Abstract
Interspecific competition influences the composition of ecological communities. Species may differ in their needs for different resources, therefore resource availability may determine the outcome of interspecific interactions. Species often compete over food, shelter or both. When more than one resource is limited, different species may prioritize different resources. To determine the impact of resource availability on the competitive relationship between an invasive and a native species, we examined interactions between groups of the invasive Argentine ant (Linepithema humile) and the native odorous ant (Tapinoma sessile) over (1) food, (2) shelter or (3) both simultaneously. We further examined the mechanisms underlying the competitive relationship, asking whether aggressive interactions, exploratory behaviour or the order of arrival at a resource explained resource use. Shelter was preferred by both species when no competitors were present. In a competitive setting, L. humile groups controlled shelter through aggressive displacement but lost control over food due to investment of workers in the control of shelter. Thus, there are tradeoffs when competing over multiple resources and aggressive interactions allow invasive species to displace native species from a preferred resource.
This article is part of the theme issue ‘Intergroup conflict across taxa’.
Keywords: collective behaviour, argentine ant, Tapinoma sessile, aggression, exploration
1. Introduction
Interspecific competition is pervasive across ecological systems, influencing species range and distribution. Heterospecific competitors can impact the survival of individuals from other species by decreasing fecundity [1] or limiting access to resources [2,3]. Competition may influence the spatial [4,5] or temporal [6,7] partitioning of resources, and resource specialization leading to coexistence. Animals may displace competitors from resources [8–10], or avoid encounters with competitors [11,12].
Animals often compete over multiple resources such as food and shelter. Interspecific interactions can limit access to both food [8,13] and shelter [14]. For example, some social insects avoid interactions near food [15,16] and heterospecifics occupying high-quality nests may lead some social insects to lower-quality nests during nest relocation [17]. Studies of interspecific competition often examine interactions over a single type of resource, however, animals require more than one type of resource and may need to prioritize which resource to compete over when their competitive effort is limited. When social species interact, individuals can be allocated to different resources to facilitate the control of multiple resources simultaneously. Large groups often outcompete smaller ones in interspecific competitions [18–20]. So, when group members are spread across multiple resources, the ability to control all resources may be compromised for small groups that might not be able to defend multiple resources or even a single resource. Here, we investigate how interspecific competition between social animals is influenced by the presence of multiple types of resources compared to competition over a single resource.
Different behavioural mechanisms may underlie animals' ability to gain and control resources. Direct aggressive interactions displace competitors and reduce the likelihood of future competition [21]. Exploratory behaviour can lead to the discovery of resources [22], and resource discovery often results in resource dominance [23,24]. Still, aggressive individuals that arrive at a resource late can outcompete the individuals who discovered the resource [25]. Although aggression and exploration can play crucial roles in interspecific encounters, it is unclear how they facilitate interspecific interactions over multiple resources. Social insects provide ample opportunities for examining these open questions because of the wide range of inter-specific competition they exhibit [26].
The Argentine ant (Linepithema humile) is an invasive species throughout the world [27] that often displaces native ant species [28,29], including the odorous house ant (Tapinoma sessile), which is native to North America. Both L. humile and T. sessile live in large polygynous and polydomous colonies [30], nest in underground cavities, have monomorphic workers of similar size and exhibit individual variation in aggression [31–33] and exploration [34,35]. In one-on-one interactions, T. sessile generally outcompete L. humile, but when entire colonies compete, L. humile is typically the dominant species [31]. The outcome of competition between small groups, in which the number of individuals that can be allocated to a resource is limited, has not been examined thus far. Interactions among small groups of L. humile and T. sessile are of particular interest because both species expand their range via budding when a propagule—a small group of workers and at least one queen—leaves the nest to found a new colony [36,37]. Examining the interspecific competition among small groups of L. humile and T. sessile may reveal limits on the dispersal and spread of a highly invasive species.
Here, we examine how interspecific competition between small groups of L. humile and T. sessile workers is affected by the availability of multiple resources—food and shelter. We hypothesize that (1) interspecific competition will influence survival and resource use and predict that the ecologically dominant species (L. humile) will have higher survival and that it will control both resources, while the less dominant species (T. sessile) will have lower survival and be excluded from both resources; (2) interspecific competition over a single resource differs from interspecific competition over multiple resources simultaneously; we predict that L. humile will successfully dominate a single resource, but will only dominate one resource when two resources are provided because of an allocation-tradeoff; and (3) interspecific competition is driven by aggression, exploration or resource discovery. Based on past work on the aggression of L. humile toward other ant species [38,39], we predict that aggression will be the predominant mechanism driving interspecific competition.
2. Methods
(a) . Collection and maintenance of ants
Throughout the study, between January and June 2018, we collected approximately 3000 L. humile workers from foraging trails at the UCLA Mildred E. Mathias Botanical Gardens, CA, USA, where L. humile are the predominant ant species present (K. Neumann 2018, personal observations). Ants were collected with an aspirator every 2–3 weeks in batches of a few hundred individuals each time to avoid maintaining ants in the laboratory for long. Ants were used in an experiment within two to three weeks of collection. While different foraging trails at the botanical gardens were used for collection, they were all within 200 m of each other so they can be considered to all belong to a single effective colony [40]. We housed workers in a 5200 ml container in the laboratory and provided them with nests, water and sugar water ad libitum. Laboratory temperature was kept constant at 23°C throughout the experiments and lights were set to a 12 : 12 D : L cycle. Similarly, throughout the study we collected approximately 3000 T. sessile workers from a persistent foraging trail at a residential area in Venice, Los Angeles, CA, USA, where no L. humile are found and where T. sessile is the predominant ant species (N. Pinter-Wollman 2018, personal observations). Again, these workers were collected in small batches every few weeks to reduce the time they spent in the laboratory. Tapinoma sessile workers were kept in identical conditions to those of L. humile workers. Although foraging trails represent only a fraction of the entire colony, workers along these foraging trails vary in their individual behaviour [33,35,41]. We did not include queens or brood in the experimental groups because we could not obtain a large enough number of brood and queens when collecting ants without destroying the nests. It is possible that ants might behave differently in the presence of queens and brood; however, workers on foraging trails (without queens and brood) are the most likely to encounter other ant species and participate in interspecific competition.
(b) . Interspecific competition assays
We examined the competitive interactions between groups of L. humile and T. sessile workers in two study apparatus. To examine interspecific competition over a single resource, we connected three plastic boxes (710 ml, 12 × 12 cm each) using plastic tubes of approximately 20 cm in length, and an inner diameter of 0.75 cm (figure 1a,b and electronic supplementary material, figure S1). In one box we placed 50 L. humile workers and in a second we placed 50 T. sessile workers—both referred to as the ‘home boxes'. In the third box, which was between the two home boxes and we referred to as the ‘middle arena’ (figure 1a, electronic supplementary material, figure S1), we placed a resource (food or nest). Groups of 50 workers were selected to represent a dispersing propagule [36,37]. To test competition over multiple resources simultaneously, we used an apparatus in which four plastic boxes were connected by plastic tubes, creating two ‘middle arenas’ (figure 1b). We introduced 50 workers of each species into the two home boxes and placed food into one middle arena and a nest into the other middle arena (figure 1c,d).
Figure 1.
Study apparatus. (a) Schematic of the experimental apparatus with one resource and (b) apparatus with two resources. Each square represents a plastic box (710 ml, 12 × 12 cm) and the connecting lines represent plastic tubes, each 20 cm long with an opening diameter of 0.75 cm (photo of the one resource apparatus can be found in electronic supplementary material, figure S1). To determine resource use we counted the number of ants within the gray area near food (c) or nest (d). Both resources were provided in a glass vial. Food (sugar water) filled the vial (c), and the nest was covered in tinfoil (not shown here for visibility) and contained a wet cotton ball at the bottom for humidity. Because the ants could enter the vial when it was a nest (d) but could not enter it when it was full of food (c), the circles indicating proximity to the resource are of slightly different sizes (nest: radius = 3 cm; food: radius = 3.4 cm) to account for the use of the area inside the vial itself. Ants are for illustration purpose only and their size is not to scale. (Online version in colour.)
To allow ants to acclimate to the study apparatus, after placing the ants in the home boxes we blocked the home boxes from the middle arena(s) for 8 h using cotton balls, which plugged the plastic tubes leading into the middle arena(s). During the acclimation period, ants were provided with a nest and sugar water in their home boxes. Each nest was made of a glass vial (10 cm) with a damp cotton ball inside and a tinfoil cover to create a dark humid space. Sugar water (20% concentration) was provided in a glass vial plugged with a cotton ball to prevent the sugar water from spilling and to allow the ants access to the sugar water that wicked through the cotton. The amount of water and sugar water in the vials was substantial and did not require replacement during the acclimation period or during the experiments.
To test competition over resources (nest, food, or both) we removed either the nest or food or both from the home boxes of both species. When examining competition over food we removed the food entirely and when examining competition over a nest we only removed the tinfoil covering the home nest to minimize the amount of disruption during the experimental manipulation. When examining competition over both food and nest simultaneously, we removed both the food and the tinfoil cover from the home boxes. We performed eight replicates for each type of competition over (1) only food, (2) only nest and (3) both food and nest simultaneously. Each replicate for each trial was conducted with a different group of 50 workers; thus, we used 24 different groups of 50 workers for each species. To control for interspecific competition, and to determine resource use without a heterospecific, we repeated these three assays four more times for each species without the heterospecific present at the end of the study, after completing the treatment experiments. Again, each control trial was conducted with a different group of 50 workers, thus 12 different groups of 50 workers from each species were used in the control trials. Test apparatus was thoroughly cleaned with ethanol between trials.
