc. R. Soc. B 286, 20191993. ( 10.1098/rspb.2019.1993) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Bernardo J, Agosta SJ. 2005. Evolutionary implications of hierarchical impacts of nonlethal injury on reproduction, including maternal effects. Biol. J. Linnean Soc. 86, 309-331. ( 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2005.00532.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Krause J, et al. 2017. Injury-mediated decrease in locomotor performance increases predation risk in schooling fish. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 372, 20160232. ( 10.1098/rstb.2016.0232) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Johns PM, Howard KJ, Breisch NL, Rivera A, Thorne BL. 2009. Nonrelatives inherit colony resources in a primitive termite. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 17 452-17 456. ( 10.1073/pnas.0907961106) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Beehner JC, Lu A. 2013. Reproductive suppression in female primates: a review. Evol. Anthropol. 22, 226-238. ( 10.1002/evan.21369) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Balshine-Earn S, Neat FC, Reid H, Taborsky M. 1998. Paying to stay or paying to breed? Field evidence for direct benefits of helping behavior in a cooperatively breeding fish. Behav. Ecol. 9, 432-438. ( 10.1093/beheco/9.5.432) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Elliot NB, Valeix M, Macdonald DW, Loveridge AJ. 2014. Social relationships affect dispersal timing revealing a delayed infanticide in African lions. Oikos 123, 1049-1056. ( 10.1111/oik.01266) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Zipple MN, Grady JH, Gordon JB, Chow LD, Archie EA, Altmann J, Alberts SC. 2017. Conditional fetal and infant killing by male baboons. Proc. R. Soc. B 284, 20162561. ( 10.1098/rspb.2016.2561) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Schneider-Crease I, Chiou KL, Snyder-Mackler N, Bergman TJ, Beehner JC, Lu A. 2020. Beyond infant death: the hidden costs of male immigration in geladas. Anim. Behav. 159, 89-95. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.11.010) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Krause J, Ruxton G. 2002. Living in groups. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Heg D, Brouwer L, Bachar Z, Taborsky M. 2005. Large group size yields group stability in the cooperatively breeding cichlid Neolamprologus pulcher. Behaviour 142, 1615-1641. ( 10.1163/156853905774831891) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Taborsky B, Skubic E, Bruintjes R. 2007. Mothers adjust egg size to helper number in a cooperatively breeding cichlid. Behav. Ecol. 18, 652-657. ( 10.1093/beheco/arm026) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Henschel JR, Skinner JD. 1991. Territorial behaviour by a clan of spotted hyaenas Crocuta crocuta. Ethology 88, 223-235. ( 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1991.tb00277.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Drewe JA. 2010. Who infects whom? Social networks and tuberculosis transmission in wild meerkats. Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 633-642. ( 10.1098/rspb.2009.1775) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Nolan MP, Delaplane KS. 2017. Distance between honey bee Apis mellifera colonies regulates populations of Varroa destructor at a landscape scale. Apidologie 48, 8-16. ( 10.1007/s13592-016-0443-9) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Alexander KA, Sanderson CE, Larsen MH, Robbe-Austerman S, Williams MC, Palmer MV. 2016. Emerging tuberculosis pathogen hijacks social communication behavior in the group-living banded mongoose (Mungos mungo). Mbio 7, e00281-00216. ( 10.1128/mBio.00281-16) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Crofoot MC. 2013. The cost of defeat: capuchin groups travel further, faster and later after losing conflicts with neighbors. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 152, 79-85. ( 10.1002/ajpa.22330) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Morris-Drake A, Christensen C, Kern JM, Radford AN. 2019. Experimental field evidence that out-group threats influence within-group behaviour. Behav. Ecol. 30, 1425-1435. ( 10.1093/beheco/arz095) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Mirville MO, Ridley AR, Samedi JPM, Vecellio V, Ndagijimana F, Stoinski TS, Grueter CC. 2020. Intragroup behavioral changes following intergroup conflict in mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei). Int. J. Primatol. 41, 382-400. ( 10.1007/s10764-020-00130-1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Samuni L, Mielke A, Preis A, Crockford C, Wittig RM. 2020. Intergroup competition enhances chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) in-group cohesion. Int. J. Primatol. 41, 342-362. ( 10.1007/s10764-019-00112-y) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Macintosh AJJ, Sicotte P. 2009. Vigilance in ursine black and white colobus monkeys (Colobus vellerosus): an examination of the effects of conspecific threat and predation. Am. J. Primatol. 71, 919-927. ( 10.1002/ajp.20730) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Mares R, Young AJ, Clutton-Brock TH. 2012. Individual contributions to territory defence in a cooperative breeder: weighing up the benefits and costs. Proc. R. Soc. B 279, 3989-3995. ( 10.1098/rspb.2012.1071) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Morris-Drake A, Linden JF, Kern JM, Radford AN. 2021. Extended and cumulative effects of experimentally induced intergroup conflict in a cooperatively breeding mammal. Proc. R. Soc. B 288, 20211743. ( 10.1098/rspb.2021.1743) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Thompson ME, Kahlenberg SM, Gilby IC, Wrangham RW. 2007. Core area quality is associated with variance in reproductive success among female chimpanzees at Kibale National Park. Anim. Behav. 