Abstract
Fatigue is a common experience in both health and disease. Yet, pathological (i.e., prolonged or chronic) and transient (i.e., exertional) fatigue symptoms are traditionally considered distinct, compounding a separation between interested research fields within the study of fatigue. Within the clinical neurosciences, nascent frameworks position pathological fatigue as a product of inference derived through hierarchical predictive processing. The metacognitive theory of dyshomeostasis (Stephan et al., 2016) states that pathological fatigue emerges from the metacognitive mechanism in which the detection of persistent mismatches between prior interoceptive predictions and ascending sensory evidence (i.e., prediction error) signals low evidence for internal generative models, which undermine an agent’s feeling of mastery over the body and is thus experienced phenomenologically as fatigue. Although acute, transient subjective symptoms of exertional fatigue have also been associated with increasing interoceptive prediction error, the dynamic computations that underlie its development have not been clearly defined. Here, drawing on the metacognitive theory of dyshomeostasis, we extend this account to offer an explicit description of the development of fatigue during extended periods of (physical) exertion. Accordingly, it is proposed that a loss of certainty or confidence in control predictions in response to persistent detection of prediction error features as a common foundation for the conscious experience of both pathological and nonpathological fatigue.
Keywords: Fatigue, Predictive processing, Allostasis, Exercise, Interoception, Metacognition
Introduction
The experience of fatigue is ubiquitous in both health and disease. Although the concept of fatigue is firmly embedded within modern life, typical lay understanding of what is meant when describing oneself as fatigued can be diverse and multifaceted. Dictionary definitions of fatigue (n.) include 1) “extreme tiredness resulting from mental or physical exertion or illness”; 2) “a reduction in the efficiency of a muscle or organ after prolonged activity”; and 3) “a lessening in one’s response to or enthusiasm for something, caused by overexposure” (Stevenson, 2010). Thus, fatigue is often used to describe a broad combination of physical, sensory, and cognitive epiphenomena. As such, colloquial understanding of fatigue may serve to complicate its formal study, exacerbate its multidimensionality (Karshikoff et al., 2017), and emphasise the need to clearly distinguish it from other related phenomena (e.g., sleepiness) which share common descriptors (e.g., “tiredness”; Shen et al., 2006).
Currently, there is no universally accepted definition or agreed standard measure for the assessment of fatigue (Dittner et al., 2004; Kluger et al., 2013). Consequently, the study of fatigue has forged increasingly reductionistic approaches across divergent fields, fragmenting fatigue research (Pattyn et al., 2018). One common distinction is the separation of pathological fatigue from the “normal,” physiological fatigue that we all experience in everyday life. The latter is a transient, nonpathological symptom with an identifiable cause (e.g., physical or cognitive exertion), which wanes with the removal of the stressor (i.e., through rest). On the other hand, pathological fatigue is the prolonged (1-5 months) or chronic (>6 months) experience of symptoms (Jason et al., 2010), which are unalleviated by rest and may present as a primary symptom, a secondary symptom or a comorbidity within neurological diseases (Penner & Paul, 2017). Qualitative reports from patients indicate differences in both the severity and quality of fatigue experienced in disease (Flinn & Stube, 2010; Repping-Wuts et al., 2008; Scott et al., 2011) supporting this distinction. However, due to the absence of a universal and standardised measure of fatigue, quantification of these differences across health and disease may be limited by the type of fatigue assessed (e.g., trait vs. state) and the heterogeneity (e.g., uni- vs. multidimensional) of symptoms considered (Dittner et al., 2004). Indeed, the separate instruments used to assess chronic/trait versus acute symptoms of fatigue may in itself aid in perpetuating distinctions and introduce confounding affective influences (e.g., depression) on the interpretation of (trait/chronic) fatigue symptoms (c.f. Tseng et al., 2010). It remains unclear whether pathological and nonpathological fatigue can be considered fully distinct phenomena. Interestingly, a common feature of pathological fatigue is an exacerbation of symptoms following some form of acute exertion (Nijs et al., 2010). It is therefore conceivable that exertional fatigue is a fundamental component encompassed within the pathological experience of fatigue, supporting a common mechanism within both disease and health that ultimately functions to regulate energy expenditure and work output (Chaudhuri & Behan, 2004).
Within the clinical neurosciences, several theories of pathological fatigue and its underpinning neurocomputations have recently been proposed, each aligned to contemporary understanding of brain function (Kuppuswamy, 2017; Stephan et al., 2016). Building upon early propositions that perception represents unconscious, knowledge-driven inference (Helmholtz, 1860), these contemporary accounts build upon the notion that our ability to perceive, act, attend, and learn may all be accounted for by viewing the brain as a self-evidencing inference machine (Dayan et al., 1995; Friston, 2005). Predictive processing, in which explanations of sensory states are not simply extracted from sensory input streams, but instead constructed through establishing causal structures that incorporate prior expectations of sensory inputs, is posited to be a core principle of brain function (Bubic et al., 2010; Clark, 2013; Friston, 2009; Hohwy, 2014). The consideration of how precisely the brain constructs perception at the forefront of emerging accounts of pathological fatigue contrasts with many current perspectives on the transient symptoms of fatigue in response to acute, protracted exertion in health. Although previous predictive processing accounts offer a common foundation to describe and formally distinguish pathological from nonpathological, exertional fatigue (Stephan et al., 2016), an explicit account of the dynamics of the inferential processes underpinning the development of the latter is yet to be clearly described. Our goal therefore is to extend the metacognitive theory of dyshomeostasis (Stephan et al., 2016) and offer a more explicit account of the changing inferential processes that give rise to the conscious experience of fatigue during acute, transient exertion. We focus on how this framework may be applied to the study of transient fatigue symptoms arising from homeostatic perturbations incurred through sustained physical exercise.1
What is Fatigue? – Fatigue vs. fatigability
Despite the absence of a standardised definition of fatigue, recent taxonomical frameworks for the study of the various causes and consequences of fatigue have been proposed (Enoka & Duchateau, 2016; Kluger et al., 2013). These taxonomies has been applied to facilitate articulation of fatigue in both neurological conditions (Kluger et al., 2013) and in response to acute, physical exertion (Enoka & Duchateau, 2016). Within it, fatigue is described as a self-reported, disabling symptom incorporating two dimensions: a subjective dimension (“perceived fatigue”), associated with broad feelings linked to weariness and exhaustion, and/or an objective dimension (“performance fatigability”), denoting a reduction in some marker of (physical and/or cognitive) performance (Kluger et al., 2013). Although these aspects of fatigue may interact, they are ultimately considered distinct and may emerge independently (Kluger et al., 2013). In the context of physical exertion, recently proposed frameworks have expanded the subjective dimension such that it includes any change in perception that aids in regulating the performer and is referred to as “perceived fatigability” (Enoka & Duchateau, 2016). Examples experienced during a task may include the perceived level of effort required (Staiano et al., 2018)and/or changes in affective valence (Hartman et al., 2019). Although combining multiple axes under one broad rubric has appeal for describing the complex and constantly changing processes involved in the control of physical performance, the concept of “perceived fatigability” may risk confusing an already complicated and multifactorial phenomenon, such as fatigue, and may detract from its functional role in the context of physical exertion. That is, the subjective component of fatigue is often conflated and used interchangeably with concepts, such as perception of effort. Although related, these represent distinct constructs (Halperin & Emanuel, 2020; Micklewright et al., 2017) with separate functional consequences (Greenhouse-Tucknott et al., 2020).
Fundamentally, fatigue represents a subjective symptom (Penner & Paul, 2017). Indeed, emphasis on changes in objective, performance-based markers of fatigue (i.e., fatigability), particularly within health, may capture only a small part of the total fatigue process since performance may be maintained through goal-orientated control processes at the expense of the perception of fatigue (Hockey, 2011; Hockey, 2013). It also is important to recognise that acute performance decrements may emerge following protracted exertion in response to other transient, affective states, such as boredom. Boredom and fatigue may be differentiated based on the level of arousal engendered by a task, corresponding to a state of under- or over-arousal, respectively (Pattyn et al., 2008). Thus, in healthy populations two conditions appear necessary for the acute, transient emergence of fatigue: 1) high task demands or arousal in which, 2) the ability to exert effective control is challenged (Hockey, 2011). In this perspective, we describe a neurocomputational account of the subjective perception of fatigue (hereafter simply referred to as fatigue) that emerges in response to changing perceptions of control efficacy during demanding physical exertion, based on a predictive processing framework.
Hierarchical Predictive Processing – A neurobiologically plausible theory of the brain
Under conceptualisations of predictive processing, the brain is formalised as a statistical organ that continuously seeks to render the external environment predictable. This is achieved through forming and testing predictions of generative models (i.e., models that explain how sensory inputs are generated probabilistically by latent states of the world) against incoming sensory evidence (Clark, 2013). By inverting generative models (i.e., computing the probability of the states of the world, given the sensory inputs), perception is described as a form of statistical inference achieved through minimizing the discrepancy (prediction error) between predictions (or perceptual hypotheses) and current sensory evidence (Stephan et al., 2016). Unlike traditional views of perception as a stimulus-response process, perception is instead highly ingrained and contextualised by our beliefs2 about the statistical nature of the world (Clark, 2015). In keeping with the Bayesian brain hypothesis (Knill & Pouget, 2004), models are cast as following an approximate form of Bayesian inference. This inferential process is implemented through predictive coding (Friston, 2005; Rao & Ballard, 1999). Together, multilevel cortical systems are proposed to transfer prediction and prediction error across a series of recurrent ‘loops,’ in which probabilistic predictions (or priors/beliefs) are passed top-down from higher to lower levels, while prediction errors (i.e., the unexplained data) travel laterally (within the same level) and ascend through forward projections to the level above and serve to update predictions (Fig. 1). This is equivalent to a belief update or the transformation of prior into posterior probabilities. In these models, the goal is to update beliefs at all levels of the hierarchy such that the discrepancy between predicted states and sensory inputs is minimal. Optimization of posterior probability therefore operates through the minimization of prediction error and enables perceptual inference, which across the multiple levels of the hierarchy can provide deep explanation of sensory data (Friston, 2009; Hohwy, 2014).
Prediction error may be minimized in two ways: 1) predictions may be updated to better explain sensory inputs (i.e., perceptual inference; described above); 2) sensory evidence that conforms to prior expectations may be selectively sampled through action. For example, within the motor system, motor commands are recast as a set of proprioceptive predictions concerning the sensory consequences of action, which are enacted through classical closed-loop reflexes enacted at lower levels of the hierarchy (Adams et al., 2013). Under the framework of active inference, perception and action are integrated functions of a coordinated process designed to minimize the experience of prediction error (Adams et al., 2013; Friston et al., 2016). Critically, understanding how perception and action emerge within predictive frameworks also requires a measure of the expected variability in the sensory data. Therefore, inferential computations must predict the sensory inputs (first-order predictions) and the reliability of this signal (second-order predictions) to make informed judgments on which to base perception and engage action (Kanai et al., 2015). The relative precision (i.e., inverse uncertainty; reflecting signal-to-noise, or more informally, confidence in the sensory signal) of prediction and prediction error determines their respective influence on posterior beliefs (Fig. 1a and b; see Friston, 2009). Predictions of high relative precision will dominate inferential processes (e.g., action), even in the presence of conflicting sensory evidence. Conversely, predictions of low precision afford greater weighting to sensory inputs which may force us to reevaluate and update model predictions (Friston, 2009, 2010). Attention has been proposed as a process of precision inference, through which estimates of precision may be optimized (Feldman & Friston, 2010; Hohwy, 2012). Therefore attention, alongside action and perception, represents a natural component of error minimization within predictive processing. The estimation of expected precision within uncertain environments corresponds to higher-order beliefs concerning the reliability of sensory evidence (Fig. 1; Clark et al., 2018). Beliefs about beliefs (i.e., beliefs about estimated precision; hyperpriors) may provide a formalised description of metacognition (Friston et al., 2013). Within predictive processing, metacognition has been described as a high-level form of inference associated with the overall performance of prediction error minimization (Petzschner et al., 2017; Stephan et al., 2016)
Inferred Dyshomeostasis – Allostasis, metacognition, and pathological fatigue.
As well as inferring the ambiguous states of our external environment, our brains also must infer the cause of hidden states of the body (Seth & Critchley, 2013). Several accounts have applied predictive processing within the domain of interoception to explain how dynamically changing visceral, metabolic, autonomic, immunological, and hormonal conditions are integrated (Ainley et al., 2016; Barrett, 2017; Barrett & Simmons, 2015; Gu et al., 2013; Seth et al., 2012; Seth & Critchley, 2013; Seth & Friston, 2016). All biological agents strive to maintain internal states within set bounds (homeostasis) and minimize the entropy of experienced sensory states (Friston, 2010). Interoceptive inference provides a potential explanation of how our brains respond to anticipated future needs of the body (allostasis; Sterling, 2012), and how those needs may be maintained across varying temporal scales; from (short-term) autonomic reflexes to (long-term) gross behavioural responses (Pezzulo et al., 2015). Allostasis and the representation of interoceptive consequences of action is a fundamental principle of all basic psychological functions (Kleckner et al., 2017), including our emotional experiences (Barrett, 2017; Critchley & Garfinkel, 2017; Joffily & Coricelli, 2013) and the conscious appreciation and recognition of self (Apps & Tsakiris, 2014; Seth et al., 2012; Seth & Friston, 2016). For a more in-depth discussion on the formulations of homeostatic and allostatic function within current predictive processing frameworks, see Corcoran and Hohwy (2019).
Increasingly, the (patho)aetiology of subjective feelings of fatigue have been attributed to altered computational inference, affecting the ability of hierarchical systems to reconcile sensorimotor/interoceptive predictions and sensory data (Clark et al., 2016; Edwards et al., 2012; Kuppuswamy, 2017; Manjaly et al., 2019; Stephan et al., 2016; Wrightson et al., 2019). This may occur at several loci within the hierarchy (Petzschner et al., 2017; Stephan et al., 2016). For example, dysfunctional sensory receptors (Light et al., 2009) may increase prediction error even in the absence of overt perturbations, while unduly precise prior beliefs may generate flawed perceptual inferences that are impervious to prediction error updates (van der Schaaf et al., 2018). In respect of the latter, abnormal prior beliefs endowed with high precision through the misallocation of attentional resources have been proposed as the theoretical basis of many medically unexplained or “psychogenic” symptoms (Edwards et al., 2012). In this context, pathological symptoms of fatigue may potentially emerge as an indirect consequence of abnormal attentional processes (Edwards et al., 2012). However, it remains unclear exactly how the experience of fatigue would emerge exactly. Alternative accounts have described fatigue as a direct outcome of metacognitive processing (Stephan et al., 2016). Here, continued experience of (interoceptive) prediction error, despite engaging in corrective action, is monitored within a metacognitive layer and forges the belief that allostatic control processes are incapable of effectively minimizing interceptive prediction error (that is, one experiences low “allostatic control self-efficacy”), which is experienced subjectively as fatigue (Stephan et al., 2016).
Stephan et al.'s (2016) compelling model provides a computational account of pathological fatigue, which the authors contend may be formally distinguished from the acute, transient exertional fatigue experienced in health (referred to by the authors as “tiredness”3). In this model, changes at a somatic level and/or within the hierarchical system’s circuitry in response to disease disrupts computational functions (which may involve various levels of the hierarchy) such that dyshomeostasis (error within the interoceptive model) is persistently experienced, engendering a perceived lack of control over bodily states which in turn fosters the conscious perception of fatigue. Importantly, in pathological fatigue, these disruptions affect the system in such a way that the detection of dyshomeostasis is not abated even through rest. Therefore, mastery over body states is not easily restored and the perception of fatigue endures. This is the fundamental difference between chronic, pathological experiences of fatigue and the acute feeling states incurred through physical activity. In the latter, prediction errors arising from metabolic responses to exercise can be effectively attenuated by action (i.e., rest), allowing the gradient of interoceptive “surprise” to turn negative and thus agency to be experienced through the restoration of homeostasis during a period of recovery.
However, Stephan and colleague’s description of acute symptoms of fatigue does not explore the dynamics of the inference-control loop during the homeostatic challenges imposed by prolonged physical exertion (Gabriel & Zierath, 2017; Jeukendrup et al., 2000; Joyner & Coyle, 2008). Thus, an explicit account of how the acute, transient symptoms of fatigue emerge in relation to protracted physical exertion remains absent. Here, we conform to the proposition forwarded by Stephan et al. (2016)—that fatigue arises from the metacognitive detection of persistent prediction error which reduces perceived capability of exerting effective control over the body (reduced self-efficacy)—and extend the metacognitive theory of dyshomeostasis to describe the changing metacognitive beliefs and inferential computations that may underlie the development of exertional fatigue.
Transient Exertional Fatigue - A predictive processing model
According to Stephan et al. (2016), pathological fatigue may initially reflect an adaptive process, promoting rest and the conservation of energetic resources when effective actions to restore homeostasis are not perceived to be present. We extend this, contending that the experience of fatigue serves a similar function during acute physical exertion, in line with previous assertions (St Clair Gibson & Noakes, 2004). The selection of (goal-directed) actions may be devolved to an inferential issue, whereby actions and their predicted outcomes are chosen for the agent to ultimately frequent predictable states long-term(Friston, 2009; Friston et al., 2014). Importantly, this may involve predicting the consequences of actions across different temporal scales and contexts (Pezzulo et al., 2018). In some instances, the active experience of uncertain states may be tolerated if it helps guide future behaviour towards the minimization of prediction error (e.g., exploitation versus exploration; Schwartenbeck et al., 2013). However, for goal-directed actions to be maintained, the precision of the driving high-level beliefs must be high in order to dominate posterior beliefs, maintain attention and enable action (Pezzulo et al., 2015). However, all physical behaviour involves energetic expenditure, providing an ever-changing homeostatic context which must also be predicted within the transition towards the said outcome. The integration of salient, motivational information (e.g., bodily states) informs inferential processes that influence the selection of control policies in the pursuit of goal states (Pezzulo et al., 2018). Accordingly, for action to continue in any given form, interoceptive prediction error (and effects on control circuits) must be suppressed. However, the act of doing so, through attentional processes4 or lower-level perceptual updates, indicates that initial predictions did not provide a good fit to the sensory data.
The continued need to suppress prediction error in the pursuit of goals is the important extension of the proposal outlined by Stephan et al. (2016), forming the basis of metacognitive inference dynamics leading to the experience of fatigue emerging from acute (physical) exertion. We propose that the suppression of prediction error at all lower-levels during the maintenance of goal-directed action driven by higher levels, is subject to the same metacognitive appraisal as described in the conceptualisation of pathological fatigue. Persistent experience of error within hierarchical models undermines (short-term) control mastery, reducing self-efficacy and is experienced phenomenologically as fatigue. Metacognition within predictive processing may be ascribed computationally to beliefs about the precision of priors (Friston et al., 2013). The metacognitive experience of fatigue may therefore be associated with an increasing uncertainty in expected sensory states, reflecting low precision beliefs cascading down from high to low levels of the hierarchy. Low precision at higher, temporally-distal, and more abstract levels of prediction may be unable to contextualise prediction error effectively. Eventually over time, this decline in precision beliefs may be such that there is a shift in the dominant control of behaviour towards lower levels, which attempt to reconcile more immediate differences between predictions and prediction errors (Pezzulo et al., 2015; Fig. 2).
To illustrate this hypothesis, we provide an example based on the experience of fatigue during sustained physical exercise. Once the decision to engage with exercise has been made, the brain must predict not only the proprioceptive consequences of action, interactions with the social and physical environment, but also the homeostatic consequences of the transition towards the intended goal state, in order to maintain biological integrity. This is obviously challenging, since protracted whole-body exercise incurs a vast array of ever-changing processes across a range of biological systems (Hawley et al., 2014); the strength of which may exceed the control capable by simple reflexes at the lowest level and therefore competes for attention and higher-level control (Fig. 2). To continue towards goals (e.g. persist with the current exercise task), precise, high-level beliefs must be maintained. Action (i.e. reducing intensity) offers a means to minimize interoceptive prediction errors during goal pursuit but in certain instances this may directly challenge the probability of the expected or intended goal state being reached.
Alternately, changes in attention and perceptual updates arising from prediction error minimization at lower levels may serve to enable goal-directed action to continue in a given form (e.g., speed). This may however, come at the expense of incurring further prediction error further on within a new inferential cycle. Regular and continued detection of error and the need for ensuing updates to maintain action increases evidence against initial model predictions and the capacity to effectively control the body’s states during exercise. As such, self-efficacy in ones' capacity to experience future goal states whilst controlling homeostatic integrity in this transition is undermined and experienced consciously as fatigue. Reduced confidence propagates, top-down, through the hierarchy, thus higher goal beliefs increasingly lose their ability to predict future outcomes and contextualise lower-level prediction errors. At this point, a shift to lower-level controllers may attempt to restore self-efficacy by minimizing the experience of prediction error on more immediate timescales (Pezzulo et al., 2015). That is, we are forced to slow down or stop altogether.
Minimization of interoceptive prediction errors may not be immediate (e.g., replenishment of resting muscle glycogen stores can take many hours following exercise; Casey et al., 1995), therefore proprioceptive predictions5 may potentially be used as proxies for predicting the states which will eventually see interoceptive prediction error minimized (Critchley & Garfinkel, 2017; Pezzulo et al., 2018). By slowing down or stopping all together, the minimization of proprioceptive (and prospective interoceptive) prediction errors signals conditions that will see the restoration of homeostasis, indicating a level of control over bodily states which serves to alleviate fatigue (Stephan et al., 2016). However, the restoration of control self-efficacy is not immediate. Indeed, allostatic predictions used to maintain homeostatic set-points likely function across different biological systems under their own individual time constants (Stephan et al., 2016), which may see the experience of dyshomeostasis prolonged during rest. In addition, incurred deficits in allostatic self-efficacy and the subsequent decline in the precision afforded to control predictions (the proposed hallmark of fatigue) may underscore the protracted experience of fatigue post-exertion even when homeostatic balance has been restored. That is, increased sensitivity to prediction error during recovery (i.e., rest) may be maintained due to the loss of precision afforded to control predictions. It is possible that full alleviation of the conscious experience of fatigue may require the detection of a similar series of events to that through which it developed (i.e., protracted experience of accurate predictions related to interoceptive states). If this is the case, the experience of fatigue may urge us to stop in order to restore confidence in allostatic control beliefs and rest may represent the most effective context in which this may occur.
Neuroanatomy of Interoceptive Monitoring During Physical Exertion – Neurophysiological evidence
The neuroanatomical circuits supporting interoception and homeostasis have been extensively described (Craig, 2002, 2003; Critchley & Harrison, 2013; Damasio & Carvalho, 2013) and incorporated within models of interoceptive inference and allostatic control (Barrett & Simmons, 2015; Gu et al., 2013; Stephan et al., 2016). Stephan et al. (2016) propose an additional metacognitive layer within established interoceptive pathways which monitors interoceptive prediction performance and is responsible for the emergence of fatigue. Putative structures include visceromotor regions, such as the anterior insula cortex (AIC) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)(Craig, 2002)and/or portions of the anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC)(Fleming & Dolan, 2012). In support of the former, the experience of fatigue has been shown to correlate with increased activity within visceromotor regions following an acute interoceptive (e.g., inflammation) challenge (Harrison et al., 2009, 2015). The dense connectivity of the AIC and ACC has seen them identified as part of the brain’s “rich-club,” enabling communication across diverse networks (e.g., default and salience) and facilitating the representation of the body and allostatic control as integral component of psychobehavioural functions (Barrett & Simmons, 2015; Kleckner et al., 2017).
Strikingly, studies of acute physical and cognitive exertion have identified a similar network involving the AIC, ACC, and portions of the dorsal lateral PFC (dlPFC), activated with demanding activity across both domains (see Müller & Apps, 2019 for summary). This network was implicated in the development of fatigue, assigned specifically with the monitoring of neural-responses within task-specific regions and the computation of the cost/benefits of continued action (Müller & Apps, 2019). Insula activity has also been shown to parallel increasing physiological demands in response to greater exercise intensities and durations (Williamson et al., 1999), with pronounced mid/AIC activation associated with the point of task failure (Hilty, Jäncke, et al., 2011a). Enhanced synchronisation of insula and primary motor cortex (M1) activity also has been observed toward the end of volitional exercise (Hilty, Langer, et al., 2011b), which may reflect the influence of the dynamically changing conditions of the body on selected control (action) policies. Indeed, one plausible hypothesis, similar to that proposed recently by McMorris et al. (2018), is that AIC is responsible for generating an awareness of the performance of interoceptive predictions and allostatic control during exercise, and therefore fatigue. The representation of allostatic control performance may be continuously influenced by inputs from the dlPFC, which may signal the level of uncertainty within (longer-term) predictions. The ACC is believed to play an important role in deciding between actions during physical exertion (Lutz, 2018) and thus may be involved in encoding the estimated precision of competing predictions and prediction errors based on inputs received regarding the performance of interoceptive predictions (see also Craig, 2009). Precision dynamics arbitrate between the dominant controllers within the hierarchy in pursuit of the suppression of (long-term) prediction error (Pezzulo et al., 2015), with the encoding of precision assigned to neuromodulatory systems (e.g., dopamine; Friston et al., 2012). It has been proposed that the development of exertional fatigue may coincide with changes in the firing of neuromodulatory neurons within the (lateral) PFC, modulating motivation and choice based on the integration of interoceptive and reward pathways (McMorris et al., 2018). The current proposal holds that changes in neuromodulatory control across various regions of the brain reflects the optimisation of precision across the hierarchy to minimize prediction error, and fatigue is associated with a progressive reduction in precision estimates of high-level predictions driving action.
Testing the Model – Current empirical support and future hypotheses
The core proposition of the proposed model contends that detection of repeated prediction error within interoceptive circuits during acute bouts of physical exertion undermines control efficacy beliefs, reductions in which provide the foundation of the perception of fatigue. Several lines of evidence in the existing literature support these central assertions and provide the basis of future investigations.
Multiple studies have investigated the association between self-efficacy, emotion (Focht et al., 2007; Magnan et al., 2013; McAuley et al., 1999), and behavioural responses to acute physical exertion (Halper & Vancouver, 2016; Hutchinson et al., 2008; McAuley & Courneya, 1992; Weinberg et al., 1981). Perceived capacity to successfully engage in further physical exertion has been demonstrated to both increase (Blacklock et al., 2010; Katula et al., 1999) and decrease (Focht et al., 2007; Katula et al., 1999; Welch et al., 2010) in response to acute bouts of exercise. The direction of this effect appears to be associated with the intensity of the bout (Blacklock et al., 2010; Katula et al., 1999). Deterioration in performance self-efficacy has been demonstrated at intensities at which a change in the metabolic environment is evident (Welch et al., 2010) and importantly, this decrement has been associated with the phenomenological experience of fatigue (Focht et al., 2007). These findings provide support for the proposition that physiological perturbations evoked by demanding physical exertion impact perceived efficacy which may be the origin for the acute, transient experience of exertional fatigue. However, future research is required to empirically establish whether unstable metabolic responses during exercise lead directly to a deterioration in self-efficacy and to confirm the temporal association between changes in perceived efficacy and the phenomenological experience of fatigue during physical exertion. Weakening of self-efficacy is proposed to evolve from continued metacognitive detection of prediction error (Stephan et al., 2016). Presently, few studies have directly examined the effect of physical exertion on metacognitive function (Palmer et al., 2019), despite recognition that it is a process fundamental to self-regulation during prolonged physical exertion (Brick et al., 2015; Brick et al., 2020; see Brick et al., 2016). Future examination of the metacognitive basis of exertional fatigue under the present proposal could see the use of a combination of metacognitive assessments alongside the modelling of precision estimates from behavioural responses (Hezemans et al., 2020; Wolpe et al., 2014). Interestingly, using this behavioural modelling approach, apathy—a construct often correlated with fatigue—has recently been associated with a low precision-weighting of performance predictions (Hezemans et al., 2020).
It is unclear whether potential reductions in self-efficacy relate to confidence in one’s ability to control or accurately perceive viscerosensory information. Experimental manipulation of prediction error within interoceptive paradigms provides a suitable challenge. The exercise science literature is replete with experimental interventions designed to manipulate physical or perceived states during exercise, with a predominant focus on altering interoceptive-associated feedback (Amann et al., 2009; Castle et al., 2012; Hartman et al., 2019; Iodice et al., 2019). However, it is important to note for future investigations that under the current framework, perception is understood only when descending beliefs and ascending sensory evidence are considered in conjunction. To highlight this point, recent evidence demonstrated that perception of a weak, exteroceptive stimulus was influenced by its relative timing within the cardiac cycle with effects of presumed heartbeat-event predictions observed throughout the somatosensory cortex (Al et al., 2020). It is expected that interoceptive self-efficacy will be most impaired when the divergence between expectations and physiological states is at its greatest.
In healthy populations, sustained contractions are perceived as more effortful when performed in a perceived state of fatigue, even in condition absent of any functional changes to the active muscles (Greenhouse-Tucknott et al., 2020). Similarly, effort perception during motor tasks has been associated with trait fatigue in clinical populations, such as stroke survivors, suffering from chronic fatigue (De Doncker et al., 2020)6. This may be explained within the proposed model as follows: lower precision estimates in prior beliefs at higher levels emerging with (exertional) fatigue (reflecting increasing uncertainty in model predictions) cause greater prediction error at lower levels. Perception of effort is associated with movement or action costs, reflecting high gain, or insufficient attenuation, of action-induced prediction error (Kuppuswamy, 2017), or more formally described under information theoretical principles, as the information cost of updating prior beliefs (Zénon et al., 2019). Therefore, the decline in precision estimates associated with fatigue form greater prediction error that generates a heightened perception of effort. What is not currently clear is whether, in healthy populations, the magnitude of fatigue experienced by an individual is directly proportional to the perceived effort during action. Further examination of the effect of increasing severity of fatigue on the perception of effort and the regulation of motor performance are warranted.
It is recognised that assessment of an individual’s interoceptive ability can span various dimensions, including the accuracy of one’s interoceptive judgments, self-reported confidence in those judgements and also insight into those judgements, i.e., metacognitive evaluation of the correspondence between objective accuracy and confidence in the accuracy of those judgements (Garfinkel et al., 2015; Garfinkel & Critchley, 2013; see also Khalsa et al., 2018). Despite its clear relevance to exertional fatigue and performance fatigability (McMorris et al., 2018; Robertson & Marino, 2016), studies on the effect of individuals’ (conscious and nonconscious) awareness of physiological states on perception and performance during physical exertion are sparse, conflicting and presently limited to measures of interoceptive accuracy (da Silva Machado et al., 2019; Herbert et al., 2007; Köteles et al., 2020). How an individual’s insight into representations of interoceptive signals influences acute physical exertion and the development of fatigue is currently unknown. Although yet to be empirically tested, it is possible that an individual’s interoceptive insight may change the relationship between a perceived state of fatigue and other perceptions arising from prediction error, such as the perception of effort, during physical exertion. That is, those who are more aware of the accuracy of their interoceptive judgments may experience smaller, relative reductions in precision beliefs under conditions of fatigue, therefore attenuating the rise in prediction error and the perception of effort. In line with this hypothesis, interoceptive insight has been shown to reduce one’s susceptibility to an exteroceptive manipulation of self-location (Bekrater-Bodmann et al., 2020), suggesting that precise higher-order representations of the body are effective at limiting the influence of (exteroceptive) prediction error on belief updates. Evaluation of how conscious insight into interoceptive representations of bodily shapes this relationship may help further define how metacognitive processes influence subjective experiences aroused during acute fatiguing exertion.
The neuroanatomical circuits supporting the development of pathological fatigue based on metacognitive detection of persistent interoceptive prediction-error have been proposed (see Stephan et al., 2016; “Neuroanatomy of interoceptive monitoring during physical exertion – neurophysiological evidence ” section). Functional resting-state connectivity and dynamic causal modelling (DCM) provides a potential means of establishing interactions between the nodes within this metacognitive interoception-network in response to fatigue (Stephan et al., 2016). Similar methods have recently been used to quantify changes in effective connectivity with the development of fatigue during protracted cognitive tasks (Wylie et al., 2020) and have indicated abnormal inhibition between hemispheres as a factor in the development of persistent fatigue in people with stroke (Ondobaka et al., 2019). In the context of the present model, it may be expected that an increased connectivity between visceromotor and prefrontal regions of the putative interoceptive metacognition network and primary interoceptive cortex (posterior insula cortex) (Barrett & Simmons, 2015) occurs as participants monitor the presence of dyshomeostasis during physical exertion. However, whether changes in effective connectivity between key nodes is associated with the development of exertional fatigue has yet to be elucidated.
Concluding Remarks
In this perspective article, we draw upon recent propositions for the computational basis of pathological fatigue and extend this framework to provide an account of the subjective experience of fatigue evoked through acute exertion. Fundamentally, whether developing in response to pathology or as an acute, transient state during physical exertion, the experience of fatigue is a product of inference associated with an increased uncertainty in one’s capacity to accurately predict and control sensory states. This framework is fundamentally entrenched within current conceptualisations of how the brain works. We hope that with this article we have highlighted and indeed emphasised the potential of predictive processing to provide a common framework for the study of fatigue across health and disease.
Acknowledgments
The authors extend sincere thanks to Dr. Paul Ansdell for his constructive and insightful comments during the preparation of this manuscript. All authors were involved in the construction of the manuscript and approved the submitted version.
Declarations
Conflict of interests
No conflict of interest
Footnotes
As described by Kuppuswamy (2017), if fatigue is the product of inference it is unitary and thus the common separation of fatigue between physical and cognitive domains may be unnecessary. Accordingly, the same principles described here in the explanation of fatigue emerging through physical exertion may also account for fatigue symptoms incurred through prolonged, demanding cognitive activity, although the exact locus of the disturbance (i.e., the level within the hierarchical system) and the specific systems (e.g., interoception vs. control) monitored may vary between domains (Müller & Apps, 2019).
The probabilistic representation of the causes of sensory data before observing the data.
We do not subscribe to the adopted terminology used by Stephan et al. (2016) in the description of feelings associated with chronic (i.e., fatigue) and acute (i.e., tiredness) experiences. Feelings of “tiredness” may be particularly problematic in the description of fatigue, because it also is used to describe a subjective state of sleepiness (Pigeon et al., 2003; Shen et al., 2006). Thus, tiredness may not accurately depict the subjective experiences reported after exertion (Leproult et al., 1997; Matsumoto et al., 2002). In response to demanding physical exertion, fatigue is a fundamental subjective experience.
We acknowledge the current understanding of volitional control of endogenous attention within predictive coding frameworks is not entirely clear (Metzinger, 2017).
An extension of the metacognitive theory of dyshomeostasis (Manjaly et al., 2019) proposes that metacognitive monitoring may not be exclusive to interoceptive circuits and may involve the detection a chronic mismatch between prediction and actual sensory evidence across different systems (e.g., proprioception; exteroception). In addition, interdependency between systems (Pezzulo et al., 2018) may see the persistent detection of error and development of fatigue permeate across different systems.
The authors propose that the experience of a high perception of effort drives the (trait) perception of fatigue in stroke survivors, rather than (state) fatigue driving elevated perceptions of effort (De Doncker et al., 2020). The model presented here suggests that the relationship between fatigue and effort may be circular; error is the basis of the perception of effort, which if persistently detected generates a feeling of fatigue. Changing precision beliefs associated with fatigue gives rise to greater error, which in turn elevates the perception of effort.
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- Adams RA, Shipp S, Friston KJ. Predictions not commands: active inference in the motor system. Brain Structure and Function. 2013;218:611–643. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0475-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ainley V, Apps MA, Fotopoulou A, Tsakiris M. “Bodily precision”: A predictive coding account of individual differences in interoceptive accuracy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2016;371(1708):20160003. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Al E, Iliopoulosa F, Forschacka N, Nierhausa T, Grunda M, Motykaf P, Gaeblera M, Nikulina VV, Villringera A. Heart–brain interactions shape somatosensory perception and evoked potentials. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2020;117(19):10575–10584. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1915629117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amann M, Proctor LT, Sebranek JJ, Pegelow DF, Dempsey JA. Opioid-mediated muscle afferents inhibit central motor drive and limit peripheral muscle fatigue development in humans. Journal of Physiology. 2009;587(1):271–283. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2008.163303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Apps MA, Tsakiris M. The free-energy self : A predictive coding account of self- recognition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2014;44:85–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.029.The. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett LF. The theory of constructed emotion: An active inference account of interoception and categorization. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2017;12(1):1–23. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett LF, Simmons WK. Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2015;16(7):419–429. doi: 10.1038/nrn3950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bekrater-Bodmann R, Azevedo RT, Ainley V, Tsakiris M. Interoceptive awareness is negatively related to the exteroceptive manipulation of bodily self-location. Frontiers in Psychology. 2020;11:562016. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.562016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blacklock R, Rhodes R, Blanchard C, Gaul C. Effects of exercise intensity and self-efficacy on state anxiety with breast cancer survivors. Oncology Nusing Forum. 2010;37(2):206–212. doi: 10.1188/10.ONF.206-212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brick NE, MacIntyre TE, Campbell MJ. Metacognitive processes in the self-regulation of performance in eliteendurance runners. Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 2015;1:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2015.02.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Brick, N. E., MacIntyre, T. E., & Campbell, M. J. (2016). Thinking and action: A cognitive perspective on self-regulation during endurance performance. Frontiers in Physiology, 7. 10.3389/fphys.2016.00159 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Brick NE, Campbell MJ, Sheehan RB, Fitzpatrick BL, MacIntyre TE. Metacognitive processes and attentional focus in recreational endurance runners. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 2020;18(3):362–379. doi: 10.1080/1612197x.2018.1519841. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bubic, A., von Cramon, D. Y., & Schubotz, R. I. (2010). Prediction, cognition and the brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 25. 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Casey A, Short AH, Hultman E, Greenhafft PL. Gycogen resynthesis in human muscle fibre types following exercise-induced glycogen depletion. Journal of Physiology. 1995;483:265–271. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1995.sp020583. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castle PC, Maxwell NS, Allchorn A, Mauger AR, White DK. Deception of ambient and body core temperature improves self paced cycling in hot, humid conditions. European Journal of Applied Physiology. 2012;112:377–385. doi: 10.1007/s00421-011-1988-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaudhuri A, Behan PO. Fatigue in neurological disorders. The Lancet. 2004;363(9413):978–988. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(04)15794-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark A. Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2013;36(3):181–204. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12000477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark A. Embodied Predicton. In: Metzinger T, Windt J, editors. Open MIND 7(T) MIND Group; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Clark JE, Fai Ng W, Watson S, Newton JL. The aetiopathogenesis of fatigue: Unpredictable, complex and persistent. British Medical Bulletin. 2016;117(1):139–148. doi: 10.1093/bmb/ldv057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark JE, Watson S, Friston KJ. What is mood? A computational perspective. Psychological Medicine. 2018;48(14):2277–2284. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718000430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corcoran, A. W., & Hohwy, J. (2019). Allostasis, interoception, and the free energy principle: Feeling our way forward. In: M. Tsarkiris & H. De Preester (Eds.), The interoceptive Mind: From homeostasis to awareness (pp. 272–292). Oxford University Press. 10.31234/osf.io/zbqnx
- Craig AD. How do you feel? Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2002;3(8):655–666. doi: 10.1038/nrn894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig AD. Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2003;13(4):500–505. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00090-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel - now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 10, 59–70. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Critchley HD, Garfinkel SN. Interoception and emotion. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2017;17:7–14. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.04.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Critchley HD, Harrison NA. Visceral influences on brain and behavior. Neuron. 2013;77(4):624–638. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- da Silva Machado DG, de Farias Junior LF, do Nascimento PHD, Tavares MPM, da Silva SKA, Agrícola PMD, do Nascimento Neto LI, Fonteles AI, Elsangedy HM, Li LM, Okano AH. Can interoceptive accuracy influence maximal performance, physiological and perceptual responses to exercise? Physiology and Behavior. 2019;204:234–240. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.02.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Damasio A, Carvalho GB. The nature of feelings: Evolutionary and neurobiological origins. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2013;14(2):143–152. doi: 10.1038/nrn3403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dayan P, Hinton GE, Neal RM, Zemel RS. The Helmholtz machine. Neural Computation. 1995;7(5):889–904. doi: 10.1162/neco.1995.7.5.889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Doncker W, Chalres L, Ondobaka S, Kuppuswamy A. Exploring the relationship between effort perception and post-stroke fatigue. Neurology. 2020;95(24):e3321–e3330. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000010985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dittner AJ, Wessely SC, Brown RG. The assessment of fatigue: A practical guide for clinicians and researchers. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2004;56(2):157–170. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00371-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edwards MJ, Adams RA, Brown H, Pareés I, Friston KJ. A Bayesian account of “hysteria”. Brain. 2012;135(11):3495–3512. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enoka RM, Duchateau J. Translating fatigue to human performance. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 2016;48:2228–2238. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman H, Friston KJ. Attention, uncertainty, and free-energy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2010;4:215. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleming SM, Dolan RJ. The neural basis of metacognitive ability. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2012;367(1594):1338–1349. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flinn NA, Stube JE. Post-stroke fatigue: Qualitative study of three focus groups. Occupational Therapy International. 2010;17(2):81–91. doi: 10.1002/oti.286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Focht BC, Knapp DJ, Gavin TP, Raedeke TD, Hickner RC. Affective and self-efficacy responses to acute aerobic exercise in sedentary older and younger adults. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity. 2007;15:123–138. doi: 10.1123/japa.15.2.123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston K. A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2005;360(1456):815–836. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston K. The free-energy principle: A rough guide to the brain? Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2009;13(7):293–301. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston K. The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2010;11(2):127–138. doi: 10.1038/nrn2787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston K, Shiner T, FitzGerald T, Galea JM, Adams R, Brown H, Dolan RJ, Moran R, Stephan KE, Bestmann S. Dopamine, affordance and active inference. PLoS Computational Biology. 2012;8(1):e1002327. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ, Lawson R, Frith CD. On hyperpriors and hypopriors: Comment on Pellicano and Burr. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2013;17(1):1. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston K, Schwartenbeck P, Fitzgerald T, Moutoussis M, Behrens T, Dolan RJ. The anatomy of choice: dopamine and decision-making. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 2014;369:20130481. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ, FitzGerald T, Rigoli F, Schwartenbeck P, O’Doherty JP, Pezzulo G. Active inference and learning. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2016;68:862–879. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabriel BM, Zierath JR. The limits of exercise physiology: From performance to health. Cell Metabolism. 2017;25:1000–1011. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2017.04.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garfinkel SN, Critchley HD. Interoception, emotion and brain: New insights link internal physiology to social behaviour. Commentary on: “Anterior insular cortex mediates bodily sensibility and social anxiety” by Terasawa et al. (2012) Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2013;8(3):231–234. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garfinkel SN, Seth AK, Barrett AB, Suzuki K, Critchley HD. Knowing your own heart: Distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biological Psychology. 2015;104:65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenhouse-Tucknott A, Wrightson JG, Raynsford M, Harrison NA, Dekerle J. Interactions between perceptions of fatigue, effort and affect decrease knee extensor endurance performance following upper body motor activity, independent of changes to neuromuscular function. Psychophysiology. 2020;57(9):e13602. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gu X, Hof PR, Friston KJ, Fan J. Anterior insular cortex and emotional awareness. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2013;521(15):3371–3388. doi: 10.1002/cne.23368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halper LR, Vancouver JB. Self-efficacy’s influence on persistence on a physical task: Moderating effect of performance feedback ambiguity. Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 2016;22:170–177. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2015.08.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Halperin I, Emanuel A. Rating of perceived effort: Methodological concerns and future directions. Sports Medicine. 2020;50:679–687. doi: 10.1007/s40279-019-01229-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrison NA, Brydon L, Walker C, Gray MA, Steptoe A, Dolan RJ, Critchley HD. Neural origins of human sickness in interoceptive responses to inflammation. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;66(5):415–422. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrison NA, Cooper E, Dowell NG, Keramida G, Voon V, Critchley HD, Cercignani M. Quantitative magnetization transfer imaging as a biomarker for effects of systemic inflammation on the brain. Biological Psychiatry. 2015;78(1):49–57. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.09.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartman ME, Ekkekakis P, Dicks ND, Pettitt RW. Dynamics of pleasure-displeasure at the limit of exercise tolerance: Conceptualizing the sense of exertional physical fatigue as an affective response. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2019;222:jeb186585. doi: 10.1242/jeb.186585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawley JA, Hargreaves M, Joyner MJ, Zierath JR. Integrative biology of exercise. Cell. 2014;159(4):738–749. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.10.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helmholtz H. In: Handbuch der Physiogischen Optik. Southhall JPC, editor. Dover; 1860. [Google Scholar]
- Herbert BM, Ulbrich P, Schandry R. Interoceptive sensitivity and physical effort: Implications for the self-control of physical load in everyday life. Psychophysiology. 2007;44(2):194–202. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00493.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hezemans FH, Wolpe N, Rowe JB. Apathy is associated with reduced precision of prior beliefs about action outcomes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2020;149(9):1767–1777. doi: 10.1037/xge0000739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hilty L, Jäncke L, Luechinger R, Boutellier U, Lutz K. Limitation of physical performance in a muscle fatiguing handgrip exercise is mediated by thalamo-insular activity. Human Brain Mapping. 2011;32(12):2151–2160. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hilty L, Langer N, Pascual-Marqui R, Boutellier U, Lutz K. Fatigue-induced increase in intracortical communication between mid/anterior insular and motor cortex during cycling exercise. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;34(12):2035–2042. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07909.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hockey, G. R. J. (2011). A motivational control theory of cognitive fatigue. In P. L. Ackerman (Ed.), Cognitive fatigue: Multidisciplinary perspectives on current research and future applications. (pp. 167–188). American Psychological Association. 10.1037/12343-008
- Hockey, R. (2013). The psychology of fatigue: Work, effort and control. Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9781139015394
- Hohwy J. Attention and conscious perception in the hypothesis testing brain. Frontiers in Psychology. 2012;3:1–14. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hohwy J. The self-evidencing brain. Noûs. 2014;50(2):259–285. doi: 10.1111/nous.12062. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hutchinson JC, Sherman T, Mertinovic N, Tenenbaum G. The effect of manipulated self-efficacy on perceived and sustained effort. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology. 2008;20:457–472. doi: 10.1080/10413200802351151. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Iodice P, Porciello G, Bufalari I, Barca L, Pezzulo G. An interoceptive illusion of effort induced by false heart-rate feedback. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2019;116(28):13897–13902. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1821032116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jason LA, Evans M, Brown M, Porter N. What is fatigue? Pathological and nonpathological fatigue. PM & R. 2010;2:327–331. doi: 10.1016/j.pmrj.2010.03.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeukendrup AE, Craig NP, Hawley JA. The bioenergetics of world class cycling. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport. 2000;3(4):414–433. doi: 10.1016/S1440-2440(00)80008-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joffily M, Coricelli G. Emotional valence and the free-energy principle. PLoS Computational Biology. 2013;9(6):e1003094. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joyner MJ, Coyle EF. Endurance exercise performance: The physiology of champions. Journal of Physiology. 2008;586(1):35–44. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.143834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanai R, Komura Y, Shipp S, Friston K. Cerebral hierarchies: Predictive processing,precision and the pulvinar. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 2015;370:20140169. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karshikoff B, Sundelin T, Lasselin J. Role of inflammation in human fatigue: Relevance of multidimensional assessments and potential neuronal mechanisms. Frontiers in Immunology. 2017;8:1–12. doi: 10.3389/fimmu.2017.00021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katula JA, Blissmer BJ, McAuley E. Exercise intensity and self-efficacy effects on anxiety reduction in healthy, older adults. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1999;22(3):233–247. doi: 10.1023/A:1018768423349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khalsa SS, Adolphs R, Cameron OG, Critchley HD, Davenport PW, Feinstein JS, Feusner JD, Garfinkel SN, Lane RD, Mehling WE, Meuret AE, Nemeroff CB, Oppenheimer S, Petzschner FH, Pollatos O, Rhudy JL, Schramm LP, Simmons WK, Stein MB, et al. Interoception and mental health: A roadmap. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. 2018;3(6):501–513. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleckner, I. R., Zhang, J., Touroutoglou, A., Chanes, L., Xia, C., Simmons, W. K., Quigley, K. S., Dickerson, B. C., & Barrett, L. F. (2017). Evidence for a large-scale brain system supporting allostasis and interoception in humans. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(5). 10.1038/s41562-017-0069 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Kluger BM, Krupp LB, Enoka RM. Fatigue and fatigability in neurologic illnesses: Proposal for a unified taxonomy. Neurology. 2013;80(4):409–416. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31827f07be. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knill DC, Pouget A. The Bayesian brain: The role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation. Trends in Neurosciences. 2004;27(12):712–719. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Köteles F, Teufel B, Körmendi J, Ferentzi E, Szemerszky R. Cardioceptive accuracy is associated with arousal but not with valence and perceived exertion under physical load. Psychophysiology. 2020;57:e13620. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuppuswamy A. The fatigue conundrum. Brain. 2017;140(8):2240–2245. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leproult R, Van Reeth O, Byrne MM, Sturis J, Van Cauter E. Sleepiness, performance, and neuroendocrine function during sleep deprivation: Effects of exposure to bright light or exercise. Journal of Biological Rhythms. 1997;12(3):245–258. doi: 10.1177/074873049701200306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Light AR, White AT, Hughen RW, Light KC. Moderate exercise increases expression for sensory, adrenergic, and immune genes in chronic fatigue syndrome patients but not in normal subjects. Journal of Pain. 2009;10(10):1099–1112. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2009.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lutz K. Functional brain anatomy of exercise regulation. Progress in Brain Research. 2018;240:341–352. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magnan RE, Kwan BM, Bryan AD. Effects of current physical activity on affective response to exercise: Physical and social-cognitive mechanisms. Psychology & Health. 2013;28(4):418–433. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2012.733704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manjaly Z-M, Harrison NA, Critchley HD, Do CT, Stefanics G, Wenderoth N, Lutterotti A, Müller A, Stephan KE. Pathophysiological and cognitive mechanisms of fatigue in multiple sclerosis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 2019;90(6):642–651. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2018-320050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsumoto Y, Mishima K, Satoh K, Shimizu T, Hishikawa Y. Physical activity increases the dissociation between subjective sleepiness and objective performance levels during extended wakefulness in human. Neuroscience Letters. 2002;326(2):133–136. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00335-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McAuley E, Courneya KS. Self-efficacy relationships with affective and exertion responses to exercise. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1992;22(4):312–326. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb01542.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McAuley E, Tablot H-M, Martinez S. Manipulating self-efficacy in the exercise environment in women: Influences on affective responses. Health Psychology. 1999;18(3):288–294. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.18.3.288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McMorris T, Barwood MJ, Corbett J. Central fatigue theory and endurance exercise: Toward an interoceptive model. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2018;93:93–107. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.03.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Metzinger, T. (2017). The problem of mental action - Predictive coding without sensory sheets. In: T. Metzinger & W. Wiese (Eds.), Philosophy and Predictive Processing (p. 19). MIND Group. 10.15502/9783958573208
- Micklewright D, St Clair Gibson A, Gladwell V, Al Salman A. Development and validity of the Rating-of-Fatigue scale. Sports Medicine. 2017;47(11):2375–2393. doi: 10.1007/s40279-017-0711-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller T, Apps M. Motivational fatigue: A neurocognitive framework for the impact of effortful exertion on subsequent motivation. Neuropsychologia. 2019;123:141–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nijs J, Van Oosterwijck J, Meeus M, Lambrecht L, Metzger K, Frémont M, Paul L. Unravelling the nature of postexertional malaise in myalgic encephalomyelitis/chronic fatigue syndrome: The role of elastase, complement C4a and interleukin-1β. Journal of Internal Medicine. 2010;267(4):418–435. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2009.02178.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ondobaka, S., Ward, N. S., & Kuppuswamy, A. (2019). Inter-hemispheric inhibition in stroke survivors is related to fatigue and cortical excitability. Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory. 10.1101/831511
- Palmer MA, Stefanidis K, Turner A, Tranent PJ, Breen R, Kucina T, Brumby L, Holt GA, Fell JW, Sauer JD. Acute physical exercise can influence the accuracy of metacognitive judgments. Scientific Reports. 2019;9(1):12412. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-48861-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pattyn N, Neyt X, Henderickx D, Soetens E. Psychophysiological investigation of vigilance decrement: Boredom or cognitive fatigue? Physiology and Behaviour. 2008;93(1–2):369–378. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pattyn N, Van Cutsem J, Dessy E, Mairesse O. Bridging exercise science, cognitive psychology, and medical practice: Is “cognitive fatigue” a remake of “the emperor’s new clothes”? Frontiers in Psychology. 2018;9:1246. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Penner IK, Paul F. Fatigue as a symptom or comorbidity of neurological diseases. Nature Reviews. Neurology. 2017;13(11):662–675. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2017.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petzschner FH, Weber LAE, Gard T, Stephan KE. Computational psychosomatics and computational psychiatry: Toward a joint Ffamework for differential diagnosis. Biological Psychiatry. 2017;82(6):421–430. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.05.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pezzulo G, Rigoli F, Friston K. Active Inference, homeostatic regulation and adaptive behavioural control. Progress in Neurobiology. 2015;134:17–35. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pezzulo G, Rigoli F, Friston KJ. Hierarchical active inference: A theory of motivated control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2018;22(4):294–306. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.01.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pigeon WR, Sateia MJ, Ferguson RJ. Distinguishing between excessive daytime sleepiness and fatigue: Toward improved detection and treatment. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2003;54(1):61–69. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00542-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rao RP, Ballard DH. Predicitive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nature - Neuroscience. 1999;2:79–87. doi: 10.1038/4580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Repping-Wuts H, Uitterhoeve R, van Riel P, van Achterberg T. Fatigue as experienced by patients with rheumatoid arthritis (RA): A qualitative study. International Journal of Nursing Studies. 2008;45(7):995–1002. doi: 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2007.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robertson CV, Marino FE. Viewpoint: A role for the prefrontal cortex in exercise tolerance and termination. Journal of Applied Physiology. 2016;120:464–466. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00363.2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartenbeck P, FitzGerald T, Dolan RJ, Friston K. Exploration, novelty, surprise, and free energy minimization. Frontiers in Psychology. 2013;4:710. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00710. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott JA, Lasch KE, Barsevick AM, Piault-Louis E. Patients’ experiences with cancer-related fatigue: A review and synthesis of qualitative research. Oncology Nusing Forum. 2011;38(3):E191–E203. doi: 10.1188/11.onf.e191-e203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seth, A. K., & Critchley, H. D. (2013). Extending predictive processing to the body: Emotion as interoceptive inference. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(227–228). [DOI] [PubMed]
- Seth AK, Friston KJ. Active interoceptive inference and the emotional brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2016;371:20160007. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seth AK, Suzuki K, Critchley HD. An interoceptive predictive coding model of conscious presence. Frontiers in Psychology. 2012;3:1–16. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shen J, Barbera J, Shapiro CM. Distinguishing sleepiness and fatigue: Focus on definition and measurement. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2006;10(1):63–76. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2005.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- St Clair Gibson A, Noakes TD. Evidence for complex system integration and dynamic neural regulation of skeletal muscle recruitment during exercise in humans. British Journal of Sports Medicine. 2004;38(6):797–806. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2003.009852. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Staiano W, Bosio AB, de Morree HM, Rampinini E, Marcora SM. The cardinal exercise stopper: Muscle fatigue, muscle pain or perception of effort? Progress in Brain Research. 2018;240:175–200. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stephan KE, Manjaly ZM, Mathys CD, Weber LAE, Paliwal S, Gard T, Tittgemeyer M, Fleming SM, Haker H, Seth AK, Petzschner FH. Allostatic self-efficacy: A metacognitive theory of dyshomeostasis-induced fatigue and depression. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2016;10:550. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sterling P. Allostasis: A model of predictive regulation. Physiology and Behavior. 2012;106(1):5–15. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevenson, A. (Ed.). (2010). Oxford Dictionary of English (3rd ed.). Oxford University Press.
- Tseng BY, Billinger SA, Gajewski BJ, Kluding PM. Exertion fatigue and chronic fatigue are two distinct constructs in people post-stroke. Stroke. 2010;41(12):2908–2912. doi: 10.1161/strokeaha.110.596064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Schaaf ME, Roelofs K, de Lange FP, Geurts DEM, van der Meer JWM, Knoop H, Toni I. Fatigue is associated with altered monitoring and preparation of physical effort in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. 2018;3(4):392–404. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.01.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinberg RS, Gould D, Yukelson D, Jackson A. The effect of preexisting and manipulated self-efficacy on a competitive muscular endurance task. Journal of Sport Psychology. 1981;3:345–354. doi: 10.1123/jsp.3.4.345. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Welch AS, Hulley A, Beauchamp M. Affect and self-efficacy responses during moderate-intensity exercise among low-active women: The effect of cognitive appraisal. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology. 2010;32:154–175. doi: 10.1123/jsep.32.2.154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williamson JW, McColl R, Mathews D, Ginsburg M, Mitchell JH. Activation of the insular cortex is affected by the intensity of exercise. Journal of Applied Physiology. 1999;87(3):1213–1219. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1999.87.3.1213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolpe N, Wolpert DM, Rowe JB. Seeing what you want to see: Priors for one’s own actionsrepresent exaggerated expectations of success. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014;8:232. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wrightson JG, Zewdie E, Kuo HC, Millet GY, Kirton A. Fatigue in children with perinatal stroke: Clinical and neurophysiological associations. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2019;62(2):234–240. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.14273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wylie GR, Yao B, Genova HM, Chen MH, DeLuca J. Using functional connectivity changes associated with cognitive fatigue to delineate a fatigue network. Scientific Reports. 2020;10:21927. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-78768-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zénon A, Solopchuk O, Pezzulo G. An information-theoretic perspective on the costs of cognition. Neuropsychologia. 2019;123:5–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]