(c) . Quantifying resource use
To quantify the use of resources in the arena(s), we manually counted the number of ants of each species at or near the resource three times a day (morning (6 : 00–8 : 00), afternoon (13 : 00–15 : 00) and evening (18 : 00–20 : 00)) for three consecutive days (i.e. nine observations) after removing the resource from the home box. Ants were considered to be using the resource when they were inside the nest vial, or within approximately 3 cm (1/4 of the length of the arena box) from the nest entrance, and when they were within approximately 3.4 cm from the food entrance (figure 1c,d). Because the ants could enter the vial of the nest but could not enter the food vial, we considered a slightly larger area around the entrance to the food vial to ensure that the resource use areas were of similar size for both resources (figure 1c,d). During each of the nine observations, we conducted four counts, one every 5 min, with each count lasting approximately 1 min. We summed these four counts to obtain a single value of the number of ants of each species using the resource in the arena for each of the nine observation periods in each of the eight replications. Some ants died over the course of the 3 days, therefore we counted the total number of ants alive of each species in each observation period to examine survival.
(d) . Statistical analysis of survival and resource use
To determine whether survival was impacted by the presence of a competitor and the number of resources available, we examined the change in the number of ants alive of each species over the course of our experiment (nine observations over 3 days). We ran a Cox proportional hazard regression model using the R package ‘coxme’ [42] in which the number of ants alive was the response variable and species (L. humile or T. sessile), type of resource(s) (food, nest, or both) and heterospecific presence/absence, i.e. trial type (experimental or control) were the explanatory variables. We included a three-way interaction term between species × type of resource(s) × trial type. For this and all following models, we performed post hoc tests for significant interaction terms with more than two levels using the emmeans() function in the R package ‘emmeans’ [43]. For interactions involving a continuous and categorical variable(s), we used the function emtrends(). We computed the marginal and conditional R2 using the R package ‘MuMIn’ [44].
To determine whether the use of resources changed over time, differed between the two species, resource types (food or nest), the number of resources (one or two), and was affected by the presence of a conspecific (experimental or control), we fit a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) using a Poisson distribution. The number of workers at a resource was the response variable, and species, resource type (food or nest), number of resources (one or two), trial type (experiment or control) and time were the explanatory variables. We included group ID and starting date of the trial as random effects to account for the use of repeated measures of groups over the 3 days of the trial and any potential effects of seasonality (date). To account for different trends for the various effects, we further included two three-way interactions: trial type × resource × species and time × resource × species. The model was fit with maximum-likelihood (Laplace approximation) and implemented using the glmer() function from the ‘lme4' package [45]. The Anova() function from the ‘car’ package [46] was used for analysis of deviance to determine the confidence of our estimates using a Type II analysis of variance. All statistical analyses were conducted in R v. 4.0.2 [47].
(e) . Mechanisms of competition
To determine the behavioural mechanism underlying resource control, we observed aggressive interactions during the competition assays, noted which species arrived at the resource first and compared the exploratory behaviour of the two species. First, to determine whether aggressive behaviour had an effect on resource use, we counted the number of aggressive interactions at the resource(s) in the arena(s) during each of the nine observations. We recorded an aggressive interaction if we observed locking mandibles, biting, fighting, limb detachment, prolonged fighting and killing, as detailed in Neumann & Pinter-Wollman [33]. Because most aggressive interactions are brief, we used the total number of aggressive interactions (rather than the average) from the four 5 min samples in each of the nine observation periods, i.e. a total of 20 min in each observation period. We analysed the aggressive behaviour using a GLMM with a Poisson distribution, fit by maximum-likelihood (Laplace approximation), with the number of aggressive interactions as the response variable, resource type (food or nest), number of resources (one or two) and time as fixed effects, and group ID and date of trial as random effects. The model was implemented using the glmer() function from the ‘lme4' package [45] and the Anova() function from the ‘car’ package [46] to determine the confidence of our estimates using a Type II analysis of variance.
To determine whether the order of arrival at a resource, i.e. resource discovery, influenced resource use, we compared resource use, i.e. the number of workers at the resource(s) at the end of the three days, between trials in which L. humile arrived at the resource first and trials in which T. sessile arrived at the resource first. We used a GLMM with a Poisson distribution for count data, with final number of workers at a resource as the response variable, and species, number of resources (one or two), resource type (food or nest) and discoverer of resource (L. humile or T. sessile first at the resource) as fixed effects, and starting date of trial as a random effect. Group ID was not included as a random effect because this model only compares a single observation for each group—the last one.
Finally, to examine whether the two species differed in their exploratory behaviour, which might influence who discovered the resource first, we quantified the exploratory behaviour of 20 individual workers of each species that were not used in any of the other assays in an 8-armed maze with a novel spice at the end of each arm, as in Hui & Pinter-Wollman [35] and Page et al. [41]. To quantify exploratory behaviour, we recorded the total number of visits made by a worker to any arm of the maze over a 5 min period. Each worker was tested once because this assay provides an accurate and quick estimate of exploration that is repeatable over time and across situations, such as an open field test [41]. We then used a Wilcoxon sum rank test to compare the exploratory behaviour of L. humile and T. sessile workers.
3. Results
The number of ants alive decreased over time and the presence of a competitor reduced the survival of both species in all three treatments. For both species, workers were more likely to die in the presence of heterospecifics than in the absence of heterospecifics (Cox proportional hazard regression, trial type: χ2 = 397.80, p < 0.0001, n = 36; figure 2). Tapinoma sessile had a greater survival rate than L. humile (χ2 = 9.97, p < 0.01, n = 36; figure 2). Specifically, there was a significant interaction between species and treatment (χ2 = 11.12, p < 0.01, n = 36; figure 2), with T. sessile having increased survival in the experimental nest treatment (blue versus red triangles in lower half of figure 2; post hoc: z ratio = 3.61, p = 0.016). For full output of this and all following models, see electronic supplementary materials.
Figure 2.
Survival of ants by species and study apparatus. Average proportion of ants surviving, relative to starting number (50 ants) at each of the nine observations over three days in experimental groups (dashed lines, N = 8) and control groups—when each species was alone in the apparatus (solid lines, N = 4). Blue lines are for T. sessile and red lines are for L. humile; different shades of blue and red and different shapes are for the different study apparatus (only nest: light blue/light red, triangles; only food: dark blue/dark red, circles; both resources presented: medium blue/medium red, squares). (Online version in colour.)
Each species controlled a different resource (GLMM: resource type × species × trial type: χ2 = 913.34, p < 0.0001, n = 36; figure 3a,b). Specifically, in the experimental groups, L. humile had significantly more workers at nests (post hoc: z ratio = 33.58, p < 0.0001), while T. sessile had significantly more workers at food (post hoc: z ratio = −28.35, p < 0.0001), regardless of the number of resources present (GLMM: number of resources: χ2 = 0.44, p = 0.50, n = 36). Over time, the number of ants at each resource changed (GLMM: time: χ2 = 18.85, p < 0.001, n = 36) and this effect differed depending on the species and resource type (GLMM: time × species × resource type: χ2 = 213.23, p < 0.0001, n = 36; figure 3a,b). Specifically, the use of nests increased over time in L. humile (up-selected emtrend = 0.27) and decreased over time in T. sessile (down-selected emtrend = −0.06) and there was a significant difference between these trends (post hoc: z ratio = 10.44, p < 0.0001). Similarly, the use of food increased over time in T. sessile (up-selected emtrend = 0.16) and decreased over time in L. humile (down-selected emtrend = −0.11) and there was a significant difference between these trends (post hoc: z ratio = −12.37, p < 0.0001).
Figure 3.
Use of resources over time. Number of L. humile workers (red) and T. sessile workers (blue) at the (a) nest and (b) food across the 3 days of the experiment (nine observations) (N = 8 replicates per treatment (food, nest or both)). Here and in following similar plots, data points are slightly jittered along the x-axis to improve visibility. Lines are the default loess fit in ggplot2 and shaded areas around the lines represent the confidence interval of the fit. (Online version in colour.)
The presence of a heterospecific altered the use of resource(s) for both species. Overall, there were significantly more workers at a resource in the control trials, when no heterospecific was present (GLMM: trial type: χ2 = 18.61, p < 0.0001, n = 36; figure 4a,b), for both species (GLMM: trial type × species: χ2 = 2.03, p = 0.15, n = 36; electronic supplementary material, table S3a). However, species’ use of resources was impacted differently by the presence of a heterospecific (GLMM: trial type × resource type × species: χ2 = 913.34, p < 0.0001, n = 36). Specifically, in the presence of T. sessile, L. humile decreased their use of food (post hoc: z ratio = 6.78, p < 0.001; solid versus dashed teal lines in figure 4a), while in the presence of L. humile, T. sessile decreased their use of the nest (post hoc: z ratio = 9.09, p < 0.0001; solid versus dashed purple lines in figure 4b). Interestingly, there was a significant interaction between resource type and trial type (GLMM: trial type × resource type: χ2 = 179.63, p < 0.0001, n = 36). Specifically, there were significantly more workers at the nest than at the food in the control groups (post hoc: z ratio = −10.84, p < 0.001), suggesting that nests were the preferred resource for both species. The fixed effects of this model explained 52% of the variance (marginal R2) and the entire model explained 93% of the variance (conditional R2).
Figure 4.
Effect of heterospecifics on resource use. Number of workers of L. humile (a) and T. sessile (b) at a resource (nest: purple; food: teal) for the different trial types (experimental: solid lines and darker colour shades; control: dashed lines and lighter colour shades). (Online version in colour.)
The most likely behavioural mechanism driving the control of resources was aggression. There were significantly more aggressive interactions between L. humile and T. sessile at the nest than at the food (GLMM: resource type: χ2 = 372.14, p < 0.0001, n = 24; figure 5a) and this pattern was not impacted by the number of resources (GLMM: number of resources: χ2 = 0.022, p = 0.88, n = 24). Aggression decreased over time (GLMM: time: χ2 = 55.63, p < 0.0001, n = 24). The fixed effects of this model explained 59% of the variance (marginal R2) and the entire model explained 71% of the variance (conditional R2).
Figure 5.
Mechanisms of resource use. (a) Number of aggressive interactions between L. humile and T. sessile at food (teal) and nests (purple) over time. (b) Exploratory behaviour of individual L. humile (red) and T. sessile (blue) workers (N = 20 each), quantified as the number of visits made to novel spices in an eight-armed maze. Number of workers at the nest (c) or food (d) on the last observation of the experiment, when either L. humile (red) or T. sessile (blue) discovered the resource first. (Online version in colour.)
We did not find evidence supporting that the order of resource discovery or exploratory behaviour are likely mechanisms of competition. The number of workers at a resource at the end of the trial (final observation on day 3) was not significantly impacted by which species discovered the resource first (GLMM: species to discover resource first: χ2 = 1.44, p = 0.23, n = 24; figure 5c,d), nor was it impacted by resource type (GLMM: resource type: χ2 = 0.18, p = 0.67, n = 24; figure 5c,d) or the number of resources (GLMM: number of resources: χ2 = 2.31, p = 0.13, n = 24; figure 5c,d). The fixed effects of this model explained 11% of the variance (marginal R2) and the entire model explained 39% of the variance (conditional R2). Finally, we did not detect a significant difference in the exploratory behaviour of L. humile and T. sessile workers. Individuals from both species made a similar number of visits to the maze arms during the exploration trials (Wilcoxon rank sum test: W = 245, p = 0.22, n = 40; figure 5b).
4. Discussion
We found that interspecific competition affected survival, and that species differed in the resource they prioritized and in their influence on one another. When a competitor was present, survival was lower than when a competitor was absent (figure 2). The two species differed in their use of resources, with L. humile controlling nests (figure 3a) and T. sessile controlling food (figure 3b). The presence of L. humile significantly reduced the number of T. sessile at nests (figure 4a), while the presence of T. sessile significantly reduced the number of L. humile at food (figure 4b). Aggressive interactions likely drove the interspecific competition (figure 5a), rather than resource discovery (figure 5c,d) or exploratory behaviour (figure 5b).
Interspecific competition reduced survival overall. However, interestingly, T. sessile had higher survival rates than L. humile when competing over nests. In previous work, when colonies of 500 L. humile and T. sessile workers competed, L. humile had higher survival rates and they controlled both food and nests [31]. However, in our work with smaller groups of 50 workers, T. sessile fared better in terms of survival, which could have important implications for interspecific interactions during dispersal by propagules that can have as few as ten workers [36]. It is possible that small groups might not be able to control multiple resources simultaneously. Future work might consider whether the presence of a queen and brood influences competition and whether there is an optimal propagule size that allows both successful establishment when faced with interspecific competition and minimal investment by the colony in the number of dispersing individuals [48].
The patterns of worker distribution and aggression that we observed suggest that nests were preferred over food. Both species had more workers at nests than at food when the heterospecific was absent and just one resource was provided (figure 4a,b) and aggressive behaviour was more frequent near nests than near food (figure 5a). Furthermore, the ecologically dominant species, L. humile [28], controlled the nests when both species were present (figure 3a). The control of food by T. sessile could have resulted from ecological release [49], which occurred when the dominant L. humile expended workers to defend the nest, leaving T. sessile to forage freely at the food source. The release of T. sessile from competition at the food may have contributed to their increased survival when food was the only resource available (figure 2). Alternatively, it is possible that their higher survival rates allowed them to control the food source. Finally, because in urban environments that L. humile and T. sessile occupy, ants can exploit human food waste for sustenance [50], future work on competition in different ecological environments might reveal if the preference we observed for nests over food changes when food is a scarce resource.
Our results comparing the resource use of each species in the presence of the heterospecifics to resource use in the control trials, when heterospecifics are absent, suggest a potential allocation–control trade-off. Under the allocation–control trade-off, groups either allocate a small number of workers to all resources at the risk of not being able to control any single resource, or allocate many workers to a single resource and control it, at the risk of not obtaining enough of the other resource(s). The finding that both species reduce the use of one resource in experimental trials (L. humile reducing use of food and T. sessile reducing use of nests; figure 4a,b) suggests that both species were impacted by this trade-off. The focus of L. humile on nests and of T. sessile on food could be explained by competitive exclusion or by different physiological needs. Indeed, nutritional deficiencies of an ant colony can affect worker foraging decisions [51]. However, we show here that both a need for another resource simultaneously (like a nest) and the presence of another species may influence such decisions. Future work could examine how physiological constraints, such as starvation, further influence resource preferences.
Aggressive interactions between heterospecifics drove the competitive dynamics we observed more than difference between the two species in resource discovery or exploratory behaviour (figure 5). There were fewer aggressive encounters near the food, perhaps because workers at the food were hungry and sucrose deprivation decreases aggressive behaviour in L. humile [52], or because workers had just eaten and were not motivated to fight. These patterns of aggressive behaviour suggest that aggression may depend not only on species identity but also on the abundance of different resources in the environment.
The order of resource discovery and exploratory behaviour did not seem to have a significant impact on the interspecific competitive outcomes we observed. It is possible that discovering the resources was not a challenge to either species because the apparatus were small. Perhaps in a larger or more spatially convoluted setting we would have detected an effect of resource discovery order on resource control. Although competing species can differ in exploratory behaviour [24,53–56], we did not detect such a difference here. The lack of difference in exploratory behaviour could be explained by the high variation among individual workers in exploratory behaviour in both species. This variation can influence the ability of L. humile colonies to find a suitable nest [35] or food [41]. Thus, future work might consider how the behavioural composition of a group influences its competitive interactions [33,57]. The groups of workers in our study were competing in the absence of a queen or brood. It is possible that the presence of queens or brood would have resulted in different outcomes that could be examined in future work. For example, it is possible that the presence of queen and brood would lead to more aggressive behaviour by workers, especially when competing over shelter, because of their importance for reproductive success. It is also possible that interspecific interactions would persist for longer in the presence of brood and queen. For example, T. sessile workers might have continued to fight for the nest, rather than shift toward using the food (figure 3) if there were a queen and brood present to ensure their protection. Group composition is important for determining intergroup interactions and certain individuals (like a queen) may have a disproportionate impact on the dynamics and outcome of intergroup conflict [58,59]. Future research should explore whether there is a difference in resource use and competition when a queen (or queens) and brood are present.
To conclude, our findings suggest that identifying the causes and consequences of interspecific competition between social species requires consideration of environmental, behavioural and temporal effects. The result of a competitive scenario over one resource might not necessarily indicate how competitors will behave in the presence of other resources, other species, or on different temporal or spatial scales.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Peter Nonacs, Julie Miller, Gabby Najm, Natalie Lemanski, Ana Rubio, Sean O'Fallon and two anonymous reviewers for helpful suggestions on previous versions of this manuscript.
Data accessibility
Data and code can be found on Figshare:
Authors' contributions
K.N.: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, visualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; N.P.-W.: conceptualization, formal analysis, funding acquisition, investigation, project administration, resources, supervision, validation, visualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing.
Both authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.
Competing interests
We declare that we have no conflict of interest.
Funding
This work was partially supported by the NFS IOS grant no. 1456010 to N.P.-W.
References
- 1.Gurnell J, Wauters LA, Lurz PWW, Tosi G. 2004. Alien species and interspecific competition: effects of introduced eastern grey squirrels on red squirrel population dynamics. J. Anim. Ecol. 73, 26-35. ( 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2004.00791.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Blaustein AR. 1980. Behavioral aspects of competition in a three-species rodent guild of coastal southern California. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 6, 247-255. ( 10.1007/BF00569206) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Knight TM, Chase JM, Goss CW, Knight JJ. 2004. Effects of interspecific competition, predation, and their interaction on survival and development time of immature Anopheles quadrimaculatus. J. Vector Ecol. 29, 277-284. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Albrecht M, Gotelli NJ. 2001. Spatial and temporal niche partitioning in grassland ants. Oecologia 126, 134-141. ( 10.1007/s004420000494) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kneitel JM, Chase JM. 2004. Trade-offs in community ecology: linking spatial scales and species coexistence. Ecol. Lett. 7, 69-80. ( 10.1046/j.1461-0248.2003.00551.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kronfeld-Schor N, Dayan T. 1999. The dietary basis for temporal partitioning: food habits of coexisting Acomys species. Oecologia 121, 123-128. ( 10.1007/s004420050913) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kitchen AM, Gese EM, Schauster ER. 1999. Resource partitioning between coyotes and swift foxes: space, time, and diet. Can. J. Zool. 77, 1645-1656. ( 10.1139/z99-143) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Tannerfeldt M, Elmhagen B, Angerbjörn A. 2002. Exclusion by interference competition? The relationship between red and arctic foxes. Oecologia 132, 213-220. ( 10.1007/s00442-002-0967-8) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Berger KM, Gese EM. 2007. Does interference competition with wolves limit the distribution and abundance of coyotes? J. Anim. Ecol. 76, 1075-1085. ( 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2007.01287.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Brown M, Steinitz R, Emery Thompson M. 2022. Wins and losses in intergroup conflicts reflect energy balance in red-tailed monkeys. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210152. ( 10.98/rstb.2021.0152) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Alatalo RV, Eriksson D, Gustafsson L, Larsson K. 1987. Exploitation competition influences the use of foraging sites by tits: experimental evidence. Ecology 68, 284-290. ( 10.2307/1939259) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Yoshimoto J. 2009. Interspecific variation in competitor avoidance and foraging success in sap-attracted insects. Eur. J. Entomol. 106, 529-533. ( 10.14411/eje.2009.066) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Davidson DW, Samson DA, Inouye RS. 1985. Granivory in the Chihuahuan Desert: interactions within and between trophic levels. Ecology 66, 486-502. ( 10.2307/1940397) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Cole NC, Jones CG, Harris S. 2005. The need for enemy-free space: the impact of an invasive gecko on island endemics. Biol. Conserv. 125, 467-474. ( 10.1016/j.biocon.2005.04.017) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Vonshak M, Dayan T, Hefetz A. 2012. Interspecific displacement mechanisms by the invasive little fire ant Wasmannia auropunctata. Biol. Invasions 14, 851-861. ( 10.1007/s10530-011-0122-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Rogers SR, Cajamarca P, Tarpy DR, Burrack HJ. 2013. Honey bees and bumble bees respond differently to inter- and intra-specific encounters. Apidologie 44, 621-629. ( 10.1007/s13592-013-0210-0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.McGlynn TP. 2012. The ecology of nest movement in social insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 57, 291-308. ( 10.1146/annurev-ento-120710-100708) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Adams ES, Mesterton-Gibbons M. 2003. Lanchester's attrition models and fights among social animals. Behav. Ecol. 14, 719-723. ( 10.1093/beheco/arg061) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Majolo B, de Bortoli Vizioli A, Schino G. 2008. Costs and benefits of group living in primates: group size effects on behaviour and demography. Anim. Behav. 76, 1235-1247. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.008) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Quirici V, Faugeron S, Hayes LD, Ebensperger LA. 2011. The influence of group size on natal dispersal in the communally rearing and semifossorial rodent, Octodon degus. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65, 787-798. ( 10.1007/s00265-010-1082-1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Peiman KS, Robinson BW. 2010. Ecology and evolution of resource-related heterospecific aggression. Q. Rev. Biol. 85, 133-158. ( 10.1086/652374) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Dingemanse NJ, de Goede P. 2004. The relation between dominance and exploratory behavior is context-dependent in wild great tits. Behav. Ecol. 15, 1023-1030. ( 10.1093/beheco/arh115) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Smith JM, Parker GA. 1976. The logic of asymmetric contests. Anim. Behav. 24, 159-175. ( 10.1016/S0003-3472(76)80110-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Davidson DW. 1998. Resource discovery versus resource domination in ants: a functional mechanism for breaking the trade-off. Ecol. Entomol. 23, 484-490. ( 10.1046/j.1365-2311.1998.00145.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Nagamitsu T, Inoue T. 1997. Aggressive foraging of social bees as a mechanism of floral resource partitioning in an Asian tropical rainforest. Oecologia 110, 432-439. ( 10.1007/s004420050178) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Rodrigues AMM, Barker JL, Robinson EJH. 2022. From inter-group conflict to inter-group cooperation: insights from social insects. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210466. ( 10.1098/srtb.2021.0466) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Suarez AV, Holway DA, Case TJ. 2001. Patterns of spread in biological invasions dominated by long-distance jump dispersal: insights from Argentine ants. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 1095-1100. ( 10.1073/pnas.98.3.1095) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Holway DA. 1999. Competitive mechanisms underlying the displacement of native ants by the invasive Argentine ant. Ecology 80, 238-251. ( 10.1890/0012-9658(1999)080[0238:CMUTDO]2.0.CO;2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Tillberg CV, Holway DA, Lebrun EG, Suarez AV. 2007. Trophic ecology of invasive Argentine ants in their native and introduced ranges. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104, 20 856-20 861. ( 10.1073/pnas.0706903105) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Holway DA, Suarez AV. 2006. Homogenization of ant communities in mediterranean California: the effects of urbanization and invasion. Biol. Conserv. 127, 319-326. ( 10.1016/j.biocon.2005.05.016) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Buczkowski G, Bennett GW. 2008. Aggressive interactions between the introduced Argentine ant, Linepithema humile and the native odorous house ant, Tapinoma sessile. Biol. Invasions. 10, 1001-1011. ( 10.1007/s10530-007-9179-9) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Salyer A, Bennett GW, Buczkowski GA. 2014. Odorous house ants (Tapinoma sessile) as back-seat drivers of localized ant decline in urban habitats. PLoS ONE 9, e113878. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0113878) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Neumann KM, Pinter-Wollman N. 2019. Collective responses to heterospecifics emerge from individual differences in aggression. Behav. Ecol. 30, 801-808. ( 10.1093/beheco/arz017) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Scholes DR, Suarez AV. 2009. Speed-versus-accuracy trade-offs during nest relocation in Argentine ants (Linepithema humile) and odorous house ants (Tapinoma sessile). Insectes Soc. 56, 413-418. ( 10.1007/s00040-009-0039-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Hui A, Pinter-Wollman N. 2014. Individual variation in exploratory behaviour improves speed and accuracy of collective nest selection by Argentine ants. Anim. Behav. 93, 261-266. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.05.006) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hee JJ, Holway DA, Suarez AV, Case TJ. 2000. Role of propagule size in the success of incipient colonies of the invasive Argentine ant. Conserv. Biol. 14, 559-563. ( 10.1046/j.1523-1739.2000.99040.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Kimball CP. 2016. Independent colony foundation in Tapinoma sessile of Northcentral Colorado. Entomol. News 126, 83-86. ( 10.3157/021.126.0203) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Suarez AV, Holway DA, Liang DS, Tsutsui ND, Case TJ. 2002. Spatiotemporal patterns of intraspecific aggression in the invasive Argentine ant. Anim. Behav. 64, 697-708. ( 10.1006/anbe.2002.4011) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Human KG, Gordon DM. 1999. Behavioral interactions of the invasive Argentine ant with native ant species. Insectes Soc. 46, 159-163. ( 10.1007/s000400050127) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Heller NE, Ingram KK, Gordon DM. 2008. Nest connectivity and colony structure in unicolonial Argentine ants. Insectes Soc. 55, 397-403. ( 10.1007/s00040-008-1019-0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Page H, Sweeney A, Pilko A, Pinter-Wollman N. 2018. Underlying mechanisms and ecological context of variation in exploratory behavior of the Argentine ant, Linepithema humile. J. Exp. Biol. 221, jeb188722. ( 10.1101/369132) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Therneau TM. 2020. coxme: Mixed Effects Cox Models. R Package. See https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/coxme/index.html.
- 43.Lenth RV. 2022. emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means. R package version 172. See https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans.
- 44.Barton K. 2022. MuMIn: Multi-Model Inference. R package version 14317. See https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=MuMIn.
- 45.Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1-48. ( 10.18637/jss.v067.i01) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Fox J, Weisberg S. 2019. An R companion to applied regression, 3rd edn. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
- 47.R Core T. 2014. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. See http://www.R-project.org/. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Hendry AP, Day T, Cooper AB. 2001. Optimal size and number of propagules: allowance for discrete stages and effects of maternal size on reproductive output and offspring fitness. Am. Nat. 157, 387-407. ( 10.1086/319316) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bolnick DI, Ingram T, Stutz WE, Snowberg LK, Lau OL, Paull JS. 2010. Ecological release from interspecific competition leads to decoupled changes in population and individual niche width. Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 1789-1797. ( 10.1098/rspb.2010.0018) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Penick Clint A, Savage Amy M, Dunn Robert R. 2015. Stable isotopes reveal links between human food inputs and urban ant diets. Proc. R. Soc. B. 282, 20142608. ( 10.1098/rspb.2014.2608) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Dussutour A, Simpson SJ. 2008. Description of a simple synthetic diet for studying nutritional responses in ants. Insectes Soc. 55, 329-333. ( 10.1007/s00040-008-1008-3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Grover CD, Kay AD, Monson JA, Marsh TC, Holway DA. 2007. Linking nutrition and behavioural dominance: carbohydrate scarcity limits aggression and activity in Argentine ants. Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 2951-2957. ( 10.1098/rspb.2007.1065) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Mettke-Hofmann C, Winkler H, Leisler B. 2002. The significance of ecological factors for exploration and neophobia in parrots. Ethology 108, 249-272. ( 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2002.00773.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Sih A, Bell A, Johnson JC. 2004. Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends Ecol. Evol. 19, 372-378. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2004.04.009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Saar M, Subach A, Reato I, Liber T, Pruitt JN, Scharf I. 2018. Consistent differences in foraging behavior in 2 sympatric harvester ant species may facilitate coexistence. Curr. Zool. 64, 653-661. ( 10.1093/cz/zox054) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Cerda X, Retana J. 1994. Food exploitation patterns of two sympatric seed-harvesting ants Messor bouvieri (Bond.) and Messor capitatus (Latr.) (Hym., Formicidae) from Spain. J. Appl. Entomol. 117, 268-277. ( 10.1111/j.1439-0418.1994.tb00735.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Pinter-Wollman N. 2012. Personality in social insects: how does worker personality determine colony personality? Curr. Zool. 58, 580-588. ( 10.1093/czoolo/58.4.580) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Smith JE, Fichtel C, Holmes RK, Kappeler PM, van Vugt M, Jaeggi AV. 2022. Sex bias in intergroup conflict and collective movements among social mammals: male warriors and female guides. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210142. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0142) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Glowacki L, McDermott R. 2022. Key individuals catalyze intergroup violence. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210141. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0141) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Data and code can be found on Figshare: