Skip to main content
Industrial Health logoLink to Industrial Health
. 2021 Oct 25;60(2):91–96. doi: 10.2486/indhealth.2021-0124

On-call work and sleep: the importance of switching on during a callout and switching off after a call

Charlotte C GUPTA 1,*, Michelle DOMINIAK 1, Katya KOVAC 1, Amy C REYNOLDS 2, Sally A FERGUSON 1, Cassie J HILDITCH 3, Madeline SPRAJCER 1, Grace E VINCENT 1
PMCID: PMC8980688  PMID: 34690251

Abstract

Due to the unpredictable nature of working time arrangements, on-call workers experience regular disruption to sleep, particularly if woken by calls. Sleep disruption can impact long term physical and mental health, next day performance, and importantly, performance immediately after waking. To reduce the impact of performance impairments upon waking (i.e., reducing sleep inertia), research has investigated strategies to promote alertness (e.g., bright light, caffeine, and exercise). This review puts forth on-call workers who are likely to return to sleep after a call, it is also important to consider the impact of these sleep inertia countermeasures on subsequent sleep. Future research should build on the preliminary evidence base for sleep inertia countermeasures by examining the impact on subsequent sleep. This research is key for both supporting alertness and performance during a call (“switching on”) and for allowing the on-call worker to return to sleep after a call (“switching off”).

Keywords: On-call, Stand-by, Work, Sleep, Performance, Emergency service, Sleep inertia

Introduction

On-call is a work arrangement where employees may be called to work outside of scheduled hours to provide 24/7 coverage1). This is particularly common for personnel working in emergency services, healthcare, maintenance, military, and information technology2). These workers are often required to respond to emergency situations around the clock, including being woken from sleep to attend a call (either in person or from home)3, 4). A main difference between on-call work and traditional shift work schedules is unpredictability, as workers often do not know if they will be called for work, and if so, when or for how long they will be required to work1).

The unpredictable nature of on-call work can lead to disrupted sleep both pre-call and post-call. Before a call, workers may have disrupted sleep due to anxiety about receiving a call or missing a call5, 6). Anxiety related to the unpredictability of on-call work can affect sleep even if the likelihood of receiving a call is low7). Post-call, the worker may be allowed to, or need to, return to sleep1), however the quality and quantity of this sleep can be impaired. In a study by our group, 70% of on-call workers reported difficulty returning to sleep after a call8). This may be due to the nature of the call, with stress, rumination, and anxiety resulting in difficulty falling back to sleep4, 9). Workers may also receive (or expect to receive) additional calls throughout the call period such that post-call sleep may also be interrupted (thereby becoming pre-call sleep). An additional consideration is that many on-call workers sleep at work, for example doctors, paramedics, and firefighters4) and thus workers may attempt to return to sleep in a compromised sleep environment (e.g., an environment with light or noise disruption)8). Work by our group has also shown that on-call work can also adversely influence the sleep of other family members, including the partners of on-call workers10). Taken together, these findings demonstrate that pre-call and post-call sleep in on-call workers can be impaired due to the nature of the work schedule. The flow-on effects are an issue for a) health and well-being, b) workplace performance and productivity, and c) performance immediately after waking. This review firstly summarises the key findings on these flow-on effects (for more detailed reviews on these topics, see4, 1113)), to provide context for the discussion on sleep inertia countermeasures and the importance of considering the impact on subsequent sleep.

There is some research to suggest that on-call work is associated with chronic physical and mental health problems that are likely influenced and exacerbated by shortened sleep2). For example, having fewer hours of sleep when on-call is associated with a greater number of physical health symptoms in junior doctors14). Further, in a sample of 414 general practitioners, being on-call was a predictive factor for poorer mental health, depression and anxiety15). Mental health remains a challenge for on-call workers, with recent studies also demonstrating a relationship between on-call work and mental health problems16, 17).

Impaired cognitive function and work performance are acutely associated with inadequate sleep18) and such changes are evident in on-call populations1, 2, 6). For example, inadequate sleep experienced by on-call workers has been linked to motor vehicle accidents, with 49% of a sample of 70 medical residents reporting incidents of falling asleep at the wheel when driving home, and 90% of these reported as occurring after a night on-call19). A study quantifying the economic cost of inadequate sleep of on-call workers in Australia estimated the cost of injury to be $2.25 billion per year1). This includes medical compensation, lost production, and staff turnover costs such as overtime and over-employment. Together, this shows that the workplace performance impacts of on-call work can be wide-reaching and range from personal safety to industry costs.

When an on-call worker is woken by a call, they are likely to experience sleep inertia, which refers to the transitional state between sleep and wake. Subjectively, sleep inertia is often experienced as ‘grogginess’ upon waking20, 21). While the most severe symptoms typically last for 15–30 minutes post-waking, full recovery can take over an hour12). Sleep inertia is characterised by reduced alertness and performance on tasks involving memory, logical reasoning, reaction time, and problem solving22, 23). Given these cognitive impairments, it is unsurprising that sleep inertia has been implicated in workplace accidents when employees must perform high stress and safety critical duties shortly after waking. For example, in an analysis of 400 United States Air Force accidents, pilot error was a contributing factor most commonly during the first hour after waking, when sleep inertia is likely impacting performance24).

Sleep inertia is particularly problematic for on-call workers who are often completing safety-critical tasks upon waking. For example, medical staff may need to make critical decisions25), or firefighters may be required to don safety equipment and drive to an incident within minutes of waking6). Sleep inertia-related cognitive impairments can be greatest during the biological night, when on-call workers are likely to be receiving calls, as sleep inertia is exacerbated by greater sleep and circadian pressures26, 27). However it is also important to note that even with an uninterrupted night’s sleep (i.e., without a call) sleep inertia is still present upon waking and presents a challenge to immediate performance and alertness28). Further, the build-up of sleep debt after multiple days of shortened sleep could exacerbate the effects of sleep inertia on subsequent days29, 30); a problem for those who work consecutive nights of on-call.

Countermeasures for reducing short-term performance impacts after receiving a call

While performance after waking is an issue due to poor sleep, addressing performance after waking may actually exacerbate sleep loss issues for on-call workers. We will review a) the efficacy of existing sleep inertia countermeasures, and b) the impact of sleep inertia countermeasures on subsequent sleep.

a) The efficacy of existing countermeasures

Several countermeasures have shown promise in reducing the impairment associated with sleep inertia. These countermeasures can be proactive and planned ahead of time, or reactive and implemented upon waking11).

Proactive sleep inertia countermeasures include, but are not limited to: a) waking naturally rather than waking to an alarm31); b) the use of dawn simulators32); and c) the use of caffeine before a short nap33, 34). While these proactive countermeasures can effectively improve performance upon waking (i.e., reduce sleep inertia), they are only suitable when workers have a planned sleep opportunity and wake time is known. Therefore, these proactive countermeasures are not applicable for on-call workers who can be woken at any time during the sleep period.

Reactive countermeasures to sleep inertia may be more effective for on-call workers as they are performed after waking and do not require any pre-sleep planning11). Research into reactive sleep inertia countermeasures (particularly for on-call workers) has grown in recent years. While potential countermeasures have been evaluated as options, more research is needed. These reactive countermeasures include but are not limited to: a) bright light35); b) caffeinated gum36); c) sound37); and d) exercise38, 39).

For example, in a proof-of-concept study by Kovac, Vincent40), exercise completed immediately upon waking during the night increased noradrenaline levels and reduced sleepiness levels after 5 minutes40). Increasing noradrenaline through exercise may reduce the cognitive impacts of sleep inertia and accelerate the transition from sleep to wake40), thereby making exercise a suitable reactive countermeasure for on-call workers. Preliminary evidence has also shown some support for bright light, sound, and caffeine as reactive countermeasures (for review see11)). One study found that exposure to 1-min of bright light (2,000 lux) after waking reduced subjective sleepiness after 15 min and improved memory and reaction time35). However, while exposure to bright light after waking may improve cognitive performance, this improvement lasted 45 mins. Another common reactive sleep inertia countermeasure is caffeine, a cost effective and easily available stimulant11). One study that explored the use of caffeine as a reactive countermeasure gave participants chewing gum containing 100 mg of caffeine or a non-caffeinated placebo gum when woken at 01:00 or 06:0036). Improved reaction time lasted between 12–18 min after chewing the caffeinated gum compared to the placebo gum, suggesting that caffeine could be a more effective countermeasure compared to bright light, given the faster cognitive performance improvements. Sound has also been investigated as a reactive countermeasure for sleep inertia11, 37) and has shown the potential to improve performance under certain conditions. In a study by Hayashi et al.41), music was played at 60 dB for 20 min after waking from a nap, with reduced subjective sleepiness observed. Cognitive performance was also improved when the music played was music that was preferred by the participant. However, it is important to note that there was not a control group in this study.

However, these other countermeasures have shown some improvements to alertness and performance upon waking, as discussed above, protecting subsequent sleep (i.e., post-call out) is an important consideration for on-call workers. Further, the feasibility of these countermeasures, particularly for on-call workers who are likely returning to bed, has not been explored. This includes both practicality of a countermeasure, and motivation of the workers. For example, an exercise countermeasure may not be practical unless workers have access to exercise equipment, such as an exercise bike or treadmill. Further, although there are positive implications for sleep inertia, there may be some workers who do not want to exercise and then go back to sleep.

b) Understanding the impact of countermeasures on subsequent sleep

In order to reduce the likelihood of chronic sleep disruption, sleep inertia countermeasures should be examined for their potential impact on subsequent sleep. To date, only caffeine has been investigated in the context of subsequent sleep. While caffeine intake during the day is known to influence subsequent sleep at night, typically prolonging sleep latency and reducing sleep time and efficiency42), caffeine consumed during the night has different effects36, 43, 44). In a cross-over study by Newman et al.36), fifteen participants slept in a laboratory and, after being woken at 01:00 and 06:00, were immediately given a gum pellet either containing 100 mg of caffeine or a placebo. Not only did the caffeine result in better cognitive performance upon waking compared to the placebo, the participants in the caffeine group displayed a relatively short sleep onset latency when returning to sleep after being woken, similar to that found for the placebo group. This is promising for on-call workers who must perform immediately upon waking and then return to sleep. However, it is important to note that there was no difference in sleep onset latency between the caffeine and placebo group at approximately 01:30. This is a time of day during which sleep pressure is maximal, which may explain the absence of an effect of caffeine on sleep onset latency, that is, participants did not find it difficult to fall asleep. However, sleep quality after caffeine administration was not measured during this study. This will be important to measure in future studies as caffeine may influence sleep quality during subsequent sleep. Further, sleep inertia testing was conducted for approximately 25 minutes after participants were woken. While this may be the length of time that some workers are awake and performing after a call, there are numerous on-call scenarios which are not covered by this simulation. These include less time awake after being awoken (e.g., a medical practitioner providing medical advice), or more time awake (e.g., a fire fighter on a call-out). These different scenarios may require different levels of caffeine and have different effects of subsequent sleep. Future research should be mindful to explore these various on-call arrangements. Additionally, while some studies reported minimal effects on sleep after caffeine taken during the day (e.g.,43, 44)), they were not conducted in on-call contexts, where the workers consume caffeine upon waking during the night and return to sleep post-call. There remains a critical need for research on the effects of countermeasures for sleep inertia on subsequent sleep.

Where to from here? Future research directions

First, further research is needed, specifically in the context of on-call work, on reactive countermeasures that have preliminary support (e.g., sound, temperature, and exercise) in reducing sleep inertia and other future novel countermeasures. Second, those countermeasures that have shown promise at reducing the impacts of sleep inertia (e.g., light and caffeine) should be investigated for their impacts on sleep. We propose the systematic experimental implementation of these countermeasures within a laboratory environment, where post-call sleep latency and sleep quality are also examined. Third, as suggested by multiple authors34, 38), qualitative measures, such as interviews and focus groups, should be implemented in future methodologies to assess implementation and feasibility of sleep inertia countermeasures in real world contexts. Qualitative measures are important for understanding the experience of on-call workers who are required to be alert after being woken by a call and also the barriers to sleep inertia countermeasure implementation20). Previous work by our group has shown that males and females employ different coping styles when managing on-call work45), therefore it is possible there may also be differences in how sleep inertia countermeasures are implemented by different genders.

Finally, until the impacts of sleep inertia countermeasures on sleep have been determined, on-call workers should be mindful that strategies to reduce sleep inertia after a call have the potential to impact subsequent sleep. Upon completion of their work commitments, workers should engage in sleep-promoting measures to improve subsequent sleep. This may include employing healthy sleep practices (i.e., sleep hygiene), such as reducing bright light, or engaging in mindfulness strategies. Furthermore, it is critical that workers are strategic in their use of sleep inertia countermeasures in situations where they know they will need to sleep after a call. For example, small amounts of caffeine could be used strategically (i.e., at times when it is expected that responding to a call will take a significant amount of time, rather than when a call may simply result in a short phone-based response) as opposed to a standard response to a call. However, future research is necessary to determine which strategies may be effective in reducing sleep inertia (for call response), but which do not disrupt subsequent sleep.

Conclusion

The sleep of on-call workers is a concern for both the health and safety of the workforce. Given the factors that exacerbate sleep inertia, on-call workers are likely to experience sleep inertia when woken by overnight calls. While several countermeasures have preliminary support for minimising the negative impact of sleep inertia on cognitive performance (e.g., bright light, exercise, sound), the impact of these countermeasures in on-call contexts where there is a high likelihood of returning to sleep is unknown. Therefore, any sleep inertia countermeasures need to be assessed not only considering their effectiveness in promoting alertness and performance, but also in relation to their impact on subsequent sleep. Such research should include laboratory and field studies to assist on-call workers to “switch on” to promote alertness, and then “switch off” to allow for subsequent sleep, thus improving health, performance, and safety in often safety critical work environments.

References

  • 1).Vincent GE, Kinchin I, Ferguson SA, Jay SM (2018) The cost of inadequate sleep among on-call workers in Australia: a workplace perspective. Int J Environ 15, 398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2).Nicol AM, Botterill JS (2004) On-call work and health: a review. Environ Health 3, 15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3).Vincent GE, Aisbett B, Hall SJ, Ferguson SA (2016) Fighting fire and fatigue: sleep quantity and quality during multi-day wildfire suppression. Ergonomics 59, 932–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4).Ferguson SA, Paterson JL, Hall SJ, Jay SM, Aisbett B (2016) On-call work: to sleep or not to sleep? It depends. Chronobiol Int 33, 678–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5).Sprajcer M, Jay SM, Vincent GE, Vakulin A, Lack L, Ferguson SA (2018) How the chance of missing the alarm during an on-call shift affects pre-bed anxiety, sleep and next day cognitive performance. Biol Psychol 137, 133–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6).Paterson JL, Aisbett B, Ferguson SA (2016) Sound the alarm: health and safety risks associated with alarm response for salaried and retained metropolitan firefighters. Saf Sci 82, 174–81. [Google Scholar]
  • 7).Sprajcer M, Jay SM, Vincent GE, Vakulin A, Lack L, Ferguson SA (2018) Uncertain call likelihood negatively affects sleep and next-day cognitive performance while on-call in a laboratory environment. Chronobiol Int 35, 838–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8).Vincent GE, Kovac K, Signal L, Reynolds AC, Paterson J, Sprajcer M, Ferguson SA (2021) What factors influence the sleep of on-call workers? Behav Sleep Med 19, 255–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9).Jay SM, Paterson JL, Aisbett B, Ferguson SA (2018) No rest for the women: understanding the impact of on-call work for women in the emergency services. Chronobiol Int 35, 827–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10).Vincent GE, Karan S, Paterson J, Reynolds AC, Dominiak M, Ferguson SA (2020) Impacts of Australian firefighters’ on-call work arrangements on the sleep of partners. Clocks & Sleep 2, 39–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11).Hilditch CJ, Dorrian J, Banks S (2016) Time to wake up: reactive countermeasures to sleep inertia. Ind Health 54, 528–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12).Hilditch CJ, McHill AW (2019) Sleep inertia: current insights. Nat Sci Sleep 11, 155–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13).Hall SJ, Ferguson SA, Turner AI, Robertson SJ, Vincent GE, Aisbett B (2017) The effect of working on-call on stress physiology and sleep: a systematic review. Sleep Med Rev 33, 79–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14).Baldwin P, Dodd M, Wrate R (1997) Young doctors’ health—I. How do working conditions affect attitudes, health and performance? Soc Sci 45, 35–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15).Rout U, Cooper CL, Rout JK (1996) Job stress among British general practitioners: predictors of job dissatisfaction and mental ill-health. Stress Med 12, 155–66. [Google Scholar]
  • 16).Heponiemi T, Puttonen S, Elovainio M (2014) On-call work and physicians’ well-being: testing the potential mediators. Occup Med (Lond) 64, 352–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17).Lee JJ, Park EC, Ji H, Jang SI (2020) The effects of on-call work on mental health issues among wage workers in the Republic of Korea. Psychol Health & Med 25, 675–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18).Reynolds AC, Banks S (2010) Total sleep deprivation, chronic sleep restriction and sleep disruption. Prog Brain Res 185, 91–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19).Marcus CL, Loughlin GM (1996) Effect of sleep deprivation on driving safety in housestaff. Sleep 19, 763–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20).Dawson D, Ferguson SA, Vincent GE (2021) Safety implications of fatigue and sleep inertia for emergency services personnel. Sleep Med Rev 55, 101386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21).Tassi P, Bonnefond A, Engasser O, Hoeft A, Eschenlauer R, Muzet A (2006) EEG spectral power and cognitive performance during sleep inertia: the effect of normal sleep duration and partial sleep deprivation. Physiol Behav 87, 177–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22).Bruck D, Pisani DL (1999) The effects of sleep inertia on decision-making performance. J Sleep Res 8, 95–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23).Burke TM, Scheer F, Ronda JM, Czeisler CA, Wright KP, Jr. (2015) Sleep inertia, sleep homeostatic and circadian influences on higher-order cognitive functions. J Sleep Res 24, 364–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24).Ribak J (1983) Diurnal rhythmicity and Air Force flight accidents due to pilot error. Aviat Space Environ Med 54, 1096–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25).Fallis WM, McMillan DE, Edwards MP (2011) Napping during night shift: practices, preferences, and perceptions of critical care and emergency department nurses. Crit Care Nurse 31, e1–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26).Scheer FAJL, Shea TJ, Hilton MF, Shea SA (2008) An endogenous circadian rhythm in sleep inertia results in greatest cognitive impairment upon awakening during the biological night. J Biol Rhythms 23, 353–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27).Dinges D, Orne MT, Orne EC (1985) Assessing performance upon abrupt awakening from naps during quasi-continuous operations. Behav Res Methods 17, 37–45. [Google Scholar]
  • 28).Kovac K, Vincent GE, Jay SM, Sprajcer M, Aisbett B, Lack L, Ferguson SA (2020) The impact of anticipating a stressful task on sleep inertia when on-call. Appl Ergon 82, 102942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29).Balkin TJ, Badia P (1988) Relationship between sleep inertia and sleepiness: cumulative effects of four nights of sleep disruption/restriction on performance following abrupt nocturnal awakening. Biol Psychol 27, 245–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30).McHill AW, Hull JT, Cohen DA, Wang W, Czeisler CA, Klerman EB (2019) Chronic sleep restriction greatly magnifies performance decrements immediately after awakening. Sleep 42, zsz032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31).Thompson A, Jones H, Gregson W, Atkinson G (2014) Effects of dawn simulation on markers of sleep inertia and post-waking performance in humans. Eur J Appl Physiol 114, 1049–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32).Gimenez MC, Hessels M, van de Werken M, de Vries B, Beersma DG, Gordijn MC (2010) Effects of artificial dawn on subjective ratings of sleep inertia and dim light melatonin onset. Chronobiol Int 27, 1219–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33).Horne JA, Reyner LA (1996) Counteracting driver sleepiness: effects of napping, caffeine, and placebo. Wiley Online Library. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34).Centofanti S, Banks S, Coussens S, Gray D, Munro E, Nielsen J, Dorrian J (2020) A pilot study investigating the impact of a caffeine-nap on alertness during a simulated night shift. Chronobiol Int 37, 1–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35).Hayashi M, Masuda A, Hori T (2003) The alerting effects of caffeine, bright light and face washing after a short daytime nap. Clin Neurophysiol 114, 2268–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36).Newman RA, Kamimori GH, Wesensten NJ, Picchioni D, Balkin TJ (2013) Caffeine gum minimizes sleep inertia. Percept Mot Skills 116, 280–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37).McFarlane SJ, Garcia JE, Verhagen DS, Dyer AG (2020) Alarm tones, voice warnings, and musical treatments: a systematic review of auditory countermeasures for sleep inertia in abrupt and casual awakenings. Clocks & Sleep 2, 416–33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38).Kovac K, Ferguson SA, Paterson JL, Aisbett B, Hilditch CJ, Reynolds AC, Vincent GE (2020) Exercising caution upon waking–can exercise reduce sleep inertia? Front physiol 11, 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39).Dominiak M, Kovac K, Reynolds AC, Ferguson SA, Vincent GE (2021) The effect of a short burst of exercise during the night on subsequent sleep. J Sleep Res 30, e13077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40).Kovac K, Vincent GE, Paterson JL, Aisbett B, Reynolds AC, Ferguson SA (2020) Can an increase in noradrenaline induced by brief exercise counteract sleep inertia? Chronobiol Int 37, 9–10 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41).Hayashi M, Uchida C, Shoji T, Hori T (2004) The effects of the preference for music on sleep inertia after a short daytime nap. Sleep Biol Rhythms 2, 184–91. [Google Scholar]
  • 42).Clark I, Landolt HP (2017) Coffee, caffeine, and sleep: a systematic review of epidemiological studies and randomized controlled trials. Sleep Med Rev 31, 70–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43).Beaumont M, Batéjat D, Coste O, Doireau P, Chauffard F, Enslen M, Lagarde D, Pierard C (2005) Recovery after prolonged sleep deprivation: residual effects of slow-release caffeine on recovery sleep, sleepiness and cognitive functions. Neuropsychobiology 51, 16–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44).Paech GM, Della Vedova C, Pajcin M, Grant C, Kamimori G, Banks S (2016) Caffeine has minimal effects on daytime recovery sleep following severe sleep deprivation. Sleep Biol Rhythms 14, 149–56. [Google Scholar]
  • 45).Roberts B, Vincent GE, Ferguson SA, Reynolds AC, Jay SM (2019) Understanding the differing impacts of on-call work for males and females: results from an online survey. Int J Environ 16, 370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Industrial Health are provided here courtesy of National Institute of Occupational Safety and Health, Japan

RESOURCES