73, 501-512. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.09.007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Mosser A, Packer C. 2009. Group territoriality and the benefits of sociality in the African lion, Panthera leo. Anim. Behav. 78, 359-370. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.04.024) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Samuni L, Preis A, Deschner T, Wittig RM, Crockford C. 2019. Cortisol and oxytocin show independent activity during chimpanzee intergroup conflict. Psychoneuroendocrinology 104, 165-173. ( 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.02.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Zanette LY, White AF, Allen MC, Clinchy M. 2011. Perceived predation risk reduces the number of offspring songbirds produce per year. Science 334, 1398-1401. ( 10.1126/science.1210908) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Mileva VR, Gilmour KM, Balshine S. 2011. Effects of maternal stress on egg characteristics in a cooperatively breeding fish. Comp. Biochem. Physiol-A 158, 22-29. ( 10.1016/j.cbpa.2010.08.017) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Vuarin P, Pillay N, Schradin C. 2019. Elevated basal corticosterone levels increase disappearance risk of light but not heavy individuals in a long-term monitored rodent population. Horm. Behav. 113, 95-102. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.05.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Lemoine S, Preis A, Samuni L, Boesch C, Crockford C, Wittig RM. 2020. Between-group competition impacts reproductive success in wild chimpanzees. Curr. Biol. 30, 312-318. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2019.11.039) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Campos FA, Archie EA, Gesquiere LR, Tung J, Altmann J, Alberts SC. 2021. Glucocorticoid exposure predicts survival in female baboons. Sci. Adv. 7, eabf6759. ( 10.1126/sciadv.abf6759) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Thompson FJ, Marshall HH, Vitikainen EIK, Cant MA. 2017. Causes and consequences of intergroup conflict in cooperative banded mongooses. Anim. Behav. 126, 31-40. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.01.017) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Brunton PJ. 2013. Effects of maternal exposure to social stress during pregnancy: consequences for mother and offspring. Reproduction 146, R175-R189. ( 10.1530/rep-13-0258) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Stein LR, Bell AM. 2012. Consistent individual differences in fathering in threespined stickleback Gasterosteus aculeatus. Curr. Zool. 58, 45-52. ( 10.1093/czoolo/58.1.45) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Alexander RD, Borgia G. 1978. Group selection, altruism, and levels of organization of life. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 9, 449-474. ( 10.1146/annurev.es.09.110178.002313) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Wrangham RW. 1980. An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour 75, 262-300. ( 10.1163/156853980x00447) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Choi JK, Bowles S. 2007. The coevolution of parochial altruism and war. Science 318, 636-640. ( 10.1126/science.1144237) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Rusch H. 2014. The evolutionary interplay of intergroup conflict and altruism in humans: a review of parochial altruism theory and prospects for its extension. Proc. R. Soc. B 281, 20141539. ( 10.1098/rspb.2014.1539) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Ashton BJ, Kennedy P, Radford AN. 2020. Interactions with conspecific outsiders as drivers of cognitive evolution. Nat. Commun. 11, 4937. ( 10.1038/s41467-020-18780-3) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Bolnick DI, et al. 2011. Why intraspecific trait variation matters in community ecology. Trends Ecol. Evol. 26, 183-192. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2011.01.009) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Des Roches S, Post DM, Turley NE, Bailey JK, Hendry AP, Kinnison MT, Schweitzer JA, Palkovacs EP. 2018. The ecological importance of intraspecific variation. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 2, 57-64. ( 10.1038/s41559-017-0402-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Helanterä H, Strassmann JE, Carrillo J, Queller DC. 2009. Unicolonial ants: where do they come from, what are they and where are they going? Trends Ecol. Evol. 24, 341-349. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2009.01.013) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Humphries DJ, Nelson-Flower MJ, Bell MBV, Finch FM, Ridley AR. 2021. Kinship, dear enemies, and costly combat: the effects of relatedness on territorial overlap and aggression in a cooperative breeder. Ecol. Evol. 11, 17 031-170 422. ( 10.1002/ece3.8342) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Thorne BL, Breisch NL, Muscedere ML. 2003. Evolution of eusociality and the soldier caste in termites: influence of intraspecific competition and accelerated inheritance. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 12 808-12 813. ( 10.1073/pnas.2133530100) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Mitani JC, Watts DP, Amsler SJ. 2010. Lethal intergroup aggression leads to territorial expansion in wild chimpanzees. Curr. Biol. 20, R507-R508. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2010.04.021) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Williams JM, Lonsdorf EV, Wilson ML, Schumacher-Stankey J, Goodall J, Pusey AE. 2008. Causes of death in the Kasekela chimpanzees of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Am. J. Primatol. 70, 766-777. ( 10.1002/ajp.20573) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Wilson ML, et al. 2014. Lethal aggression in Pan is better explained by adaptive strategies than human impacts. Nature 513, 414-417. ( 10.1038/nature13727) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Cant MA, Nichols HJ, Thompson FJ, Vitikainen EIK. 2016. Banded mongooses: demography, life history, and social behavior. In Cooperative breeding in vertebrates: studies of ecology, evolution and behavior (eds Koenig WD, Dickinson JL), pp. 318-337. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Strong MJ, Sherman BL, Riehl C. 2018. Home field advantage, not group size, predicts outcomes of intergroup conflicts in a social bird. Anim. Behav. 143, 205-213. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.07.006) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Wrangham RW. 1999. Evolution of coalitionary killing. J. Phys. Anthropol. 42, 1-30. () [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Aureli F, et al. 2008. Fission-fusion dynamics: new research frameworks. Curr. Anthropol. 49, 627-654. ( 10.1086/586708) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Martínez-Íñigo L, Engelhardt A, Agil M, Pilot M, Majolo B. 2021. Intergroup lethal gang attacks in wild crested macaques, Macaca nigra. Anim. Behav. 180, 81-91. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2021.08.002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Isbell LA, Bidner LR, Loftus JC, Kimuyu DM, Young TP. 2021. Absentee owners and overlapping home ranges in a territorial species. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 75, 21. ( 10.1007/s00265-020-02945-7) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Furuichi T. 2020. Variation in intergroup relationships among species and among and within local populations of African apes. Int. J. Primatol. 41, 203-223. ( 10.1007/s10764-020-00134-x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Cant MA, English S, Reeve HK, Field J. 2006. Escalated conflict in a social hierarchy. Proc. R. Soc. B 273, 2977-2984. ( 10.1098/rspb.2006.3669) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Roberts EK, Lu A, Bergman TJ, Beehner JC. 2012. A Bruce effect in wild geladas. Science 335, 1222-1225. ( 10.1126/science.1213600) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Wenseleers T, Alves DA, Francoy TM, Billen J, Imperatriz-Fonseca VL. 2011. Intraspecific queen parasitism in a highly eusocial bee. Biol. Lett. 7, 173-176. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2010.0819) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Barve S, Lahey AS, Brunner RM, Koenig WD, Walters EL. 2020. Tracking the warriors and spectators of acorn woodpecker wars. Curr. Biol. 30, R982-R983. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2020.07.073) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Packer C, Pusey AE. 1983. Adaptations of female lions to infanticide by incoming males. Am. Nat. 121, 716-728. ( 10.1086/284097) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Raihani NJ, Nelson-Flower MJ, Golabek KA, Ridley AR. 2010. Routes to breeding in cooperatively breeding pied babblers Turdoides bicolor. J. Avian Biol. 41, 681-686. ( 10.1111/j.1600-048X.2010.05211.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Zahavi A. 1990. Arabian babblers: The quest for social status in a cooperative breeder. In Cooperative breeding in birds: long-term studies of ecology and behavior (eds Stacey PB, Koenig WD), pp. 105-130. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Doolan SP, Macdonald DW. 1996. Dispersal and extra-territorial prospecting by slender-tailed meerkats (Suricata suricatta) in the south-western Kalahari. J. Zool. 240, 59-73. ( 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1996.tb05486.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Clutton-Brock TH, Gaynor D, McIlrath GM, Maccoll ADC, Kansky R, Chadwick P, Manser M, Skinner JD, Brotherton PNM. 1999. Predation, group size and mortality in a cooperative mongoose, Suricata suricatta. J. Anim. Ecol. 68, 672-683. ( 10.1046/j.1365-2656.1999.00317.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Li XY, Kokko H. 2019. Sex-biased dispersal: a review of the theory. Biol. Rev. 94, 721-736. ( 10.1111/brv.12475) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Lazaro-Perea C. 2001. Intergroup interactions in wild common marmosets, Callithrix jacchus: territorial defence and assessment of neighbours. Anim. Behav. 62, 11-21. ( 10.1006/anbe.2000.1726) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Leclaire S, Nielsen JF, Sharp SP, Clutton-Brock TH. 2013. Mating strategies in dominant meerkats: evidence for extra-pair paternity in relation to genetic relatedness between pair mates. J. Evol. Biol. 26, 1499-1507. ( 10.1111/jeb.12151) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Nichols HJ, Cant MA, Hoffman JI, Sanderson JL. 2014. Evidence for frequent incest in a cooperatively breeding mammal. Biol. Lett. 10, 20140898. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2014.0898) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.White PCL, Harris S, Smith GC. 1995. Fox contact behaviour and rabies spread: a model for the estimation of contact probabilities between urban foxes at different population densities and its implications for rabies control in Britain. J. Appl. Ecol. 32, 693-706. ( 10.2307/2404809) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Sarkar A, et al. 2020. Microbial transmission in animal social networks and the social microbiome. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 4, 1020-1035. ( 10.1038/s41559-020-1220-8) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Rasa OAE. 1987. The dwarf mongoose: a study of behavior and social structure in relation to ecology in a small, social carnivore. Adv. Stud. Behav. 17, 121-163. ( 10.1016/s0065-3454(08)60178-3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Seiler N, Boesch C, Mundry R, Stephens C, Robbins MM. 2017. Space partitioning in wild, non-territorial mountain gorillas: the impact of food and neighbours. R. Soc. Open Sci. 4, 170720. ( 10.1098/rsos.170720) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Peck DT, Seeley TD. 2019. Mite bombs or robber lures? The roles of drifting and robbing in Varroa destructor transmission from collapsing honey bee colonies to their neighbors. PLoS ONE 14, e0218392. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0218392) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Arseneau TJM, Taucher AL, van Schaik CP, Willems EP. 2015. Male monkeys fight in between-group conflicts as protective parents and reluctant recruits. Anim. Behav. 110, 39-50. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.09.006) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Wittig RM, Crockford C, Weltring A, Langergraber KE, Deschner T, Zuberbuhler K. 2016. Social support reduces stress hormone levels in wild chimpanzees across stressful events and everyday affiliations. Nat. Commun. 7, 13361. ( 10.1038/ncomms13361) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Silk JB, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Crockford C, Engh AL, Moscovice LR, Wittig RM, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. 2010. Female chacma baboons form strong, equitable, and enduring social bonds. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 64, 1733-1747. ( 10.1007/s00265-010-0986-0) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Braga Goncalves I, Radford AN. 2019. Experimental evidence that intruder and group member attributes affect out-group defence and associated within-group interactions in a social fish. Proc. R. Soc. B 286, 20191261. ( 10.1098/rspb.2019.1261) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Radford AN. 2008. Duration and outcome of intergroup conflict influences intragroup affiliative behaviour. Proc. R. Soc. B 275, 2787-2791. ( 10.1098/rspb.2008.0787) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Birch G, Cant MA, Thompson FJ. 2019. Behavioural response of workers to repeated intergroup encounters in the harvester ant Messor barbarus. Insect. Soc. 66, 491-500. ( 10.1007/s00040-019-00710-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Preston EFR, Thompson FJ, Ellis S, Kyambulima S, Croft DP, Cant MA. 2020. Network-level consequences of outgroup threats in banded mongooses: grooming and aggression between the sexes. J. Anim. Ecol. 90, 153-167. ( 10.1111/1365-2656.13323) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Yi Y, Fichtel C, Kim E, Choe JC. 2020. Impacts of intergroup interactions on intragroup behavioral changes in Javan gibbons (Hylobates moloch). Int. J. Primatol. 41, 363-381. ( 10.1007/s10764-019-00116-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Majolo B, Vizioli AD, Lehmann J. 2016. The effect of intergroup competition on intragroup affiliation in primates. Anim. Behav. 114, 13-19. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.01.009) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Gardner A, Grafen A. 2009. Capturing the superorganism: a formal theory of group adaptation. J. Evol. Biol. 22, 659-671. ( 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01681.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Bruintjes R, Lynton-Jenkins J, Jones JW, Radford AN. 2016. Out-group threat promotes within-group affiliation in a cooperative fish. Am. Nat. 187, 274-282. ( 10.1086/684411) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Thompson FJ, Hunt KL, Wright K, Rosengaus RB, Cole EL, Birch G, Maune AL, Cant MA. 2020. Who goes there? Social surveillance as a response to intergroup conflict in a primitive termite. Biol. Lett. 16, 20200131. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2020.0131) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Putland DA, Goldizen AW. 1998. Territorial behaviour in the Tasmanian native hen: group and individual performance. Anim. Behav. 56, 1455-1463. ( 10.1006/anbe.1998.0937) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Kerhoas D, Perwitasari-Farajallah D, Agil M, Widdig A, Engelhardt A. 2014. Social and ecological factors influencing offspring survival in wild macaques. Behav. Ecol. 25, 1164-1172. ( 10.1093/beheco/aru099) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Braga Goncalves I, Radford AN. In press. Experimental evidence that chronic outgroup conflict reduces reproductive success in a cooperatively breeding fish. eLife. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 107.Beldade R, Blandin A, O'Donnell R, Mills SC. 2017. Cascading effects of thermally-induced anemone bleaching on associated anemonefish hormonal stress response and reproduction. Nat. Commun. 8, 716. ( 10.1038/s41467-017-00565-w) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Creel S, Winnie JA, Christianson D. 2009. Glucocorticoid stress hormones and the effect of predation risk on elk reproduction. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 12 388-12 393. ( 10.1073/pnas.0902235106) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Hellmann JK, Hamilton IM. 2019. Intragroup social dynamics vary with the presence of neighbors in a cooperatively breeding fish. Curr. Zool. 65, 21-31. ( 10.1093/cz/zoy025) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Johnson DDP, MacKay NJ. 2015. Fight the power: Lanchester's laws of combat in human evolution. Evol. Hum. Behav. 36, 152-163. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.11.001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Schindler S, Radford AN. 2018. Factors influencing within-group conflict over defence against conspecific outsiders seeking breeding positions. Proc. R. Soc. B 285, 20181669. ( 10.1098/rspb.2018.1669) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Rusch H, Gavrilets S. 2020. The logic of animal intergroup conflict: a review. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 178, 1014-1030. ( 10.1016/j.jebo.2017.05.004) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Willems EP, van Schaik CP. 2015. Collective action and the intensity of between-group competition in nonhuman primates. Behav. Ecol. 26, 625-631. ( 10.1093/beheco/arv001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Clutton-Brock T, Sheldon BC. 2010. Individuals and populations: the role of long-term, individual-based studies of animals in ecology and evolutionary biology. Trends Ecol. Evol. 25, 562-573. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2010.08.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Heinsohn R. 1997. Group territoriality in two populations of African lions. Anim. Behav. 53, 1143-1147. ( 10.1006/anbe.1996.0316) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Resetarits EJ, Torchin ME, Hechinger RF. 2020. Social trematode parasites increase standing army size in areas of greater invasion threat. Biol. Lett. 16, 20190765. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2019.0765) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Decellieres M, Zuberbuhler K, Leon J. 2021. Habitat-dependent intergroup hostility in Diana monkeys, Cercopithecus diana. Anim. Behav. 178, 95-104. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2021.06.001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Ligon JD, Ligon SH. 1990. Green woodhoopoes: Life history traits and sociality. In Cooperative breeding in birds: long-term studies of ecology and behavior (eds Stacey PB, Koenig WC), pp. 33-65. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Hamilton WJ, Buskirk RE, Buskirk WH. 1976. Defense of space and resources by chacma (Papio ursinus) baboon troops in an African desert and swamp. Ecology 57, 1264-1272. ( 10.2307/1935050) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Van de Vliert E. 2020. The global ecology of differentiation between us and them. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 270-278. ( 10.1038/s41562-019-0783-3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Gilchrist JS, Otali E. 2002. The effects of refuse-feeding on home-range use, group size, and intergroup encounters in the banded mongoose. Can. J. Zool. 80, 1795-1802. ( 10.1139/z02-113) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Mech LD, Barber-Meyer SM. 2017. Seasonality of intraspecific mortality by gray wolves. J. Mammal. 98, 1538-1546. ( 10.1093/jmammal/gyx113) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Brandell EE, Fountain-Jones NM, Gilbertson MLJ, Cross PC, Hudson PJ, Smith DW, Stahler DR, Packer C, Craft ME. 2021. Group density, disease, and season shape territory size and overlap of social carnivores. J. Anim. Ecol. 90, 87-101. ( 10.1111/1365-2656.13294) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Golabek KA, Ridley AR, Radford AN. 2012. Food availability affects strength of seasonal territorial behaviour in a cooperatively breeding bird. Anim. Behav. 83, 613-619. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.11.034) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Lemoine S, Boesch C, Preis A, Samuni L, Crockford C, Wittig RM. 2020. Group dominance increases territory size and reduces neighbour pressure in wild chimpanzees. R. Soc. Open Sci. 7, 200577. ( 10.1098/rsos.200577) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Strahl SD, Schmitz A. 1990. Hoatzins: cooperative breeding in a folivorous neotropical bird. In Cooperative breeding in birds: long-term studies of ecology and behavior (eds Stacey PB, Koenig WC), pp. 133-155. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Jordan NR, Cherry MI, Manser MB. 2007. Latrine distribution and patterns of use by wild meerkats: implications for territory and mate defence. Anim. Behav. 73, 613-622. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.06.010) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Gese EM, Ruff RL. 1997. Scent-marking by coyotes, Canis latrans: the influence of social and ecological factors. Anim. Behav. 54, 1155-1166. ( 10.1006/anbe.1997.0561) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Zann RA, Morton SR, Jones KR, Burley NT. 1995. The timing of breeding by zebra finches in relation to rainfall in central Australia. Emu 95, 208-222. ( 10.1071/mu9950208) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Harris TR. 2006. Between-group contest competition for food in a highly folivorous population of black and white colobus monkeys (Colobus guereza). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 61, 317-329. ( 10.1007/s00265-006-0261-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Janson CH, van Schaik CP. 1988. Recognizing the many faces of primate food competition: methods. Behaviour 105, 165-186. () [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Scarry CJ. 2017. Male resource defence during intergroup aggression among tufted capuchin monkeys. Anim. Behav. 123, 169-178. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.10.015) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Boesch C, Crockford C, Herbinger I, Wittig R, Moebius Y, Normand E. 2008. Intergroup conflicts among chimpanzees in Tai National Park: lethal violence and the female perspective. Am. J. Primatol. 70, 519-532. ( 10.1002/ajp.20524) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Oi DH. 2006. Effect of mono- and polygyne social forms on transmission and spread of a microsporidium in fire ant populations. J. Invertebr. Pathol. 92, 146-151. ( 10.1016/j.jip.2006.04.010) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Buechel SD, Wurm Y, Keller L. 2014. Social chromosome variants differentially affect queen determination and the survival of workers in the fire ant Solenopsis invicta. Mol. Ecol. 23, 5117-5127. ( 10.1111/mec.12915) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Crofoot MC, Gilby IC, Wikelski MC, Kays RW. 2008. Interaction location outweighs the competitive advantage of numerical superiority in Cebus capucinus intergroup contests. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 577-581. ( 10.1073/pnas.0707749105) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Drewe JA, Madden JR, Pearce GP. 2009. The social network structure of a wild meerkat population: 1. Inter-group interactions. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 63, 1295-1306. ( 10.1007/s00265-009-0782-x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Sakamaki T, Ryu H, Toda K, Tokuyama N, Furuichi T. 2018. Increased frequency of intergroup encounters in wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) around the yearly peak in fruit abundance at Wamba. Int. J. Primatol. 39, 685-704. ( 10.1007/s10764-018-0058-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Steenbeek R. 1999. Tenure related changes in wild Thomas's langurs I: between-group interactions. Behaviour 136, 595-625. ( 10.1163/156853999501487) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Ridley AR. 2016. Southern pied babblers: the dynamics of conflict and cooperation in a group-living society. In Cooperative breeding in vertebrates: studies of ecology, evolution and behavior (eds Koenig WC, Dickinson JL), pp. 115-132. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Van Belle S, Scarry CJ. 2015. Individual participation in intergroup contests is mediated by numerical assessment strategies in black howler and tufted capuchin monkeys. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 370, 20150007. ( 10.1098/rstb.2015.0007) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Mirville MO, Ridley AR, Samedi JPM, Vecellio V, Ndagijimana F, Stoinski TS, Grueter CC. 2018. Low familiarity and similar ‘group strength’ between opponents increase the intensity of intergroup interactions in mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 72, 178. ( 10.1007/s00265-018-2592-5) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Scarry CJ. 2020. Against all odds: numerical assessment by tufted capuchin monkeys. Am. J. Primatol. 82, e23094. ( 10.1002/ajp.23094) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Brown M. 2014. Patch occupation time predicts responses by grey-cheeked mangabeys (Lophocebus albigena) to real and simulated neighboring groups. Int. J. Primatol. 35, 491-508. ( 10.1007/s10764-014-9762-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Morrison RE, Hirwa JP, Mucyo JPS, Stoinski TS, Vecellio V, Eckardt W. 2020. Inter-group relationships influence territorial defence in mountain gorillas. J. Anim. Ecol. 89, 2852-2862. ( 10.1111/1365-2656.13355) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Giraldeau LA, Ydenberg R. 1987. The center-edge effect: the result of a war of attrition between territorial contestants. Auk 104, 535-538. ( 10.2307/4087559) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Falls JB. 1982. Individual recognition by sounds in birds. In Acoustic communication in birds (eds Kroodsma DE, Mille REH), pp. 237-278. New York, NY: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Roth AM, Cords M. 2016. Effects of group size and contest location on the outcome and intensity of intergroup contests in wild blue monkeys. Anim. Behav. 113, 49-58. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.11.011) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Isbell LA, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM. 1990. Costs and benefits of home range shifts among vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops) in Amboseli National Park, Kenya. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 27, 351-358. ( 10.1007/bf00164006) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Markham AC, Alberts SC, Altmann J. 2012. Intergroup conflict: ecological predictors of winning and consequences of defeat in a wild primate population. Anim. Behav. 