Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2022 Apr 6;12:5743. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-09834-1

Resident birds are more behaviourally plastic than migrants

Federico Morelli 1,, Yanina Benedetti 1, Daniel T Blumstein 2
PMCID: PMC8986783  PMID: 35388121

Abstract

Species subjected to more variable environments should have greater phenotypic plasticity than those that are more restricted to specific habitat types leading to the expectation that migratory birds should be relatively more plastic than resident birds. We tested this comparatively by studying variation in flight initiation distance (FID), a well-studied antipredator behaviour. We predicted that variation in FID would be greater for migratory species because they encountered a variety of locations during their lives and therefore had less predictable assessments of risk compared to more sedentary species. Contrary to our prediction, we found that non-migratory species (sedentary) had greater variation in FID than migratory ones. Migratory and partially migratory birds had greater average FIDs than sedentary birds, suggesting that they were generally more wary. These results suggest that the predictability associated with not migrating permits more nuanced risk assessment which was seen in the greater variation in FID of sedentary bird species.

Subject terms: Ecology, Zoology, Ecology, Environmental sciences

Introduction

Flight initiation distance (FID), a component of escape behaviour, is commonly used as a proxy of fear in animals and has provided fundamental insights into predator–prey interactions15. Fear is defined by ethologists as "a motivational state aroused by specific stimuli that give rise to defensive behaviour or escape"6. FID is considered a reliable metric quantifying the level of risk-taking in animals because it reflects the trade-off between costs of premature escape and benefits from staying7,8. Thus, FID indicates if individuals take more risk (i.e. delay their escape) or take less risk (i.e. escape earlier) when facing a potential threat9. Escape behaviour and longer times to take-off could be associated with increasing bird's vulnerability10.

FID is particularly well-studied in birds and many factors that influence escape have been identified (summarised in11). For instance, it is well known that the FID is strongly positively associated with a species’ body size since larger species need more time to get airborne and escape1215. Additionally, changes in FID are associated with a variety of different factors (e.g. predator starting distance16,17, the density of potential predators18, the population density of the species19, flock size3, where individuals are along a rural–urban gradient4, season20,21 and other life-history traits as the type of diet5.

Most previous studies have focused on the average FID, but variation in FID could also be considered a measure of plasticity at the individual and population level2224. These studies have shown that intraspecific variation in FID is associated with a greater variety of habitat use and is associated with a variety of life history traits suggesting that it is part of an antipredator/life history syndrome. At the intraspecific level, diel variation in FID may be associated with foraging success. We extend these previous insights by focusing on the coefficient of variation in FID as a measure of behavioural plasticity for avian species. We then asked how this measure of variation was associated with whether or not a species was migratory or not. The migratory behavior of birds is a regular behaviour involving billions of individuals, which travel across diverse regions of the planet25,26. Migratory birds are quantitatively different than non-migratory species in several ways. For instance, migratory birds face a thermal niche change between breeding and non-breeding seasons26, and they tend to have smaller brains than sedentary species27. We expected that migratory species would be significantly more plastic in their FID responses. This was based on the assumption that they must experience more habitat types and less predictable risks during their lives compared with more sendintary species. To isolate this effect, we controlled for other variables that are known to explain variation in FID. Because previous studies have identified significant phylogenetic signals in life history and escape behaviour, we compared, using AIC (Akaike's information criterion), models that did or did not incorporate phylogenetic information.

Results

A total of 1240 FID observations were collected in USA, while 2359 observations were collected in Australia from species with complete information on the set of our independent variables, with 115 observations recorded for two bird species present in both countries (Table S1). Overall, 539 observations were collected for migratory birds, 528 for partially migratory birds, while 2647 observations were collected for sedentary birds (Table S1). A total of 15 different migratory species, 9 partially migratory and 9 sedentary species were studied in the USA, while 2 migratory, 9 partially migratory and 49 sedentary species were studied in Australia (Tables S1 and S2).

The mean values of FID were higher for migratory and partially migratory birds than for sedentary species in USA, while in Australia partially migratory birds fled at the greatest distance (Table S1, Fig. 1). Plasticity was unrelated to the number of observations (Fig. S1), and was relatively higher in sedentary and partially migratory birds than in migratory species in USA and Australia (Table S1, Fig. 2). Of the top ten most plastic birds (Table S1, Fig. S2), 60% were classified as sedentary species, with 2 partially migratory species for USA (Corvus corax and Molothrus ater) and 2 for Australia (Falco cenchroides and Elanus axillaris)(Table S2). No migratory species were selected in the top ten most behaviorally plastic birds (Table S2).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Difference in flight initiation distance (FID in metres) among migratory, partially migratory and sedentary birds, split by country where data were collected. The box plots show medians (horizontal bar), quartiles, 5- and 95-percentiles, jittered points (small-grey dots) and extreme values (small-coloured dots). Mean values are indicated with black rhombus.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Difference in plasticity of escape behaviour (coefficient of variation of FID) migratory, partially migratory and sedentary birds, split by country where data were collected. The box plots show medians (horizontal bar), quartiles, 5- and 95-percentiles, jittered points (small-grey dots) and extreme values (small-coloured dots). Mean values are indicated with black rhombus.

We found no significant phylogenetic signal in behavioural plasticity using any of the phylogenetic signal statistics, while the phylogenetic signal was significant for the other life-history traits analysed (Table 1). Any way analysed, we found a consistent result: there was a significantly positive association between plasticity and being sedentary, and partially migratory when compared with migratory birds (Table 2). The non-phylogenetic model had a substantially lower AIC (delta AIC = 50.5). Interestingly, the phylogenetic model also suggested that longer-lived bird species have greater behavioural plasticity (Table 2) while the other explored variables were not statistically significant (Table 2, Figs. S3 and S4).

Table 1.

Results of phylogenetic signal of escape behavior plasticity (coefficient of variation of FID) and other life history traits of the 95 bird species included in this study.

Variables Abouheif’s I K K * Lambda
Stat p Stat p Stat p Stat p Stat p
Relative variance of FID 0.037 0.216  − 0.0005 0.129 0.288 0.223 0.336 0.279 0.174 0.589
Habitat breadth 0.244 0.002 0.019 0.006 0.441 0.011 0.533 0.003 0.821 0.001
Diet breadth 0.333 0.001 0.031 0.002 0.393 0.002 0.440 0.006 0.699 0.001
Body mass 0.288 0.010 0.0260 0.009 0.942 0.002 0.727 0.008 0.857 0.001
Life span max 0.399 0.001 0.0369 0.002 0.465 0.022 0.565 0.012 0.791 0.001

The table shows Abouheif’s Cmean, Moran’s I, Blomberg’s K and K*, and Pagel’s Lambda statistics (Stat) and associated p values for each value. Significant variables are highlighted in bold.

Table 2.

Results of models explaining plasticity in escape behaviour in birds as a function of starting distance, the number of observations (FID count), habitat and diet breadth, migratory behaviour (migratory, partially migratory and sedentary birds), body mass, life span and diet type of species, considering also the country where data were collected.

Variable/Model Estimate 2.50% 97.50% SE t value p
Model 1 (GLM)
Intercept 59.237 35.910 82.564 11.902 4.977  < 0.001
Starting distance (mean)  − 0.208  − 0.545 0.130 0.172  − 1.206 0.231
FID (count)  − 0.032  − 0.161 0.098 0.066  − 0.479 0.633
Habitat breadth 0.054  − 3.146 3.254 1.633 0.033 0.974
Diet breadth  − 49.251  − 148.363 49.862 50.569  − 0.974 0.333
Migration: partially migratory 17.323 0.366 34.280 8.652 2.002 0.048
Migration: sedentary 21.238 4.858 37.618 8.357 2.541 0.013
Body mass  − 0.001  − 0.008 0.006 0.004  − 0.218 0.828
Life span (max) 0.225  − 0.113 0.563 0.172 1.306 0.196
Diet: Omnivore  − 1.280  − 15.719 13.160 7.367  − 0.174 0.863
Diet: Plant/seed/nectar 7.277  − 6.387 20.942 6.972 1.044 0.300
Diet: Vertebrates 13.766  − 6.926 34.458 10.557 1.304 0.196
Country: Australia/USA 0.500  − 35.091 36.091 18.159 0.028 0.978
Country: USA 4.084  − 9.037 17.205 6.695 0.610 0.544
Model 2 (PGLS)
Intercept 51.702 1.917 101.486 25.401 2.035 0.045
Starting distance (mean) 0.120 0.014 0.227 0.054 2.216 0.030
FID (count)  − 0.195  − 0.692 0.302 0.254  − 0.768 0.445
Habitat breadth 0.681  − 3.127 4.488 1.943 0.350 0.727
Diet breadth  − 27.274  − 134.606 80.058 54.762  − 0.498 0.620
Migration: partially migratory 21.136 6.421 35.851 7.508 2.815 0.006
Migration: sedentary 19.233 2.784 35.681 8.392 2.292 0.025
Body mass  − 0.005  − 0.015 0.004 0.005  − 1.083 0.282
Life span (max) 0.569 0.106 1.032 0.236 2.408 0.018
Diet: Omnivore  − 4.052  − 20.566 12.462 8.426  − 0.481 0.632
Diet: Plant/seed/nectar 12.432  − 3.351 28.215 8.053 1.544 0.127
Diet: Vertebrates 12.796  − 13.941 39.532 13.641 0.938 0.351
Country: Australia/USA  − 20.829  − 54.873 13.216 17.370  − 1.199 0.234
Country: USA  − 5.248  − 21.005 10.509 8.040  − 0.653 0.516

