Abstract
Across vertebrate species, intergroup conflict confronts individuals with a tension between group interests best served by participation in conflict and personal interest best served by not participating. Here, we identify the neurohormone oxytocin as pivotal to the neurobiological regulation of this tension in distinctly different group-living vertebrates, including fishes, birds, rodents, non-human primates and humans. In the context of intergroup conflict, a review of emerging work on pro-sociality suggests that oxytocin and its fish and birds homologues, isotocin and mesotocin, respectively, can elicit participation in group conflict and aggression. This is because it amplifies (i) concern for the interests of genetically related or culturally similar ‘in-group’ others and (ii) willingness to defend against outside intruders and enemy conspecifics. Across a range of social vertebrates, oxytocin can induce aggressive behaviour to ‘tend-and-defend’ the in-group during intergroup contests.
This article is part of the theme issue ‘Intergroup conflict across taxa’.
Keywords: parochial altruism, in-group, out-group, neuromodulation, decision-making, vertebrates
1. Introduction
Interactions between groups of conspecifics can be cooperative and benign but also hostile, for example, when groups compete for (access to) food, mating opportunities and territory [1,2]. Moreover, across species and all else equal, groups are more likely to be victorious when their members contribute to the collective aggression of rivalling other groups and prevent defeat when they contribute to the collective defence against enemy attacks [3,4]; and yet, joining conflict requires investing personal resources and increases the risk of injury. Participating in out-group aggression and in-group defence thus requires individuals to solve a tension between personal interests on the one hand and group interests on the other [4–7].
The tension between personal interests, served by withholding participation in conflict, and group interests served by pro-actively contributing, is seen in several species across taxa [1,7]. Perhaps there are evolutionary preserved biological mechanisms that regulate individual participation in intergroup conflict. Here, we examine this possibility at the neurobiological level by focusing on the role of oxytocin (and its homologues isotocin and mesotocin [8,9]) in regulating key parameters underlying conflict participation. We uncover a remarkable cross-species commonality in how isotocin in social fishes, mesotocin in gregarious birds, and oxytocin in group-living mammals biologically prepares for a ‘tend-and-defend’ response during intergroup conflict and not, or less so, for the aggressive subordination and exploitation of rivalling groups of conspecifics.
2. Evolution and neurobiology of oxytocin
Oxytocin is a nine-amino acid peptide (i.e. nonapeptide) synthesised primarily in the brain. It can act centrally as a neuromodulator and/or peripherally as a hormone [9]. Across taxa and species, the mammalian oxytocin has several homologues, such as ‘isotocin’ in bony fishes [10], ‘mesotocin’ in nonmammalian tetrapods (lungfish, amphibians, reptiles and birds) [11], and up to five structural variants of oxytocin recently sequenced in new world monkeys [12] (figure 1).
Figure 1.
Oxytocin amino acids sequences across taxa/species. A simplified phylogenetic tree shows oxytocin sequence in different species and taxa with the common ancestors of oxytocin and vasopressin in invertebrates. To differentiate oxytocins in mammals, the variant amino acid and its position are indicated as a prefix. The leu8-oxytocin is taken here as a reference to see which amino acid(s) differ from this structure, where non-matching amino acids are colour coded. NWM refers to new world monkeys (e.g. marmosets, spider monkeys, capuchin monkeys, etc.). Nonapeptides sequences are from [8,12]. Illustrations by Z. Triki.
Oxytocin can thus be viewed as an ancient peptide widely preserved across taxa. It shares common ancestors with another nonapeptide, ‘vasopressin’, that can be traced back all the way to snails and insects (figure 1). However, not all insects have this nonapeptide gene ancestor, such as silkworms, fruit flies, mosquitos, spiders and honeybees, suggesting a potential loss of such genes [13]. The vertebrates witnessed the emergence of oxytocin and its sister nonapeptide vasopressin (and their homologues) about 500 million years ago through gene duplication of a common ancestral gene, presumably in jawless fishes [8]. Since then, the oxytocin structure has remained highly preserved, from bony fishes to mammals, where the structural differences between oxytocin and its homologues and the recently discovered mammalian variants occur in one or two amino acids (figure 1). These differences notwithstanding, oxytocin variants and homologues thus share important structural and functional elements. For readability, we use here the nomenclature ‘oxytocin’ for all mammalian oxytocins, isotocin and mesotocin [14].
In all vertebrates, oxytocin is synthesised mainly in the magnocellular and parvocellular hypothalamic neurons. From here, oxytocin can be released centrally or relayed to the posterior pituitary gland, where oxytocin is released into the bloodstream and eventually cleared out in other fluids such as saliva and urine (box 1). In teleost and amphibians, the hypothalamic parvocellular and magnocellular neurons are located in the preoptic area and anterior hypothalamus. In other vertebrates, such as reptiles, birds and mammals, rather two separate nuclei, the paraventricular and supraoptic nuclei, harbour the oxytocin neurons (for further details, see [50]).
Box 1. Measuring and manipulating oxytocin in social vertebrates.
The box summarises the most common techniques used in biology to manipulate and measure oxytocin levels across taxa. To study causal effects, several methods exist to manipulate oxytocin (left panel). Non-invasive intranasal administration of oxytocin or an antagonist is commonly used in (non-)human primates. Invasive techniques include injections and intracerebroventricular infusions and are more commonly used in small mammals, fishes, and birds. Finally, causality is studied by comparing models with versus without intact oxytocin circuitry (i.e. knockdown (out) models). To examine correlations between naturally occurring oxytocin release and behaviour, central oxytocin is obtained from cerebrospinal fluid or directly from the brain, and peripheral oxytocin can be obtained from blood plasma, saliva, or urine (top right panel). Various assaying techniques exist to detect the presence of the nonapeptide in the sample (bottom right panel). For further details on the different techniques' accuracy and validity and the extent to which endogenous/exogenous oxytocin levels can be an informative tool in behavioural studies, please see [15–22].
manipulating (oxytocin agonist/antagonist) | measuring | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
non-invasive | nasal spray | nebuliser | nasal drops | central | cerebrospinal fluid | microdialysis | brain harvesting |
primates [23] | primates [24] | rodents [25] | primates [26] | rodents [25] | rodents [27] | ||
birds [28] | rodents [29] | birds [30] | |||||
fishes [31] | |||||||
invasive | intravenous/intra-muscular/intraperitoneal | intracerebroventricular infusion | peripheral | blood plasma | saliva | urine | |
primates [32] | primates [33] | primates [26] | primates [23] | primates [34] | |||
rodents [25] | rodents [35] | carnivores [36] | birds [37] | carnivores [38] | |||
birds [30] | birds [39] | rodents [25] | |||||
fishes [40] | fishes [41] | birds [42] | |||||
fishes [43] | |||||||
genetic | knockout models | knock-down models | behavioural phenotypes (selection experiments) | assay | |||
rodents [44] | rodents [27] | rodents [45] | enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) [23] | ||||
birds [46] | radioimmunoassay (RIA) [19] | ||||||
liquid chromatography-mass spectrometry (LC-MS) [47] | |||||||
immunohistochemistry (IHC) [48] | |||||||
mRNA quantification [49] |
Upon its release from neuronal soma, axons and dendrites, oxytocin exerts widespread effects in the brain via an oxytocin-specific G protein-coupled receptor [9]. Oxytocin binding on this receptor activates a set of signalling cascades that can quickly modulate the evolutionary ancient and structurally and functionally preserved social decision-making network in the vertebrate brain [51,52]. This network includes various brain nuclei known for their crucial roles in regulating social recognition, affiliation and parental behaviour, responses to social stressors and aggression [2,52–54].
3. Oxytocin and participation in group conflict
There is growing evidence that in a range of species, oxytocin plays a significant role in forming and solidifying social structures (e.g. [15, 55]. In particular, affiliation among conspecifics is often associated with higher oxytocin levels. For example, studies that use oxytocin levels from blood plasma, urine or saliva as an informative tool on central oxytocin release, have recorded elevated oxytocin following affiliative touch [56–58] and cooperative exchange [59–61] in mammalian species such as (human) primates and dogs. Also, strongly bonded marmoset monkeys showed synchronized fluctuations of oxytocin over a six-week period [62] (also see [63]). Similar positive effects of affiliation on oxytocin levels are found in gregarious birds [64], lizards [65] and fishes [31]. Other work observed links between oxytocin levels in distinct brain regions on the one hand, and a range of social behaviours on the other, including suckling (in rats and sheep [66–68] and mating (e.g. in voles [69]).
At first blush, the mutually reinforcing relationship between affiliation and oxytocin may appear antagonistic to the possibility that oxytocin prepares individuals for participation in hostile group conflict with conspecifics. However, for group conflict to be won, or not lost, individuals within rivalling groups need to contribute to their group's fighting capacity at some personal cost (figure 2) (also see [2,5–7,70,71]). Making such costly contributions serves the group and can thus be seen as a form of pro-social behaviour towards one's in-group. Indeed, as we [2,72] and others (e.g. [1,5,7]) have argued and shown, in many group-living species an individual's conflict participation pi is a function of concern for in-group (henceforth αI) and out-group interests (henceforth αO), expected out-group threat (henceforth β), and compliance with group norms for participation (viz. reputation concerns; henceforth γ (see also [73]. If we set each parameter to vary between −1 and 1 inclusive, participation likelihood increases when there is a positive concern for in-group interests (αI > 0), negative concern for out-group interests (αO < 0), perceived out-group threat (β > 0) or when the animal expects participation returns reputation benefits (γ > 0) [1,2,4,73]. This means that participation can be expected when and because oxytocin increases (i) in-group concern αI, and/or (ii) creates negative out-group concern (αO < 0), and/or (iii) increases perceived out-group threat β, and/or (iv) increases expectation of reciprocity and reputation benefits from participating (γ). In the remainder of this section, we examine the evidence for the role of oxytocin on each of these parameters underlying participation in conflict (also see [68,74–77]) (box 2).
Figure 2.
Intergroup conflict as a multilevel contest game of strategy. (a) Individuals nested in two groups (circle: ai … an and square: bi … bn) can contribute personal resources (e.g. skills, time and energy) to their group's capacity for out-group attack A; red) and/or to protect against enemy attacks (in-group defence D; blue). Conflict participation is risky–the individual may get injured–and resources contributed are ‘wasted’. (b) Conflict participation increases the likelihood of victory with concomitant ‘spoils of war’ (a win/lose outcome; top panel), and of surviving out-group attacks (a stalemate outcome; bottom panel). Because (participating in) conflict is wasteful, even winning groups typically are less wealthy post-conflict.
Box 2. Inferring conflict participation parameters from vertebrate decision-making.
Concern for in-group (αI) and out-group (αO) can be inferred from behavioural choices, neural activation in, e.g. mesolimbic reward circuitry and, in humans, self-reports. In humans, social concerns can be inferred from economic decision-making games such as the Dictator Game (DG), wherein participants donate x out of an endowment e to an anonymous recipient (with 0 ≤ x ≤ e). Higher donations to in-group rather than out-group members reflect stronger concern for in-group (αI) than out-group interests (αO) [78,79]. Variants of such games have been used to infer social preferences in non-human primates [80]. In nonmammalian vertebrates, such as social fishes, social preferences are inferred from time spent in proximity of a conspecific [40] or from costly helping of a conspecific [81]. To infer expectations of reciprocity (viz. β), studies with humans used trust games. Participants can transfer x out of an endowment e to a recipient (with 0 ≤ x ≤ e). The recipient then receives 3x and can return y to the participant (with 0 ≤ y ≤ 3x). Greater transfers reflect expectations of reciprocity (or ‘trust’), and greater back-transfers reflect a willingness to reciprocate (or ‘trustworthiness’) [79]. Vice versa, expectations of competition can be inferred from partner choice, with rejecting partners who did not cooperate on earlier occasions as a measure of negative expectations (in humans[79]; in birds [82]; in fishes [83]). Finally, reputation concerns have been inferred from third-party punishment games [80], where participants, after decision-making, express through punishment social disapproval of the others' (non-cooperative) behaviour and/or induce a norm for cooperation on future trials [84]. Punishment and behavioural adjustments to (threat of) punishment are seen across social vertebrates, including chimpanzees [85] and social fishes [86].
(a) . Parochial preferences (in-group interest αI > out-group interest αO)
Studies with human participants revealed that concerns for genetically related or culturally similar conspecifics (in-group) are typically stronger than for unrelated and unfamiliar (out-group) conspecifics [78] (box 2). Oxytocin has a mechanistic role to play in such in-group biased preferences (i.e. αI > αO) [87,88]. For instance, in-group participants in a foraging game helped each other more often compared to out-group participants, a behaviour that was mediated by endogenous oxytocin (i.e. measured in saliva) [59]. Similarly, Chinese males had a frontocentral positive activity of larger amplitude in response to the pain expressions of in-group (Asian targets) but not out-group members (Caucasian targets), especially following intranasal administration of oxytocin rather than placebo [89] (also [90]).
In humans, oxytocin seems to amplify αI and neither increases nor decreases αO—oxytocin makes humans like their in-group more and does not condition (dis)liking out-groups. This was shown, for example, when human participants indicated their liking for individuals from their own nationality (i.e. Dutch citizens) and individuals from a more or less rivalling nationality (e.g. Germans). Compared to placebo-treated individuals, those given intranasal oxytocin expressed a greater liking for in-group members (an increase in αI) but did not increase or decrease their liking for out-group members (i.e. αO was similar in oxytocin and placebo conditions) [87]. Recent work on wild chimpanzees suggests that these effects may generalize to other species, including voles [91,92], sheep [93] and chimpanzees [94]). In another series of experiments with human participants, individuals were organised in two groups of three and could contribute to club goods A and B out of a personal endowment. Whereas contributions to A and B equally benefitted the members of one's own group, contributions to B (but not A) also imposed a cost on the out-group members. Intranasal oxytocin (versus placebo) increased contributions to club good A, reflecting an increase in αI. However, oxytocin neither increased nor decreased contributions to club good B, suggesting oxytocin did not affect αO [95,96] (also see [97,98]).
Although follow-up experiments in humans sometimes show that oxytocin can increase αO (e.g. [99,100]), this effect is rarely as strong as the oxytocin-induced increase on αI. This mirrors findings with non-human vertebrates. For example, marmosets treated with marmoset-specific pro8-oxytocin reduced pro-sociality towards strangers compared to those treated with saline or consensus-mammalian leu8-oxytocin [101] (see also [102]). Chimpanzees had higher urinary oxytocin concentrations before and after hostile intergroup encounters, which predicted within-group affiliative behaviours [34]. Resident male mice exhibit higher attack bites against intruders of different strains (viz. out-group) than against intruders of their own strain. Yet compared to oxytocin receptor wild-type mice, oxytocin receptor-null residents exhibited greater aggression towards intruders of their own strain, suggesting that oxytocin modulates αI more than αO [44]. In a monogamous zebra finch, affiliation towards one's partner requires the activation of the oxytocin receptor [39], while oxytocin knockdown birds and those treated with an oxytocin antagonist experienced affiliation behaviour deficit [46,64] (for similar findings in pinyon jays, see [25]). Finally, work on the mutualistic cleaner fish and its various coral reef fish clients showed that cleaners injected with oxytocin break less often the already engaged cleaner-client social interaction to initiate a new interaction with a newly arrived client [103].
Together, there is growing evidence for the possibility that across social vertebrates, oxytocin appears to increase a positive concern for the interests of familiar conspecifics more than for the interests of genetically or culturally unfamiliar, out-group conspecifics: αI > αO. At least in humans, this parochial preference is also reflected in in-group-biased expectations of reciprocity derived from trust games (box 1; [74,95]). In short, when individuals with elevated levels of oxytocin participate in conflict this is more likely owing to an increase in αI than because of a decrease in αO.
(b) . Responding to out-group threat (β)
Nursing rats protect their offspring against intruders by aggressing them with fast attacks directed towards the intruder's neck or back region, lateral threats to force the intruder aside, and standing in an upright posture in front of the intruder, sometimes using the front legs to hold the intruder down [104]. Such ‘maternal defence’ rests on oxytocin, where oxytocin knockout rats and those treated with oxytocin antagonists abstain from aggressing intruders [54,104,105].
A suite of follow-up studies shows oxytocin-mediated aggression towards threatening outsiders is not confined to (female) rodents. For example, when groups of wild meerkats were given intravenous oxytocin (or placebo), individuals spent over twice as much time ‘on guard’; a personally costly behaviour that helps to protect the group against an outside threat from predators and hostile conspecifics [106] (see [102] for similar results in marmoset monkeys). Likewise, estrildid finches that form year-round male-female pairs aggressively defend their territories from intruders. Yet, such aggressive defence is significantly reduced following the blockade of oxytocin receptors in the avian brain [30]). Also, in social fishes such as cichlids and sticklebacks, the presence of an intruder incites higher oxytocin neuronal activity [48] (also see [107]), and higher brain oxytocin levels associate with an aggressive defence of nest and territory [31].
Experiments with human participants confirmed that oxytocin could elicit defensive aggression and suggest that such aggression is closely tied to rivalling out-group threats. For example, several studies showed that oxytocin increases competition against out-group members if, and only if, out-group hostility would hurt the individual and/or its in-group members [95,108] (also see [76]). Other studies using different experimental tasks produced similar results. For example, individuals given oxytocin more quickly (and less accurately) aggressed ethnically different rather than ethnically similar intruders [109].
Taken together, there is converging evidence across social vertebrates that oxytocin upregulates attention and aggressive responses towards predators and rivalling conspecifics. In addition to parochial preferences (§3a), individuals with elevated levels of oxytocin may increase their conflict participation because of enhanced perception of out-group threat and increased readiness to protect and defend genetically related and culturally familiar conspecifics [110].
(c) . Reputation and group norms for participation (γ)
Individuals within groups adapt behaviour to other group members' choices, including those of ‘first-movers’ and group leaders [111,112]. Such behavioural alignment or ‘compliance’ enables the individual to benefit from the protection offered by the group and, in addition, facilitates the coordination of collective action towards some group goal [79,113]. Behavioural alignment thus is functional towards both individual and group survival and prosperity both in general and in the context of intergroup conflict. Furthermore, groups are more likely to win intergroup contests when individual contributions are well-coordinated and aligned with leader initiatives [88,98,114] (also see [73]).
There is some evidence that oxytocin facilitates behavioural alignment and compliance with group norms. Humans, for instance, change their private views in the direction of their group members' opinions more when given oxytocin rather than placebo [115–118]. Likewise, oxytocin mediates interpersonal synchronization at both the neural and behavioural levels in humans [119–123], marmoset monkeys [62], dogs [124] and social fishes [125]. In one study with humans, individuals within groups aligned their contributions to group conflict better when given oxytocin than placebo. As a result, their groups won greater ‘spoils of war’ [98]. Oxytocin may, therefore, prepare the individual for conflict participation because it increases sensitivity to and compliance with leader initiatives and group norms for participation.
Because compliance can have adaptive functionality to the group, individuals are willing to enforce compliance in other group members [79,84]. For example, humans punish those who fail to contribute to group conflict, and such (threat of) punishment increases subsequent conflict participation [114]. At least in humans, there is some evidence that oxytocin prepares the individual for such norm enforcement. For instance, in one study, participants as neutral third parties punished group members who had exploited another person's trust more when given oxytocin rather than placebo [126] (also see [127–130]). In short, oxytocin facilitates interpersonal synchronization and alignment across various social vertebrates at the neural, physiological and behavioural levels. Possibly, and especially when collaborations require strong synchrony in space and time [131], individuals with elevated oxytocin may participate in group conflict because of amplified γ—the readiness to align with and follow other group members' initiatives.
4. Conclusion
Our review reveals converging evidence for the possibility that oxytocin has a ‘tend-and-defend’ functionality that prepares for active conflict participation through an increase in parochial in-group preferences (αI) and perceived threat from out-groups (β). We observed little to no evidence that oxytocin modulates (negative) concern for out-groups (αO) and concomitant aggression aimed at exploiting and sub-ordinating outsiders (figure 3).
Figure 3.
Oxytocin and conflict participation parameters across social vertebrates. Oxytocin creates parochial preferences (αI > αo) because it upregulates αI (concern for genetically and culturally related conspecifics) and less αO (concern for genetically and culturally unrelated conspecifics). Oxytocin also upregulates β (the willingness to defend aggressively against intruders and groups of rivalling conspecifics). At least in primates, oxytocin increases γ (behavioural alignment with group norms for participation). Arrows indicate the direction of an effect of, or association with oxytocin. Empty cells indicate no or too little evidence is available. Illustrations by C. De Dreu.
Our conclusion comes with some limitations. First, we allowed for some degrees of freedom in interpreting animal behaviour as reflective of social preferences (α), threat-responding (β) and norm compliance (γ). Such ‘heuristic’ treatment ignores that both animal behaviour and hormones are often equifinal—different behaviours or hormones serving the same function—and multi-final—the same behaviour or hormone serving several functions [132–134]. Future experiments could try to isolate these parameters further and, in addition, examine possible interactions (e.g. social preferences upregulated threat-responding). Second, not all parameters in the conflict participation function have been covered across social vertebrates, and there are a range of context-dependencies that can complicate straightforward predictions. Conclusive evidence for oxytocin-induced reputation concerns and compliance with group norms, for example, appears limited to humans (figure 3). Third, our analysis collapsed across various measurements and manipulations of oxytocin, and some evidence is strictly correlational. For example, research with humans mostly relied on upregulating oxytocin and has not examined how oxytocin antagonists reduce conflict participation. Also, research often either considered only females or males, while some effects might be sex-specific.
The converging evidence for ‘tend-and-defend’ functionality across social vertebrates should not be taken as if oxytocin is required for participation in group conflict to emerge. Some highly social species such as bees engage in lethal intergroup conflict [135] yet lack oxytocin homologues. Whereas social vertebrates may have co-opted the oxytocinergic circuitry to support a ‘tend-and-defend’ response during the intergroup conflict, other species may rely on different neuroendocrine systems to produce strategic engagement in intergroup conflict. In addition, in social vertebrates, other neurohormonal mechanisms may contribute to conflict participation. For example, oxytocin and vasopressin co-evolved, where vasopressin differs in two amino acids compared to oxytocin [14]. Yet, like oxytocin, vasopressin regulates affiliative behaviour and context-dependent aggressive behaviour (e.g. competition, territory defence) [40,136]. Furthermore, the sex steroid testosterone mediates aggressive behaviour, which can influence group conflict outcomes [137], and the stress hormone cortisol mediates the natural ‘fight-or-flight’ response to threatening conspecifics [34]. Future work into the neurohormonal underpinnings of conflict participation is needed, particularly in how distinctly different neurotransmitters and hormones interact in producing prosocial behaviour towards genetically related and culturally similar conspecifics and aggression towards more or less rivalling out-groups.
Data accessibility
This article has no additional data.
Authors' contributions
Z.T.: conceptualization, funding acquisition, visualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; K.D.: conceptualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; C.K.D.D.: conceptualization, funding acquisition, visualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing.
All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.
Competing interests
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
This project has received funding from the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant no. P2NEP3_188240 and P400PB_199286) to Z.T. and from the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (AdG agreement no. 785635) to C.K.W.D.D.
References
- 1.Radford AN, Majolo B, Aureli F. 2016. Within-group behavioural consequences of between-group conflict: a prospective review. Proc. R. Soc. B 283, 20161567. ( 10.1098/rspb.2016.1567) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.De Dreu CKW, Gross J, Fariña A, Ma Y. 2020. Group cooperation, carrying-capacity stress, and intergroup conflict. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 760-776. ( 10.1016/j.tics.2020.06.005) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wilson DS, Wilson EO. 2007. Rethinking the theoretical foundation of sociobiology. Q. Rev. Biol. 82, 327-348. ( 10.1086/522809) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.De Dreu CKW, Gross J. 2019. Revisiting the form and function of conflict: neurobiological, psychological, and cultural mechanisms for attack and defense within and between groups. Behav. Brain Sci. 42, e116. ( 10.1017/S0140525X18002170) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Bornstein G. 2003. Intergroup conflict: individual, group, and collective interests. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. Off. J. Soc. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Inc 7, 129-145. ( 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0702_129-145) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Choi J-K, Bowles S. 2007. The coevolution of parochial altruism and war. Science 318, 636-640. ( 10.1126/science.1144237) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Rusch H, Gavrilets S. 2020. The logic of animal intergroup conflict: a review. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 178, 1014-1030. ( 10.1016/j.jebo.2017.05.004) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Stoop R. 2012. Neuromodulation by oxytocin and vasopressin. Neuron 76, 142-159. ( 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.025) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Jurek B, Neumann ID. 2018. The oxytocin receptor: from intracellular signaling to behavior. Physiol. Rev. 98, 1805-1908. ( 10.1152/physrev.00031.2017) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Acher R, Chauvet J, Chauvet M, Crepy D. 1968. Molecular evolution of neurohypophysial hormones: comparison of the active principles of three bony fishes. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 11, 535-538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Acher R, Chauvet J, Chauvet M. 1970. Phylogeny of the neurohypophysial hormones: the avian active peptides. Eur. J. Biochem. 17, 509-513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ren D, Lu G, Moriyama H, Mustoe AC, Harrison EB, French JA. 2015. Genetic diversity in oxytocin ligands and receptors in new world monkeys. PLoS ONE 10, e0125775. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0125775) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Stafflinger E, Hansen KK, Hauser F, Schneider M, Cazzamali G, Williamson M, Grimmelikhuijzen CJP. 2008. Cloning and identification of an oxytocin/vasopressin-like receptor and its ligand from insects. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 3262-3267. ( 10.1073/pnas.0710897105) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Theofanopoulou C, Gedman G, Cahill JA, Boeckx C, Jarvis ED. 2021. Universal nomenclature for oxytocin–vasotocin ligand and receptor families. Nature 592, 747-755. ( 10.1038/s41586-020-03040-7) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Crockford C, Deschner T, Ziegler T, Wittig R. 2014. Endogenous peripheral oxytocin measures can give insight into the dynamics of social relationships: a review. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 68. ( 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00068) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.McCullough ME, Churchland PS, Mendez AJ. 2013. Problems with measuring peripheral oxytocin: can the data on oxytocin and human behavior be trusted? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 1485-1492. ( 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.04.018) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Graustella AJ, MacLeod C. 2012. A critical review of the influence of oxytocin nasal spray on social cognition in humans: evidence and future directions. Horm. Behav. 61, 410-418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Engel S, Laufer S, Miller R, Niemeyer H, Knaevelsrud C, Schumacher S. 2019. Demographic, sampling-and assay-related confounders of endogenous oxytocin concentrations: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 54, 100775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Szeto A, McCabe PM, Nation DA, Tabak BA, Rossetti MA, McCullough ME, Schneiderman N, Mendez AJ. 2011. Evaluation of enzyme immunoassay and radioimmunoassay methods for the measurement of plasma oxytocin. Psychosom. Med. 73, 393-400. ( 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31821df0c2) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Valstad M, Alvares GA, Andreassen OA, Westlye LT, Quintana DS. 2016. The relationship between central and peripheral oxytocin concentrations: a systematic review and meta-analysis protocol. Syst. Rev. 5, 1-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Churchland PS, Winkielman P. 2012. Modulating social behavior with oxytocin: how does it work? What does it mean? Horm. Behav. 61, 392-399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Evans SL, Dal Monte O, Noble P, Averbeck BB. 2014. Intranasal oxytocin effects on social cognition: a critique. Brain Res. 1580, 69-77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Daughters K, Manstead ASR, Hubble K, Rees A, Thapar A, Goozen SHM van. 2015. Salivary oxytocin concentrations in males following intranasal administration of oxytocin: a double-blind, cross-over study. PLoS ONE 10, e0145104. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0145104) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Martins DA, et al. 2020. Effects of route of administration on oxytocin-induced changes in regional cerebral blood flow in humans. Nat. Commun. 11, 1160. ( 10.1038/s41467-020-14845-5) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Neumann ID, Maloumby R, Beiderbeck DI, Lukas M, Landgraf R. 2013. Increased brain and plasma oxytocin after nasal and peripheral administration in rats and mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 1985-1993. ( 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.03.003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Striepens N, Kendrick KM, Hanking V, Landgraf R, Wüllner U, Maier W, Hurlemann R. 2013. Elevated cerebrospinal fluid and blood concentrations of oxytocin following its intranasal administration in humans. Sci. Rep. 3, 3440. ( 10.1038/srep03440) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Keebaugh AC, Barrett CE, Laprairie JL, Jenkins JJ, Young LJ. 2015. RNAi knockdown of oxytocin receptor in the nucleus accumbens inhibits social attachment and parental care in monogamous female prairie voles. Soc. Neurosci. 10, 561-570. ( 10.1080/17470919.2015.1040893) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Duque JF, Leichner W, Ahmann H, Stevens JR. 2018. Mesotocin influences pinyon jay prosociality. Biol. Lett. 14, 20180105. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2018.0105) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Dogterom J, Greidanus TBVW, Swaab DF. 1977. Evidence for the release of vasopressin and oxytocin into cerebrospinal fluid: measurements in plasma and CSF of intact and hypophysectomized rats. Neuroendocrinology 24, 108-118. ( 10.1159/000122702) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Goodson JL, Schrock SE, Kingsbury MA. 2015. Oxytocin mechanisms of stress response and aggression in a territorial finch. Physiol. Behav. 141, 154-163. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.01.016) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kleszczyńska A, Sokołowska E, Kulczykowska E. 2012. Variation in brain arginine vasotocin (AVT) and isotocin (IT) levels with reproductive stage and social status in males of three-spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 175, 290-296. ( 10.1016/j.ygcen.2011.11.022) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Freeman SM, Samineni S, Allen PC, Stockinger D, Bales KL, Hwa GG, Roberts JA. 2016. Plasma and CSF oxytocin levels after intranasal and intravenous oxytocin in awake macaques. Psychoneuroendocrinology 66, 185-194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Saito A, Nakamura K. 2011. Oxytocin changes primate paternal tolerance to offspring in food transfer. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural. Behav. Physiol. 197, 329-337. ( 10.1007/s00359-010-0617-2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Samuni L, Preis A, Mundry R, Deschner T, Crockford C, Wittig RM. 2017. Oxytocin reactivity during intergroup conflict in wild chimpanzees. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 268-273. ( 10.1073/pnas.1616812114) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Lukas M, Toth I, Reber SO, Slattery DA, Veenema AH, Neumann ID. 2011. The neuropeptide oxytocin facilitates pro-social behavior and prevents social avoidance in rats and mice. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 2159-2168. ( 10.1038/npp.2011.95) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.MacLean EL, Gesquiere LR, Gee NR, Levy K, Martin WL, Carter CS. 2017. Effects of affiliative human–animal interaction on dog salivary and plasma oxytocin and vasopressin. Front. Psychol. 8, 1606. ( 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01606) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Stocker M, Prosl J, Vanhooland L-C, Horn L, Bugnyar T, Canoine V, Massen JJM. 2021. Measuring salivary mesotocin in birds - seasonal differences in ravens' peripheral mesotocin levels. Horm. Behav. 134, 105015. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2021.105015) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wirobski G, Range F, Schaebs FS, Palme R, Deschner T, Marshall-Pescini S. 2021. Endocrine changes related to dog domestication: comparing urinary cortisol and oxytocin in hand-raised, pack-living dogs and wolves. Horm. Behav. 128, 104901. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104901) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Klatt JD, Goodson JL. 2013. Oxytocin-like receptors mediate pair bonding in a socially monogamous songbird. Proc. R. Soc. B 280, 20122396. ( 10.1098/rspb.2012.2396) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Triki Z, Bshary R, Grutter AS, Ros AFH. 2017. The arginine-vasotocin and serotonergic systems affect interspecific social behaviour of client fish in marine cleaning mutualism. Physiol. Behav. 174, 136-143. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.03.011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Thompson RR, Walton JC. 2004. Peptide effects on social behavior: effects of vasotocin and isotocin on social approach behavior in male goldfish (Carassius auratus). Behav. Neurosci. 118, 620-626. ( 10.1037/0735-7044.118.3.620) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Bottje WG, Wang S, Kinzler S, Neldon HL, Koike TI. 1990. Plasma concentrations of arginine vasotocin and mesotocin following pentobarbital anaesthesia and carotid cannulation in domestic fowl. Br. Poult. Sci. 31, 189-195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Gozdowska M, Kulczykowska E. 2004. Determination of arginine–vasotocin and isotocin in fish plasma with solid-phase extraction and fluorescence derivatization followed by high-performance liquid chromatography. J. Chromatogr. B 807, 229-233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Hattori T, Kanno K, Nagasawa M, Nishimori K, Mogi K, Kikusui T. 2015. Impairment of interstrain social recognition during territorial aggressive behavior in oxytocin receptor-null mice. Neurosci. Res. 90, 90-94. ( 10.1016/j.neures.2014.05.003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Modi ME, Young LJ. 2012. The oxytocin system in drug discovery for autism: animal models and novel therapeutic strategies. Horm. Behav. 61, 340-350. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.010) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Kelly AM, Goodson JL. 2014. Hypothalamic oxytocin and vasopressin neurons exert sex-specific effects on pair bonding, gregariousness, and aggression in finches. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111, 6069-6074. ( 10.1073/pnas.1322554111) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Franke AA, Li X, Menden A, Lee MR, Lai JF. 2019. Oxytocin analysis from human serum, urine, and saliva by orbitrap liquid chromatography-mass spectrometry. Drug Test. Anal. 11, 119-128. ( 10.1002/dta.2475) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Weitekamp CA, Solomon-Lane TK, Del Valle P, Triki Z, Nugent BM, Hofmann HA. 2017. A role for oxytocin-like receptor in social habituation in a teleost. Brain. Behav. Evol. 89, 153-161. ( 10.1159/000464098) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Calcagnoli F, de Boer SF, Beiderbeck DI, Althaus M, Koolhaas JM, Neumann ID. 2014. Local oxytocin expression and oxytocin receptor binding in the male rat brain is associated with aggressiveness. Behav. Brain Res. 261, 315-322. ( 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.12.050) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Knobloch HS, Grinevich V. 2014. Evolution of oxytocin pathways in the brain of vertebrates. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 31. ( 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00031) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.O'Connell LA, Hofmann HA. 2011. The vertebrate mesolimbic reward system and social behavior network: a comparative synthesis. J. Comp. Neurol. 519, 3599-3639. ( 10.1002/cne.22735) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Rilling JK, Young LJ. 2014. The biology of mammalian parenting and its effect on offspring social development. Science 345, 771-776. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Goodson JL. 2005. The vertebrate social behavior network: evolutionary themes and variations. Horm. Behav. 48, 11-22. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.02.003) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.de Moura Oliveira VE, et al. .2021. Oxytocin and vasopressin within the ventral and dorsal lateral septum modulate aggression in female rats. Nat. Commun. 12, 1-15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Lemoine S, Samuni L, Crockford C, Wittig R. 2022. Parochial cooperation in wild chimpanzees: a model to explain the evolution of parochial altruism. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210149. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0149) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Morhenn VB, Park JW, Piper E, Zak PJ. 2008. Monetary sacrifice among strangers is mediated by endogenous oxytocin release after physical contact. Evol. Hum. Behav. 29, 375-383. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.04.004) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Gordon I, Zagoory-Sharon O, Leckman JF, Feldman R. 2010. Oxytocin and the development of parenting in humans. Biol. Psychiatry 68, 377-382. ( 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.005) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Ogi A, Mariti C, Baragli P, Sergi V, Gazzano A. 2020. Effects of stroking on salivary oxytocin and cortisol in guide dogs: preliminary results. Anim. Open Access J. MDPI 10, 708. ( 10.3390/ani10040708) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.McClung JS, Triki Z, Clément F, Bangerter A, Bshary R. 2018. Endogenous oxytocin predicts helping and conversation as a function of group membership. Proc. R. Soc. B 285, 20180939. ( 10.1098/rspb.2018.0939) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Wittig RM, Crockford C, Deschner T, Langergraber KE, Ziegler TE, Zuberbühler K. 2014. Food sharing is linked to urinary oxytocin levels and bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proc. R. Soc. B 281, 20133096. ( 10.1098/rspb.2013.3096) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Rincon AV, Deschner T, Schülke O, Ostner J. 2020. Oxytocin increases after affiliative interactions in male Barbary macaques. Horm. Behav. 119, 104661. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.104661) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Finkenwirth C, van Schaik C, Ziegler TE, Burkart JM. 2015. Strongly bonded family members in common marmosets show synchronized fluctuations in oxytocin. Physiol. Behav. 151, 246-251. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.07.034) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Feldman R. 2012. Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans. Horm. Behav. 61, 380-391. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Pedersen A, Tomaszycki ML. 2012. Oxytocin antagonist treatments alter the formation of pair relationships in zebra finches of both sexes. Horm. Behav. 62, 113-119. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.05.009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Kabelik D, Magruder DS. 2014. Involvement of different mesotocin (oxytocin homologue) populations in sexual and aggressive behaviours of the brown anole. Biol. Lett. 10, 20140566. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2014.0566) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Rossoni E, Feng J, Tirozzi B, Brown D, Leng G, Moos F. 2008. Emergent synchronous bursting of oxytocin neuronal network. PLoS Comput. Biol. 4, e1000123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Neumann ID, Landgraf R. 2019. Tracking oxytocin functions in the rodent brain during the last 30 years: from push-pull perfusion to chemogenetic silencing. J. Neuroendocrinol. 31, e12695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Kavaliers M, Choleris E. 2017. Out-group threat responses, in-group bias, and nonapeptide involvement are conserved across vertebrates: (a comment on Bruintjes et al., ‘out-group threat promotes within-group affiliation in a cooperative fish’). Am. Nat. 189, 453-458. ( 10.1086/690838) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Walum H, Young LJ. 2018. The neural mechanisms and circuitry of the pair bond. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 643-654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Lehmann L, Keller L. 2006. The evolution of cooperation and altruism–a general framework and a classification of models. J. Evol. Biol. 19, 1365-1376. ( 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01119.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Green PA, Briffa M, Cant MA. 2021. Assessment during intergroup contests. Trends Ecol. Evol. 36, 139-150. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2020.09.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.De Dreu CK, Fariña A, Gross J, Romano A. 2021. Pro-sociality as a foundation for intergroup conflict. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 44, 112-116. ( 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.09.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.De Dreu C, Triki Z. 2022. Intergroup conflict: origins, dynamics, and consequences across taxa. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210134. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0134) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.De Dreu CKW, Kret ME. 2016. Oxytocin conditions intergroup relations through upregulated in-group empathy, cooperation, conformity, and defense. Biol. Psychiatry 79, 165-173. ( 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.020) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.De Dreu CKW. 2012. Oxytocin modulates cooperation within and competition between groups: an integrative review and research agenda. Horm. Behav. 61, 419-428. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Ziegler TE, Crockford C. 2017. Neuroendocrine control in social relationships in non-human primates: field based evidence. Horm. Behav. 91, 107-121. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.03.004) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Piva M, Chang SWC. 2018. An integrated framework for the role of oxytocin in multistage social decision-making. Am. J. Primatol. 80, e22735. ( 10.1002/ajp.22735) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Balliet D, Wu J, De Dreu CKW. 2014. Ingroup favoritism in cooperation: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 140, 1556-1581. ( 10.1037/a0037737) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.van Dijk E, De Dreu CKW. 2021. Experimental games and social decision making. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 72, 415-438. ( 10.1146/annurev-psych-081420-110718) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Jaeggi AV, Burkart JM, Van Schaik CP. 2010. On the psychology of cooperation in humans and other primates: combining the natural history and experimental evidence of prosociality. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 365, 2723-2735. ( 10.1098/rstb.2010.0118) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Bshary R, Grutter AS. 2006. Image scoring and cooperation in a cleaner fish mutualism. Nature 441, 975-978. ( 10.1038/nature04755) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Fraser ON, Bugnyar T. 2012. Reciprocity of agonistic support in ravens. Anim. Behav. 83, 171-177. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.10.023) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Milinski M. 1987. Tit for tat in sticklebacks and the evolution of cooperation. Nature 325, 433-435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Fehr E, Schurtenberger I. 2018. Normative foundations of human cooperation. Nat . Hum. Behav. 2, 458-468. ( 10.1038/s41562-018-0385-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Schweinfurth MK, Call J. 2019. Revisiting the possibility of reciprocal help in non-human primates. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 104, 73-86. ( 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.026) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Raihani NJ, Grutter AS, Bshary R. 2010. Punishers benefit from third-party punishment in fish. Science 327, 171. ( 10.1126/science.1183068) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.De Dreu CKW, Greer LL, Van Kleef GA, Shalvi S, Handgraaf MJJ. 2011. Oxytocin promotes human ethnocentrism. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 1262-1266. ( 10.1073/pnas.1015316108) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Yang X, Wang W, Wang XT, Wang YW. 2021. A meta-analysis of hormone administration effects on cooperative behaviours: oxytocin, vasopressin, and testosterone. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 126, 430-443. ( 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.03.033) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Sheng F, Liu Y, Zhou B, Zhou W, Han S. 2013. Oxytocin modulates the racial bias in neural responses to others' suffering. Biol. Psychol. 92, 380-386. ( 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.11.018) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Levy J, Goldstein A, Influs M, Masalha S, Zagoory-Sharon O, Feldman R. 2016. Adolescents growing up amidst intractable conflict attenuate brain response to pain of outgroup. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 13 696-13 701. ( 10.1073/pnas.1612903113) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Ross HE, Young LJ. 2009. Oxytocin and the neural mechanisms regulating social cognition and affiliative behavior. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 30, 534-547. ( 10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.05.004) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Marlin BJ, Mitre M, D'amour JA, Chao MV, Froemke RC. 2015. Oxytocin enables maternal behaviour by balancing cortical inhibition. Nature 520, 499-504. ( 10.1038/nature14402) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Kendrick KM, Da Costa AP, Broad KD, Ohkura S, Guevara R, Lévy F, Keverne EB. 1997. Neural control of maternal behaviour and olfactory recognition of offspring. Brain Res. Bull. 44, 383-395. ( 10.1016/s0361-9230(97)00218-9) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Crockford C, Wittig RM, Langergraber K, Ziegler TE, Zuberbühler K, Deschner T. 2013. Urinary oxytocin and social bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proc. R. Soc. B 280, 20122765. ( 10.1098/rspb.2012.2765) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.De Dreu CKW, Greer LL, Handgraaf MJJ, Shalvi S, Van Kleef GA, Baas M, Ten Velden FS, Van Dijk E, Feith SWW. 2010. The neuropeptide oxytocin regulates parochial altruism in intergroup conflict among humans. Science 328, 1408-1411. ( 10.1126/science.1189047) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Velden F, Daughters K, Dreu C. 2017. Oxytocin promotes intuitive rather than deliberated cooperation with the in-group. Horm. Behav. 92, 164-171. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.06.005) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Israel S, Weisel O, Ebstein RP, Bornstein G. 2012. Oxytocin, but not vasopressin, increases both parochial and universal altruism. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 1341-1344. ( 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.02.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Zhang H, Gross J, De Dreu C, Ma Y. 2019. Oxytocin promotes coordinated out-group attack during intergroup conflict in humans. ELife 8, e40698. ( 10.7554/eLife.40698) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Terris ET, Beavin LE, Barraza JA, Schloss J, Zak PJ. 2018. Endogenous oxytocin release eliminates in-group bias in monetary transfers with perspective-taking. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12, 35. ( 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00035) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Schiller B, Domes G, Heinrichs M. 2020. Oxytocin changes behavior and spatio-temporal brain dynamics underlying inter-group conflict in humans. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 31, 119-130. ( 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2019.12.109) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Mustoe AC, Cavanaugh J, Harnisch AM, Thompson BE, French JA. 2015. Do marmosets care to share? Oxytocin treatment reduces prosocial behavior toward strangers. Horm. Behav. 71, 83-90. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.04.015) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Cavanaugh J, Mustoe A, French JA. 2018. Oxytocin regulates reunion affiliation with a pairmate following social separation in marmosets. Am. J. Primatol. 80, e22750. ( 10.1002/ajp.22750) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Soares MC, Bshary R, Mendonça R, Grutter AS, Oliveira RF. 2012. Arginine vasotocin regulation of interspecific cooperative behaviour in a cleaner fish. PLoS ONE 7, e39583. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0039583) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Bosch OJ. 2013. Maternal aggression in rodents: brain oxytocin and vasopressin mediate pup defence. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 368, 20130085. ( 10.1098/rstb.2013.0085) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Pedersen CA, Ascher JA, Monroe YL, Prange AJ. 1982. Oxytocin induces maternal behavior in virgin female rats. Science 216, 648-650. ( 10.1126/science.7071605) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Madden JR, Clutton-Brock TH. 2011. Experimental peripheral administration of oxytocin elevates a suite of cooperative behaviours in a wild social mammal. Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 1189-1194. ( 10.1098/rspb.2010.1675) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Reddon AR, O'Connor CM, Marsh-Rollo SE, Balshine S. 2012. Effects of isotocin on social responses in a cooperatively breeding fish. Anim. Behav. 84, 753-760. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.07.021) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 108.De Dreu CKW, Shalvi S, Greer LL, Van Kleef GA, Handgraaf MJJ. 2012. Oxytocin motivates non-cooperation in intergroup conflict to protect vulnerable in-group members. PLoS ONE 7, e46751. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0046751) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Egito JH, Nevat M, Shamay-Tsoory SG, Osório AAC. 2020. Oxytocin increases the social salience of the outgroup in potential threat contexts. Horm. Behav. 122, 104733. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104733) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Samuni L, Preis A, Deschner T, Wittig RM, Crockford C. 2019. Cortisol and oxytocin show independent activity during chimpanzee intergroup conflict. Psychoneuroendocrinology 104, 165-173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Glowacki L, McDermott R. 2022. Key individuals catalyze intergroup violence. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210141. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0141) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Smith J, Fichtel C, Holmes R, Kappeler P, van vugt M, Jaeggi A. 2022. Sex bias in intergroup conflict and collective movements among social mammals: male warriors and female guides. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210142. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0142) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Shamay-Tsoory SG, Saporta N, Marton-Alper IZ, Gvirts HZ. 2019. Herding brains: a core neural mechanism for social alignment. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 174-186. ( 10.1016/j.tics.2019.01.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.De Dreu CKW, Gross J, Méder Z, Giffin M, Prochazkova E, Krikeb J, Columbus S. 2016. In-group defense, out-group aggression, and coordination failures in intergroup conflict. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 10 524-10 529. ( 10.1073/pnas.1605115113) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Stallen M, De Dreu CKW, Shalvi S, Smidts A, Sanfey AG. 2012. The herding hormone: oxytocin stimulates in-group conformity. Psychol. Sci. 23, 1288-1292. ( 10.1177/0956797612446026) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Edelson MG, Shemesh M, Weizman A, Yariv S, Sharot T, Dudai Y. 2015. Opposing effects of oxytocin on overt compliance and lasting changes to memory. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 966-973. ( 10.1038/npp.2014.273) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Aydogan G, Jobst A, D'Ardenne K, Müller N, Kocher MG. 2017. The detrimental effects of oxytocin-induced conformity on dishonesty in competition. Psychol. Sci. 28, 751-759. ( 10.1177/0956797617695100) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Xu L, Becker B, Kendrick KM. 2019. Oxytocin facilitates social learning by promoting conformity to trusted individuals. Front. Neurosci. 13, 56. ( 10.3389/fnins.2019.00056) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Arueti M, Perach-Barzilay N, Tsoory MM, Berger B, Getter N, Shamay-Tsoory SG. 2013. When two become one: the role of oxytocin in interpersonal coordination and cooperation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 1418-1427. ( 10.1162/jocn_a_00400) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Gebauer L, Witek MAG, Hansen NC, Thomas J, Konvalinka I, Vuust P. 2016. Oxytocin improves synchronisation in leader-follower interaction. Sci. Rep. 6, 38416. ( 10.1038/srep38416) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Mu Y, Guo C, Han S. 2016. Oxytocin enhances inter-brain synchrony during social coordination in male adults. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 1882-1893. ( 10.1093/scan/nsw106) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Kret ME, De Dreu CKW. 2017. Pupil-mimicry conditions trust in partners: moderation by oxytocin and group membership. Proc. R. Soc. B 284, 20162554. ( 10.1098/rspb.2016.2554) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Spengler FB, Scheele D, Marsh N, Kofferath C, Flach A, Schwarz S, Stoffel-Wagner B, Maier W, Hurlemann R. 2017. Oxytocin facilitates reciprocity in social communication. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 1325-1333. ( 10.1093/scan/nsx061) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Nagasawa M, Mitsui S, En S, Ohtani N, Ohta M, Sakuma Y, Onaka T, Mogi K, Kikusui T. 2015. Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. Science 348, 333-336. ( 10.1126/science.1261022) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Ataei Mehr B, Garner SR, Neff BD. 2020. Effect of isotocin on shoaling behaviour of the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Anim. Cogn. 23, 827-831. ( 10.1007/s10071-020-01381-4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Daughters K, Manstead ASR, Ten Velden FS, De Dreu CKW. 2017. Oxytocin modulates third-party sanctioning of selfish and generous behavior within and between groups. Psychoneuroendocrinology 77, 18-24. ( 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.11.039) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Aydogan G, Furtner NC, Kern B, Jobst A, Müller N, Kocher MG. 2017. Oxytocin promotes altruistic punishment. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 1740-1747. ( 10.1093/scan/nsx101) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Marsh N, Scheele D, Feinstein JS, Gerhardt H, Strang S, Maier W, Hurlemann R. 2017. Oxytocin-enforced norm compliance reduces xenophobic outgroup rejection. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 9314-9319. ( 10.1073/pnas.1705853114) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Stallen M, Rossi F, Heijne A, Smidts A, Dreu CKWD, Sanfey AG. 2018. Neurobiological mechanisms of responding to injustice. J. Neurosci. 38, 2944-2954. ( 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1242-17.2018) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Han X, et al. 2020. A neurobiological association of revenge propensity during intergroup conflict. eLife 9, e52014. ( 10.7554/eLife.52014) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Samuni L, Preis A, Deschner T, Crockford C, Wittig RM. 2018. Reward of labor coordination and hunting success in wild chimpanzees. Commun. Biol. 1, 1-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.von Bertalanffy L. 1969. General system theory: foundations, development, applications. New York, NY: George Braziller.
- 133.Young LJ, Wang Z. 2004. The neurobiology of pair bonding. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 1048-1054. ( 10.1038/nn1327) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Oliveira RF. 2009. Social behavior in context: hormonal modulation of behavioral plasticity and social competence. Integr. Comp. Biol. 49, 423-440. ( 10.1093/icb/icp055) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Cunningham JP, Hereward JP, Heard TA, De Barro PJ, West SA. 2014. Bees at war: interspecific battles and nest usurpation in stingless bees. Am. Nat. 184, 777-786. ( 10.1086/678399) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Goodson JL. 2008. Nonapeptides and the evolutionary patterning of sociality. In Progress in brain research (eds Neumann ID, Landgraf R), pp. 3-15. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Rose RM, Bernstein IS, Gordon TP. 1975. Consequences of social conflict on plasma testosterone levels in rhesus monkeys. Psychosom. Med. 37, 50-61. ( 10.1097/00006842-197501000-00006) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
This article has no additional data.