84, 399-403. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.05.009) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Radford AN. 2005. Group-specific vocal signatures and neighbour-stranger discrimination in the cooperatively breeding green woodhoopoe. Anim. Behav. 70, 1227-1234. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.04.002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Harris TR. 2010. Multiple resource values and fighting ability measures influence intergroup conflict in guerezas (Colobus guereza). Anim. Behav. 79, 89-98. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.10.007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Kitchen DM, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM. 2004. Factors mediating inter-group encounters in savannah baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus). Behaviour 141, 197-218. ( 10.1163/156853904322890816) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Cassidy KA, MacNulty DR, Stahler DR, Smith DW, Mech LD. 2015. Group composition effects on aggressive interpack interactions of gray wolves in Yellowstone National Park. Behav. Ecol. 26, 1352-1360. ( 10.1093/beheco/arv081) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 155.Robinson JG. 1988. Group-size in wedge-capped capuchin monkeys, Cebus olivaceus, and the reproductive success of males and females. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 23, 187-197. ( 10.1007/bf00300353) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Scarry CJ. 2013. Between-group contest competition among tufted capuchin monkeys, Sapajus nigritus, and the role of male resource defence. Anim. Behav. 85, 931-939. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.02.013) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Furrer RD, Kyabulima S, Willems EP, Cant MA, Manser MB. 2011. Location and group size influence decisions in simulated intergroup encounters in banded mongooses. Behav. Ecol. 22, 493-500. ( 10.1093/beheco/arr010) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Benadi G, Fichtel C, Kappeler P. 2008. Intergroup relations and home range use in Verreaux's sifaka (Propithecus verreauxi). Am. J. Primatol. 70, 956-965. ( 10.1002/ajp.20588) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 159.Harris TR. 2006. Within- and among-male variation in roaring by black and white colobus monkeys (Colobus guereza): what does it reveal about function? Behaviour 143, 197-218. ( 10.1163/156853906775900702) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Crofoot MC, Wrangham RW. 2010. Intergroup aggression in primates and humans: the case for a unified theory. In Minding the gap: tracing the origins of human universals (eds Kappeler PM, Silk JB), pp. 171-196. Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag. [Google Scholar]
  • 161.Ridley AR, Raihani NJ, Nelson-Flower MJ. 2008. The cost of being alone: the fate of floaters in a population of cooperatively breeding pied babblers Turdoides bicolor. J. Avian Biol. 39, 389-392. ( 10.1111/j.2008.0908-8857.04479.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 162.Cant MA, Otali E, Mwanguhya F. 2001. Eviction and dispersal in co-operatively breeding banded mongooses (Mungos mungo). J. Zool. 254, 155-162. ( 10.1017/s0952836901000668) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 163.Plowes NJR, Adams ES. 2005. An empirical test of Lanchester's square law: mortality during battles of the fire ant Solenopsis invicta. Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 1809-1814. ( 10.1098/rspb.2005.3162) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 164.Brown M, Waser PM. 2018. Group movements in response to competitors' calls indicate conflicts of interest between male and female grey-cheeked mangabeys. Am. J. Primatol. 80, e22918. ( 10.1002/ajp.22918) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 165.Cameron EZ, Setsaas TH, Linklater WL. 2009. Social bonds between unrelated females increase reproductive success in feral horses. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 13 850-13 853. ( 10.1073/pnas.0900639106) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 166.Watts DP, Mitani JC. 2001. Boundary patrols and intergroup encounters in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 138, 299-327. ( 10.1163/15685390152032488) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 167.Powell S, Donaldson-Matasci M, Woodrow-Tomizuka A, Dornhaus A. 2017. Context-dependent defences in turtle ants: resource defensibility and threat level induce dynamic shifts in soldier deployment. Funct. Ecol. 31, 2287-2298. ( 10.1111/1365-2435.12926) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 168.Cassidy KA, Mech LD, MacNulty DR, Stahler DR, Smith DW. 2017. Sexually dimorphic aggression indicates male gray wolves specialize in pack defense against conspecific groups. Behav. Process. 136, 64-72. ( 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.01.011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 169.Cooper MA, Aureli F, Singh M. 2004. Between-group encounters among bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 56, 217-227. ( 10.1007/s00265-004-0779-4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 170.Cords M. 2007. Variable participation in the defense of communal feeding territories by blue monkeys in the Kakamega Forest, Kenya. Behaviour 144, 1537-1550. ( 10.1163/156853907782512100) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 171.Arseneau-Robar TJM, Taucher AL, Schnider AB, van Schaik CP, Willems EP. 2017. Intra- and interindividual differences in the costs and benefits of intergroup aggression in female vervet monkeys. Anim. Behav. 