The model 1 is a generalized linear model while the model 2 is a phylogenetic generalized least squares model (PGLS) incorporating a phylogenetic correlation term among bird species. Models are based on 3714 observations of FID collected in USA (1240 records), Australia (2359 records) and both (115 records) for 95 bird species. The table shows the values of estimates, the lower (2.5%) and upper (97.5%) limits of confidence intervals, standard error (SE), t and p values. Significant variables are highlighted in bold. Model 1 AIC: 863.93. Model 2 AIC: 914.40.

Discussion

Using the coefficient of variation of FID, a potentially species-specific value that is independent of the FID range and/or a species’ body size, we found that sedentary and partially migratory birds were comparatively more plastic in their escape behaviour than migratory species. This was contrary to our expectation, but we speculate that sedentary and partially migratory birds, that spend their entire life cycle in the same habitat or in a relatively narrow distribution range than migratory species, benefit from more variable and perhaps nuanced responses to threats. Interestingly, migratory and partially migratory birds had relatively longer escape responses relatively than sedentary species in USA, while such differences were less clear in Australia, where partially migratory species had longer average FID’s. Greater wariness could be associated with the relatively smaller brain that characterised the migratory birds if compared to the sedentary species27. A smaller brain in migratory birds could be an energy saving strategy for species’ that have relatively high energetic needs associated with a highly mobile life cycle27. If generally true in other species, this suggests that migratory birds may be particularly vulnerable to human disturbance.

While there are significant effects of a species’ evolutionary history on specific traits, our non-phylogenetic model was a more efficient model than the phylogenetic one suggesting that the statistical relationship between migration and plasticity is not influenced by evolutionary relationships between species. Interestingly, in the phylogenetically-informed analysis, we also found a positive and significant association between behavioural plasticity and species' life span (Fig. S5) that was independent of migratory status: longer-lived species were more plastic in their escape responses. While this makes sense, we are unable to explain why the result did not emerge from the better-supported non-phylogenetic analysis.

A shortcoming of our study is that we assigned migratory status at the species level, but there is intraspecific variation in migratory behavior in some species28. If anything, this would make it more difficult to detect the effect of migration on variable escape responses. Any classification of birds as “migratory”, “partially migratory” and “non-migratory” species could be considered too reductionist because there are sedentary species that live within the same home ranges throughout the calendar year, while other birds shift subtly their environments among seasons within the same geographic area. Furthermore, there are short-distance migrants that may experience relatively similar threat environments because the assemblage of predators may not differ substantially from summer to winter, and finally long-distance migrants that potentially could experience very different threat among seasons. However, the notion that migrants need to be adapted to a wider range of dangers across their annual cycle remains to be demonstrated. In fact, some studies indicate that long-distance migratory species tend to select mostly similar environments throughout their annual cycle29, which may reduce the variation in predation risk between sedentary and migratory birds.

Finally, our findings suggest that migratory species may be more vulnerable to human disturbance than non-migratory species because of their increased wariness and less flexible response to threats. This suggests that future work studying variation in tolerance to disturbance in migratory and non-migratory species would be valuable30. If migratory behaviour were also associated with reduced tolerance to humans, this would further strengthen the need to protect migratory species from human disturbance.

Methods

Escape behaviour data collection

Flight initiation distance (FID) is defined as the distance at which animals take flight from approaching threats31,32. Following standardised methods5, trained observers identified a non-alarmed bird (i.e. resting or foraging) and directly approached at a measured pace of 0.5 m/s. The observer dropped a flag where they started the approach, a second flag where the bird moved its head to look in response to the approach, and a third flag where the bird began to escape. Escapes included both walking or running away and flying away. A final flag was dropped at the initial position of the bird when the experimental approach began. From these, the observer measured (with a meter tape, rangefinder, or a well-calibrated pace) the starting distance (first flag to bird’s initial location), alert distance (second flag to bird’s initial location), flight-initiation distance (third flag to bird’s initial location). The data were collected in a variety of habitats along the East coast of Australia as well as some other locations in Southern and Western Australia, central Colorado near Crested Butte, Colorado, and in Los Angeles, and Orange Counties in Southern California. Most data were collected in protected areas and reserves where the species were not harassed by humans, during the period 2000–2003.

Bird life-history traits and measuring a proxy of behavioural plasticity

For each bird species, we collected the following life history data. Species body mass and habitat breadth were extracted from33 while diet breadth, life span (max) were extracted from34 and migratory status (resident or sedentary, partially migratory or migratory) was extracted from35. The main type of diet for each species was extracted from36.

We calculated the mean, maximum, minimum and standard devition of flight initiation distance for each bird species with > 5 observations, to avoid potential overinflated standard deviation estimates from species with only a few observations9. Then, for each species, we calculated our metric of plasticity based on the coefficient of variation (CV) of FID,

CVFID=FIDSD100FID.

Because this is a ratio of the variance to the mean, plasticity is independent of body size.

Statistical analyses

Life-history traits, including FID, often have a strong phylogenetic signal37,38. To create a phylogeny of the 95 species we had data on, we downloaded 1000 phylogenetic trees with the avian phylogenetic relationships from ‘www.birdtree.org’, selecting the backbone tree based on Ericson et al.39. The consensus tree was obtained applying the 50% majority rule (i.e. the proportion of a split to be present in all trees). We used the following R packages: ‘ape’40, ‘phangorn’41 and ‘Rphylip’42 to manipulate trees. With these trees, we tested the strength and significance of the phylogenetic signal43 for our metric of plasticity using all available indices in the package ‘phylosignal’ for R44: Abouheif’s Cmean45,46, Moran’s I46, Blomberg’s K statistic and statistic K* 47, and Pagel’s Lambda48.

To formally test the relationship between migratory behaviour and plasticity, we compared models with and without phylogenetic information and used AIC to identify the more efficient models to interpret49. First, we fitted a Generalized Linear Model50. Plasticity (CVFID) was modelled as a function of the following fixed effects: mean starting distance (included because it often explains substantial variation in FID16), the number of FID observations (to account for variation in our certainty of estimates), habitat breadth, diet breadth, whether a species are mainly sedentary, partially migratory or migratory, body mass, and maximum longevity. We included also country (USA or Australia) and diet type (classified as plant/seed/nectar, invertebrates, vertebrates or omnivore) as fixed effects to account for potential effects on variation in FID related to the region where data were collected and to different strategies of foraging. Then, we fitted a Phylogenetic Generalized Least Squares (PGLS) model with the same list of fixed effects but including also the phylogeny, and assumed a Brownian motion model of evolution (i.e. K < 1)47. The models were fitted by maximum likelihood, using the package “nlme” for R51. A test to detect potential multicollinearity issues among predictor variables was performed using the test of variance inflation factors (VIF) on the full generalized linear model, using the package ‘fmsb’ for R52. All variables had VIF < 3 and then were modelled together. The overall value of VIF for the full model was 1.21, suggesting no strong multicollinearity.

All statistical tests were performed using the R software53. The confidence interval for each variable was estimated by the Wald method from the package ‘MASS’54.

Ethics

By design this work was minimally invasive. Research protocols were approved by the Macquarie University Animal Research Committee (99,021), the Rocky Mountain Biological Laboratory, and the UCLA Animal Research Committee (2000–147-01). Permission to work on land was acquired when required.

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information. (795.1KB, docx)

Author contributions

F.M. conceived the study. D.T.B. collected the data which was analysed by F.M. and Y.B. F.M. and D.T.B. interpreted the results and wrote the manuscript. All authors gave final approval for publication and agree to be held accountable for the work performed therein.

Funding

D.T.B. thanks the Australian Research Council, Macquarie University, and UCLA for support of field data collection.

Data availability

The dataset used in this study is provided in the Electronic Supplementary Material, Table S2 and also published in Dryad Digital Repository. Dryad https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.ns1rn8pv3. Additional information regarding the raw data can be provided directly by the authors, under reasonable request.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-022-09834-1.

References

  • 1.Hall MJ, Burns AL, Martin JM, Hochuli DF. Flight initiation distance changes across landscapes and habitats in a successful urban coloniser. Urban Ecosyst. 2020 doi: 10.1007/s11252-020-00969-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Møller AP, Samia DSM, Weston MA, Guay PJ, Blumstein DT. Flight initiation distances in relation to sexual dichromatism and body size in birds from three continents. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2016;117:823–831. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Morelli F, et al. Contagious fear: Escape behavior increases with flock size in European gregarious birds. Ecol. Evol. 2019;9:6096–6104. doi: 10.1002/ece3.5193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Samia DSM, et al. Rural-urban differences in escape behavior of European birds across a latitudinal gradient. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2017;5:66. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Blumstein DT. Developing an evolutionary ecology of fear: How life history and natural history traits affect disturbance tolerance in birds. Anim. Behav. 2006;71:389–399. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.McFarland, D. Oxford companion to animal behavior. (Oxford University Press, 1987).
  • 7.Stankowich T, Blumstein DT. Fear in animals: A meta-analysis and review of risk assessment. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2005;272:2627–2634. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Lima SL. Maximizing feeding efficiency and minimizing time exposed to predators: a trade-off in the black-capped chickadee. Oecologia. 1985;66:60–67. doi: 10.1007/BF00378552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Sol D, et al. Risk-taking behavior, urbanization and the pace of life in birds. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2018;72:59. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Lockwood R, Swaddle JP, Rayner JMV. Avian Wingtip Shape Reconsidered: Wingtip Shape Indices and Morphological Adaptations to Migration. J. Avian Biol. 1998;29:273–292. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Møller, A. P. Birds. in Escaping from predators: An integrative view of escape decisions and refuge use (eds. Cooper, W. E. J. & Blumstein, D. T.) 88–112 (Cambridge University Press, 2015).
  • 12.Møller AP. Flight distance of urban birds, predation and selection for urban life. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2008;63:63–75. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Fernández-Juricic E, et al. Relationships of anti-predator escape and post-escape responses with body mass and morphology: a comparative avian study. Evol. Ecol. Res. 2006;8:731–752. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Weston MA, Mcleod EM, Blumstein DT, Guay PJ. A review of flight-initiation distances and their application to managing disturbance to Australian birds. Emu. 2012;112:269–286. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Hemmingsen A. The relation of shyness (flushing distance) to body size. Spolia Zool Musei Hauniensis. 1951;11:74–76. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Blumstein DT. Flight-initiation distance in birds is dependent on intruder starting distance. J. Wildl. Manage. 2013;67:852–857. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Glover HK, Weston MA, Maguire GS, Miller KK, Christie BA. Towards ecologically meaningful and socially acceptable buffers: Response distances of shorebirds in Victoria, Australia, to human disturbance. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2011;103:326–334. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Geist C, Liao J, Libby S, Blumstein DT. Does intruder group size and orientation affect flight initiation distance in birds? Anim. Biodivers. Conserv. 2001;28:69–73. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Mikula P. Pedestrian density influences flight distances of urban birds. Ardea. 2014;102:53–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Piratelli, A. J., Favoretto, G. R. & de Almeida Maximiano, M. F. Factors affecting escape distance in birds. Zoologia32, 438–444 (2015).
  • 21.Burger J, Gochfeld M. Human activity influence and diurnal and nocturnal foraging of Sanderlings (Calidris alba) Condor. 1991;93:259–265. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Møller AP, Garamszegi LZ. Between individual variation in risk-taking behavior and its life history consequences. Behav. Ecol. 2012;23:843–853. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Ferguson SM, Gilson LN, Bateman PW. Look at the time: diel variation in the flight initiation distance of a nectarivorous bird. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2019;73:147. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Garamszegi LZ, Møller AP. Partitioning within-species variance in behaviour to within- and between-population components for understanding evolution. Ecol. Lett. 2017;20:599–608. doi: 10.1111/ele.12758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bauer S, Hoye BJ. Migratory animals couple biodiversity and ecosystem functioning worldwide. Science. 2014;344:1242552. doi: 10.1126/science.1242552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Dufour P, et al. Reconstructing the geographic and climatic origins of long-distance bird migrations. J. Biogeogr. 2020;47:155–166. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Sol D, et al. Evolutionary divergence in brain size between migratory and resident birds. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e9617. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Bonnet-Lebrun AS, Somveille M, Rodrigues ASL, Manica A. Exploring intraspecific variation in migratory destinations to investigate the drivers of migration. Oikos. 2021;130:187–196. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Zurell D, Gallien L, Graham CH, Zimmermann NE. Do long-distance migratory birds track their niche through seasons? J. Biogeogr. 2018;45:1459–1468. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Samia DSM, Nakagawa S, Nomura F, Rangel TF, Blumstein DT. Increased tolerance to humans among disturbed wildlife. Nat. Commun. 2015;6:8877. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Ydenberg RC, Dill LM. The economics of fleeing from predators. Adv. Study Behav. 1986;16:229–249. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Cooper, W. E. J. & Blumstein, D. T. Escape behavior: importance, scope, and variables. in Escaping from predators: An integrative view of escape decisions (eds. Cooper, W. E. J. & Blumstein, D. T.) 3–14 (Cambridge University Press, 2015). 10.1017/CBO9781107447189.002.
  • 33.Sayol F, Sol D, Pigot AL. Brain size and life history interact to predict urban tolerance in birds. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2020;8:58. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Sayol F, Downing PA, Iwaniuk AN, Maspons J, Sol D. Predictable evolution towards larger brains in birds colonizing oceanic islands. Nat. Commun. 2018;9:2820. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-05280-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Tobias JA, Pigot AL. Integrating behaviour and ecology into global biodiversity conservation strategies. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2019;374:20190012. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Wilman H, et al. EltonTraits 1.0: Species-level foraging attributes of the world’s birds and mammals. Ecology. 2014;95:2027. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Kamilar JM, Cooper N. Phylogenetic signal in primate behaviour, ecology and life history. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2013;368:20120341–22012034. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Machado JP, Antunes A, Borges R, Gomes C, Rocha AP. Measuring phylogenetic signal between categorical traits and phylogenies. Bioinformatics. 2018 doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/bty800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ericson PGP, et al. Diversification of Neoaves: integration of molecular sequence data and fossils. Biol. Lett. 2006;2:543–547. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2006.0523. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Paradis E, Claude J, Strimmer K. APE: analyses of phylogenetics and evolution in R language. Bioinformatics. 2004;20:289–290. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btg412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Schliep KP. phangorn: phylogenetic analysis in R. Bioinformatics. 2011;27:592–593. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btq706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Revell, L. J. & Chamberlain, S. A. Rphylip: An R interface for PHYLIP R package. (2014).
  • 43.Blomberg SP, Garland T. Tempo and mode in evolution: phylogenetic inertia, adaptation and comparative methods. J. Evol. Biol. 2003;15:899–910. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Keck F, Rimet F, Bouchez A, Franc A. Phylosignal: An R package to measure, test, and explore the phylogenetic signal. Ecol. Evol. 2016;6:2774–2780. doi: 10.1002/ece3.2051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Münkemüller T, et al. How to measure and test phylogenetic signal. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2012;3:743–756. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Kot M. Adaptation: Statistics and a null model for estimating phylogenetic effects. Syst. Zool. 1990;39:227–241. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Blomberg SP, Garland TJ, Ives AR. Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data: behavioral traits are more labile. Evolution (N. Y.) 2003;57:717–745. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2003.tb00285.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Pagel M. Inferring the historical patterns of biological evolution. Nature. 1999;401:877–884. doi: 10.1038/44766. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach. Springer; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.McCullagh, P. & Nelder, J. A. Generalized Linear Models. (Chapman and Hall, 1989).
  • 51.Pinheiro, J. et al. nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 3.1-140. 1–117 (2019).
  • 52.Nakazawa, M. ‘fmsb’ Functions for Medical Statistics Book with some Demographic Data - R package version 0.6.1. (2017).
  • 53.R Development Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. (2021).
  • 54.Venables, W. N. & Ripley, B. D. Modern Applied Statistics with S. (Springer, 2002). 10.1007/978-0-387-21706-2.

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Information. (795.1KB, docx)

Data Availability Statement

The dataset used in this study is provided in the Electronic Supplementary Material, Table S2 and also published in Dryad Digital Repository. Dryad https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.ns1rn8pv3. Additional information regarding the raw data can be provided directly by the authors, under reasonable request.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES