Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Apr 8.
Published in final edited form as: J Sch Violence. 2019 Dec 19;19(1):62–76. doi: 10.1080/15388220.2019.1703716

A Longitudinal Study of Gun Violence Attitudes: Role of Childhood Aggression and Exposure to Violence, and Early Adolescent Bullying Perpetration and Victimization

Amanda B Nickerson a, Shannon M Shisler b, Rina D Eiden c, Jamie M Ostrov d, Pamela Schuetze e, Stephanie A Godleski f, Alan M Delmerico g
PMCID: PMC8993126  NIHMSID: NIHMS1546406  PMID: 35401061

Abstract

This prospective study examined the effects of early childhood physical aggression and violence exposure on bullying victimization/perpetration and attitudes toward guns and violence in early adolescence (EA) in a high-risk sample. Participants included 216 mother-child dyads from an ongoing longitudinal study using multi-method assessments (e.g., classroom observations, laboratory assessment, parent-, teacher-, and child self-reports). Results supported a developmental pathway from early adversity (i.e., prenatal substance use) to aggression at kindergarten age to bullying perpetration and gun violence attitudes (aggressive responses to shame) in EA. Higher peer victimization was also associated with aggressive responses to shame in EA. Results are discussed in light of the complexity of the motives for aggression and the need for prevention and early intervention.

Keywords: Aggression, gun violence, bullying, longitudinal


Gun violence is a major public health concern, and positive attitudes toward weapon carrying and violence may be a marker of later gun violence. Research is needed to examine the significant early risk factors for these attitudes, using longitudinal designs with high-risk samples (Grych & Swan, 2012; Institute of Medicine & National Research Council, 2013; Spano, 2012). A persistent pattern of aggressive behavior and positive attitudes may initiate a proactive aggression pathway, whereby these behaviors persist and escalate over time and generalize across contexts (Crick & Dodge, 1996; Vitaro, Brendgen, & Barker, 2006). However, this has rarely been assessed in relation to gun violence risk. Early childhood exposure to violence and related comorbid risks may also place children at risk for later forms of peer victimization (e.g., bullying victimization), which could lead adolescents to have more positive views of gun violence and engage in aggression for protective functions (Reid, Richards, Loughran, & Mulvey, 2017).

From a social-ecological perspective, early exposure to violence and adversity may be part of overarching multi-systemic environmental interactions and create contexts for children that perpetuate generational transmission of aggression (Bronfenbrenner, 1977). Strain theory also suggests that persistent, negative experiences that are unjust, high in magnitude, associated with low social control, and model violence and crime lead youth to engage in violence and crime to cope (Agnew, 2001, 2007). These strain experiences include but are not limited to child abuse, parental rejection, negative school experiences, and abusive peer relations, although the latter has been understudied (Agnew, 2001). In addition, behavioral teratology models suggest that prenatal substance exposure increases biological vulnerability to children’s regulation physiologically and in aggression and conduct problems (Eiden, Godleski, Schuetze, & Colder, 2015; Mayes, 2002). There is also strong theoretical and empirical support for transmission of violence through interpersonal networks via exposure to violence and experiences of individual victimization or perpetration (Tracy, Braga, & Papachristos, 2016). Being in high-risk environments with shared violent experiences and norms may also increase the risk of further victimization by accepting violence as a norm, predisposing one to engage in violence in defense or retaliation, and sharing violent experiences with others in their network (Tracy et al., 2016). From a stress process model, children living in disadvantaged environments are more likely to be exposed to violence, with co-occurrence of violence and victimization resulting in a variety of adverse outcomes (Foster & Brooks-Gunn, 2009). As depicted by the theoretical model in Figure 1, we conceptualized that early adversity (i.e., prenatal substance exposure, socioeconomic adversity) would lead to increased violence exposure and stable childhood aggression throughout early childhood and kindergarten, which would then lead to bullying victimization and perpetration, as well as more positive attitudes toward guns and violence in EA in a low-income, diverse sample of mostly substance using mothers and their children.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Theoretical model.

Attitudes toward guns and violence predict aggression (Bryant, 2011) and youth gun ownership (Shapiro, Dorman, Burkey, Welker, & Clough, 1997). These findings are consistent with social-cognitive theory and related research demonstrating that cognitive processes (e.g., pro-aggression attitudes, hostile attribution biases) predict aggressive behavior and mediate the relation between exposure to violence and aggressive behavior (Bradshaw, Rodgers, Ghandour, & Garbarino, 2009; Houston & Grych, 2016). Factor analysis of the Attitudes Toward Guns and Violence Questionnaire (Shapiro, 2000) has identified four subscales to assess this construct: (a) aggressive response to shame, or beliefs that aggression is the only way to counter shame arising from being insulted; (b) comfort with aggression (i.e., general values about aggression and guns); (c) excitement, or the intrinsic stimulation associated with guns; and (d) power/safety (i.e., viewpoint that violence and guns give power and increase safety). Aggressive response to shame has been most consistently related to aggressive behavior in youth (Bryant, 2011; Hart, O’Toole, Price-Sharps, & Shaffer, 2007). Relatedly, shame displacement, including blaming others, and having a desire to retaliate aggressively, increases bullying behavior (Ahmed & Braithwaite, 2004). Feelings of power and safety also discriminate between violent, non-violent delinquent, and non-delinquent male youth (Hart et al., 2007).

These distinct attitudes toward guns and violence may develop from different developmental pathways, although there is a dearth of research testing models with high-risk samples (Myers et al., 2018; Reid et al., 2017). The majority of research on the transmission of violence through interpersonal networks via exposure to violence and individual victimization or perpetration experiences is cross-sectional, based on single-informant self-reports, or retrospective (Tracy et al., 2016). Developmental studies of aggressive behavior are often conducted with predominantly White middle-class samples, despite the fact that violence is most prominent for African-American youth in low-income neighborhoods (Bryant, 2011; Stoddard, Zimmerman, & Bauermeister, 2012). In this study, we examined two pathways to predict attitudes toward guns and violence in a high-risk, diverse sample of youth using multi-method assessments of major constructs from infancy through EA.

Early Childhood Aggression, Bullying Perpetration, and Gun Violence

Physical aggression is common in the toddler years and declines sharply in early childhood (Côté et al., 2007), although a subset of children exhibits persistent patterns of aggressive and antisocial behavior across time and contexts (Loeber & Farrington, 2000; Moffit, 2003). Aggression is associated with higher levels of community violence exposure (Foster & Brooks-Gunn, 2009), developed and maintained in part by modeling, exposure to, and endorsement of pro-aggressive attitudes and behaviors (Fite et al., 2010; Fite, Schwartz, & Hendrickson, 2012; Vitaro et al., 2006). This pattern of aggression, and having friends who carry weapons, increase the risk of youth weapon carrying (Dijkstra, Gest, Lindenberg, Veenstra, & Cillessen, 2012; Dijkstra et al., 2010).

Although there is a voluminous body of developmental research on aggression, longitudinal studies of bullying perpetration are sparser (Rodkin, Espelage, & Hanish, 2015). Bully perpetrators are more likely than their uninvolved peers to carry weapons (Dukes, Stein, & Zane 2010; van Geel, Vedder, & Tanilon, 2014), but due to a dearth of longitudinal studies, it remains unclear whether bullying perpetration is incidental to underlying aggression or if it mediates the association with later antisocial outcomes (Rodkin et al., 2015). Interestingly, a recent study by Schwartz, Lansford, Dodge, and Pettit (2018) found that high levels of peer victimization attenuated the association between early aggression and adult arrest, suggesting that the link between bullying experiences and adult criminal outcomes may be rooted in childhood adversity.

The current study addresses a gap in the literature by examining the relation of early and ongoing aggressive and problem behavior across contexts (e.g., home and school) to bullying perpetration and attitudes toward guns and violence in EA in a high-risk, predominantly low-income African-American sample. We hypothesized that this pattern may be particularly associated with comfort with aggression, as youth may view violence as a way to achieve goals (Fite et al., 2010, 2012), suggested by strain theory (Agnew, 2001).

Exposure to Violence, Bullying, and Gun Violence

Multiple studies have found an association between violence exposure and youth weapon carrying (Shapiro et al., 1997; Spano & Bolland, 2013). Some research has suggested that the link between exposure to violence and aggression occurs through indirect associations with anxiety (Myer et al., 2018). However, Reid and colleagues (2017) found that violence exposure and psychological distress predicted adolescents’ later gun carrying, although the effect of psychological distress was reduced after accounting for exposure to violence.

A link has also been found between peer victimization (e.g., bullying) and weapon carrying, particularly for males (Esselmont, 2014; Goldstein, Young, & Boyd, 2008). This issue has received national attention, due in part to findings that 71% of school shooters had been victims of bullying (Vossekuil, Fein, Reddy, Borum, & Modzeleski, 2002). There is growing research support indicating that victimized youth are more likely to carry weapons and perceive their schools as less safe (Esselmont, 2014; Goldstein et al., 2008). These reduced perceptions of school safety mediate the relation between peer victimization and weapon carrying, particularly for boys (Goldstein et al., 2008). Thus, we examined if childhood exposure to violence was prospectively predictive of peer bullying and victimization in EA, and its further association with aggressive responses to shame and power and safety attitudes toward guns and violence.

Current Study

We examined two pathways, informed by social-ecological, strain theory, and biological teratology models of the effects of prenatal substance exposure, to specific attitudes toward guns and violence (e.g., aggressive response to shame, comfort with aggression, power/safety) from infancy to EA for a sample at high risk due to prenatal substance exposure and socioeconomic adversity. The first pathway examined persistent aggressive behavior from toddler age to kindergarten to EA bullying perpetration and attitudes toward guns and violence. The second pathway examined the association between exposure to violence from toddler age to kindergarten to bullying victimization in EA and its association with attitudes toward guns and violence. Covariates were included in the model given their associations with the constructs under examination, including maternal race and being in non-biological parent care, which has been linked to more negative outcomes in some samples (Linares et al., 2006; Minnes et al., 2010) and more positive outcomes for others (Eiden, Foote, & Schuetze, 2007).

Method

Participants and Procedure

The sample included 216 caregiver–child dyads (49% boys) participating in a longitudinal study since the child’s birth, recruited from two urban hospitals serving a predominantly African American low-income population. All mothers were screened after delivery to identify a sample of participants with high rates of prenatal cocaine use. Exclusionary criteria consisted of maternal age < 18 years, use of illicit substances other than cocaine or marijuana during pregnancy, plural births, and significant infant medical problems. The sample consisted of 116 infants exposed to cocaine and other substances (alcohol, cigarettes, marijuana) in utero, and 100 not exposed to cocaine but may have been exposed to other substances (see Eiden, Coles, Schuetze, & Colder,2014 for more information). Caregiver-infant dyads were group matched on maternal race/ethnicity and education (see Table 1 for demographics). Caregiver-infant dyads were assessed beginning at 1 month of infant age (corrected for prematurity), with follow-up assessments every 6 months until and including kindergarten age, at 2nd grade, and in EA (M = 13.6 years, SD = 1.22). Although the primary caregiver of the child was identified at each assessment and invited to participate, the term mother is used for ease of presentation. In EA, adolescents self-identified their race/ethnicity as 57.6% African American, 17.9% Mixed Race, 7.9% Hispanic, 6.6% Caucasian, 2.0% American Indian, 0.7% Asian, and 7.3% other.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations.

M/% SD 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15.
1. Prenatal Substance Exposure .07 .12 1
2. Socioeconomic Adversity .56 .22 .23** 1
3. Non-biological care (% ever in NBC) 31.5% .28** .20** 1
4. Maternal Race (% minority) 84.2% .01 .29** .13+ 1
5. Maternal Exposure to Violence 64.2% −.13+ −.06 −.08 −.01 1
6. Child Aggression 18 to 48 months (MR) 2.96 1.69 .07 .04 −.06 .06 .13+ 1
7. Child Exposure to Violence (K – MR) (% exposed) 33.1% .04 .14+ .11 .01 .18* .19* 1
8. Child Aggression (K – TR BASC) 4.86 5.77 .18* .15+ .09 .17* .04 .18* .12 1
9. Child Aggression/Defiance (K – TR SNAP) 2.51 3.30 .15+ .13 .06 .24* .09 .11 .09 .85** 1
10. Classroom Problem Behavior (K – OBS) 8.76 9.75 .03 .05 .13 .17* .05 .05 .09 .44** .50** 1
11. Bully Perpetration (EA - CR) 1.19 .35 .15+ .07 .03 .18* .04 .19* .12 .34** .23** .08 1
12. Bully Victimization (EA-CR) 1.32 .46 .04 .02 .06 .10 −.01 .10 .13 −.01 −.01 .10 .38** 1
13. Power/Safety Attitudes (EA-CR) 4.09 2.31 .12 .10 .13 .05 −.01 −.06 .13 .03 −.02 .07 .16+ .04 1
14. Aggressive Response to Shame Attitudes (EA-CR) 4.18 3.67 .09 .19* .09 .19* −.02 .06 .20* .12 .11 .12 .30** .31** .18* 1
15. Comfort with Aggression Attitudes (EA-CR) 2.72 2.86 .15+ −.14+ .04 −.12 .02 .01 −.01 .14 .16+ .24** .02 −.06 −.10 −.09 1

Descriptive Statistics and Bivariate Correlations

Note. NBC = Non-biological care; MR = mother report; K = kindergarten; TR = teacher report; OBS = observation; EA = early adolescence; CR = child report. Maternal race coded 0 = Caucasian, 1 = minority. Sample sizes for correlations ranged from n = 126 to 216.

+

p < .10

*

p < .05

**

p < .01

Measures

Early Adversity and Sociodemographic Risk.

Maternal prenatal substance use included cocaine use (0 = no, 1 = yes) assessed by a combination of urine toxicology, maternal hair analysis, and self-report (see Eiden, Coles, Schuetze, & Colder, 2014 for additional details), as well as cigarette and alcohol use (measured by the Timeline Follow-Back Interview [TLFB]; Sobell et al., 1986). Socioeconomic adversity was assessed at 1-month of age based on a composite variable consisting of low maternal education, single parenthood, and low family income. Mothers reported their highest level of education, dichotomized to reflect those who had not completed high school (40%). Mothers who reported not living with a partner in the past month were coded as single (67%). Income-to-needs ratios were computed by dividing each family’s total income (including public assistance) by the number of family members supported by this income. This variable was reverse-coded; higher scores reflected lower income-to-needs, turned into a proportion by dividing each family’s income by the sample maximum. To create the composite socioeconomic adversity score, an average of all indicators was computed (Moran et al., 2016).

Violence Exposure.

Maternal exposure to violence was assessed at each appointment between 1 and 48 months of child age (9 total assessments) using the TLFB (Sobell et al., 1986) where mothers reported about violence they witnessed, experienced, or perpetrated in the six months prior. Child exposure to violence (e.g., shootings, drug deals, beatings) was assessed with the 26-item Survey of Exposure to Community Violence (Richters & Saltzman, 1990) from maternal report when the child was in kindergarten (α = .84). Due to high skewness and kurtosis, which persisted despite transformation, both maternal and child exposure variables were dummy-coded 0 = non-exposed and 1 = exposed to violence.

Child Aggression.

Early childhood aggression was assessed using maternal reports on the Child Behavior Checklist, 1.5- to 5-year version (CBCL; Achenbach, 1992) every 6 months from 18 to 48 months. Only the six items related to pure physical aggression (e.g., attacks people) were utilized in the composite (see NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2004), averaged across the waves of data collection (αs = .75 to .82).

In kindergarten, teacher ratings and classroom observations were used as measured indicators of a latent construct for child aggression. Specifically, teachers rated students on 10 items from the Behavioral Assessment for Children Teacher Rating Scales (BASC TRS; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1992) assessing classroom aggression (e.g., “Hits other children”) on a scale of 0 (Never) to 3 (Almost Always). Responses were summed to create the aggression score (α = .93). Teachers also reported on children’s aggression/defiance using 5 items from the Swanson, Nolan and Pelham – IV (SNAP-IV; Swanson, Nolan, & Pelham, 1982) rated on a scale of 0 (Never) to 3 (Very Much). Scores were averaged, and the internal consistency of the subscale was good (α = .90). Finally, classroom problem behaviors were assessed using the BASC Student Observation System (BASC SOS; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1992). Trained research assistants blind to group status observed the child in the kindergarten classroom for 90 minutes, and recorded the percentage of time students were engaged in problem behaviors across six 15-minute episodes. Coders were trained to a minimum inter-rater reliability criterion of r = .80. Inter-rater reliability checks occurred at 4-week intervals on 4–5 school observations (Cohen’s Κappa = .87). Confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) revealed sufficient standardized factor loading for the BASC aggression scale (.87), the SNAP-IV aggression/defiance scale (.98), and the BASC SOS problem behavior scale (.51). This factor was labeled child aggression and was included as a latent indicator in subsequent analyses.

Bullying Perpetration and Victimization.

In EA, bullying victimization and perpetration in the previous 30 days were assessed with the California Bullying Victimization Scale (Felix et al., 2011). Each subscale consists of eight items assessing the frequency of bullying victimization (e.g., “How often have you been hit, pushed, or physically hurt in a mean or hurtful way?”) or perpetration (e.g., “How have you hit, pushed, or physically hurt another student in a mean or hurtful way?”) on 5-point Likert scale (1; not in the past month, 2; once in the past month, 3; 2 or 3 times in the past month, 4; about once a week, or 5; several times a week). Total scores were obtained by averaging across items (Hase, Godlberg, Smith, Stuck, & Campain, 2015), with α = .76 for victimization and .73 for perpetration in the current study.

Attitudes Towards Guns and Violence.

Adolescents completed the Attitudes Toward Guns and Violence Questionnaire (ATGVQ; Shapiro, 2000), a measure assessing comfort with aggression (6 items, e.g., “I wish there weren’t any guns in my neighborhood”; α = .77), excitement about guns (5 items, e.g., “It would be exciting to hold a loaded gun in my hand”; α = .65), power/safety derived from the violence and guns (4 items, e.g., “Carrying a gun makes people feel powerful and strong”; α = .70), and aggressive responses to shame (8 items, e.g., “If somebody insults you and you don’t want to be a chump, you have to fight”; α = .78). Each item was rated from 0 (disagree) to 2 (agree), and a summed score was calculated for each subscale; higher scores indicated more positive attitudes about guns, aggression, and violence. Due to the questionable reliability of the excitement subscale and its low base rate (70% of the sample scored 0), this subscale was not included in further analyses. The ATGVQ has been associated with handgun ownership (Shapiro et al., 1997). In the current sample, more positive attitudes toward guns and violence correlated significantly with Youth Self-Report (YSR; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) of aggression (r = .34, p < .01) and the Adolescent Delinquency scale (Huizinga, Esbensen, & Weiher, 1991) assault subscale (r = .29, p < .01) in EA.

Covariates.

Maternal race and non-biological parent care were covariates in the model. These variables were assessed through caregiver interview.

Data Analysis

First, associations between variables were examined using Pearson’s correlations or Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) as appropriate. Covariates that had bivariate associations with early childhood variables (1–48 months) or outcome variables (attitudes towards guns and violence) at p < .10 were included in the analysis. Data were analyzed using structural equation modeling (SEM) in Mplus version 8 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2017). Model fit was evaluated using the comparative fit index (CFI), the root-mean-square-error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR); nested models were compared using the chi-square difference test. Modification indices were examined to determine if respecification of the model was warranted and theoretically relevant paths indicated by the modification indices were added.

Missing Data

As with any longitudinal study and in particular studies involving at-risk samples (e.g., Sturge-Apple et al., 2017), there were incomplete data for some participants at one or more of the assessment points. Of the 216 participants enrolled in the study, 164 completed the kindergarten assessment (155 of these had complete teacher and school observation data), and 152 participants completed all EA assessments. Families with missing data in EA were not significantly different on maternal age, partner status, or prenatal substance exposure, but 42.2% of children missing data for the EA assessment were in non-biological parent care at some point between birth and kindergarten, compared to only 24.2% of children without missing EA data, Pearson χ2 = 7.46, p < .01, Cohen’s d = .38. Little’s test indicated data fit criteria for missing-completely at-random χ2 (190) = 168.00, p = .87. Full information maximum likelihood estimation procedures (Arbuckle, 1996), robust to violations of normality were used to estimate model parameters (Schafer & Graham, 2002).

Results

Bivariate associations and descriptive statistics for study variables are presented in Table 1. Child aggression in both early childhood and kindergarten were positively associated with bullying perpetration in EA, and bullying perpetration and victimization in EA were associated with aggressive responses to shame, but not to the other attitudes about guns and violence.

Model Testing

The first step in SEM analyses was to fit the measurement model for the latent variable for child aggression (see CFA results in Method). The second step was to test the structural model. We tested the hypothesized model (Figure 1) with prenatal substance use, maternal race, and socioeconomic adversity as exogenous variables. The model included predictive paths from prenatal substance exposure, race, and socioeconomic adversity to three early childhood variables: maternal violence exposure (0 = never, 1 = ever from 1 to 48 months), child aggression (composite scores from 1 to 48 months), and non-biological parent care status (0 = never in non-biological care, 1 = ever in non-biological care through EA). This was followed by predictive paths from all three early childhood variables to the two kindergarten variables (i.e., child violence exposure, child aggression). Predictive paths from the kindergarten variables included paths to EA bullying perpetration and victimization, and finally, there were associations from bullying and victimization to the three attitude outcomes (i.e., power/safety, aggressive response to shame, comfort with aggression). All within time residuals were covaried. This model fit the data well (χ2(63) = 76.84, p = .11, CFI = .96, RMSEA = .03, CI [.00-.05], SRMR = .06).

Modification indices suggested the addition of theoretically meaningful predictive paths from the latent construct of child aggression in kindergarten to comfort with guns and violence and a path from maternal socioeconomic adversity to kindergarten violence exposure.

Addition of these two paths resulted in a significant improvement in model fit Δχ2(2) = 7.23, p > .05, and the new model fit the data well (i.e., χ2(61) = 69.61, p = .21, CFI = .98, RMSEA = .05, CI [.00-.05]), SRMR = .05). Overall, this model explained 2.4% of the variance in power and safety attitudes, 3% of the variance in comfort with guns and violence, and 13.2% of the variance in aggressive response to shame (see Figure 2 and Table 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Results of respecified SEM model. EA = early adolescence; TR = teacher report. Standardized coefficients are presented.

All within time covariances were included in the model, but are not shown for ease of presentation. Significant covariances included prenatal substance use with socioeconomic adversity (β = .227, p < .001), socioeconomic adversity with race (β = .287, p < .001), early childhood aggression with maternal violence exposure (β = .153, p = .035), non-biological care with child aggression (β = −.625, p < .001) and bullying with victimization (β = .416, p < .001). Significant results are denoted by solid lines and marginal results are denoted by dashed lines. Nonsignificant pathways are not included in the figure for ease of presentation, but these estimates are reported in Table 2.

** p < .01, *p < .05, + p < .10

Table 2.

Nonsignificant Model Paths

Path Standardized Estimate SE p-value
Prenatal Substance Use to Early Childhood Aggression 0.068 0.071 .338
Race to Early Childhood Aggression 0.045 0.072 .536
Socioeconomic Adversity to Early Childhood Aggression 0.007 0.077 .926
Race to Maternal Violence Exposure 0.005 0.071 .943
Socioeconomic Adversity to Maternal Violence Exposure −0.034 0.073 .638
Socioeconomic Adversity to Non-biological Care 0.092 0.060 .125
Early Childhood Aggression to Non-biological Care −0.079 0.068 .242
Maternal Violence Exposure to Non-biological Care −0.036 0.066 .582
Maternal Violence Exposure to Childhood Aggression (K) 0.112 0.101 .266
Child Violence Exposure (K) to Bullying 0.110 0.080 .166
Non-biological Care to Bullying −0.011 0.082 .891
Child Violence Exposure (K) to Victimization 0.135 0.089 .129
Childhood Aggression (K) to Victimization −0.028 0.091 .760
Non-biological Care to Victimization 0.052 0.085 .539
Bullying to Comfort Attitudes −0.012 0.094 .896
Victimization to Comfort Attitudes −0.048 0.089 .585
Victimization to Power and Safety Attitudes −0.026 0.087 .768

Discussion

The findings of this study support a social-ecologically informed developmental pathway from early risk (i.e., prenatal substance exposure, non-biological care) to child aggression in kindergarten to early adolescents’ endorsement of engaging in aggressive responses to shame; bullying perpetration in EA mediated the association between kindergarten child aggression and these attitudes in EA. Victimization by bullying was also related to aggressive response to shame, but not through the anticipated pathway from exposure to violence from infancy through middle childhood. These results highlight both the impact of bullying perpetration and victimization on attitudes toward guns and violence, as well as the aggressive behavior in the school and peer contexts as a potential pathway toward gun violence.

