Abstract
Objectives
Psychological well-being (PWB) may be a potential modifiable risk factor of age-related diseases. We aimed to determine associations of PWB with sensorineural and cognitive function and neuronal health in middle-aged adults.
Methods
This study included 2039 Beaver Dam Offspring Study participants. We assessed PWB, hearing, visual acuity, contrast sensitivity impairment, olfactory impairment, cognition and retinal (macular ganglion cell inner-plexiform layer,mGCIPL) thickness. Age-sex-education-adjusted multivariable linear, logistic regression and generalized estimating equation models were used and then further adjusted for health-related confounders.
Results
Individuals with higher PWB had better hearing functions, visual acuity and thicker mGCIPL and reduced odds for hearing, contrast sensitivity and olfactory impairment in age-sex-education-adjusted models. Effects on mGCIPL and visual and olfactory measures decreased with adjustment. Higher PWB was associated with better cognition, better combined sensorineural-cognitive function and decreased cognitive impairment.
Discussion
PWB was associated with sensorineural-cognitive health indicating a potential of PWB interventions for healthy aging.
Keywords: Purpose in Life, Positive Relations, Cognition, Senses, Retinal Thickness
Introduction
Age-related sensory losses and cognitive decline and dementia are common in older adults and public health challenges in aging populations. Sensory and cognitive declines might influence each other over the lifespan and are affected by neurodegeneration, sharing common risk factors (Albers et al., 2015; Merten, Fischer, et al., 2020; Pronk et al., 2019; Schubert et al., 2019). Recently, retinal nerve cells and, specifically, macular ganglion cell inner-plexiform layer thickness (mGCIPL) was found to be associated with sensorineural (hearing, vision and olfactory function) and cognitive function and has been discussed as a marker of neuronal health (Merten, Paulsen, et al., 2020; Ward et al., 2020). Neurodegenerative processes start in midlife, decades before clinical symptom onset, indicating a need for early intervention and prevention (Jack et al., 2013).
In the search for modifiable risk factors for age-related diseases, psychological well-being (PWB) has received growing interest. PWB is an overarching term for a multifaceted concept including eudaimonic (e.g., purpose, fulfillment) and hedonic (e.g., feeling good) components. Having a purpose in life and positive relations with others are two PWB key aspects that have been associated with mental and physical health (Ryff, 2014; Windsor et al., 2015). Purpose in life represents a self-organizing life aim that provides a sense of meaning, stimulates goals and manages behaviors (McKnight & Kashdan, 2009). Positive relations and social support through deep, trusting interpersonal relations are important for PWB (Ryff, 2014).
While in previous research reduced PWB has often been considered a result of reduced physical health, growing research operationalizes PWB as a determinant of various health outcomes in order to investigate its protective utility for human health (Ryff, 2014). PWB has been associated with reduced mortality (Cohen et al., 2016; Diener & Chan, 2011; Laugesen et al., 2018; Pantell et al., 2013) and better physical health (Ryff, 2014; Windsor et al., 2015; Yoo & Ryff, 2019), including vascular, metabolic and inflammation factors (Boylan & Ryff, 2015; Brooks et al., 2014; Cohen et al., 2016; Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2010; Kim et al., 2013; Radler et al., 2018; Smith & Ruiz, 2002; Yang et al., 2016; Yu et al., 2015; Zilioli et al., 2015). Multiple pathways that may explain how PWB might positively affect human health have been suggested (Ryff & Kim, 2020). One pathway might be through better lifestyle and health behaviors. Higher purpose in life has been associated with better physical function (Kim et al., 2017) and more health-protective behaviors, i.e. physical activity and vegetable intake (Hill et al., 2018; Hooker & Masters, 2016), preventive health care, medical check-ups and cancer screenings (Chen et al., 2019; Kim et al., 2014). Another mechanism might be through the contribution of enriched and stimulating environments, i.e., social activities (Lu et al., 2003) that could improve brain reserve (Stern, 2009). Finally, psychological and social resources could be protective against negative emotional effects and stress; and effects of PWB on physiological processes of immune function and stress hormones are plausible (Berkman et al., 2013). Vascular, metabolic and inflammation factors have been shown to be important risk factors for sensory and cognitive decline (Schubert et al., 2019; Whitson et al., 2018) and system-wide neurodegenerative processes may play a role in declining sensory and cognitive function (Albers et al., 2015). Therefore, potential better lifestyles and more brain reserve in individuals with higher PWB could also contribute to better sensory and cognitive function. However, few cohort studies on sensory function and PWB exist (Boesveldt et al., 2017; Kramer et al., 2002; Mick et al., 2018; Mick & Pichora-Fuller, 2016; Strawbridge et al., 2000). Limitations of such previous studies are self-reported sensory assessments and/or a focus on social functions, while other aspects of PWB, such as purpose in life, and more complex constructs remain unstudied. Some epidemiological studies have been investigating PWB and cognition and found that PWB was associated with better cognitive function, less decline (Barnes et al., 2004; James et al., 2011; Kim et al., 2019; Lewis et al., 2017; Seeman et al., 2001; Windsor et al., 2015), and reduced risk for mild cognitive impairment and dementia in older adults (Boyle et al., 2010; Kuiper et al., 2015; Scarmeas et al., 2001; Xu et al., 2019). However, previous work often relied on verbal-only cognitive assessments, which can be confounded e.g. by hearing loss, and few studies in midlife exist (Kats et al., 2016; Lewis et al., 2017; Nakanishi et al., 2019). Furthermore, existing research on PWB and sensory and cognitive functions largely featured self-reported measures of confounding risk factors for sensory and cognitive decline. Studies with a thorough characterization of participants’ health are lacking. To our knowledge, studies on the association between PWB and multiple measures of sensorineural functioning and measures of neuronal health, such as mGCIPL, have not been conducted.
Importantly, PWB can be modified by group interventions and environmental changes (Chan et al., 2017; Friedman et al., 2019; Ryff, 2014). Purpose in life can be renewed through career counseling, higher education or volunteer activities. Regular group exercises can improve social support satisfaction (Chan et al., 2017). PWB interventions might, therefore, promote healthy brain aging. With neuropathologic changes occurring decades prior to clinically manifested symptoms, interventions to prevent sensory and cognitive decline and neurodegenerative diseases may be most beneficial in midlife.
The aim of this study was to determine the association of PWB with sensorineural and cognitive function and mGCIPL thickness as a marker of neuronal health in midlife.
Materials and Methods
Study Population
This cross-sectional study is based on participants of the 10-year follow-up of the Beaver Dam Offspring Study (BOSS), a prospective cohort study of aging (Nash et al., 2011). The adult offspring of the population-based Epidemiology of Hearing Loss Study (EHLS) participants were eligible for the baseline BOSS examination (2005-2008) and have been followed every 5 years. PWB was assessed for the first time at the 10-year follow-up (2015-2017). We included all BOSS 10-year follow-up participants in this study who had a measure of PWB and at least one of the sensory or cognitive outcomes or mGCIPL. The study was approved by the University of Wisconsin Health Sciences Institutional Review Board (2014-1337, Beaver Dam Offspring Study) with written informed consent from all participants before each examination.
Measurements
With the exception of long-term inflammation, all measures included in this study were based on the assessment at the 10-year follow-up.
Psychological Well-being
Assessment of PWB was based on participants’ responses to the two subscales Positive Relations with Others and Purpose in Life of the Ryff Psychological Well-Being inventory (Ryff, 2014). Each subscale had 7 items. Scores could vary between 7-49 (from low to high). PWB was defined as the sum of both scales’ score (14-98 points) and z-standardized. Internal validity alphas for Purpose in Life and Positive Relations with Others and for the combined PWB scale were acceptable and good (0.73, 0.77 and 0.83, respectively).
Sensorineural Measures
Hearing Measures
Hearing thresholds for both ears were obtained at 0.5,1,2,3,4,6 and 8kHz (air-conduction) and 0.5 and 2kHz (bone-conduction) using clinical audiometers with TDH-50P earphones and ER-3A insert earphones (in cases of probable ear-canal collapse). Testing followed American National Standards Institute standards for equipment and American Speech-Language-Hearing Association guidelines (American National Standards Institute, 1999, 2010; American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA), 1978) and was conducted in a sound-treated booth. In the very few instances when testing was outside a booth, the insert earphones were used. When necessary, masking was done. The pure-tone average (PTA) of air-conduction thresholds at 0.5,1,2 and 4kHz in decibel hearing level (dB HL) for each ear was used to measure hearing sensitivity, with impairment defined as PTA>25dB HL (Merten, Paulsen, et al., 2020). Higher-order auditory function was assessed with two additional hearing tests. The dichotic digits test (DDT) was administered binaurally with 25 sets of triple-digit pairs. 3 digits were presented to each ear simultaneously at 70dB HL. The participant’s task was to repeat as many of the 6 digits as possible. The Word Recognition in Competing Message (WRCM) with the Northwestern University Auditory Test Number 6 was administered to the better ear. In the WRCM, 25 words were presented by a single female speaker to the better hearing ear at 36dB HL above the individual’s hearing threshold at 2kHz. If thresholds at 2kHz were equal, the right ear was tested. The competing message (single male speaker) was added at a level 8dB HL below the female speaker’s level in that same ear (Wiley et al., 1998; Wilson et al., 1990). The percentage of correctly repeated digits from both ears in the free recall condition was the DDT outcome and the percentage of correctly repeated words was the WRCM outcome (Merten, Paulsen, et al., 2020).
Vision Measures
Contrast sensitivity (CS) was measured in each eye separately using a Pelli-Robson letter chart (Pelli et al., 1988). Participants were instructed to read as many letters as possible until 2 letters in a triplet were missed. We used the last triplet in which a participant correctly identified at least 2 of the 3 letters to assign a log CS score. Impairment was defined as log CS score≤1.55 (Paulsen et al., 2018). We measured best-corrected monocular visual acuity (VA) in each eye with the Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study chart R modified for a 2-m distance (ETDRS Coordinating Center Department of Epidemiology and Preventive Medicine University of Maryland School of Medicine, 1980; Paulsen et al., 2018). We asked participants to read the letters from the 2-m chart until they could no longer correctly identify at least 3 of 5 letters on a line (scores 0 – 70). For those unable to read any letters at 2-m, a 1-m chart was used (scores − 30 – −1). People, who could not read any letter on the 1-m chart were assigned values of −40. The number of correctly identified letters was the VA outcome.
Olfaction Measure
To test olfaction, the San Diego Odor Identification Test (SDOIT) was used. Eight common odorants were randomly presented with a 45s interstimulus interval. A picture board including the 8 odorants and additional 12 distractor items was used to aid identification (verbal response or pointing on item on picture board). The number of correctly identified odors were summed and impairment defined as <6 out of 8 correct (Merten, Paulsen, et al., 2020).
Cognitive Measures
We conducted a neurocognitive test battery consisting of: Trail-Making Tests A and B (TMTA, TMTB), modified Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT), Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST), and Verbal Fluency Test (VFT) measuring the cognitive domains of attention, speed, executive function, memory and verbal fluency (Merten, Paulsen, et al., 2020). Participants were allowed to use assistive devices when performing cognitive tasks (their own glasses and/or hearing aids and we provided reading glasses and/or a Pocket Talker (Pocket Talker Pro™, Williams Sound, Eden Prairie, MN) if they needed them).
In TMTA, consecutive numbers were to be connected and in TMTB, consecutive numbers and letters were to be connected in an alternating fashion. Completion time in seconds was the outcome with longer durations indicating poorer performance. Inability to complete each subtest in 5min resulted in a score of 301s (Reitan, 1992). In DSST, participants convert numbers to symbols based on a key. The number of correctly converted numbers in 90s served as outcome score. For the AVLT, subjects were asked to recall as many words as they could from a list of 15 verbally presented words. Three trials with the same 15-word list were administered followed by a new 15 words distractor list. Immediately after recalling words from the distractor list, the participant was asked to recall as many words as they could from the first word list. The number of words correctly recalled from the first list in the final trial was our measure of memory function (González et al., 2015; Ivnik et al., 1992a; Zhong et al., 2014). In the VFT, the task is to produce as many words as possible starting with the letters F,A and S, within 60s for each letter. Total numbers of words provided for the three letters were summed as test outcome (Strauss et al., 2006).
The Mini-Mental-State Examination (MMSE) was completed by participants aged >50 years. Memory concerns were ascertained by two questions. Cognitive impairment was defined as memory concerns and impairment in at least one cognitive domain (executive function, memory, verbal function), self- or surrogate report of physician diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease or dementia, or a MMSE score<24 (Merten, Paulsen, et al., 2020). We used cut-offs of 1.5 standard deviations (SD) below the mean; or roughly equivalent, 3rd-5th percentile of published age-specific norm distributions of the tests to determine impairments on the cognitive tests (Ivnik et al., 1992b, 1992a, 1996; Tombaugh, 1999).
Measure of Neuronal Health
Optical coherence tomography (OCT) utilizing automated segmentation techniques was used to measure the thickness of the retinal nerve cell layers. We conducted a Macular Cube 512x128 scan on each eye, centered on the fovea, using the Cirrus 5000 HD-OCT (Carl Zeiss Meditec, Inc). The mGCIPL represents a combination of neuronal cell bodies and dendrites in the macula. Average mGCIPL thicknesses (in μm) from each eye were used in analyses (Merten, Paulsen, et al., 2020).
Other Variables
We assessed participants’ age, sex, education, smoking status, history of heavy alcohol consumption (ever been drinking more than four alcoholic beverages per day), diabetes (history of diabetes diagnosis and/or glycated hemoglobin≥6.5%), history of cardiovascular disease (stroke, myocardial infarction, angina, congestive heart failure, transient ischemic attack, peripheral vascular disease, thrombosis, angioplasty or a stent operation, coronary bypass, and/or carotid arteries surgery), waist circumference (in cm), regular exercise (at least once a week long enough to work up a sweat) and long-term inflammatory status. We measured high-sensitivity c-reactive protein, interleukin-6, vascular cell adhesion protein 1 and intercellular adhesion molecule 1 from stored blood samples and defined long-term elevated inflammation as being repeatedly in the highest group for at least one inflammation marker at baseline and 5-year follow-up (Nash et al., 2011; Paulsen et al., 2018). For high-sensitivity c-reactive protein, participants were divided into three risk groups (<1, 1–3, and >3mg/L) according to established cut-points and we compared the highest risk group (>3mg/L ) to the rest (Pearson et al., 2003). For the other three markers, we compared the highest tertile against the rest.
Statistical Analyses
Statistical analyses were conducted using SAS software v.9.4 (SAS Institute, Inc, Cary, NC). The composite cognitive function score (in SD) was created using a principal component analysis on neurocognitive test data (TMTA, TMTB, DSST, AVLT, VFT) (Merten, Paulsen, et al., 2020). Moreover, using principal component analysis, we calculated a combined sensorineural-cognitive function (brain aging) score in order to represent the integrity of the entire sensorineural system by combining sensorineural (PTA, CS, SDOIT) and neurocognitive (TMTA, TMTB, DSST, AVLT, VFT) test performances (Schubert et al., 2019).
We used multivariable linear regression, logistic regression and generalized estimating equation (GEE) models to assess the strength of associations of the determinant PWB with sensorineural and cognitive function, mGCIPL thickness and the combined measure of the brain aging as outcomes. Secondary analyses with individual cognitive tests are shown in Appendix 1. The GEE approach was used for paired measures (hearing sensitivity, hearing impairment, CS, VA, mGCIPL) to include both measures in a single model adjusting for the correlation between paired measures. There was insufficient variation in CS and olfactory test performance to support continuous analyses in this middle-aged cohort. Models were adjusted for age, sex and education and repeated additionally adjusting for chronic health conditions and behaviors (smoking, history of heavy alcohol consumption, diabetes, cardiovascular disease, long-term inflammation, waist circumference and exercise). We tested for sex interactions. Regression diagnostic plots were used to evaluate the fit and whether data satisfied the assumptions of linear regression. In order to visualize our results, we generated forest plots of the associations of psychological well-being (per 1SD Increase) and the odds for impairment in sensory and cognitive function (Appendix 2).
Results
This study included 2039 participants (mean age=58 years; SD=10, range=27-89; 55% women; Table 1). There were no significant sex interactions.
Table 1.
n(%) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Sex | ||||
Women | 1118(54.8) | |||
Men | 921(45.2) | |||
Education | ||||
0-12 years | 552(27.1) | |||
13-15 years | 707(34.7) | |||
16 years and more | 780(38.3) | |||
Smoking | ||||
Never | 1172(57.9) | |||
Former | 637(31.4) | |||
Current | 217(10.7) | |||
History of heavy alcohol | 374(19.0) | |||
Cardiovascular Disease | 232(11.5) | |||
Diabetes | 252(12.4) | |||
Long-term elevated inflammation | 765(46.8) | |||
Exercise at least once a week | 1369(67.7) | |||
Visual Acuity, letters correct, better eye | 59.2(5.8) | |||
Hearing impairment | 533(27.4) | |||
Contrast Sensitivity impairment | 137(7.0) | |||
Olfactory impairment | 110(5.7) | |||
Cognitive impairment | 83(4.1) | |||
M(SD) | Range | Skewness | Kurtosis | |
| ||||
Age, yrs | 57.9(9.6) | 27–89 | 0.13 | −0.17 |
Waist circumference, cm | 103.2(16.5) | 64–179.5 | 0.46 | 0.40 |
Hearing sensitivity, PTA worse ear, dB HL | 21.0(15.0) | −3.8–125 | 2.08 | 7.56 |
DDT score, % correct | 78.8(11.4) | 35.3–100 | −0.31 | −0.27 |
WRCM score, % correct | 54.5(17.2) | 0–92 | −0.88 | 0.66 |
Cognitive function | 0(1) | −6.4–3.5 | −1.24 | 4.45 |
Brain aging | 0(1) | −6.2–3.2 | −1.29 | 4.19 |
mGCIPL, worse eye, μm | 77.1(9.0) | 23–98 | −1.57 | 5.58 |
Psychological well-being | 81.1(12.5) | 24–98 | −0.94 | 0.54 |
Note. Sample sizes differ slightly due to missing data; If not stated otherwise measure was collected at the 10-year follow-up; Long-term elevated inflammation was based on measures from stored blood samples from the baseline and 5-year follow-up, where we measured high-sensitivity c-reactive protein, interleukin-6, vascular cell adhesion protein 1 and intercellular adhesion molecule 1. Long-term elevated inflammation was defined as being repeatedly in the highest group for at least one inflammation marker at baseline and 5-year follow-up. For high-sensitivity c-reactive protein, participants were divided into three risk groups (<1, 1–3, and >3mg/L) according to established cut-points and we compared the highest risk group to the rest. For the other three markers, we compared the highest tertile against the rest.
DDT, dichotic digits test; M, mean; mGCIPL, macular ganglion cell-inner plexiform layer; SD, standard deviation; PTA, pure-tone average 0.5-4 kHz; WRCM, word recognition in competing message test.
Higher PWB was associated with better hearing sensitivity (−0.91dB HL decrease per 1SD increase in PWB; 95% confidence interval (CI) −1.64,−0.18; p=.01) and dichotic digits performance (0.91% increase per 1SD increase in PWB; 95% CI 0.35,1.48; p=.002) in fully-adjusted models. Effect sizes were comparable to 1.5 years and 3.5 years of less aging, respectively. Individuals with higher PWB had reduced odds of hearing impairment (odds ratio OR=0.88 per 1SD increase in PWB; 95% CI 0.77,0.99; p=.04) in fully-adjusted models (Table 2, Appendix 2). In fully-adjusted models, PWB did not show associations with WRCM, VA, or risk for CS or olfactory impairment (Appendix 2).
Table 2.
Model 1 a |
Model 2 b |
|
---|---|---|
B (95% CI) | B (95% CI) | |
|
|
|
Hearing sensitivity score, in dB HL c | −1.00 (−1.61,−0.40)** | −0.91 (−1.64,−0.18)* |
|
|
|
Dichotic digits test score, in % d | 1.02 (0.52,1.53)*** | 0.91 (0.35,1.48)** |
|
|
|
Word recognition score, in % d | 0.48 (−0.22,1.18) | 0.56 (−0.26,1.37) |
|
|
|
Visual acuity score, in letters correct c | 0.31 (0.02,0.60)* | 0.20 (−0.14,0.54) |
|
|
|
OR (95% CI) | OR (95% CI) | |
|
|
|
Hearing sensitivity impairment c | 0.88 (0.79,0.98)* | 0.88 (0.77,0.99)* |
|
|
|
Contrast sensitivity impairment c | 0.87 (0.77,0.97)* | 0.92 (0.81,1.05) |
|
|
|
Olfactory impairment e | 0.82 (0.68,1.00) † | 0.88 (0.69,1.11) |
p < .10;
p < .05;
p < .01;
p < .001.
Note: CI, confidence interval; dB HL, decibel hearing level; OR, odds ratio.
Model adjusted for age, sex and education
Model adjusted for age, sex, education, chronic health conditions and health behaviors (smoking, history of heavy alcohol consumption, diabetes, cardiovascular disease, long-term elevated inflammation, exercise and waist circumference)
Generalized estimating equation model
Multivariable linear regression model
Multivariable logistic regression model
Higher PWB was strongly associated with better cognitive function (0.07SD increase per 1SD increase in PWB; 95% CI 0.03,0.11; p=.0006), which compared to an effect of 2 years of less aging and decreased odds for cognitive impairment (OR=0.60 per 1SD increase in PWB; 95% CI 0.47,0.76; p<.0001) in fully-adjusted models (Table 3, Appendix 2). PWB was also associated with better cognitive test scores on the TMTA, TMTB and DSST (Appendix 1).
Table 3.
Model 1 a |
Model 2 b |
|
---|---|---|
OR (95% CI) | OR (95% CI) | |
|
|
|
Cognitive impairment c | 0.56 (0.46, 0.69)*** | 0.60 (0.47, 0.76)*** |
|
|
|
B (95% CI) | B (95% CI) | |
|
|
|
Cognitive score, in SD d | 0.09 (0.06,0.13)*** | 0.07 (0.03, 0.11)*** |
|
|
|
MGCIPL thickness, in μm e | 0.36 (0.03,0.70)* | 0.33 (−0.05,0.71)† |
|
|
|
Brain aging score, in SD d | 0.11 (0.07,0.14)*** | 0.08 (0.04,0.12)*** |
p < .10;
p < .05;
p < .01;
p < .001.
Note: SD, standard deviation; OR, odds ratio; mGCIPL, macular ganglion cell-inner plexiform layer
Model adjusted for age, sex and education
Model adjusted for age, sex, education, chronic health conditions and health behaviors (smoking, history of heavy alcohol consumption, diabetes, cardiovascular disease, long-term elevated inflammation, exercise and waist circumference)
Multivariable logistic regression model
Multivariable linear regression model
Generalized estimating equation model
Individuals with higher PWB had thicker mGCIPL in age-sex-education-adjusted models, which was comparable to an effect of 1.5 years of less aging, but the effect size slightly decreased and was no longer statistically significant in the fully-adjusted model (0.33μm increase per 1SD increase in PWB; 95% CI −0.05,0.71; p=.08; Table 3).
Moreover, higher PWB was associated with higher scores on our combined sensorineural-cognitive measure of brain aging (0.08SD increase per 1SD increase in PWB; 95% CI 0.04,0.12; p<.0001; Table 3), comparable to 2 years of less aging.
Discussion
We found that higher PWB was associated with better sensorineural and cognitive function in middle-aged adults. These associations remained after adjusting for education, chronic health conditions and health behaviors. Notably, with every 1 SD increase in PWB participants showed benefits in sensorineural-cognitive function that were comparable to a difference of around 2 years of less aging.
These results extend previous findings to a study of multiple objectively and comprehensively assessed sensory and cognitive functions and associations in midlife, a time when interventions should be more efficacious (Boesveldt et al., 2017; Boyle et al., 2010; Kim et al., 2019; Kramer et al., 2002; Kuiper et al., 2015; Lewis et al., 2017; Mick et al., 2018; Mick & Pichora-Fuller, 2016; Nakanishi et al., 2019; Strawbridge et al., 2000; Windsor et al., 2015). Importantly, we were able to show that associations were independent of confounding of multiple physiologically assessed health factors such as inflammation and vascular risks, exceeding studies based on self-report. Moreover, we could compare associations across different sensorineural functions and found that among the senses, associations with hearing measures were strongest.
Our results are in line with research on the connection between PWB and various health functions. Since PWB is not simply the reverse of psychological distress its associations with physical and neuronal health are likely complicated and bi-directional (Ryff, 2014). Most previous studies have shown that PWB is often compromised in those with chronic health conditions or illness. However, growing research has also investigated its protective utility for human health (Boylan & Ryff, 2015; Brooks et al., 2014; Cohen et al., 2016; Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2010; Kim et al., 2013; Radler et al., 2018; Ryff, 2014; Ryff & Kim, 2020; Smith & Ruiz, 2002; Windsor et al., 2015; Yoo & Ryff, 2019; Yu et al., 2015; Zilioli et al., 2015). A better lifestyle and health behaviors (Hooker & Masters, 2016; Kim et al., 2014) and enriched and stimulating environments, i.e., social activities (Lu et al., 2003) could improve brain reserve (Stern, 2009) and are considered to contribute to these effects. Moreover, psychological and social resources could be protective against negative emotional effects and stress(Berkman et al., 2013). Importantly, PWB is considered modifiable (Chan et al., 2017; Friedman et al., 2019; Ryff, 2014), emphasizing a potential of PWB interventions to slow or stabilize sensory and cognitive decline. Strategies to maintain PWB might be most beneficial when applied early in the process of declining sensory and cognitive functions. However, future longitudinal studies are needed to assess the direction of effects between PWB and sensory and cognitive health. Moreover, future research needs to determine the efficacy of PWB intervention for improvement in neuronal health (Ryff, 2014).
We found associations of PWB with hearing and cognitive impairment that remained when adjusting for health-related confounders, suggesting an association independent of potential confounding through better health. This is consistent with three cohort studies showing associations between self-reported hearing impairment and reduced social functioning (Kramer et al., 2002; Mick et al., 2018; Strawbridge et al., 2000). We extend this research to objectively assessed hearing and confounder variables and we used a more multifaceted measure of PWB, which incorporated positive relations and purpose in life. Our results are also in line with existing epidemiological research primarily in older adults that shows associations of PWB with better cognition, less decline and reduced risk for impairment (Barnes et al., 2004; Boyle et al., 2010; James et al., 2011; Kats et al., 2016; Kim et al., 2019; Kuiper et al., 2015; Lewis et al., 2017; Nakanishi et al., 2019; Scarmeas et al., 2001; Seeman et al., 2001; Windsor et al., 2015; Xu et al., 2019). This study expands upon the limited research on midlife and extends findings to associations of the multifaceted PWB construct with performance on a verbal and visual cognitive test battery while adjusting for well-characterized potential health confounders.
There is always some task-impurity when assessing sensory and cognitive function using behavioral tests. To minimize measurement error due to sensory problems, participants used assistive devices when performing cognitive tasks (their own glasses and/or hearing aids and we provided reading glasses and/or a Pocket Talker (Pocket Talker Pro™, Williams Sound, Eden Prairie, MN) if they needed them). Longitudinal research has shown associations between sensory and cognitive decline and system-wide neurodegeneration can be considered a source of these co-occurring losses (Albers et al., 2015; Merten, Fischer, et al., 2020; Pronk et al., 2019; Schubert et al., 2019). It is difficult to disentangle the task-impurity problem from the co-occurring age-related declines in these systems with cross-sectional data. We did not include sensory measures in the analyses on cognitive function to avoid overcontrolling. Our results on an association of PWB with brain aging, which includes multiple dimensions of sensorineural and cognitive function even after confounder adjustment reflect this idea of system-wide neurodegeneration.
We did not find associations between PWB and visual function (VA or CS impairment) or olfactory impairment, which were objectively assessed in our study. One study reported associations of self-reported vision loss with lower social network diversity (among men only), and reduced social participation and support (Mick et al., 2018). Another cohort study found an association of social network size and socializing frequency and odor identification performance (Boesveldt et al., 2017). Our measure of PWB was not restricted to a social dimension but also entails purpose in life and these earlier studies incorporated potential confounder variables based on self-report. We found associations in the age-sex-education-adjusted models, which diminished in fully-adjusted models. This might mean that associations between PWB and vision and olfaction might primarily be confounded by healthier lifestyles and health status, or we might be over-adjusting for pathways through which PWB would promote sensory and cognitive. Similarly, associations of PWB with mGCIPL were shown in age-sex-education-adjusted models and decreased to a trend level when adding health variables, which could be due to confounding or over-adjustment.
Limitations and Strengths
We used inflammation markers from earlier BOSS waves, as these were not measured at the 10-year follow-up. Due to the cross-sectional design, we were not able to investigate the temporality of effects of PWB and our functional outcomes. The BOSS cohort is primarily non-Hispanic White, which may limit the generalizability of results to other populations. Strengths of this study include the large middle-aged sample with a variety of objectively assessed sensorineural and cognitive functions and the mGCIPL. Furthermore, we were able to address numerous potential confounders assessed with physical examination data.
Conclusion
Higher PWB was associated with better function in multiple sensorineural and cognitive functions in midlife, a time when prevention and intervention methods should be more efficacious. The remaining associations after adjusting for various potential confounders emphasizes its potential for healthy aging interventions. However, future studies will be needed to determine the temporality of effects and the efficacy of midlife PWB interventions for healthy sensory and cognitive aging and neurodegeneration.
Supplementary Material
Funding
This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health under Grant [number R01 AG021917 to KJC]; and an Unrestricted Grant from Research to Prevent Blindness, Inc. to the UW-Madison Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. The funding agencies had no role in study design; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the article for publication.
Footnotes
Disclosure of Interest
The Authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.
Data availability statement
The datasets for this manuscript are not publicly available because of data protection regulations. Requests to access the datasets though data sharing agreements should be directed to Dr. Karen J. Cruickshanks.
References
- Albers MW, Gilmore GC, Kaye J, Murphy C, Wingfield A, Bennett DA, Boxer AL, Buchman AS, Cruickshanks KJ, Devanand DP, Duffy CJ, Gall CM, Gates GA, Granholm A-C, Hensch T, Holtzer R, Hyman BT, Lin FR, McKee AC, … Zhang LI (2015). At the interface of sensory and motor dysfunctions and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia : The Journal of the Alzheimer’s Association, 11(1), 70–98. 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.04.514 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American National Standards Institute. (1999). Maximum Permissible Ambient Noise Levels for Audiometric Test Rooms. In ANSI, S3.1. ANSI, S3.1. [Google Scholar]
- American National Standards Institute. (2010). Specification for audiometers. ANSI, S3.6. [Google Scholar]
- American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA). (1978). Guidelines for manual pure-tone threshold audiometry. ASHA, 20, 297–301. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes LL, Mendes De Leon CF, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, & Evans DA (2004). Social resources and cognitive decline in a population of older African Americans and whites. Neurology, 63(12), 2322–2326. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000147473.04043.B3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berkman LF, Glass T, Brissette I, & Seeman TE (2013). From social integration to health: Durkheim in the new millennium. The Sociology of Health and Illness: A Reader, 51, 99–111. 10.4324/9781315013015-18 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boesveldt S, Yee JR, McClintock MK, & Lundström JN (2017). Olfactory function and the social lives of older adults: a matter of sex. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 45118. 10.1038/srep45118 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boylan JM, & Ryff CD (2015). Psychological Well-Being and Metabolic Syndrome. Psychosomatic Medicine, 77(5), 548–558. 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000192 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyle PA, Buchman AS, Barnes LL, & Bennett DA (2010). Effect of a Purpose in Life on Risk of Incident Alzheimer Disease and Mild Cognitive Impairment in Community-Dwelling Older Persons. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(3), 304. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.208 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks KP, Gruenewald T, Karlamangla A, Hu P, Koretz B, & Seeman TE (2014). Social relationships and allostatic load in the MIDUS study. Health Psychology, 33(11), 1373–1381. 10.1037/a0034528 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan A, Yu D, & Choi K (2017). Effects of tai chi qigong on psychosocial well-being among hidden elderly, using elderly neighborhood volunteer approach: a pilot randomized controlled trial. Clinical Interventions in Aging, 12, 85–96. 10.2147/CIA.S124604 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen Y, Kim ES, Koh HK, Frazier AL, & Vanderweele TJ (2019). Sense of Mission and Subsequent Health and Well-Being among Young Adults: An Outcome-Wide Analysis. American Journal of Epidemiology, 188(4), 664–673. 10.1093/aje/kwz009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen R, Bavishi C, & Rozanski A (2016). Purpose in Life and Its Relationship to All-Cause Mortality and Cardiovascular Events. Psychosomatic Medicine, 78(2), 122–133. 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000274 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diener E, & Chan MY (2011). Happy People Live Longer: Subjective Well-Being Contributes to Health and Longevity. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 3(1), 1–43. 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2010.01045.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- ETDRS Coordinating Center Department of Epidemiology and Preventive Medicine University of Maryland School of Medicine. (1980). Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study. (ETDRS) Manual of Operations. [Google Scholar]
- Friedman EM, Ruini C, Foy CR, Jaros L, Love G, & Ryff CD (2019). Lighten UP! A Community-Based Group Intervention to Promote Eudaimonic Well-Being in Older Adults: A Multi-Site Replication with 6 Month Follow-Up. Clinical Gerontologist, 42(4), 387–397. 10.1080/07317115.2019.1574944 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- González HM, Tarraf W, Gouskova N, Gallo LC, Penedo FJ, Davis SM, Lipton RB, Argüelles W, Choca JP, Catellier DJ, & Mosley TH (2015). Neurocognitive Function Among Middle-aged and Older Hispanic/Latinos: Results from the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 30(1), 68–77. 10.1093/arclin/acu066 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill PL, Edmonds GW, & Hampson SE (2018). Purpose to Self-Rated Health through Multiple Health Behaviors. Journal of Health Psychology, 24(10), 1–12. 10.1177/1359105317708251.A [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hooker SA, & Masters KS (2016). Purpose in life is associated with physical activity measured by accelerometer. Journal of Health Psychology, 21(6), 962–971. 10.1177/1359105314542822 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivnik RJ, Malec JF, Smith GE, Tangalos EG, & Petersen RC (1996). Neuropsychological tests’ norms above age 55: COWAT, BNT, MAE token, WRAT-R reading, AMNART, STROOP, TMT, and JLO. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 10(3), 262–278. 10.1080/13854049608406689 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ivnik RJ, Malec JF, Smith GE, Tangalos EG, Petersen RC, Kokmen E, & Kurland LT (1992a). Mayo’s older americans normative studies: Updated AVLT norms for ages 56 to 97. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6(sup001), 83–104. 10.1080/13854049208401880 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ivnik RJ, Malec JF, Smith GE, Tangalos EG, Petersen RC, Kokmen E, & Kurland LT (1992b). Mayo’s older americans normative studies: WAIS-R norms for ages 56 to 97. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6(sup001), 1–30. 10.1080/13854049208401877 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Jack CR, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, Petersen RC, Weiner MW, Aisen PS, Shaw LM, Vemuri P, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, Lesnick TG, Pankratz VS, Donohue MC, & Trojanowski JQ (2013). Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer’s disease: an updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers. The Lancet Neurology, 12(2), 207–216. 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70291-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- James BD, Wilson RS, Barnes LL, & Bennett DA (2011). Late-Life Social Activity and Cognitive Decline in Old Age. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(6), 998–1005. 10.1017/S1355617711000531 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kats D, Patel MD, Palta P, Meyer ML, Gross AL, Whitsel EA, Knopman D, Alonso A, Mosley TH, & Heiss G (2016). Social support and cognition in a community-based cohort: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study. Age and Ageing, 45(4), 475–480. 10.1093/ageing/afw060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Gouin J-P, & Hantsoo L (2010). Close relationships, inflammation, and health. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(1), 33–38. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.09.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim ES, Kawachi I, Chen Y, & Kubzansky LD (2017). Association Between Purpose in Life and Objective Measures of Physical Function in Older Adults. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(10), 1039. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.2145 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim ES, Strecher VJ, & Ryff CD (2014). Purpose in life and use of preventive health care services. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(46), 16331–16336. 10.1073/pnas.1414826111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim ES, Sun JK, Park N, & Peterson C (2013). Purpose in life and reduced incidence of stroke in older adults: “The Health and Retirement Study.” Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 74(5), 427–432. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.01.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim G, Shin SH, Scicolone MA, & Parmelee P (2019). Purpose in Life Protects Against Cognitive Decline Among Older Adults. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 27(6), 593–601. 10.1016/j.jagp.2019.01.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kramer SE, Kapteyn TS, Kuik DJ, & Deeg DJH (2002). The Association of Hearing Impairment and Chronic Diseases with Psychosocial Health Status in Older Age. Journal of Aging and Health, 14(1), 122–137. 10.1177/089826430201400107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuiper JS, Zuidersma M, Oude Voshaar RC, Zuidema SU, van den Heuvel ER, Stolk RP, & Smidt N (2015). Social relationships and risk of dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Ageing Research Reviews, 22, 39–57. 10.1016/j.arr.2015.04.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laugesen K, Baggesen LM, Schmidt SAJ, Glymour MM, Lasgaard M, Milstein A, Sørensen HT, Adler NE, & Ehrenstein V (2018). Social isolation and all-cause mortality: a population-based cohort study in Denmark. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 4731. 10.1038/s41598-018-22963-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis NA, Turiano NA, Payne BR, & Hill PL (2017). Purpose in life and cognitive functioning in adulthood. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 24(6), 662–671. 10.1080/13825585.2016.1251549 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu L, Bao G, Chen H, Xia P, Fan X, Zhang J, Pei G, & Ma L (2003). Modification of hippocampal neurogenesis and neuroplasticity by social environments. Experimental Neurology, 183(2), 600–609. 10.1016/S0014-4886(03)00248-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKnight PE, & Kashdan TB (2009). Purpose in Life as a System that Creates and Sustains Health and Well-Being: An Integrative, Testable Theory. Review of General Psychology, 13(3), 242–251. 10.1037/a0017152 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Merten N, Fischer ME, Tweed TS, Breteler MMB, & Cruickshanks KJ (2020). Associations of Hearing Sensitivity, Higher-Order Auditory Processing, and Cognition Over Time in Middle-Aged Adults. The Journals of Gerontology: Series A, 75(3), 545–551. 10.1093/gerona/glz189 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merten N, Paulsen AJ, Pinto AA, Chen Y, Dillard LK, Fischer ME, Huang G-H, Klein BEK, Schubert CR, & Cruickshanks KJ (2020). Macular Ganglion Cell-Inner Plexiform Layer as a Marker of Cognitive and Sensory Function in Midlife. The Journals of Gerontology: Series A, 75(9), e42–e48. 10.1093/gerona/glaa135 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mick P, Parfyonov M, Wittich W, Phillips N, Guthrie D, & Kathleen Pichora-Fuller M (2018). Associations between sensory loss and social networks, participation, support, and loneliness: Analysis of the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging. Canadian Family Physician Medecin de Famille Canadien, 64(1), e33–e41. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mick P, & Pichora-Fuller MK (2016). Is Hearing Loss Associated with Poorer Health in Older Adults Who Might Benefit from Hearing Screening? Ear & Hearing, 37(3), e194–e201. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakanishi M, Yamasaki S, Nishida A, & Richards M (2019). Midlife Psychological Well-being and its Impact on Cognitive Functioning Later in Life: An Observational Study Using a Female British Birth Cohort. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 72(3), 835–843. 10.3233/JAD-190590 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nash SD, Cruickshanks KJ, Klein R, Klein BEK, Nieto FJ, Huang GH, Pankow JS, & Tweed TS (2011). The Prevalence of Hearing Impairment and Associated Risk Factors. Archives of Otolaryngology–Head & Neck Surgery, 137(5), 432–439. 10.1001/archoto.2011.15 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pantell M, Rehkopf D, Jutte D, Syme SL, Balmes J, & Adler N (2013). Social Isolation: A Predictor of Mortality Comparable to Traditional Clinical Risk Factors. American Journal of Public Health, 103(11), 2056–2062. 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301261 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulsen AJ, Schubert CR, Johnson LJ, Chen Y, Dalton DS, Klein BEK, Klein R, Pinto A, & Cruickshanks KJ (2018). Association of Cadmium and Lead Exposure With the Incidence of Contrast Sensitivity Impairment Among Middle-aged Adults. JAMA Ophthalmology, 136(12), 1342–1350. 10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2018.3931 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearson TA, Mensah GA, Alexander RW, Anderson JL, Cannon RO, Criqui M, Fadl YY, Fortmann SP, Hong Y, Myers GL, Rifai N, Smith SC, Taubert K, Tracy RP, Vinicor F, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, & American Heart Association. (2003). Markers of inflammation and cardiovascular disease: application to clinical and public health practice: A statement for healthcare professionals from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and the American Heart Association. Circulation, 107(3), 499–511. 10.1161/01.cir.0000052939.59093.45 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelli DG, Robson JG, & Wilkins AJ (1988). The design of a new letter chart for measuring contrast sensitivity. Clinical Vision Sciences, 2(3), 187–199. [Google Scholar]
- Pronk M, Lissenberg-Witte BI, van der Aa HPA, Comijs HC, Smits C, Lemke U, Zekveld AA, & Kramer SE (2019). Longitudinal Relationships Between Decline in Speech-in-Noise Recognition Ability and Cognitive Functioning: The Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62(4S), 1167–1187. 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-H-ASCC7-18-0120 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radler BT, Rigotti A, & Ryff CD (2018). Persistently high psychological well-being predicts better HDL cholesterol and triglyceride levels: findings from the midlife in the U.S. (MIDUS) longitudinal study. Lipids in Health and Disease, 17(1), 1. 10.1186/s12944-017-0646-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reitan RM (1992). Trail Making Test Manual for Administration and Scoring. Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory. [Google Scholar]
- Ryff CD (2014). Psychological Well-Being Revisited: Advances in the Science and Practice of Eudaimonia. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 83(1), 10–28. 10.1159/000353263 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryff CD, & Kim ES (2020). Extending Research Linking Purpose in Life to Health: The Challenges of Inequality, the Potential of the Arts, and the Imperative of Virtue. In Burrow AL & Hill PL (Eds.), The Ecology of Purposeful Living Across the Lifespan (pp. 29–58). Springer International Publishing. 10.1007/978-3-030-52078-6_3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Scarmeas N, Levy G, Tang M-X, Manly J, & Stern Y (2001). Influence of leisure activity on the incidence of Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology, 57(12), 2236–2242. 10.1212/WNL.57.12.2236 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schubert CR, Fischer ME, Pinto AA, Chen Y, Klein BEK, Klein R, Tsai MY, Tweed TS, & Cruickshanks KJ (2019). Brain Aging in Midlife: The Beaver Dam Offspring Study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 67(8), 1610–1616. 10.1111/jgs.15886 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seeman TE, Lusignolo TM, Albert M, & Berkman L (2001). Social relationships, social support, and patterns of cognitive aging in healthy, high-functioning older adults: MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging. Health Psychology, 20(4), 243–255. 10.1037/0278-6133.20.4.243 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith TW, & Ruiz JM (2002). Psychosocial influences on the development and course of coronary heart disease: Current status and implications for research and practice. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(3), 548–568. 10.1037/0022-006X.70.3.548 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stern Y (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia, 47(10), 2015–2028. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strauss E, Sherman EMS, & Spreen O (2006). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests (Strauss E, Sherman EMS, & Spreen O (eds.); 3rd ed.). Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Strawbridge WJ, Wallhagen MI, Shema SJ, & Kaplan GA (2000). Negative consequences of hearing impairment in old age: A longitudinal analysis. Gerontologist, 40(3), 320–326. 10.1093/geront/40.3.320 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tombaugh T (1999). Normative Data Stratified by Age and Education for Two Measures of Verbal Fluency FAS and Animal Naming. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14(2), 167–177. 10.1016/S0887-6177(97)00095-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward DD, Mauschitz MM, Bönniger MM, Merten N, Finger RP, & Breteler MMB (2020). Association of retinal layer measurements and adult cognitive function: A population-based study. Neurology, 95(9), e1144–e1152. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000010146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitson HE, Cronin-Golomb A, Cruickshanks KJ, Gilmore GC, Owsley C, Peelle JE, Recanzone G, Sharma A, Swenor B, Yaffe K, & Lin FR (2018). American Geriatrics Society and National Institute on Aging Bench-to-Bedside Conference: Sensory Impairment and Cognitive Decline in Older Adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 66(11), 2052–2058. 10.1111/jgs.15506 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiley TL, Cruickshanks KJ, Nondahl DM, Tweed TS, Klein R, & Klein BE (1998). Aging and word recognition in competing message. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 9(3), 191–198. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson RH, Zizz CA, Shanks JE, & Causey GD (1990). Normative data in quiet, broadband noise, and competing message for Northwestern University Auditory Test No. 6 by a female speaker. The Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55(4), 771–778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Windsor TD, Curtis RG, & Luszcz MA (2015). Sense of purpose as a psychological resource for aging well. Developmental Psychology, 51(7), 975–986. 10.1037/dev0000023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu H, Yang R, Qi X, Dintica C, Song R, Bennett DA, & Xu W (2019). Association of Lifespan Cognitive Reserve Indicator with Dementia Risk in the Presence of Brain Pathologies. JAMA Neurology, 76(10), 1184–1191. 10.1001/jamaneurol.2019.2455 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang YC, Boen C, Gerken K, Li T, Schorpp K, & Harris KM (2016). Social relationships and physiological determinants of longevity across the human life span. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(3), 578–583. 10.1073/pnas.1511085112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoo J, & Ryff CD (2019). Longitudinal Profiles of Psychological Well-Being and Health: Findings From Japan. Frontiers in Psychology, 10(December), 1–8. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02746 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu L, Boyle PA, Wilson RS, Levine SR, Schneider JA, & Bennett DA (2015). Purpose in Life and Cerebral Infarcts in Community-Dwelling Older People. Stroke, 46(4), 1071–1076. 10.1161/STROKEAHA.114.008010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhong W, Cruickshanks KJ, Schubert CR, Carlsson CM, Chappell RJ, Klein BEK, Klein R, & Acher CW (2014). Pulse Wave Velocity and Cognitive Function in Older Adults. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders, 28(1), 44–49. 10.1097/WAD.0b013e3182949f06 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zilioli S, Slatcher RB, Ong AD, & Gruenewald TL (2015). Purpose in life predicts allostatic load ten years later. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 79(5), 451–457. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2015.09.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets for this manuscript are not publicly available because of data protection regulations. Requests to access the datasets though data sharing agreements should be directed to Dr. Karen J. Cruickshanks.