Abstract
Background
Reconsolidation impairment using propranolol is a novel intervention for mental disorders with an emotional memory at their core. In this systematic review and meta-analysis, we examined the evidence for this intervention in healthy and clinical adult samples.
Methods
We searched 8 databases for randomized, double-blind studies that involved at least 1 propranolol group and 1 placebo group. We conducted a meta-analysis of 14 studies (n = 478) in healthy adults and 12 studies in clinical samples (n = 446).
Results
Compared to placebo, reconsolidation impairment under propranolol resulted in reduced recall of aversive material and cue-elicited conditioned emotional responses in healthy adults, as evidenced by an effect size (Hedges g) of −0.51 (p = 0.002, 2-tailed). Moreover, compared to placebo, reconsolidation impairment under propranolol alleviated psychiatric symptoms and reduced cue-elicited reactivity in clinical samples with posttraumatic stress disorder, addiction or phobia (g = −0.42, p = 0.010).
Limitations
Methodological differences between studies posed an obstacle for identifying sources of heterogeneity.
Conclusion
Reconsolidation impairment is a robust, well-replicated phenomenon in humans. Its clinical use is promising and deserves further controlled investigation.
Introduction
Increased activation of the stress response during emotional experiences plays the adaptive role of enhancing memory consolidation and facilitating subsequent recall.1–3 Preclinical evidence suggests that the consolidation of both aversive and appetitive memories is dependent on the activation of the noradrenergic system in the basolateral amygdala. Heightened activity in these mechanisms may lead to an overly powerful emotional memory thought to underlie certain psychiatric disorders.3,4 For instance, it is thought that exposure to highly stressful or traumatic experiences that accentuate the stress response may underlie aversive memories in fear-based psychiatric disorders, such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), adjustment disorder and specific phobia, among others.2,5 As well, overly consolidated appetitive memories of drug-use experiences arguably maintain compulsive drug-seeking behaviours in people with addiction.6 More precisely, overly consolidated memories become too easily reactivated, perpetuating symptoms of intrusion, avoidance and hypervigilance in trauma- and stressor-related disorders and phobias,2,7 and craving and relapse in substance dependence.8 Considering the role of emotional memories in such disorders, reducing the strength of these memories would have obvious clinical value and deserves to be attempted.
Memory reconsolidation theory suggests the presence of a neuroplastic process whereby the reactivation of previously consolidated memories induces a temporary state of lability, during which time such memories may be altered before restabilizing.9 It is argued that this mechanism may be implicated in updating or impairing long-term memories by interfering with their restabilization back to long-term storage (i.e., reconsolidation). As with initial consolidation, reconsolidation is also dependent on de novo protein synthesis and activation of the noradrenergic system.10,11 Evidence suggests that propranolol — a synthetic noradrenergic β-receptor blocker that crosses the blood–brain barrier and exerts central inhibitory effects — can oppose the enhancement of memory conferred by emotion when administered before or shortly after new learning (consolidation) or the recall of established memories (reconsolidation).12–14 Although the mechanisms of reconsolidation require clarification, one possibility is that by blocking noradrenergic activity in the amygdala, propranolol disrupts the cyclic AMP/protein kinase A pathway, indirectly interfering with the synthesis of new proteins required for memory (re)consolidation.15–17
Therapeutic approaches based on reconsolidation interference theory offer hope for a paradigm shift in the treatment of mental disorders that have an emotional memory at their core.18–22 A previous meta-analysis of studies of experimentally acquired and previously learned emotional memories demonstrated that the reconsolidation blocker propranolol, paired with memory reactivation, weakened such memories compared to memory reactivation under placebo in healthy adults.14
Based on such findings, reconsolidation impairment procedures are being adapted increasingly into treatment protocols for several psychiatric conditions. For instance, one protocol involves reactivating a traumatic memory by writing a narrative and reading it aloud after the administration of propranolol. This protocol has shown efficacy in improving both trauma- and stressor-related and substance-dependence symptoms.23,24 Similar protocols that elicit memory retrieval (reactivation) using videos, words or in vivo exposure to substance cues or fearful stimuli have also shown efficacy.7,25,26 However, some studies have failed to find such effects.27,28 One meta-analysis examined the reconsolidation of naturalistic memories in clinical and subclinical samples; although the authors reported promising results, they concluded that heterogeneity among studies limited the strength of the findings.29
An up-to-date review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials that have examined the impairment of emotional memory reconsolidation using propranolol in healthy and clinical samples may help to further explore this issue. We hypothesized that, compared to placebo, memory reactivation under the influence of or followed by the reconsolidation blocker propranolol would reduce recall, recognition memory and psychophysiological responding, and that it would decrease symptom severity in clinical samples.
Methods
Study inclusion criteria
We included studies in the qualitative synthesis if they were published (with no date constraint) and involved memory reactivation under propranolol in human adults. We included studies in the meta-analysis if they met the criteria above and were double-blind, placebo-controlled trials.
Experimental paradigms
The protocols typically used in reconsolidation studies that involve healthy volunteers consist of behavioural learning (e.g., conditioned responses to emotional material), physiologic reactivity to autobiographical emotional memories using script-driven imagery (i.e., symptom provocation) or declarative memory tasks. Protocols typically involve 3 phases: (1) participants undergo initial learning of emotionally valenced or neutral material (e.g., word lists, pictures or conditioned stimuli); (2) at least 24 hours later, propranolol or placebo is administered 60–90 minutes before or immediately after memory reactivation achieved by the brief presentation of a cue; and (3) after a washout period of 24 hours, participants’ memory is tested via a recall or recognition task, or via physiologic reactivity to the conditioned stimulus.
In single-treatment clinical studies, memory reactivation is achieved by exposing participants to salient fear- or drug-related cues before or after the administration of propranolol (or placebo), and declarative memory or physiologic reactivity is tested after a washout period of at least 24 hours. In other clinical studies, the typical treatment protocol involves drug administration 60–90 minutes before participants are asked to write and read aloud a 1-page narrative of the traumatic experience over multiple6 treatment sessions, after which symptom severity or physiologic reactivity is measured.23,30
Outcome measures
For healthy samples, the outcome of interest for studies involving declarative memory tasks was memory performance for emotional material on the test day, as measured by free recall or percent correct on a recognition task. The outcome of interest for studies involving conditioned responses to emotional material consisted of physiologic responses. For clinical samples, the primary outcomes of interest were symptom severity as measured by self-report or clinician-rated measures, physiologic responding to symptom provocation (i.e., heart rate, skin conductance or electromyogram), or both.
Search strategy and data extraction
We searched PsycINFO, Ovid Medline, PTSDpubs, Web of Science, Google Scholar, PubMed, Cochrane Central and clinicaltrials.org up to September 2021, to find relevant studies using various combinations of the following key words: propranolol, reconsolidation, reactivation, emotion*, memory, fear, reward, trauma, anxiety, post-traumatic, post-traumatic stress disorder, PTSD, acute stress disorder, adjustment disorder, phobia, specific phobia, spider phobia, addict*, substance abuse and substance dependence. We exported the results of this search to a reference-management database and removed duplicates.
Three investigators (S.P., O.R. and a research assistant) independently screened the titles and abstracts, excluding irrelevant articles and assessing all relevant full-text articles. The investigators met to compare results, and disagreements were resolved by consensus. We also screened the reference sections of included articles for other articles. We conducted separate searches and analyses for the healthy and clinical samples. Finally, we contacted researchers in the field to determine whether they had unpublished data that could be included in the meta-analysis.
Data from the included studies were extracted by 3 independent raters (S.P., O.R. and a research assistant) and double-checked by a fourth party (M.L.). When studies reported incomplete results, we contacted the authors. If the authors could not provide the missing data, we excluded the study. Then, 2 research assistants used the Jadad Scale31 to conduct a quality assessment of the included studies.
Statistical analyses
To examine between-group mean differences on the test day (healthy volunteers) or post-treatment (clinical samples), we used Hedges g, because it produces an adjusted effect-size estimate for small samples.32,33 Hedges g is interpreted similarly to Cohen d: g < 0.2 represents a small effect size, 0.2 to 0.5 represents a moderate effect size and 0.6 to 0.8 represents a large effect size.32,33 For studies that reported several outcomes of interest, we averaged Hedges g across outcomes to control for outcome selection bias. Tests of the pooled Hedges g estimate were 2-sided (α < 0.05), which provided a more conservative test of the overall pooled effect. Owing to methodological heterogeneity among studies, we used a random-effects model to test the hypotheses.34 In contrast to a fixed-effects model (which assumes that each included study represents only one “true” effect), a random-effects model assumes that there is a distribution of “true” effects that varies between studies as a function of study characteristics.34 We also conducted homogeneity analyses using Q and I2 statistics to identify outliers and sources of heterogeneity.34,35
We assessed publication bias by visual inspection of funnel plot symmetry; asymmetry indicated possible publication bias. In addition, we used the trim and fill method36 to examine the impact of potential publication bias on the pooled effect size estimate. Finally, we used the Egger test to evaluate the presence of bias reflected in the funnel plot (a 2-tailed p-value < 0.05 indicated significant bias).34,36 We performed analyses using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software (version 3; Biostat Inc.).
Results
Figure 1 displays the PRISMA flow diagram for study selection. Across 31 publications (36 studies) included in the qualitative synthesis (see qualitative results), 17 studies involved healthy samples,37–51 and 19 involved clinical samples.7,23–28,30,52–59
Figure 1.
Study selection flow chart. *Thomas and colleagues45 and Deng and colleagues51 each had 2 studies in healthy samples. Roullet and colleagues58 had 2 separate clinical samples. Brunet and colleagues30 had 3 clinical studies.
We excluded 5 clinical studies from the meta-analysis because they did not include a placebo group.30,55,57 We excluded 3 studies involving healthy samples49,51 and 2 involving clinical samples25,59 from the meta-analysis because they did not report data in a usable format, and the raw data could not be retrieved from the authors. Moreover, 1 study58 provided between-group effect sizes for the sample at 3 months post-treatment based on the PTSD status of participants at study entry (i.e., moderate PTSD symptoms and severe PTSD symptoms). We included these as such in the meta-analysis. Thus, across 24 publications, 26 studies were included in the meta-analysis: 14 involving healthy samples37–48,50 and 12 involving clinical samples.7,23,24,26–28,52–54,56,58
Qualitative results
Table 1 summarizes the characteristics of the studies included in the qualitative synthesis, but not in the meta-analysis. Table 2 and Table 3 summarize the characteristics of the healthy adult and clinical studies, respectively, that were included in the quantitative synthesis. Across all studies, 618 participants received propranolol and 527 participants received placebo.
Table 1.
Characteristics of studies included in the qualitative review but excluded from the meta-analysis
Study | Population or clinical diagnosis | Propranolol/placebo | Male/ female, % | Age, yr, mean ± SD | Study protocol | Outcome measures of interest | Primary results of interest | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||||
No. enrolled | No. on test day or post-treatment | |||||||
Brunet et al.30 (2011; study 1) | Chronic PTSD | 28/0 | 28/0 | 32/68 | 37.9 ± 9.5 | Week 1: pre-treatment assessment Week 2: 0.67 mg/kg (short-acting) followed by 1 mg/kg (long-acting), script preparation 90 min later Weeks 3–7: propranolol (short-and long-acting, same dose) 90 min before reactivation Week 8: post-treatment assessment Follow-up: 6 mo after pre-treatment |
CAPS and PCL pre-treatment, post-treatment and follow-up |
Scores pre-treatment, post-treatment and follow-up (mean ± SD) PCL:60.4 ± 11.4, 37.9 ± 14.9 and 36.0 ± 15.1 CAPS:71.8 ± 18.6, 45.8 ± 21.9 and 42.7 ± 24.6 |
Brunet et al.30 (2011; study 2) | Chronic PTSD | 7/0 | 7/0 | 29/71 | 40.1 ± 11.8 | Week 1: pre-treatment assessment Week 2: 40 mg (short-acting) followed by 80 mg (long-acting) and oral script preparation 90 min later Weeks 3–7: propranolol (short-and long-acting, same dose) 90 min before reactivation Week 8: post-treatment assessment Follow-up: 6 mo after pre-treatment |
CAPS pre-treatment, post-treatment and follow-up |
Scores pre-treatment, post-treatment and follow-up (mean ± SD) CAPS:68.4 ± 15.8, 35.6 ± 31.2 and 34.1 ± 33.2 |
Brunet et al.30 (2011; study 3) | Chronic PTSD | 7/0 | 7/0 | 29/71 | 47.9 ± 15.7 | Week 1: pre-treatment assessment Week 2: 40 mg (short-acting) followed by 80 mg (long-acting) and script preparation 90 min later Weeks 3–7: 80 mg (long-acting) 90 min before reactivation Week 8: post-treatment assessment Follow-up: 6 mo after pre-treatment |
PCL 6 mo post-disaster, pre-treatment, post-treatment and follow-up |
Score 6 mo post-disaster, pre-treatment, post-treatment and follow-up (mean ± SD) PCL: 60.9 ± 5.3, 60.7 ± 4.1, 41.0 ± 4.3 and 38.4 ± 3.6 |
Deng et al.51 (2020; experiment 1) | Differential fear conditioning | 15/16 | 15/16 |
% female Propranolol: 53.3 Placebo: 50 |
Propranolol: 23.71 ± 0.19 Placebo: 23.47 ± 0.46 |
Day 1: learning Day 2: 40 mg 60 min before reactivation Day 3: long-term memory test |
SCR to unconditioned stimulus on day 3 at reinstatement | Propranolol < placebo on SCR to unconditioned stimulus on day 3 at reinstatement |
Deng et al.51 (2020; experiment 2) | Differential fear conditioning | 18/17 | 18/17 |
% female Propranolol: 42.9 Placebo: 52.9 |
Propranolol: 23.67 ± 0.48 Placebo: 23.24 ± 0.50 |
Day 1: learning 2 wk later: 40 mg propranolol before reactivation 24 h later: long-term memory test |
SCR to unconditioned stimulus stimulus at visit 3 | Unconditioned stimulus retrieval + propranolol blocked the return of fear (SCR) at reinstatement |
Kroes et al.49 (2016) | Differential fear conditioning | 23/24 | 22/24 | 41/59 | 21.72 ± 2.2 | Day 1: learning Day 2: 40 mg 60 min before reactivation Day 3: long-term memory task |
SCR, explicit memory and subjective experience of fear on day 3 | Propranolol < placebo on SCR and explicit memory, but not subjective experience of fear at day 3 |
Lin et al.59 (2021) | Nicotine dependence | 27/25 | 27/25 | 100/0 | Propranolol: 27.8 ± 6.69 Placebo: 28.24 ± 7.94 |
Day 1: baseline and cue-induced craving Day 2: 40 mg 60 min before reactivation (smoking-related pictures) Day 3: baseline and cue-induced craving |
Baseline craving measured with FNDT; cue-induced craving measured with brain imaging on day 3 | Significant reduction in craving in propranolol group only; propranolol < placebo on FNDT and cue-induced reactivity on day 3 |
Mahabir et al.55 (2015) | Chronic PTSD | 9/0 | 7/0 | 29/71 | 33.1 ± 7.0 | Week 1: pre-treatment assessments and script preparation Week 2: fMRI session Weeks 3–8: 1 mg/kg 75 min before reactivation with script Week 9: fMRI session Week 10: diagnostic assessment |
CAPS and IES-R pre-treatment and post-treatment |
Score pre-treatment and post-treatment (mean ± SD) CAPS: 80.4 ± 17.6 and 41.0 ± 27.2 IES-R: 62.8 ± 12.9 and 24.4 ± 23.4 |
Saladin et al.25 (2013) | Cocaine dependence | 35/32 | 26/24 | 66/34 | Propranolol: 39.1 ± 8.2 Placebo: 40.8 ± 9.8 |
Day 1: 40 mg immediately after CCE sequence Day 2: CCE session with no medication Follow-up: 1 wk |
CDMS, heart rate and SCR to CCE on day 2 and at follow-up | Propranolol < placebo on CDMS and heart rate but not SCR |
Wood et al.57 (2015) | Chronic PTSD | 12/0 | 10/0 (n = 8 no reactivation + propranolol) | 100/0 | 38.7 ± 14.9 | Day 2: 0.67 mg/kg (short-acting) 90 min before reactivation and 1 mg/kg (long-acting) immediately before reactivation (script preparation) Day 8: script-driven imagery |
Heart rate, SCR and IES-R to script on day 8 |
Propranolol = placebo on heart rate, SCR and IES-R on day 8 |
CAPS = Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale; CCE = cocaine cue exposure; CDMS = Craving/Distress/Mood States scale; fMRI = functional MRI; FNDT = Fagerstrom Nicotine Dependence Test; IES-R = Impact of Event Scale–Revised; PCL = PTSD Checklist; PTSD = posttraumatic stress disorder; SCR = skin conductance response; SD = standard deviation.
Table 2.
Characteristics of included reconsolidation interference studies — healthy samples
Study | Materials | Propranolol/placebo | Male/ female, % | Age, yr, mean ± SD or SEM | Study protocol | Outcome measures of interest | Primary results of interest | Quality | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||||||
No. randomized | No. analyzed on test day | ||||||||
Bos et al.38 (2012) | Differential fear conditioning | 15/15 | 15/15 | 33/67 | 21 ± 2.6 | Day 1: learning Day 2: 40 mg 80 min before reactivation Day 3: long-term memory test |
SCR and fear-potentiated startle to conditioned stimulus during extinction/ reinstatement on day 3 | Propranolol = placebo on SCR and fear-potentiated startle to conditioned stimulus on day 3 | 4.5 |
Chalkia et al.37 (2019) | Differential fear conditioning | 15/15 | 15/15 | 27/73 | Propranolol: 22.3 ± 4.33 Placebo: 21.5 ± 2.45 |
Day 1: learning Day 2: 40 mg post-reactivation Day 3: long-term memory test |
Fear-potentiated startle and unconditioned stimulus expectancy ratings to conditioned stimulus during extinction on day 3 | Propranolol = placebo on fear-potentiated startle and unconditioned stimulus to conditioned stimulus on day 3 | 4.5 |
de Quervain et al.39 (2007) | Emotionally valenced word list | 14/14 | 14/14 | 50/50 | 23.9 ± 2.9 | Day 1: learning Day 2: 40 mg 60 min before long-term memory test 2 wk later: second long-term memory test |
Free recall no. of words during second long-term memory test | Propranolol = placebo on free recall of words during second long-term memory test | 3 |
Kindt et al.40 (2009) | Differential fear conditioning | 20/20 | 20/20 | 28/72 | 20.70 ± 2.4 | Day 1: learning Day 2: 40 mg 90 min before reactivation Day 3: long-term memory test |
Fear-potentiated startle to conditioned stimulus during extinction/ reinstatement on day 3 | Propranolol < placebo on fear-conditioned startle on day 3 | 3 |
Kroes et al.41 (2010) | Emotionally valenced word list | 12/12 | 11/12 | 58/42 | 24.4 | Day 1: learning Day 2: 40 mg 90 min before reactivation Day 3: long-term memory test |
% free recall on day 3 | Propranolol < placebo on % free recall on day 3 | 3 |
Schwabe et al.42 (2012) | Emotionally valenced images | 13/13 | 13/13 | 50/50 | 18–30 | Day 1: learning Day 2: 40 mg 90 min before reactivation Day 3: long-term memory test |
% recognition of images on day 3 | Propranolol < placebo on % recognition of images on day 3 | 2 |
Sevenster et al.50 (2012) | Differential fear conditioning | 20/20 | 18/18 | 68/32 | 21.1 ± 2.6 | Day 1: learning Day 2: 40 mg 90 min before reactivation Day 3: long-term memory test |
SCR, fear-potentiated startle and unconditioned stimulus expectancy ratings to conditioned stimulus during extinction on day 3 | Propranolol < placebo on fear-potentiated startle, but not SCR or unconditioned stimulus expectancy ratings to conditioned stimulus on day 3 | 3 |
Soeter et al.43 (2010) | Differential fear conditioning | 20/20 | 20/20 | 25/75 | 20.4 ± 3.8 | Day 1: learning Day 2: 40 mg 90 min before reactivation Day 3: long-term memory test |
Fear-potentiated startle to conditioned stimulus during extinction/ reinstatement on day 3 | Propranolol < placebo on fear-potentiated startle to conditioned stimulus on day 3 | 4.5 |
Soeter et al.44 (2012) | Differential fear conditioning | 12/12 | 12/12 | 12/88 | 20.9 ± 3.5 | Day 1: learning Day 2: 40 mg 90 min before reactivation Day 3: long-term memory test |
Fear-potentiated startle to conditioned stimulus during extinction/ reinstatement on day 3 | Propranolol < placebo on fear-potentiated startle to conditioned stimulus on day 3 | 4.5 |
Thomas et al.45 (2017, experiment 1) | Emotional story paradigm | 14/14 | 14/12 | 59/41 | 32.6 ± 11.7 | Week 1: learning Week 2: 0.67 mg/ kg, immediately after reactivation Week 3: long-term memory test |
% recognition of content of slides at week 3 | Propranolol = placebo on % recognition content of slides at week 3 | 4.5 |
Thomas et al.45 (2017, experiment 2) | Emotional story paradigm | 18/16 | 17/16 | 31/69 | 25.4 ± 7.8 | Week 1: learning Week 2: 0.67 mg/kg, 60–75 min before reactivation Week 3: long-term memory test |
% recognition of content of slides at week 3 | Propranolol < placebo on % recognition content of slides at week 3 | 4.5 |
Thome et al.46 (2016) | Differential fear conditioning | 20/20 | 20/19 | 0/100 | Propranolol: 25.5 ± 3.71 Placebo: 23.9 ± 3.1 |
Day 1: learning Day 2: 40 mg 5 min post-reactivation Day 3: long-term memory test |
Fear-potentiated startle to conditioned stimulus during extinction on day 3 | Propranolol = placebo on fear-potentiated startle to conditioned stimulus on day 3 | 3.5 |
Tollenaar et al.47 (2009) | Emotionally valenced word list | 26/27 | 26/27 | 100/0 | Propranolol: 20.6 ± 2.1 Placebo: 19.5 ± 1.4 |
Week 1: learning Week 2: 80 mg 75 min before reactivation Week 3: long-term memory test |
% recognition of words in week 3 | Propranolol = placebo on % recognition of words at week 3 | 4.5 |
Tollenaar et al.48 (2009) | Script-driven imagery | 27/26 | 27/26 | 100/0 | Propranolol: 20.7 ± 2.2 Placebo: 19.5 ± 1.4 |
Week 1: script preparation Week 2: 80 mg 90 min before reactivation Week 3: heart rate and SCR to script |
Heart rate and SCR at week 3 | Propranolol = placebo on % recognition of words at week 3 | 4 |
SCR = skin conductance response; SD = standard deviation; SEM = standard error of the mean.
Table 3.
Characteristics of included reconsolidation interference studies — clinical samples
Study | Clinical diagnosis | Propranolol/placebo | Male/ female, % | Age, yr, mean ± SD | Study protocol | Outcome measures of interest | Primary results of interest | Quality | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||||||
No. randomized | No. analyzed on test day | ||||||||
Brunet et al.52 (2008) | Chronic PTSD | 9/10 | 9/10 | 47/53 | Propranolol: 34.8 ± 10.1 Placebo: 35.1 ± 10.5 |
Week 1: script preparation, 40 mg (short-acting) immediately after reactivation and 60 mg (long-acting) after 2 h Week 2: script-driven imagery |
Heart rate SCR to personal script at week 2 | Propranolol < placebo on heart rate and SCR to personal script | 4 |
Brunet et al.56 (2014) | Chronic PTSD | 28/10 | 22/10* | 32/68 | 37.9 ± 9.5 | Week 1: 0.67 mg/kg (short-acting) followed by 1 mg/kg (long-acting), script preparation 90 min later Weeks 2–6: 0.67 mg/kg (short-acting) and 1 mg/kg (long-acting) 90 min before reactivation Week 7: script-driven imagery 4 mo follow-up: script-driven imagery |
Heart rate and SCR to personal script at 1 wk post-treatment | Propranolol < placebo on heart rate and SCR to personal script at 1 wk post-treatment | N/A |
Brunet et al.23 (2018) | Chronic PTSD | 30/30 | 21/23 | 42/58 | Propranolol: 37.0 ± 11.3 Placebo: 41.8 ± 11.1 |
Week 1: 0.67 mg/kg (short-acting) then 1.0 mg/kg (long-acting) 60 min before script preparation Weeks 2–6: 0.67 mg/kg (short-acting) plus 1.0 mg/kg (long-acting) 90 min before reactivation Week 7: Post-treatment assessment |
PCL-S and CAPS at post-treatment | Propranolol < placebo on PCL-S and CAPS at post-treatment | 5 |
Elsey et al.54 (2020) | Fear of public speaking | 40/20 | 40/20 | 17/83 | Propranolol: 21.65 ± 2.78 Placebo: 22.10 ± 1.92 |
Week 1: Baseline measures, speech preparation; 40 mg of propranolol administered < 5 min post-speech (reactivation) Week 2: stress-inducing speech task (0 to 9 min) as reactivation 1 mo follow-up: symptom evaluation 3 mo follow-up: symptom evaluation |
GPSP, SUDS and PRPSA at week 2 | Propranolol = placebo on GPSP, SUDS and PRPSA at week 2 | 4 |
Jobes et al.27 (2015) | Cocaine abuse in poly-drug dependence | 19/16 | 18/15 | 48/52 | Propranolol: 41.6 Placebo: 42.1 |
Week 1: personalized cocaine script, tactile drug-related paraphernalia Week 2: 40 mg 120 min before reactivation Week 3: re-exposure test session Week 7: re-exposure test session |
CCQ and VAS at 1 wk post-intervention | Propranolol > placebo on CCQ and VAS at 1 wk post-intervention | 3.5 |
Lonergan et al.24 (2016) | Substance dependence | 9/8 | 6/4 | 71/29 | Propranolol: 44.78 ± 18.66 Placebo: 35.63 ± 16.09 |
Week 0: assessment and craving script preparation Week 1–3: 6 biweekly sessions (separated by 48 h) of 1 mg/kg of propranolol 60 min before craving-memory reactivation Week 4: post-treatment assessment |
Self-report craving questionnaires post-treatment | Propranolol < placebo on self-reported craving at post-treatment (in intention-to-treat analysis) | 4.5 |
Pachas et al.28 (2015) | Nicotine dependence | 35/39 | 23/31 | 73/27 | Propranolol: 41.6 ± 10.9 Placebo: 42.5 ± 9.8 |
Week 1: screening and evaluation; 0.67 mg/kg (short-acting), 1 mg/kg (long-acting) 90 min later, followed by reactivation (personal script) Week 2: script-driven imagery |
Heart rate, SCR and self-reported craving at week 2 | Propranolol = placebo on heart rate, SCR and self-reported craving at week 2 | 3.5 |
Roullet et al.58 (2021; severe symptom group, PCL-S score > 65) | PTSD | 18/15 | Not reported | Unknown† | Unknown† | Week 1: 0.67 mg/kg (short-acting) then 1.0 mg/kg (long-acting) 60 min before script preparation Weeks 2–6: 0.67 mg/kg (short-acting) plus 1.0 mg/kg (long-acting) 90 min before reactivation Week 7: post-treatment assessment |
PCL-S 3 mo post-treatment |
Propranolol < placebo on the PCL-S 3 mo post-treatment |
5 |
Roullet et al.58 (2021; moderate symptom group (PCL-S score > 45 < 65) | PTSD | 11/14 | Not reported | Unknown† | Unknown† | Week 1: 0.67 mg/kg (short-acting) then 1.0 mg/kg (long-acting) 60 min before script preparation Weeks 2–6: 0.67 mg/kg (short-acting) plus 1.0 mg/kg (long-acting) 90 min before reactivation Week 7: post-treatment assessment |
PCL-S 3 mo post-treatment |
Propranolol = placebo on the PCL-S 3 mo post-treatment |
5 |
Soeter et al.7 (2015) | Spider phobia | 15/15 | 15/15 | 9/91 | 21.6 ± 3.2 | Day 1: pre-treatment assessments and BAT with baby tarantula Day 5: 40 mg post-reactivation exposure to tarantula Day 16: post-treatment assessment Follow-up: 3 mo and 1 yr |
BAT to tarantula and SPQ at post-treatment day 16 | Propranolol showed > approach BAT to tarantula than placebo, but = SPQ scores at day 16 | 4 |
Xue et al.53 (2017) | Nicotine dependence | 96 randomized; 27 excluded Of 69 included: 24 placebo + reactivation, 23 propranolol + reactivation, 22 non-placebo control |
23/24 | 100/0 | Propranolol: 24.96 ± 5.9 Placebo: 23.00 ± 4.0 |
Day 1: screening and baseline tests of preference and craving for pre-existing nicotine conditioned stimulus Days 2–4: conditioning Day 5: post-conditioning test of preference and craving for the conditioned stimulus Day 6: 40 mg 60 min before unconditioned stimulus memory reactivation Day 7: post-treatment test |
Subjective ratings of nicotine craving VAS on day 7 | Propranolol < placebo on subjective ratings of nicotine craving VAS on day 7 | 4.5 |
Zhao et al.26 (2011) | Heroin dependence | 18/18 | 18/18 | 100/0 | Propranolol: 38.11 ± 1.25 Placebo: 38.00 ± 1.30 |
Day 1: Learning drug-related words Day 2: 40 mg 60 min before reactivation Day 3: long-term memory task |
Free recall no. of drug-related words on day 3 | Propranolol < placebo on free recall of drug-related words on day 3 | 3.5 |
BAT = Behavioural Approach Test; CAPS = Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale; CCQ = Cocaine Craving Questionnaire; GPSP = Global Perception of Speech Performance; PCL-S = PTSD Checklist–Specific; PRPSA = Personal Report of Public Speaking Anxiety; PTSD = posttraumatic stress disorder; SCR = skin conductance response; SD = standard deviation; SPQ = Spider Phobia Questionnaire; SUDS = Subjective Units of Distress; VAS = visual analogue scale.
Data from 10 placebo participants from Brunet and colleagues52 (2008) were included in the between-group analysis in Brunet and colleagues56 (2014). These data were included in the meta-analysis; analyses were conducted with and without these data.
Full sample demographics: propranolol n = 33 randomized, n = 29 treatment completers, n = 20 women, age (mean ± SD) = 35.6 ± 12.8 yr, n = 26 at 3 mo follow-up; placebo n = 33 randomized, n = 29 treatment completers, n = 21 women, age (mean ± SD) = 42.2 ± 12.7 yr, n = 25 at 3 mo follow-up. Analyses were carried out and presented on the intention-to-treat sample.
Studies involving healthy participants
In 11 of the 17 studies involving healthy participants, a differential fear-conditioning paradigm was used. Of these, 8 found that propranolol administered during memory reactivation lowered physiologic responses to fear-conditioned stimuli compared to placebo.40,43,44,46,49–51 In one of these studies, the effect was observed only among participants who expected to receive the unconditioned stimulus during the reactivation trial on day 2 (i.e., the shock), but did not (i.e., prediction error).50 In another study, the between-group effect of propranolol was also observed for when the unconditioned stimulus was the reactivation cue.51
The other 6 studies employed standard emotional material (i.e., a video, a slide show or pictures) or asked participants to write a personal narrative detailing a negative event as the memory task. Of the 6 studies, 4 found that participants who received propranolol remembered less emotional material than those who received a placebo.39,41,42,45
Studies involving clinical samples
Of the 19 studies involving a clinical sample, 8 involved participants with chronic PTSD,23,30,52,55–57 2 involved participants with chronic PTSD comorbid with symptoms of major depression,58 7 involved participants with substance dependence24–28,53,59 and 2 involved participants with specific phobias.7,54
Ten clinical studies used a single reactivation session7,25–28,52–54,57,59 and 9 used several (4–6) sessions.23,24,30,55,56,58 To reactivate the aversive or appetitive memories, 13 studies used written or audiorecorded trauma or drug-use narratives,23,24,27,28,30,52,55–58 4 used other exposure-based or stress-inducing tasks (e.g., public speaking)7,25,53,54 and 2 employed cues from a previously learned list of drug-related words or smoking-related pictures.26,59
Of the 13 studies that used written or auditory trauma or drug-use narratives, 8 found that propranolol-treated participants showed improvements in trauma-related or substance-dependence symptoms.23,24,30,52,55,56 In 1 publication,58 no treatment effect was found for participants with moderate PTSD symptoms at 3 months post-treatment; however, a significant effect was observed for participants with severe PTSD symptoms. Three other studies failed to find an effect of reconsolidation impairment with propranolol compared to placebo.27,28,57 Finally, for 5 of the 6 clinical studies that implemented other exposure-based methods and the studies that used a word list or pictures, propranolol paired with reactivation improved clinical symptoms compared to placebo paired with reactivation at the post-treatment test phase.7,25,26,53,59 The remaining study did not find an effect for propranolol.54
Quantitative results
Quality assessment of studies in the meta-analysis
Of the 26 total studies included in the meta-analysis, 18 had a Jadad score of 3.5 or higher, indicating good methodological quality. To maximize the sample size, we retained studies with lower methodological quality in the meta-analysis and examined study quality as a moderator variable. See Tables 1 and 2 for individual quality assessment ratings.
Studies involving healthy participants
As shown in Figure 2, propranolol-treated participants (n = 242) remembered less aversive material than placebo-treated participants (n = 236; g = −0.51, 95 % CI −0.84 to −0.19; p = 0.002). However, statistical heterogeneity was significant (Q13 = 40.06, p < 0.001; I2 = 67.55 %). Sensitivity analyses revealed no outliers. Effect sizes varied between −1.82 and 0.22.
Figure 2.
Reconsolidation interference in healthy samples. CI = confidence interval; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit; SE = standard error.
Studies involving clinical samples
As shown in Figure 3, 236 participants received propranolol and 210 participants received placebo. The studies were heterogeneous (Q11 = 29.96, p = 0.002; I2 = 63.28 %).
Figure 3.
Reconsolidation interference in clinical samples. CI = confidence interval; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit; SE = standard error.
Memory reactivation under propranolol reduced symptom severity and cue-induced physiologic reactivity compared to memory reactivation under placebo, with a moderate to strong effect size in 7 of 12 studies (g = −1.28 to −0.51).7,23,26,52,53,56,58 The overall effect size was moderate, and the difference was significant in favour of propranolol (g = −0.42, 95 % CI −0.74 to −0.10; p = 0.010). Effect sizes ranged between −1.28 and 0.60. According to sensitivity analyses, no single study explained the heterogeneity we observed.
Publication bias
Analysis of publication bias in the healthy samples revealed a relatively symmetrical funnel plot, and results from the trim and fill analysis under a random-effects model indicated that no studies to the right of the mean (i.e., null or negative findings) were missing from the funnel plot. However, the result of the Egger test was statistically significant (p = 0.023), suggesting a risk of publication bias (i.e., small studies reporting large effects).
Concerning the clinical samples, analyses revealed a relatively symmetric funnel plot, and results from the trim and fill analysis under a random-effects model indicated that no studies to the right of the mean were missing from the funnel plot. The result of the Egger test was not statistically significant (p = 0.31), suggesting little risk of publication bias.
Exploring sources of heterogeneity
We conducted post hoc analyses to explore whether moderating variables explained the heterogeneity we observed. Specifically, we examined the following variables separately in both healthy and clinical samples: outcome measured (i.e., declarative memory v. physiologic reactivity in healthy samples; symptom severity v. physiologic reactivity in clinical samples); medication dosage (40 mg v. other dosages); medication timing (pre-reactivation v. post-reactivation); sex (male v. female); and study quality (good v. poor). We used the delay before the memory test or symptom measurement (1 d v. 1 wk) as a moderator for analysis of only the healthy samples because a limited number of clinical studies measured outcomes after 1 day. None of these variables explained the observed heterogeneity.
Finally, for clinical samples only, we examined whether memory valence (aversive v. appetitive) moderated the strength of the effect size. The overall effect size was larger for studies that involved aversive memories (e.g., trauma or phobia; g = −0.53, p = 0.016) than for those involving appetitive memories (e.g., drug-related; g = −0.27, p = 0.29), although the between-group difference was not statistically significant (Q1 = 0.587, p = 0.44).
Discussion
Emotional memories can be successfully weakened by administration of the reconsolidation blocker propranolol paired with memory reactivation. Cue-induced physiologic reactivity and declarative memory for emotional material was reduced in healthy individuals who reactivated a memory under propranolol compared to those who did it under placebo. This finding was consistent with the findings of the meta-analysis by Lonergan and colleagues14 and was congruent with reconsolidation interference theory.
In clinical samples, reactivating a pathogenic memory under propranolol reduced symptom severity and related physiologic reactivity in participants with PTSD, specific phobia or substance dependence, using a personal trauma or drug-use narrative or other exposure-based methods. Arguably (and similar to studies involving healthy samples in fearconditioning or cue-induced reactivity paradigms), a reduction in physiologic responding to traumatic, phobic or drug-using memory can be considered an indicator of a change in the memory (e.g., reduced salience), such that the memory is less capable of symptom provocation.60 This finding was also congruent with reconsolidation interference theory and supported the clinical efficacy of this intervention. Importantly, the strength of the effect size in clinical samples was in line with the efficacy of established psychotherapies for the same disorders, such as cognitive behavioural therapy for PTSD.61,62 In addition, of the 7 clinical studies that were excluded from meta-analysis, 530,55,59 found a treatment effect in favour of propranolol, suggesting that incorporating these studies into a meta-analysis may have increased the overall effect.
One study of participants with PTSD and comorbid depression58 did not find an effect of propranolol that surpassed their placebo group at 1 week post-treatment; however, a treatment effect was observed among participants with severe PTSD and at 3 months post-treatment. It is possible that for severe cases of PTSD, reconsolidation impairment with propranolol may be efficacious in the longer term, considering that, unlike other exposure-based therapies, reconsolidation is unlikely to be prone to relapse. However, there was no mention in this study of a protocolized treatment manual or treatment adherence monitoring and rating. Thus, the influence of such factors on the observed effect could not be assessed.
Contrary to our previous meta-analysis,14 the current results for reconsolidation impairment in healthy samples showed no moderation effect for medication dosage (40 mg v. 80 mg), delay between drug administration and recall (24 h v. 1 wk) or participant sex. Considering that the moderation observed in the earlier meta-analysis was driven by 2 studies, it seems that increasing the number of included studies watered down the observed effects. Moreover, the results of the meta-analysis of studies in clinical samples did not reveal significant moderation by any sample characteristic (e.g., sex, type of psychological disorder) or methodological difference (e.g., type of outcome measured, medication dosage, medication timing, delay before memory test or symptom measurement).
Although methodological differences did not moderate the findings in either analysis, a great deal of variation existed across the examined studies that was related mainly to the memory-reactivation procedures (i.e., stimuli used, drug dosage or timing, number of sessions). This variability complicated our ability to contrast studies and identify the ideal protocol for conducting reconsolidation impairment. Although the included studies were deemed sufficiently homogeneous to provide a meaningful synthesis under a random-effects model,34,63 one issue may have been related to the type of disorder under study. In this analysis, fear-based (i.e., trauma or phobia) studies demonstrated a larger overall effect than drug-related studies. Although this may have been related to nuances in the neurobiological substrates involved in drug-related learning,8,28 it may also have been because of methodological factors within studies (i.e., the duration of the reactivation session; see also Walsh and colleagues29). Thus, establishment of a standardized treatment protocol that can be implemented in research settings is desirable to identify variables related to treatment efficacy.
One last factor that may have contributed to the observed heterogeneity was the notion of mismatch or prediction error.64 For memories to enter a state of lability, new or unexpected information must be provided.50,65 Without this mismatch, the memory would not destabilize, and retrieval would not set the stage for reconsolidation, thus preventing propranolol from playing its role of reconsolidation impairment. Therefore, it may be necessary for mismatch to be integrated into the memory-reactivation procedures to improve the validity and efficacy of reconsolidation impairment as a psychiatric treatment.66 Of the studies included in this analysis, only one intentionally incorporated prediction error into a study involving healthy volunteers.50 The absence of mismatch in some reconsolidation studies, particularly in clinical samples, may explain some of the negative findings.67–69
Limitations
As discussed, the relatively small number of included studies, and the methodological differences between them, precluded an in-depth assessment of potential moderators of between-group effects of reconsolidation impairment using propranolol. In line with this, reporting biases within and between included studies may have not only contributed to the observed heterogeneity, but also introduced bias in the effect-size estimates. We specifically chose Hedges g for this analysis because it corrects for overestimation of between-group effects associated with small samples, and because we wanted to maintain consistency with previously published meta-analyses.14,29 However, Hedges g performs best under parametric assumptions in the source studies. Future meta-analyses may opt to explore nonparametric measures of effect size (e.g., probability of superiority) to further expand on our findings.70
Conclusion
Findings from this meta-analysis suggest that compared to placebo, propranolol paired with memory reactivation can weaken emotional memories in healthy participants and clinical samples. The findings of this meta-analysis highlight the need to identify the essential procedural components required to successfully induce and impair memory reconsolidation in clinical samples to maximize treatment efficacy. Clarifying the ideal method for impairing reconsolidation would extend our understanding of the mechanisms through which memory reconsolidation occurs and help shed light on how to consistently ensure better treatment outcomes. Furthermore, future studies should test how to systematically incorporate mismatch into reconsolidation impairment, and whether this improves outcome.
The current findings bridge an important gap between pre-clinical experimental findings and the clinical implementation of reconsolidation impairment. Reconsolidation impairment using propranolol is an innovative intervention with far-reaching clinical implications in the field of psychiatry, considering that a number of mental disorders stem from pathological memories.
Acknowledgements
The authors wish to thank Reagan Schoenholz and Zainab Yahya for their participation in data collection, data extraction, and quality assessment.
Footnotes
Competing interests: None declared.
Contributors: S. Pigeon, M. Lonergan, R.K. Pitman and A. Brunet designed the study. S. Pigeon and O. Rotondo acquired the data, which S. Pigeon, M. Lonergan, O. Rotondo and A. Brunet analyzed. All authors wrote the article, which S. Pigeon, M. Lonergan, R.K. Pitman and A Brunet reviewed. All authors approved the final version to be published and can certify that no other individuals not listed as authors have made substantial contributions to the paper.
Funding: M. Lonergan received a doctoral award from the Fonds de Recherche du Québec (FRQ-S). S. Pigeon received a master’s award from FRQ-S and from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research while working on this project.
References
- 1.Mishkin M, Malamut B, Bachevalier J, et al. Neurobiology of learning and memory. New York: Guilford Press; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Pitman RK. Post-traumatic stress disorder, hormones, and memory. Biol Psychiatry 1989;26:221–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.McGaugh JL. Making lasting memories: remembering the significant. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2013;110:10402–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.McGaugh JL. The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annu Rev Neurosci 2004; 27:1–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Mineka S, Zinbarg R. A contemporary learning theory perspective on the etiology of anxiety disorders: it’s not what you thought it was. Am Psychol 2006;61:10–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Everitt BJ, Dickinson A, Robbins TW. The neuropsychological basis of addictive behaviour. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 2001;36:129–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Soeter M, Kindt M. An abrupt transformation of phobic behavior after a post-retrieval amnesic agent. Biol Psychiatry 2015;78:880–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Torregrossa MM, Corlett PR, Taylor JR, et al. Aberrant learning and memory in addiction. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2011;96:609–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Beckers T, Kindt M. Memory reconsolidation interference as an emerging treatment for emotional disorders: strengths, limitations, challenges, and opportunities. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2017;13:99–121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Elsey JW, Van Ast VA, Kindt M. Human memory reconsolidation: a guiding framework and critical review of the evidence. Psychol Bull 2018;144:797–848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Przybyslawski J, Roullet P, Sara SJ. Attenuation of emotional and nonemotional memories after their reactivation: role of β adrenergic receptors. J Neurosci 1999;19:6623–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.O’Carroll RE, Drysdale E, Cahill L, et al. Stimulation of the noradrenergic system enhances and blockade reduces memory for emotional material in man. Psychol Med 1999;29:1083–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Przybyslawski J, Roullet P, Sara SJ. Attenuation of emotional and nonemotional memories after their reactivation: Role of β adrenergic receptors. J Neurosci 1999;19:6623–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Lonergan MH, Olivera-Figueroa LA, Pitman RK, et al. Propranolol’s effects on the consolidation and reconsolidation of long-term emotional memory in healthy participants: a meta-analysis. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2013;38:222–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Huang Y-Y, Kandel ER. Low-frequency stimulation induces a pathway-specific late phase of LTP in the amygdala that is mediated by PKA and dependent on protein synthesis. Learn Mem 2007;14:497–503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hurlemann R, Walter H, Rehme A, et al. Human amygdala reactivity is diminished by the β-noradrenergic antagonist propranolol. Psychol Med 2010;40:1839–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Tully K, Bolshakov VY. Emotional enhancement of memory: how norepinephrine enables synaptic plasticity. Mol Brain 2010;3:15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Alberini CM, LeDoux JE. Memory reconsolidation. Curr Biol 2013; 23:R746–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Dunbar AB, Taylor JR. Garcinol blocks the reconsolidation of multiple cocaine-paired cues after a single cocaine-reactivation session. Neuropsychopharmacology 2017;42:1884–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Misanin JR, Miller RR, Lewis DJ. Retrograde amnesia produced by electroconvulsive shock after reactivation of a consolidated memory trace. Science 1968;160:554–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Sara SJ. Retrieval and reconsolidation: toward a neurobiology of remembering. Learn Mem 2000;7:73–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Diergaarde L, Schoffelmeer AN, De Vries TJ. Pharmacological manipulation of memory reconsolidation: towards a novel treatment of pathogenic memories. Eur J Pharmacol 2008;585:453–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Brunet A, Saumier D, Liu A, et al. Reduction of PTSD symptoms with pre-reactivation propranolol therapy: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Psychiatry 2018;175:427–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lonergan M, Saumier D, Tremblay J, et al. Reactivating addiction-related memories under propranolol to reduce craving: a pilot randomized controlled trial. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2016;50:245–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Saladin ME, Gray KM, McRae-Clark AL, et al. A double blind, placebo controlled study of the effects of post-retrieval propranolol on reconsolidation of memory for craving and cue reactivity in cocaine dependent humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2013;226:721–37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Zhao LY, Sun LL, Shi J, et al. Effects of beta-adrenergic receptor blockade on drug-related memory reconsolidation in abstinent heroin addicts. Drug Alcohol Depend 2011;118:224–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Jobes ML, Aharonovich E, Epstein DH, et al. Effects of pre-reactivation propranolol on cocaine craving elicited by imagery script/cue sets in opioid-dependent polydrug users: a randomized study. J Addict Med 2015;9:491–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Pachas GN, Gilman J, Orr SP, et al. Single dose propranolol does not affect physiologic or emotional reactivity to smoking cues. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2015;232:1619–28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Walsh KH, Das RK, Saladin ME, et al. Modulation of naturalistic maladaptive memories using behavioural and pharmacological reconsolidation-interfering strategies: a systematic review and meta-analysis of clinical and “sub-clinical” studies. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2018;235:2507–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Brunet A, Poundja J, Tremblay J, et al. Trauma reactivation under the influence of propranolol decreases posttraumatic stress symptoms and disorder: 3 open-label trials. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2011;31:547–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Jadad AR, Moore RA, Carroll D, et al. Assessing the quality of reports of randomized clinical trials: is blinding necessary? Control Clin Trials 1996;17:1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Lipsey MW, Wilson DB. Practical meta-analysis. New York: Sage Publications; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hedges LV. Distribution theory for Glass’s estimator of effect size and related estimators. J Educ Stat 1981;6:107–28. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JP, et al. Introduction to meta-analysis. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 2021. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Higgins JP, Thomas J, Chandler J, et al. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 2019. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Shi L, Lin L. The trim-and-fill method for publication bias: practical guidelines and recommendations based on a large database of meta-analyses. Medicine (Baltimore) 2019;98:e15987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Chalkia A, Weermeijer J, Van Oudenhove L, et al. Acute but not permanent effects of propranolol on fear memory expression in humans. Front Hum Neurosci 2019;13:51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Bos MG, Beckers T, Kindt M. The effects of noradrenergic blockade on extinction in humans. Biol Psychol 2012;89:598–605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.de Quervain DJ-F, Aerni A, Roozendaal B. Preventive effect of β-adrenoceptor blockade on glucocorticoid-induced memory retrieval deficits. Am J Psychiatry 2007;164:967–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Kindt M, Soeter M, Vervliet B. Beyond extinction: erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Nat Neurosci 2009;12:256–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kroes MCW, Strange BA, Dolan RJ. β-adrenergic blockade during memory retrieval in humans evokes a sustained reduction of declarative emotional memory enhancement. J Neurosci 2010;30:3959–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Schwabe L, Nader K, Wolf OT, et al. Neural signature of reconsolidation impairments by propranolol in humans. Biol Psychiatry 2012;71:380–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Soeter M, Kindt M. Dissociating response systems: erasing fear from memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2010;94:30–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Soeter M, Kindt M. Erasing fear for an imagined threat event. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2012;37:1769–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Thomas E, Saumier D, Pitman RK, et al. Consolidation and reconsolidation are impaired by oral propranolol administered before but not after memory (re)activation in humans. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2017;142:118–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Thome J, Koppe G, Hauschild S, et al. Modification of fear memory by pharmacological and behavioural interventions during reconsolidation. PLoS One 2016;11:e0161044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Tollenaar MS, Elzinga BM, Spinhoven P, et al. Psychophysiological responding to emotional memories in healthy young men after cortisol and propranolol administration. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2009;203:793–803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Tollenaar MS, Elzinga BM, Spinhoven P, et al. Immediate and prolonged effects of cortisol, but not propranolol, on memory retrieval in healthy young men. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2009;91:23–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Kroes MCW, Tona K-D, den Ouden HEM, et al. How administration of the beta-blocker propranolol before extinction can prevent the return of fear. Neuropsychopharmacology 2016;41:1569–78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Sevenster D, Beckers T, Kindt M. Retrieval per se is not sufficient to trigger reconsolidation of human fear memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2012;97:338–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Deng J, Shi L, Yuan K, et al. Propranolol-induced inhibition of unconditioned stimulus-reactivated fear memory prevents the return of fear in humans. Transl Psychiatry 2020;10:1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Brunet A, Orr SP, Tremblay J, et al. Effect of post-retrieval propranolol on psychophysiologic responding during subsequent script-driven traumatic imagery in post-traumatic stress disorder. J Psychiatr Res 2008;42:503–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Xue Y-X, Deng J-H, Chen Y-Y, et al. Effect of selective inhibition of reactivated nicotine-associated memories with propranolol on nicotine craving. JAMA Psychiatry 2017;74:224–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Elsey JWB, Filmer AI, Galvin HR, et al. Reconsolidation-based treatment for fear of public speaking: a systematic pilot study using propranolol. Transl Psychiatry 2020;10:179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Mahabir M, Tucholka A, Shin LM, et al. Emotional face processing in post-traumatic stress disorder after reconsolidation impairment using propranolol: a pilot fMRI study. J Anxiety Disord 2015;36:127–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Brunet A, Thomas É, Saumier D, et al. Trauma reactivation plus propranolol is associated with durably low physiological responding during subsequent script-driven traumatic imagery. Can J Psychiatry 2014;59:228–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Wood NE, Rosasco ML, Suris AM, et al. Pharmacological blockade of memory reconsolidation in posttraumatic stress disorder: three negative psychophysiological studies. Psychiatry Res 2015;225:31–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Roullet P, Vaiva G, Véry E, et al. Traumatic memory reactivation with or without propranolol for PTSD and comorbid MD symptoms: a randomised clinical trial. Neuropsychopharmacology 2021; 46:1643–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Lin X, Deng J, Yuan K, et al. Neural substrates of propranolol-induced impairments in the reconsolidation of nicotine-associated memories in smokers. Transl Psychiatry 2021;11:441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Pineles SL, Suvak MK, Liverant GI, et al. Psychophysiologic reactivity, subjective distress, and their associations with PTSD diagnosis. J Abnorm Psychol 2013;122:635–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Kar N. Cognitive behavioral therapy for the treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder: a review. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2011; 7:167–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Bisson JI, Ehlers A, Matthews R, et al. Psychological treatments for chronic post-traumatic stress disorder: systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 2007;190:97–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Deeks JJ, Higgins JP, Altman DG, et al. Analysing data and undertaking meta-analyses. In: Higgins J, Thomas J, Chandler J, et al., editors. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 6.2. London (UK): Cochrane Collaboration; 2021: 241–84. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Pedreira ME, Pérez-Cuesta LM, Maldonado H. Mismatch between what is expected and what actually occurs triggers memory reconsolidation or extinction. Learn Mem 2004;11:579–85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Lee JL. Reconsolidation: maintaining memory relevance. Trends Neurosci 2009;32:413–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Ecker B. Memory reconsolidation understood and misunderstood. Int J Neuropsychotherapy 2015;3:2–46. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Cammarota M, Bevilaqua LR, Medina JH, et al. Retrieval does not induce reconsolidation of inhibitory avoidance memory. Learn Mem 2004;11:572–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Hernandez PJ, Kelley AE. Long-term memory for instrumental responses does not undergo protein synthesis-dependent reconsolidation upon retrieval. Learn Mem 2004;11:748–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Mileusnic R, Lancashire CL, Rose SP. Recalling an aversive experience by day-old chicks is not dependent on somatic protein synthesis. Learn Mem 2005;12:615–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Marfo P, Okyere GJH. The accuracy of effect-size estimates under normals and contaminated normals in meta-analysis. Heliyon 2019;5:e01838. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]