123, 129-137. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.10.034) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 172.Mirville MO, Ridley AR, Samedi JPM, Vecellio V, Ndagijimana F, Stoinski TS, Grueter CC. 2018. Factors influencing individual participation during intergroup interactions in mountain gorillas. Anim. Behav. 144, 75-86. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2018.08.003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 173.Garvy KA, Hellmann JK, Ligocki IY, Reddon AR, Marsh-Rollo SE, Hamilton IM, Balshine S, O'Connor CM. 2015. Sex and social status affect territorial defence in a cooperatively breeding cichlid fish, Neolamprologus savoryi. Hydrobiologia 748, 75-85. ( 10.1007/s10750-014-1899-0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 174.Lewis RJ, Sandel AA, Hilty S, Barnett SE. 2020. The collective action problem but not numerical superiority explains success in intergroup encounters in Verreaux's sifaka (Propithecus verreauxi): implications for individual participation and free-riding. Int. J. Primatol. 41, 305-324. ( 10.1007/s10764-020-00155-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 175.Van Belle S. 2015. Female participation in collective group defense in black howler monkeys (Alouatta pigra). Am. J. Primatol. 77, 595-604. ( 10.1002/ajp.22380) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 176.Watts DP. 2002. Reciprocity and interchange in the social relationships of wild male chimpanzees. Behaviour 139, 343-370. ( 10.1163/156853902760102708) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 177.Brown ED, Macdonald DW, Tew TE, Todd IA. 1994. Apodemus sylvaticus infected with Heligmosomoides polygyrus (Nematoda) in an arable ecosystem: epidemiology and effects of infection on the movements of male mice. J. Zool. 234, 623-640. ( 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1994.tb04869.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 178.Cross PC, et al. 2016. Energetic costs of mange in wolves estimated from infrared thermography. Ecology 97, 1938-1948. ( 10.1890/15-1346.1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 179.Comte S, Carver S, Hamede R, Jones M. 2020. Changes in spatial organization following an acute epizootic: Tasmanian devils and their transmissible cancer. Global Ecol. Conserv. 22, e00993. ( 10.1016/j.gecco.2020.e00993) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 180.Samuni L, Crockford C, Wittig RM. 2021. Group-level cooperation in chimpanzees is shaped by strong social ties. Nat. Commun. 12, 539. ( 10.1038/s41467-020-20709-9) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 181.Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM. 1987. The influence of intergroup competition on the survival and reproduction of female vervet monkeys. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 21, 375-386. ( 10.1007/bf00299932) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 182.Lardy S, Allaine D, Bonenfant C, Cohas A. 2015. Sex-specific determinants of fitness in a social mammal. Ecology 96, 2947-2959. ( 10.1890/15-0425.1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 183.Pusey AE, Packer C. 1987. The evolution of sex-biased dispersal in lions. Behaviour 101, 275-310. () [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 184.Ridley AR, Raihani NJ. 2008. Task partitioning increases reproductive output in a cooperative bird. Behav. Ecol. 19, 1136-1142. ( 10.1093/beheco/arn097) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 185.Matos RJ, McGregor PK. 2002. The effect of the sex of an audience on male-male displays of siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens). Behaviour 139, 1211-1221. ( 10.1163/15685390260437344) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 186.Fitzsimmons LP, Bertram SM. 2013. Playing to an audience: the social environment influences aggression and victory displays. Biol. Lett. 9, 20130449. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2013.0449) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 187.Braga Goncalves I, Radford AN. In press. Intraspecific variation in audience effects during outgroup conflict in a cooperatively breeding fish. Anim. Behav.
  • 188.Meunier H, Molina-Vila P, Perry S. 2012. Participation in group defence: proximate factors affecting male behaviour in wild white-faced capuchins. Anim. Behav. 83, 621-628. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 189.Bowles S. 2009. Did warfare among ancestral hunter-gatherers affect the evolution of human social behaviors? Science 324, 1293-1298. ( 10.1126/science.1168112) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 190.Majolo B. 2019. Warfare in an evolutionary perspective. Evol. Anthropol. 28, 321-331. ( 10.1002/evan.21806) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 191.Fry DP. 2006. The human potential for peace: an anthropological challenge to assumptions about war and violence. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 192.Burke MB, Miguel E, Satyanath S, Dykema JA, Lobell DB. 2009. Warming increases the risk of civil war in Africa. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 20 670-20 674. ( 10.1073/pnas.0907998106) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

This article has no additional data.


Articles from Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES