Skip to main content
Sage Choice logoLink to Sage Choice
. 2020 Jun 6;37(7-8):NP4212–NP4237. doi: 10.1177/0886260520925781

Empowerment or Threat: Perceptions of Childhood Sexual Abuse in the #MeToo Era

Melissa S de Roos 1,, Daniel N Jones 2
PMCID: PMC8980449  PMID: 32508234

Abstract

The rise of the #MeToo movement has shed light on the prominence of sexual violence, and its victims who often remain silent. Despite increasing awareness, victims or survivors of sexual violence who disclose may be faced with negative reactions such as disbelief or blame. Such reactions extend to child victims of sexual abuse. This study aimed to shed light on gender differences in responses to sexual violence against a backdrop of #MeToo. Through an online survey (N = 253) on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk, we assessed participants’ exposure to and perception of the #MeToo movement. In addition, we measured proximity to a victim or perpetrator of sexual violence. The effect of these variables on participants’ response to a disclosure of childhood sexual abuse was examined. Results indicated that men are more likely to perceive the movement as threatening than women. Furthermore, a discrepancy in proximity to sexual violence emerged, with women more likely to know a victim and men more likely to know a perpetrator. In response to a disclosure of childhood sexual abuse, men were more likely to respond in a skeptical manner than women. Positive perceptions of the #MeToo movement translated into more supportive responses to a disclosure. Proximity to a victim of sexual violence did not impact how people responded to a disclosure, but proximity to a perpetrator was associated with a more negative response. Although the aim of this movement is to give a voice to victims of sexual violence, it may trigger a defensive response from men, which makes them more skeptical toward disclosures of victimization.

Keywords: sexual abuse, child abuse, reporting/disclosure, sexual assault, cultural contexts


The #MeToo movement gained widespread attention on Twitter in October 2017. It was an attempt to highlight the prevalence of sexual violence and to give a voice to victims particularly in the workplace, many of whom had stayed silent before (Smartt, 2017). Ten years before then, counselor Tarana Burke started this movement to draw attention to sexual violence, especially as experienced by members of racial and ethnic minority groups (Snyder & Lopez, 2017).

The hashtag was used as a way to empower female victims of sexual violence and to highlight the prevalence of sexual victimization (Ohlheiser, 2017). In the first 24 hours, 4.7 million people shared the hashtag on Facebook, and 45% of Facebook users had at least one friend who shared the hashtag (Santiago & Criss, 2017). The hashtag trended on Twitter in at least 85 countries (Strum, 2017), showing the international impact of the movement.

Since the rise of the #MeToo movement, responses to the movement have not been universally positive. Criticisms of #MeToo take several forms. For example, people have questioned what its ultimate purpose is (Wilhelm, 2017) and whether it accomplishes any goal. Furthermore, some people have characterized the movement as a witch hunt, and some think that the focus should be only on the worst types of abuse, rather than on mere misconduct, to prevent the public from becoming desensitized (Stephens, 2017). Some have criticized the movement for putting the burden of having to come forward on the victims, with a risk of retraumatizing them in the process (Gerson, 2017). Finally, concerns have been expressed regarding the possibility of false allegations and blaming accused perpetrators without due process (e.g., Cromwell, 2017; Stephens, 2017).

Although it has been suggested that victims should be referred to as survivors, there is some disagreement over the appropriate term (Schwark & Bohner, 2019). It should be the individual’s choice whether they want to be referred to as a victim or survivor. Both terms are imperfect, but “survivor” suggests recovery (Thompson, 2000), which is a complex, ongoing process that the authors cannot assume pertains to all individuals who participated in research. For this reason, we have chosen to use the term “victim”.

Gender Differences in Responses to Sexual Violence

There is a well-documented gender difference in how people respond to sexual violence, with men more likely to respond skeptically to a disclosure, and to blame the person who makes the disclosure (for a review, see Suarez & Gadalla, 2010). Several explanations for this gender difference have been proposed, and this article will focus on defensive attribution theory (DAT; Shaver, 1970) and proximity to sexual victimization. DAT might be particularly relevant because the high visibility of the #MeToo movement may increase the likelihood of defensive responses.

Research has shown that when people feel threatened because of their membership of a social category, they feel more strongly connected to that category, especially if it represents a minority in society (e.g., Brown & Ross, 1982; Grant, 1992). Social categories people readily identify with include ethnicity and gender (Tajfel & Turner, 1979). Although women have traditionally represented a minority group, initiatives such as the #MeToo movement may operate in a manner that makes women feel empowered in a way they have not felt before. Based on research on social identity, we may expect that because of this shift, some men will feel a stronger identification with other men. This identification may include those accused of sexual violence, when they are confronted with accounts or repostings of #MeToo. The increased attention on mostly women’s experiences of sexual victimization may serve as a threat that triggers some male observers to make defensive attributions in response to disclosures. These defensive attributions (e.g., Walster, 1966) may stem from the perception that they may become the victim of an erroneous claim of causing sexual trauma, which could potentially create perceptions of persecution (Lyon et al., 1994).

Another proposed explanation for gender differences is proximity to sexual victimization, whether through own experiences or those of close others. Several studies have found that people who experienced (sexual) victimization were more likely to believe allegations of sexual violence (Cromer & Freyd, 2007; Miller & Cromer, 2015; Nuttall & Jackson, 1994). These findings fit with the DAT to explain gender differences and expand on it. Women tended to have closer proximity to traumatic events than men, either through their own experiences or the experiences of others close to them (Miller & Cromer, 2015). After controlling for this proximity, gender differences were no longer significant. This finding poses the question with regard to the #MeToo movement: Does the raised awareness increase people’s proximity to sexual violence thereby making them more supportive of disclosures from victims?

On the other side of proximity to victimization, proximity to perpetration may play a role in shaping responses to sexual violence. Antisocial peers are a well-established risk factor for criminal behavior (Gendreau et al., 1992; Humphrey & Kahn, 2000). The chance of engaging in a behavior increases if the individual and their peers have positive views of the behavior (Bagozzi & Burnkrant, 1979). Thus, the importance of association with sexual perpetrators may then play a vital role in how people respond to sexual violence.

Defensive attributions are not limited to victims but may extend to perpetrators. Indeed, if the observer perceives themselves as similar to the perpetrator of a crime, the observer expresses more lenient attitudes toward the perpetrator (Feather, 1996). Back and Lips (1998) hypothesized that the reliable gender differences found in victim-blaming research are a reflection of DAT, with men more likely to perceive similarities between themselves and the usually male perpetrator. More recent studies have found a similar effect when assessing whether perceived similarity indeed affects attributions of blame (e.g., Kahn et al., 2011). These studies found that male observers attribute more blame to female victims than female observers because they do not identify with the female victim to the same extent that female observers do (Davies & Rogers, 2009; Donovan, 2007; Kelly, 2009).

Childhood Sexual Abuse (CSA) Disclosures

The #MeToo movement has been accused of having too narrow a focus, highlighting victims who “fit” a certain narrative, while excluding, for example, nonheterosexual victims, people of color, and people who are viewed as “undeserving” of empathy (Gill & Orgad, 2018). Another group of victims who may be overlooked is a group who may be at an even greater disadvantage of speaking up and being heard: child victims. It is estimated that one in eight people worldwide are victims of sexual abuse during their childhood (Stoltenborgh et al., 2011). The majority of CSA victims do not disclose the abuse in childhood. Several studies have found that disclosure during childhood is rare, with most victims not disclosing until several years after the abuse took place (Alaggia, 2010; McElvaney et al., 2012).

Unfortunately, even if children do disclose, there is no guarantee that the abuse will stop, as evidenced by high-profile cases such as the Jimmy Savile (“Savile and Hall: BBC ‘missed chances to stop attacks,’” 2016) sexual abuse scandal, and the Larry Nassar case (Chowdhury, 2018). Despite victims disclosing to adults, the abuse was allowed to continue for decades (Bilefsky, 2016; Lavigne & Noren, 2018). These experiences are not exceptions. A recent study showed that victims who came forward with experiences of sexual abuse were only believed in one third of cases, and, in almost half of these cases, no steps were taken to stop the abuse (Stiller & Hellmann, 2017).

The Present Study

The aim of the present research was to explore people’s exposure to sexual violence and how this relates to their perceptions of sexual violence as well as the #MeToo movement. A recent study explored perceptions of #MeToo and found that men had more negative views of the movement than women (Kunst et al., 2019). The results of this study indicated that underlying ideologies such as sexism shaped people’s perceptions, and that proximity to sexual violence had a comparatively small influence. However, this study did not ask participants about their responses to a specific disclosure scenario, nor did it include proximity to perpetration other than participants’ own perpetration. Case vignettes are the preferred way to assess responses to sexual violence (Font, 2013). Whereas self-report questionnaires have been criticized for pertaining to abstract, artificial, or generalized situations (Alexander & Becker, 1978), vignettes allow for more natural responses to realistic scenarios.

The key question in the present study is whether increased awareness of sexual violence is perceived as a threat, and/or whether it is able to increase perceived proximity to sexual victimization, which may make people more supportive. This study explored exposure to and perceptions of the #MeToo movement and examined the role that proximity to victimization or perpetration plays in shaping responses to a CSA disclosure. Furthermore, with regard to perceptions of the #MeToo movement, we selected the response categories from news articles as well as social media, where the #MeToo movement has been characterized as both positive and negative. Although the victim depicted in the scenario was a child when the abuse happened, at the time of disclosure she is an adult. Given that this is the time when most victims will disclose the abuse for the first time, we feel that this is an important first step in examining how people respond to such a disclosure. In the future, we would like to extend this to vignettes where children disclose sexual abuse. We expected that most participants would have had some exposure to the #MeToo movement. Furthermore, we expected that participants with a proximity to victimization would be more supportive of the movement and a disclosure. In contrast, we predicted that participants with a proximity to perpetration would be more skeptical of the movement and a disclosure. Finally, we expected that gender differences in response to sexual violence could be triggered by a video on #MeToo, with male participants responding more negatively than female participants.

Method

Participants

Participant recruitment occurred through Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. Use of MTurk in this study was preferential to college students because college students may represent a well-adjusted, homogeneous population that is not representative of the general population (Buhrmester et al., 2011). Furthermore, data collected on MTurk is found to be equally reliable as data collected through student participant pools (Buhrmester et al., 2011), with some studies suggesting that MTurk participants may be more attentive than more traditional samples (e.g., Hauser & Schwarz, 2016).

A power analysis indicated that 200 participants were required to detect the desired effect size (R-squared = .12). To account for missing data and participants who may fail attention checks, 275 participants were recruited for this study. Participants were compensated US$0.80 for their participation. The sample was restricted to participants from the United States, with an approval rate of their work on MTurk of at least 95%, and with at least 50 approved answer submissions.

Any participant who did not stay on the page displaying the video for at least the duration of that video was excluded, as was any participant who could not correctly answer the questions about the video they watched. A total of 253 participants were included in the final sample. The mean age was 35.44 years (SD = 11.32), with an age range from 18 to 75 years. There were slightly more male participants (54.2%) and the majority of participants identified as straight (88.5%), bisexual (9.5%), or homosexual (1.2%) (See Supplemental Material online for detailed demographics).

Materials

#MeToo questionnaire

To assess exposure to, and perceptions of, the #MeToo movement, we developed a brief questionnaire. The first four questions ask about exposure to the movement. Two of these ask about proximity to sexual violence either through knowing someone who perpetrated sexual violence or who was victimized. The Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale (IOSS; Aron et al., 1992) is included to assess the degree of perceived closeness. This scale displays seven images of increasingly overlapping circles, representing the participant and someone the participant knows. The participant is then asked to pick the set of circles that best represents their relationship with this person.

The subsequent questions ask about perceptions of and feelings toward the movement. These questions were based upon the criticisms of the movement, as well as on its proposed objectives, to determine the extent to which participants agree with different characterizations of the movement that have been proposed by news and social media. Perceptions include six items (three positive and three negative) that are scored on a 1 to 5 Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Feelings include six feelings (three positive and three negative) that are scored on a 1 to 5 Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The feelings included emotions that might trigger defensive responses, such as feeling attacked or persecuted.

#MeToo manipulation

A 5-min video from Time Magazine about the Silence Breakers and the #MeToo movement were used as the manipulation for the #MeToo condition (Time, 2017). It includes various accounts, from men and women of different ethnicities and with different backgrounds. It discusses the importance of giving a voice to victims and it encourages viewers to stand up against sexual violence. For the control condition, a 6-min video about pollution in the ocean was watched (National Geographic, 2016).

CSA vignettes

When using vignettes, it is vital to avoid language that creates demand effects in participants. Examples highlighted by Cromer and Goldsmith (2010) include words like “assault” or “rape” that prime people to think about violence. On the contrary, words like “relationship” or “affair” paint a picture of a consensual relationship. The key then is to focus on neutral, factual language with behavioral descriptors. For this study, the following 125-word vignette was used:

You’re talking with your friend Zoe about #MeToo. She was one of the people who reposted the status on her Facebook wall. She tells you she reposted the status because from when she was thirteen until she was fifteen, her mom’s boyfriend would come into her bedroom at night when everyone was asleep. She tells you he would undress and get under the covers with her. He would touch her and rub against her and “do some other stuff.” It only stopped when her mom broke up with him because she had to move to a different city for her job. Zoe has never told anyone about what happened to her, but now that more people are speaking up, she feels that she can too.

After the vignette, participants were asked the extent to which they believed the CSA account (rate on scale of 1 to 100), how harmful they thought this experience was (1 = not at all harmful, 5 = very harmful), how much Zoe, Zoe’s mom, and Zoe’s mom’s boyfriend are to blame (rate on scale of 0–100), and how avoidable the experience was (0 = not at all avoidable, 5 = completely avoidable). These responses to disclosure variables were based on barriers to disclosure as identified by victims. Such barriers include a fear of not being believed or of being blamed, as well as their experience being minimized (e.g., Alaggia, 2010).

Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS)

The PANAS–Short Form (PANAS-SF; Thompson, 2007) is a brief assessment of positive and negative emotions. It uses five items to measure positive emotions and five items to measure negative emotions. Each item is rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 5 (extremely). It was used to assess participants’ feelings after watching one of the videos. The internal reliability in this sample was good (α = .84)

Procedure

Participants were randomly assigned to the #MeToo manipulation condition or the control condition. After watching the video, participants answered three questions to ensure they paid attention to the video. On the next screen, participants were presented with the vignette. They were asked to carefully read the vignette before moving onto the next screen. The following screens asked the participants to what extent they believed the vignette, how harmful they thought the experience was, who was to blame, and how avoidable they thought the experience described in the vignette was. On the final screen, participants were asked to complete the #MeToo questionnaire.

Results

What Are Participants’ Exposure to, and Perceptions of, the #MeToo Movement?

Exposure

The majority of participants (91.3%) had heard of the #MeToo movement. The 22 participants who reported they had not heard of the movement were excluded from all analyses that included questions about #MeToo. Nearly 17% of participants stated they had reposted #MeToo. Two out of three participants knew someone who had reposted #MeToo, and nearly 43% of participants knew someone who had done something that would make someone else repost #MeToo.

Next, the closeness to someone who reposted was assessed using the IOSS. A series of seven images depict two circles that increase in overlap, and participants are asked to choose the image that best represents their closeness with the other person. These images were recoded as a 1 to 7 scale, ranging from 1 (no overlap) to 7 (complete overlap). Participants reported a mean closeness to someone who reposted #MeToo of 3.48 (SD = 1.84). The same question was asked for closeness to someone who did something that would make someone else repost #MeToo. A slightly lower mean closeness was reported (M = 2.72, SD = 1.94).

Independent sample t tests were conducted to assess the difference in exposure to the #MeToo movement between men and women. Details are displayed in Table 1. Women were significantly more likely than men to know someone who had reposted #MeToo. Furthermore, the difference in closeness to someone who had done something that would cause someone else to repost #MeToo was marginally significant (p = .09), with men reporting a greater closeness than did women.

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics and Independent Sample t Tests of #MeToo Exposure Variables.

Exposure Men Women t
Frequency (%) Frequency (%)
Heard of #MeToo 124 (90.5%) 107 (92.2%) −0.49
Reposted #MeToo 22 (16.1%) 20 (17.2%) −1.25
 Prefer not to say 3 (2.2%) 6 (5.2%)
Do you know anyone who reposted #MeToo 84 (61.3%) 87 (75%) −2.33*
Do you know anyone who did something to make someone else repost #MeToo 57 (41.7%) 51 (44.0%) −0.39

p < .10. *p < .05.

Finally, correlations were calculated between the exposure variables for men and women.

Details are displayed in Table 2. For both men and women, all exposure variables were significantly, positively correlated.

Table 2.

Pearson Correlations Between Exposure to #MeToo Variables for Men and Women.

Perceptions 1 2 3 4 5
1. Reposted #MeToo .29*** .45*** .25** .49***
2. Knowing someone who reposted #MeToo .25* .33** .24** .32**
3. Closeness to someone who reposted #MeToo .26* .19*** .24* .73***
4. Knowing someone who did something to make someone else repost #MeToo .22* .33** .27** .28**
5. Closeness to someone who did something to make someone else repost #MeToo .35** .27** .39*** .30**

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Men above diagonal.

Perceptions

Perceptions of #MeToo were assessed by asking participants the extent to which the movement could be described by the first six variables displayed in Table 3 and to what extent the movement made them feel the last six variables in Table 3. Each answer was scored on a 5-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (disagree strongly) to 5 (agree strongly). We conducted independent sample t tests to compare men and women in their perceptions of the #MeToo movement. The results are displayed in Table 4. Every variable showed a significant difference between men and women except divisive and worried.

Table 3.

Descriptive Statistics and Independent Sample t Tests of Perceptions of #MeToo for Men and Women.

Perceptions Men
Women
t
M SD M SD
#MeToo is
 Helpful 3.89 1.00 4.24 .88 −2.84**
 Important 4.07 1.04 4.50 .78 −3.53**
 Divisive 3.15 1.29 2.99 1.40 0.92
 Witch hunt 2.68 1.37 2.06 1.32 3.51**
 Empowering 3.90 1.02 4.30 .91 −3.9**
 Gone too far 2.67 1.38 2.21 1.30 2.56*
#MeToo makes me feel
 Supported 3.15 1.19 3.94 1.00 −5.51***
 Persecuted 2.44 1.32 1.78 1.14 4.07***
 Worried 2.42 1.31 2.08 1.30 1.96
 Empowered 3.08 1.25 4.02 1.13 −5.92***
 Heard 3.09 1.30 3.96 1.10 −5.44***
 Attacked 2.32 1.38 1.70 1.07 3.86***

p < . 10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Table 4.

Factor Loadings Based on Principal Component Analysis for Perceptions of #MeToo.

Perceptions Component
1 2
Helpful .701
Important .579 −.315
Divisive .576
Witch hunt −.279 .705
Empower .682
Gone too far −.363 .672
Supported .910
Persecuted .904
Worried .795
Empowered .918
Heard .903
Attacked .912

The perception variables are highly correlated with each other (significant correlations .19–.84; nonsignificant correlations: feeling heard with feeling attacked and feeling heard with feeling worried). Negative perceptions show a strong positive correlation with each other, strong negative correlations with positive perceptions, and vice versa.

Due to these high correlations, a principal component analysis with varimax rotation was conducted to assess the uniqueness of the perception variables. A Varimax rotation was chosen to allow for the possibility that positive and negative perceptions were uncorrelated. Two components with eigenvalues above 1 emerged, and these components explained 68.11% of the variance. The component matrix is displayed in Table 4. Loadings below .25 are not displayed.

The identified factors appear to be positive perceptions and negative perceptions. We created a composite score for positive perceptions by summing the items that indicated positive perceptions of the movement (α = .90). Similarly, a composite score was created for negative perceptions by summing the items indicative of negative perceptions (α = .89). We used these composite scores in all subsequent analyses where perceptions of #MeToo are mentioned. We ran independent sample t tests to examine gender differences on positive and negative perceptions. Women (M = 25.00, SD = 4.97) were significantly more likely than men (M = 21.15, SD = 5.22) to hold positive perceptions of the movement (t = –5.68, p < .001). Similarly, men (M = 15.70, SD = 6.50) were significantly more likely than women (M = 12.82, SD = 5.91) to hold negative perceptions of the movement (t = 3.49, p = .001).

Vignette variables

Participants were asked seven questions about the vignette describing the CSA disclosure.

Correlations between variables are displayed in Table 5.

Table 5.

Pearson Correlations Between Response to Disclosure Variables for Men and Women.

Response to Disclosure 1 2 3 4 5 6
Believe −.34*** .54*** −.08 .42*** −.13
Blame Zoe −.32*** −.49*** .26** −.41*** .16
Blame mom’s boyfriend .33*** −.64*** .11 .61*** .03
Blame Zoe’s mom −.23* .28** −.09 .08 .19*
Harmful .38*** −.41*** .63*** −.04 −.06
Avoidable .01 .09 <.01 .35*** .07

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Men above the diagonal.

Finally, we examined the link between perceptions of #MeToo and responses to the vignette. Positive perceptions of #MeToo were associated with more positive responses than negative perceptions (Table 6). Interestingly, participants with more negative perceptions were more likely to blame Zoe’s mom, and to find the vignette more avoidable.

Table 6.

Correlations Between Perceptions of #MeToo and Responses to a CSA Vignette.

Variable Positive Perceptions Negative Perceptions
Believe .39*** −.38***
Blame Zoe −.14* .56***
Blame boyfriend .15* −.32**
Blame mom −.21** .33***
Harmful .26*** −.33***
Avoid −.16* .19**

Note. CSA = childhood sexual abuse.

p < .1. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Does Proximity to Victimization or Perpetration Affect How Participants Respond to Sexual Violence?

Proximity to victimization

First, we computed correlations between proximity to victimization and perceptions of the movement. For men, having reposted #MeToo was associated with both stronger positive (r = .19, p = .04) and negative (r = .38, p < .001) perceptions, whereas for women, reposting was only associated with negative perceptions (r = .27, p < .01). Knowing someone who reposted the status and closeness to such a person was linked to positive perceptions of the movement for both men (r = .36, p < .001) and women (r = .32, p = .001).

Second, we performed analyses to assess the effect of proximity to victimization on responses to a CSA disclosure. A series of stepwise multiple linear regressions were conducted for believing the vignette, harmfulness of the vignette, avoidability of the vignette, and amount of blame allocated to Zoe, Zoe’s mom, or Zoe’s mom’s boyfriend. For each regression, the entered variables were knowing someone who reposted the status, closeness to that person, as well as positive and negative perceptions of the movement and gender. Due to skewness of the blame variables, normality assumptions were violated and, as such, a robust estimator (MLR) was used. No significant equations with the proximity to victimization variables emerged.

Proximity to perpetration

Correlational analyses showed that men who knew someone who had done something that would make someone else repost #MeToo were more likely to have negative perceptions of the movement (r = .25, p = .006). Following this, a similar approach, as for proximity to victimization, was taken for the effect of proximity to perpetration. We performed stepwise multiple regression analyses to assess the effect of proximity to perpetration on responses to a CSA disclosure. The variables entered were knowing someone who had done something that would make someone else repost, closeness to this person, positive and negative perceptions toward the movement, and gender. Two significant equations with proximity to perpetration variables emerged. Believing the disclosure was predicted by knowing someone who did something that would make someone else repost (β = .16, p = .03) even with positive (β = .24, p < .01) and negative (β = –.28, p = .001) perceptions entering the model. Similarly, for harmfulness, closeness to someone who did something that might make someone else repost entered the model (β = –.19, p = .01) in addition to negative perceptions of the movement (β = –.29, p < .001).

A regression with an MLR was run for the blame variables due to skewness of these variables. Knowing someone who did something that would make someone else repost predicted the amount of blame allocated to Zoe (β = –.01, p = .04).

How Do Participants Respond to a Potentially Threatening Video of #MeToo?

Emotional response

Of this sample, 123 participants watched the #MeToo video (48.6%) and 130 participants watched the environmental video (51.4%). The PANAS was completed by each participant, after watching the video, to rate how the video made them feel. We conducted independent sample t tests to assess whether men and women differed in their emotional response to either video. The results are displayed in Table 7. For the #MeToo video, three significant differences emerged. Men recorded significantly more shame than women. Furthermore, women reported higher levels of feeling both inspired and determined. No significant differences emerged for the environmental video.

Table 7.

Descriptive Statistics of PANAS-Scores for Men and Women for Both Videos and Independent Sample t tests.

PANAS #MeToo
Environmental
Men
Women
t Men
Women
t
M SD M SD M SD M SD
Upset 2.82 1.30 3.11 1.10 −1.29 2.62 1.15 2.97 1.26 −1.62
Hostile 2.18 1.22 2.07 1.05 0.51 2.04 1.25 1.93 1.28 0.49
Alert 3.35 1.17 3.65 1.02 −1.50 3.75 .98 3.85 1.15 −0.53
Ashamed 2.19 1.32 1.65 1.00 2.56* 2.71 1.35 2.77 1.32 −0.27
Inspired 2.88 1.34 3.60 1.36 −2.93** 3.55 1.22 3.80 1.15 −1.21
Nervous 2.01 1.20 1.91 1.09 0.50 2.36 11.32 2.43 1.23 −0.32
Determined 2.84 1.30 3.33 1.29 −2.08* 3.53 1.28 3.59 1.06 −0.30
Attentive 3.56 1.10 3.85 1.15 −1.46 3.91 1.01 4.07 .96 −0.88
Afraid 1.94 1.27 1.65 .96 1.41 2.33 1.27 2.56 1.37 −0.97
Active 2.94 1.26 2.87 1.23 0.30 3.52 1.12 3.23 1.28 1.39

Note. PANAS = Positive and Negative Affect Schedule.

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

We performed two-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) for each PANAS variable to determine whether there was an interaction effect of gender and video for any of the variables, but no significant interactions emerged.

Disclosure response

We examined the interaction effects of gender and video on the vignette variables. For belief in the vignette, a two-way ANOVA with gender and video as predictors and belief in vignette as the outcome variable was used. No significant interaction effect was found. Similar analyses were conducted for the harmful and avoidable variables. No significant interaction effects were found.

With respect to the blame variables, a multiple regression with dummy variables was conducted (with a robust maximum likelihood estimator for the blame allocated to Zoe and Zoe’s mom’s boyfriend). None of the interaction effects were significant.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to examine exposure to and perceptions of the #MeToo movement, and to assess how these factors related to participants’ responses to sexual violence. Most participants had heard of the #MeToo movement, and 17% reported they had reposted the status. Most participants knew someone who had reposted the status and close to half knew someone who had done something to make someone else repost #MeToo.

These findings suggest that #MeToo is a well-known movement and most people have had first- or secondhand exposure to it. As hypothesized, a gender difference emerged with women more likely to know someone who had reposted the status. Interestingly, the results indicated that men were marginally more likely to be close to someone who had done something to cause someone else to repost, highlighting the importance of further exploring the concept of proximity to victimization as well as perpetration.

The observed gender differences are in line with previous research that suggests women tend to have a closer proximity to victimization through relationships with victims (Miller & Cromer, 2015). Although a gender difference in reposting the status was not found in this study, the number of participants who reposted was small. Furthermore, results suggested that people who reposted the status were more likely to know someone who had done something to make someone else repost. It may be that participants were thinking of the person who did something to them that made them repost the status. Given that most people are victimized by someone they know well (National Institute of Justice, 2003), that could explain this finding. Future studies should attempt to tease out the overlap between one’s own victimization and knowing a perpetrator.

The perceptions of men and women of the #MeToo movement varied in nearly every aspect. Men held significantly more negative views than women, and women reported significantly more positive feelings associated with the movement. This finding is in line with the study by Kunst et al. (2019) who found a similar gender difference in perceptions of the movement. The negative perceptions held by men may be indicative of an underlying defensive response. It would appear then, that although the #MeToo movement seems to have a positive effect on women, making them feel empowered and heard, the opposite is true for men. This finding highlights the polarizing nature of the movement and suggests that men may feel attacked by the movement. These feelings might make them less likely to engage in a conversation about sexual violence and to emerge as allies to women rather than as opponents. Indeed, Casey (2010) posed that defensiveness reduces the likelihood of people stepping up as allies. It may be the case that polarization, as a result of #MeToo, is leading to more skepticism, resulting in a less supportive response to a disclosure of sexual violence.

With regard to the responses participants gave to the CSA disclosure, the responses skewed toward positive, with most participants responding in a non-skeptical, supportive manner. However, several differences in responses did emerge, and they highlight how perceptions of a broader movement may shape responses in a specific situation. This finding is similar to findings from research into the Black Lives Matter movement, which suggest attitudes toward the movement are related to how specific situations are perceived (e.g., Reinka & Leach, 2017). Indeed, participants who reported having more positive views of #MeToo were also more likely to respond to a disclosure in a supportive manner. From the specific vignette questions, it appeared that when participants held more negative views of the movement, they were more likely to blame Zoe’s mom and to find the vignette more avoidable. Further research should investigate the nature of this seeming diffusion of blame. Furthermore, because we did not ask participants about their perceptions of sexual violence before the #MeToo movement, we cannot be certain that it is the movement itself that shaped their perceptions. A future study might ask participants explicitly whether their views on sexual violence have changed as a result of the #MeToo movement.

When looking at victimization proximity and perceptions, results showed that reposting the status was, on one hand, more likely to be associated with negative perceptions of the movement for female participants but, on the other hand, male participants were more likely to feel positive as well as negative emotions about the movement. These results may have emerged due to the small number of participants who reported having reposted the status. However, at least in part, these results may also suggest a difference in the individual experiences of the movement and their victimization. Kunst et al. (2019) also found that people who had a personal history of sexual victimization perceived the movement as less beneficial than those who did not.

Perhaps, #MeToo is viewed as not doing enough to address victimization or as not providing any tangible support for victims. Rape, Abuse, & Incest National Network’s (RAINN) guidelines on how to respond to victims of sexual violence emphasize the importance of continued support. A very public reposting without subsequent ongoing support may end up being more harmful than helpful to a victim (Ullman, 2002). Furthermore, participants who reposted the status may simply have stronger perceptions of the movement in general due to the personal relevance of the movement for them (e.g., Lang & Bradley, 2010). A further explanation may be drawn from an article on hashtag feminism and, specifically, a similar hashtag that trended on social media in 2014 (Mendes et al., 2018). The hashtag #BeenRapedNeverReported similarly sought to draw attention to the prevalence of sexual violence and to highlight the problem of underreporting. The researchers interviewed people who had posted the hashtag on social media and they found a similar tension between positive and negative emotions, with participants reporting finding the experience comforting on one hand, but also triggering on the other hand. Similarly, the interviews showed that the public nature of a hashtag leads to support from outside one’s own social circle, but for 72% of participants, it also led to hostility, threats, and misogynistic online abuse. The present findings of mixed emotions may be the result of similar conflicting processes. Finally, having reposted the status provides no further information of the motivations for doing so. Instead, it means the person identifies as a victim of some form of sexual violence and/or harassment. As such, the range of victimization experiences is likely to vary, and victims of different types of experiences may experience the #MeToo movement differently. Reposting the status suggests that the person is an “acknowledged” victim (Koss, 1985), meaning they identify as a victim.

However, the experience of unacknowledged victims, who do not view their experience as a victimization, remains hidden although they experience similar negative consequences as acknowledged victims (Frazier & Seales, 1997; Kahn & Mathie, 2000; Orlando & Koss, 1983). In short, the heterogeneous nature of victimization likely plays a role in the range of emotional responses reported in this study.

Knowing someone who reposted the status was linked with more positive perceptions of #MeToo, and closeness to someone who did something to cause someone else to repost the status was linked with more negative perceptions. It is important to point out that the phrasing of this question may have led some people to confuse posting something about #MeToo, with reposting the status, which is synonymous with a public disclosure of sexual victimization. Interestingly, proximity to victimization did not have an effect on the vignette variables, whereas proximity to perpetration did.

This may be due to the nature of #MeToo, and closeness to someone who reposted may not be strong enough to create differences in responses. #MeToo is very public by nature, as evidenced by the public reposting of the status on social media with the intent of giving visibility to sexual victimization. However, due to the public nature of reposting the status, it is likely not a great measure of proximity to victimization because no further conversation with the person who reposted may have occurred. A better indicator of proximity to victimization would be to ask whether the participant knows anyone who has been a victim of sexual violence or harassment.

Interestingly, participants who knew someone who had done something to make someone else repost #MeToo were more likely to believe the vignette, but closeness to this person was not a factor. This finding suggests there may be an availability bias at play, with people who know someone who caused someone else to repost the status being more likely to recall an incident of sexual violence, which in turn could increase belief in the vignette. Such a bias may indeed occur following media exposure to a phenomenon (e.g., Iyengar, 1990). On the contrary, closeness to such a person was linked to viewing the vignette as less harmful and to allocating more blame to the victim. The dissonance between belief that the disclosure is real and simultaneous minimization of harm seem indicative of a type of defensiveness, or at least cognitive dissonance, whereby the participant tries to resolve the conflict of believing in victimization while also knowing someone who victimized someone else.

Interestingly, gender differences were found for the #MeToo video in how it made participants feel, with men reporting more shame and women reporting being inspired and determined. These differences suggest that, to an extent, the video did have the desired effect of eliciting different, if not opposite, responses in men and women. Future studies may wish to further examine the nature of this shame reported by men. For example, shame is defined as a painful feeling with negative effects on interpersonal relations. However, shame and guilt are often confused, with guilt having comparatively more adaptive behaviors such as taking responsibility for one’s actions (Tangney & Dearing, 2003). It is likely that these emotions would have a different effect on how people respond to movements such as #MeToo.

This study had some limitations. First, at no point were participants asked whether they had ever done something that would cause someone else to repost #MeToo. If so, this would be the closest possible proximity to perpetration and that would likely skew the results. Similarly, participants who reposted #MeToo were also asked whether they knew someone who did something that caused someone else to repost the status, which means they could have thought of the person who caused them to repost. This is clearly a different kind of proximity to perpetration than the target of this study, and thus, this should be considered in future research. Second, we presented participants with response options regarding their perceptions of the movement and, in doing so, we may have superimposed bias onto them. A future study may ask participants to write in their views on the movement instead to allow for a more objective reflection of their views. Third, the #MeToo movement is very public in nature. Reposting a status for other people to see means that proximity to victimization increases for everyone who uses social media as it takes away the intimate nature of such disclosure in a more personal manner. As such, knowing someone who reposted the status may not be the best indication of proximity to victimization. Furthermore, although use of Amazon’s Mechanical Turk may result in a more diverse sample than an undergraduate student sample, MTurk samples are also not representative of the U.S. population (Arditte et al., 2016). Finally, we did not explicitly ask people whether the #MeToo movement had changed their perceptions of sexual violence and harassment, and thus we cannot conclude whether the movement directly influenced participants’ responses to the vignette.

This study confirmed that the #MeToo movement is well-known, and that most people have heard of it. We studied well-established gender differences in response to sexual violence in the broader context of the #MeToo movement. It appears that women are more likely to have positive perceptions of the movement than men, and these perceptions are reflected in responses to a CSA disclosure. Future research should explore whether men experience greater fear of being accused of sexual violence in light of increased disclosures as a result of the movement. Self-identified victims of sexual violence may experience the movement in different ways. Beyond the scope of #MeToo, this article sheds light on the challenges victims of sexual violence may face when disclosing. Closeness to a victim increases the likelihood for positive responses, whereas closeness to a perpetrator has the opposite effect. This finding makes a case for the importance of social relationships in creating a safe environment for victims to disclose. Although belief in a disclosure seemed unaffected, the impact on blame allocation and perceived harmfulness suggests a secondary, defensive response to disclosure that should be further explored.

Supplemental Material

sj-pdf-1-jiv-10.1177_0886260520925781 - Supplemental material for Empowerment or Threat: Perceptions of Childhood Sexual Abuse in the #MeToo Era

Supplemental material, sj-pdf-1-jiv-10.1177_0886260520925781 for Empowerment or Threat: Perceptions of Childhood Sexual Abuse in the #MeToo Era by Melissa S. de Roos and Daniel N. Jones in Journal of Interpersonal Violence

Author Biographies

Melissa S. de Roos is a lecturer in the MSc forensic psychology program at Roehampton University in London, United Kingdom. Her research focuses on childhood sexual abuse, specifically barriers survivors face in disclosing the abuse, and how people respond to such disclosures. She studies grooming behaviors sex offenders use and how these behaviors complicate the disclosure process for child victims or survivors.

Daniel N. Jones is an assistant professor of management and interdisciplinary social psychology at the University of Nevada, Reno. Prior to his appointment at UNR, he was an assistant professor of legal psychology at the University of Texas at El Paso. He received his PhD from the University of British Columbia in personality and social psychology in 2011.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests: The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding: The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Supplemental Material: Supplemental material for this article is available online.

ORCID iD

Melissa S. de Roos https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3469-6870

References

  1. Alaggia R. (2010). An ecological analysis of child sexual abuse disclosure: Considerations for child and adolescent mental health. Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry , 19, 32–39. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alexander C. S., Becker H. J. (1978). The use of vignettes in survey research. Public Opinion Quarterly , 42, 93–104. [Google Scholar]
  3. Arditte K. A., Çek D., Shaw A. M., Timpano K. R. (2016). The importance of assessing clinical phenomena in Mechanical Turk research. Psychological Assessment , 28, 684–691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Aron A., Aron E. N., Smollan D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 63, 596–612. [Google Scholar]
  5. Back S., Lips H. M. (1998). Child sexual abuse: Victim age, victim gender, and observer gender as factors contributing to attributions of responsibility. Child Abuse & Neglect , 22, 1239–1252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bagozzi R. P., Burnkrant R. E. (1979). Attitude organization and the attitude–behavior relationship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 37, 913–929. [Google Scholar]
  7. BBC. (2016, February 25). BBC ‘missed chances to stop attacks’. BBC News. https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/uk-35658398 [Google Scholar]
  8. Bilefsky D. (2016, February 25). Jimmy Savile inquiry accuses BBC of failing to report sexual abuse. https://www.nytimes.com/2016/02/26/world/europe/jimmy-savile-report-bbc.html [Google Scholar]
  9. Brown R. J., Ross G. R. (1982). The battle for acceptance: An exploration into the dynamics of intergroup behavior. In Tajfel H. (Ed.), Social identity and intergroup relations (pp. 155–178). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  10. Buhrmester M., Kwang T., Gosling S. D. (2011). Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science , 6, 3–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Casey E. (2010). Strategies for engaging men as anti-violence allies: Implications for ally movements. Advances in Social Work , 11, 267–282. [Google Scholar]
  12. Chowdhury, S. (2018, January 16). Larry Nassar: The doctor involved in gymnastics’ abuse scandal. BBC Sport. https://www.bbc.co.uk/sport/gymnastics/42710921. [Google Scholar]
  13. Cromer L. D., Freyd J. J. (2007). What influences believing child sexual abuse disclosures? The roles of depicted memory persistence, participant gender, trauma history, and sexism. Psychology of Women Quarterly , 31, 13–22. [Google Scholar]
  14. Cromer L. D., Goldsmith R. E. (2010). Child sexual abuse myths: Attitudes, beliefs, and individual differences. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse , 19, 618–647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cromwell M. (2017, December 19). #MeToo movement goes too far. http://www.baltimoresun.com/news/opinion/oped/bs-ed-op-1220-metoo-toofar-20171219-story.html [Google Scholar]
  16. Davies M., Rogers P. (2009). Perceptions of blame and credibility toward victims of childhood sexual abuse: Differences across victim age, victim-perpetrator relationship, and respondent gender in a depicted case. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse , 18, 78–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Donovan R. A. (2007). To blame or not to blame: Influences of target race and observer sex on rape blame attribution. Journal of Interpersonal Violence , 22, 722–736. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Feather N. T. (1996). Reactions to penalties for an offense in relation to authoritarianism, values, perceived responsibility, perceived seriousness, and deservingness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 71, 571–587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Font S. A. (2013). Perceptions of juvenile sexual abuse victims: A meta-analysis on vignette based studies on the effects of victims’ age and respondents’ gender. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse , 22, 593–611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Frazier P. A., Seales L. M. (1997). Acquaintance rape is real rape. In Schwartz M. D. (Ed.), Researching sexual violence against women: Methodological and personal perspectives (pp. 54–64). Sage. [Google Scholar]
  21. Gendreau, P., Andrews, D. A., Goggin, C., & Chanteloupe, F. (1992). The development of clinical and policy guidelines for the prediction of criminal behaviour in criminal justice settings [Programs branch user report]. Ministry of the Solicitor General of Canada.
  22. Gerson J. (2017, October 17). Jen Gerson: I mean no disrespect when I say that I have a problem with #MeToo. http://nationalpost.com/opinion/jen-gerson-the-problemwith-metoo-it-holds-the-wrong-people-responsible-for-sex-assault [Google Scholar]
  23. Gill R., Orgad S. (2018). The shifting terrain of sex and power: From the “sexualization of culture” to# MeToo. Sexualities , 21, 1313–1324. [Google Scholar]
  24. Grant P. R. (1992). Ethnocentrism between groups of unequal power in response to perceived threat to social identity and valued resources. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science , 24, 28–44. [Google Scholar]
  25. Hauser D. J., Schwarz N. (2016). Attentive Turkers: MTurk participants perform better on online attention checks than do subject pool participants. Behavior Research Methods , 48, 400–407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Humphrey S. E., Kahn A. S. (2000). Fraternities athletic teams rape: Importance of identification with a risky group. Journal of Interpersonal Violence , 15, 1313–1322. [Google Scholar]
  27. Iyengar S. (1990). The accessibility bias in politics: Television news and public opinion. International Journal of Public Opinion Research , 2, 1–15. [Google Scholar]
  28. Kahn A. S., Mathie V. A. (2000). Understanding the unacknowledged rape victim. In Travis C. B., White J. W. (Eds.), Psychology of women: Volume 4. Sexuality, society, and feminism (pp. 377–403). American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
  29. Kahn A. S., Rodgers K. A., Martin C., Malick K., Claytor J., Gandolfo M., Webne E. (2011). Gender versus gender role in attributions of blame for a sexual assault. Journal of Applied Social Psychology , 41, 239–251. [Google Scholar]
  30. Kelly, T. (2009). Judgments and perceptions of blame: The impact of benevolent sexism and rape type on attributions of responsibility in sexual assault [Doctoral dissertation]. https://tspace.library.utoronto.ca/handle/1807/17781
  31. Koss M. P. (1985). The hidden rape victim: Personality, attitudinal, and situational characteristics. Psychology of Women Quarterly , 9, 193–212. [Google Scholar]
  32. Kunst J. R., Bailey A., Prendergast C., Gundersen A. (2019). Sexism, rape myths and feminist identification explain gender differences in attitudes toward the #MeToo social media campaign in two countries. Media Psychology , 22, 818–843. [Google Scholar]
  33. Lang P. J., Bradley M. M. (2010). Emotion and the motivational brain. Biological Psychology , 84, 437–450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lavigne P., Noren N. (2018, January 27). OTL: Michigan State secrets extend far beyond Larry Nassar case. https://www.espn.com/espn/story/_/id/22214566/pattern-denial-inaction-information-suppression-michigan-state-goes-larry-nassar-case-espn [Google Scholar]
  35. Lyon H. M., Kaney S., Bentall R. P. (1994). The defensive function of persecutory delusions: Evidence from attribution tasks. The British Journal of Psychiatry , 164, 637–646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. McElvaney R., Greene S., Hogan D. (2012). Containing the secret of child sexual abuse. Journal of Interpersonal Violence , 27, 1155–1175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Mendes K., Ringrose J., Keller J. (2018). # MeToo and the promise and pitfalls of challenging rape culture through digital feminist activism. European Journal of Women’s Studies , 25, 236–246. [Google Scholar]
  38. Miller K. E., Cromer L. D. (2015). Beyond gender: Proximity to interpersonal trauma in examining differences in believing child abuse disclosures. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation , 16, 211–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. National Geographic. (2016, September 15). https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=o0AOkDpzNBM
  40. National Institute of Justice. (2003). Youth victimization: Prevalence and implications (DOJ Publication No. NCJ 194972). U.S. Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs.
  41. Nuttall R., Jackson H. (1994). Personal history of childhood abuse among clinicians. Child Abuse & Neglect , 18, 455–472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Ohlheiser A. (2017, October 19). The woman behind “Me Too” knew the power of the phrase when she created it—10 years ago. https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/the-intersect/wp/2017/10/19/the-woman-behindme-too-knew-the-power-of-the-phrase-when-she-created-it-10-years-ago/ [Google Scholar]
  43. Orlando J. A., Koss M. P. (1983). The effects of sexual victimization on sexual satisfaction: A study of the negative-association hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 92, 104–106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Reinka M. A., Leach C. W. (2017). Race and reaction: Divergent views of police violence and protest against. Journal of Social Issues , 73, 768–788. [Google Scholar]
  45. Santiago C., Criss D. (2017, October 17). An activist, a little girl, and the heartbreaking origin of #MeToo. https://www.cnn.com/2017/10/17/us/me-too-tarana-burkeorigin-trnd/index.html [Google Scholar]
  46. Schwark S., Bohner G. (2019). Sexual violence—”Victim” or “survivor”: News images affect explicit and implicit judgments of blame. Violence Against Women , 25, 1491–1509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Shaver K. G. (1970). Defensive attribution: Effects of severity and relevance on the responsibility assigned for an accident. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 14, 101–113. [Google Scholar]
  48. Smartt N. (2017, December 20). Sexual harassment in the workplace in A #MeToo world. https://www.forbes.com/sites/forbeshumanresourcescouncil/2017/12/20/sexualharassment-in-the-workplace-in-a-metoo-world/#16fc34575a42 [Google Scholar]
  49. Snyder C., Lopez L. (2017, December 13). Tarana Burke on why she created the #MeToo movement—And where it’s headed. http://www.businessinsider.com/howthe-metoo-movement-started-where-its-headed-tarana-burke-time-person-of-yearwomen-2017-12 [Google Scholar]
  50. Stephens B. (2017, December 20). When #MeToo goes too far. https://www.nytimes.com/2017/12/20/opinion/metoo-damon-too-far.html [Google Scholar]
  51. Stiller A., Hellmann D. F. (2017). In the aftermath of disclosing child sexual abuse: Consequences, needs, and wishes. Journal of Sexual Aggression , 23, 251–265. [Google Scholar]
  52. Stoltenborgh M., Van Ijzendoorn M. H., Euser E. M., Bakermans-Kranenburg M. J. (2011). A global perspective on child sexual abuse: Meta-analysis of prevalence around the world. Child Maltreatment , 16, 79–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Strum L. (2017, October 26). Twitter chat: What #MeToo says about sexual abuse in society. https://www.pbs.org/newshour/nation/twitter-chat-what-metoo-saysabout-sexual-abuse-in-society [Google Scholar]
  54. Suarez E., Gadalla T. M. (2010). Stop blaming the victim: A meta-analysis on rape myths. Journal of Interpersonal Violence , 25, 2010–2035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Tajfel H., Turner J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations , 33, 74–88. [Google Scholar]
  56. Tangney J. P., Dearing R. L. (2003). Shame and guilt. Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  57. Thompson E. R. (2007). Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology , 38, 227–242. [Google Scholar]
  58. Thompson M. (2000). Life after rape: A chance to speak? Sexual and Relationship Therapy , 15, 325–343. [Google Scholar]
  59. Time. (2017, December 6). https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=MkR8GY2Y BAU
  60. Ullman S. E. (2002). Social reactions to child sexual abuse disclosures: A critical review. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse , 12, 89–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Walster E. (1966). Assignment of responsibility for an accident. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 3, 73–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Wilhelm H. (2017, October 23). Where #MeToo goes off the rails. http://www.chicagotribune.com/news/opinion/commentary/ct-perspec-me-too-sexualassault-wilhelm-1023-story.html [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

sj-pdf-1-jiv-10.1177_0886260520925781 - Supplemental material for Empowerment or Threat: Perceptions of Childhood Sexual Abuse in the #MeToo Era

Supplemental material, sj-pdf-1-jiv-10.1177_0886260520925781 for Empowerment or Threat: Perceptions of Childhood Sexual Abuse in the #MeToo Era by Melissa S. de Roos and Daniel N. Jones in Journal of Interpersonal Violence


Articles from Journal of Interpersonal Violence are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

RESOURCES