Supporting behavioral teratology models (Mayes, 2002), prenatal substance exposure directly predicted the latent variable of child aggression in kindergarten. Although not examined specifically in the current study, past research has found that maternal aggression, negative affect (Eiden, Schuetze, Colder, & Veira, 2011), and lower sensitivity (Ettekal, Eiden, Nickerson, Molnar, & Schuetze, 2019), as well as lower infant autonomic regulation (Schuetze, Eiden, & Danielwicz, 2011) are processes involved in the relation between prenatal substance exposure and child aggression, in addition to potential direct associations due to increased biological vulnerability. The current study extends the examination of the adverse impact of prenatal risk and childhood aggression into early adolescence, finding that these earlier risks predicted both bullying perpetration and aggressive attitudes supporting gun violence. Supporting an early starter pattern, results indicated a pattern of aggression beginning in early childhood that was sustained across time and contexts (Loeber & Farrington, 2000; Moffit, 2003).

Results of this study respond to calls for research examining the mediating role of bullying perpetration in early aggression and later antisocial outcomes (Rodkin et al., 2015), indicating that aggression in kindergarten is indirectly related to endorsement of gun violence attitudes through bullying perpetration. The only specific attitude this pathway predicted was aggressive response to shame, as previous research has found (Bryant, 2011; Hart et al., 2007). Aligned with the transmission of violence through interpersonal networks theories (Tracy et al., 2016), it is possible that these youth who display ongoing aggressive behavior in schools and with peers have accepted violence as a norm and as a way to achieve goals or cope with stresses. In addition, there was a direct, concurrent association between victimization by peers and endorsement of aggressive responses to shame in EA, consistent with previous research (Esselmont, 2014; Goldstein, Young, & Boyd, 2008). According to strain theory, youth who experience a persistent, negative experience (in this case, peer victimization) may show a proclivity toward violence and guns to cope (Agnew, 2001). The specific relations between bullying perpetration and victimization to aggressive response to shame fits within a larger body of research and theory about hostile attribution biases, which may mediate the association between peer victimization and later externalizing behaviors (Perren, Ettekal, & Ladd, 2013).

Although there was a marginal nonsignificant relation between kindergarten aggression and comfort with guns and violence, bullying perpetration did not relate to this construct and it was marginally but inversely predictive of power/safety attitudes. Taken together, findings from this sample suggest that bullying perpetrators did not endorse proactive or premeditated beliefs (e.g., comfort with guns/violence, power), but viewed violence as a retaliatory coping mechanism (Ahmed & Braithwaite, 2004). Research that assesses functions of aggression (e.g., reactive and proactive) and cognitive processes (e.g., hostile attribution biases) in relation to bullying and gun violence attitudes may be fruitful in further elucidating these processes.

Findings partially supported a stress process model (Foster & Brooks-Gunn, 2009) in that socioeconomic adversity predicted exposure to violence, yet violence exposure to violence did not relate to later adverse outcomes (e.g., bullying, attitudes toward guns and violence). Past research has found associations between exposure to violence with later aggression, violence, and weapon carrying in youth (Myer et al., 2018; Spano & Bolland, 2013). Our measure of exposure to violence assessed serious and life-threatening events experienced or witnessed by the mother or child (e.g., seeing someone shot, being threatened with serious physical harm) and due to high skewness and kurtosis that persisted despite transformations we used a dichotomous score. It is possible that lack of variability led to the lack of effects. It is also possible that we did not find effects because the direct effect tested was from exposure to violence and bullying, as opposed to gun violence or other aggressive behavior. There is little research on exposure to violence and bullying, and it is possible that other individual and contextual factors are more strongly predictive of bullying (see Cook, Williams, Guerra, Kim, & Sadek, 2010). The association between exposure to violence and bullying may also be moderated by other variables, such as child temperament, parenting, or association with deviant peers.

Implications for Practice, Limitations, and Future Research Directions

Findings about early childhood physical aggression predicting ongoing violence and violence-related attitudes highlight the importance of early identification and prevention. Parent training programs have been found to reduce children’s conduct problems while improving prosocial behavior and socioemotional competence (Menting, Orobio de Castroa, & Matthys, 2013; Posthumus, Raaijmakers, Maassen, van Engeland, & Matthys, 2012). For youth in middle childhood and early adolescence, cognitive-behavioral therapy focusing on developing emotional awareness, social problem-solving skills, and anger management can reduce aggression and improve social competence (Lochman, Powell, Boxmeyer, & Jimenez-Camargo, 2011; Sukhodolsky, Kassinove, & Gorman, 2004), which may be particularly relevant given this sample’s endorsement of seeing guns and violence as responses to difficult emotions. Aggressive behavior in kindergarten as well as bullying perpetration and victimization were predictors of positive attitudes toward guns and violence, which highlights the importance of multi-tiered bullying prevention and intervention in the school setting (Nickerson, 2019).

Despite the strengths of this study, including the longitudinal design using multiple methods and sources, there are limitations. The relatively small sample size precluded the inclusion of other important potential mediating and moderating variables in the model due to power considerations. For example, we did not examine sex differences or protective factors, which are critical for enhancing our understanding of processes of resilience. Supporting a risk-protective factor model of resilience, individual, peer, and family promotive factors have been found as protective in youth violent behavior for individuals with high cumulative risk (Stoddard et al., 2012).

Future research should focus on different forms (i.e., relational, physical) and functions (e.g., reactive, proactive) of aggression (Ostrov & Godleski, 2013), given potential differential associations with outcomes. Another limitation was that bullying and victimization, as well as gun violence attitudes, were collected via self-report at the same EA assessment. Future work should test this prospective model at different time points and with independent informants to avoid shared method variance concerns. In addition, our design may have missed important intervening changes in peer relations or child behavior as there were no assessments from roughly ages 5 to 12 years old. Although the missing and non-missing data did not differ on many variables, children in non-biological care were less likely to continue in the study, as it was more difficult to locate these families. Moreover, despite controlling for earlier levels of aggression, future models should test for changes in bullying and victimization as well as gun violence attitudes. More comprehensive models should also incorporate family, peer, school, and neighborhood influences. Although the examination of these attitudes is important, the actual behaviors of weapon carrying and use of guns to perpetuate violence are vital to assess in order to advance our understanding of how gun violence is developed and to guide prevention efforts.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the families who participated in this study, the research staff who collected and coded data, Amol Lele and Michael Ray for collaboration on data collection at the hospitals, and Claire Coles for collaboration on the larger study. Research reported in this article was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) under Awards R01DA013190 and R01DA041231. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH.

References

  1. Achenbach TM (1992). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/2–3 and 1992 profile Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont, Burlington, VT. [Google Scholar]
  2. Achenbach TM, & Rescorla LA (2001). Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families. [Google Scholar]
  3. Agnew R (2001). Building on the foundation of general strain theory: Specifying the types of strain most likely to lead to crime and delinquency. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 38, 319–361. doi: 10.1177/0022427801038004001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Agnew RS (2007). Strain theory and violent behavior. In Flannery DJ, Vazsonyi AT, & Waldman ID (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of violent behavior and aggression (pp. 519–529). New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  5. Ahmed E, & Braithwaite V (2004). “What, me ashamed?” Shame management and school bullying. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 41, 269–294. doi: 10.1177/0022427804266547 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Arbuckle JL (1996). Full information estimation in the presence of incomplete data. In Marcoulides GA and Schumacker RE (Eds.)., Advanced structural equation modeling: Issues and techniques (pp. 243–277). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bradshaw CP, Rodgers CRR, Ghandour LA, & Garbarino J (2009). Social–cognitive mediators of the association between community violence exposure and aggressive behavior. School Psychology Quarterly, 24, 199–210. doi: 10.1037/a0017362 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bronfenbrenner U (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychologist, 32, 513–531. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.32.7.513 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bryant WW (2011). Internalized racism’s affiliation with African American youth’s propensity for violence. Journal of Black Studies, 42, 690–707. doi: 10.1177/0021934710393243 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cook CR, Williams KR, Guerra NG, Kim TE, & Sadek S (2010). Predictors of bullying and victimization in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analytic investigation. School Psychology Quarterly, 25, 65–83. doi: 10.1037/a0020149 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Côté SM, Boivin M, Nagin DS, Japel C, Xu Q, Zoccolillo M, & Tremblay RE (2007). The role of maternal education and non-maternal care services in the prevention of children’s physical aggression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 1305–1312. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.11 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Crick NR, & Dodge KA (1996). Social information-processing mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development, 67, 993–1002. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01778.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dijkstra JK, Gest SD, Lindenberg S, Veenstra R, & Cillessen AH (2012). Testing three explanations of the emergence of weapon carrying in peer context: the roles of aggression, victimization, and the social network. Journal of Adolescent Health, 50, 371–376. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.08.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dijkstra JK, Lindenberg S, Veenstra R, Steglich C, Isaacs J, Card NA, & Hodges EVE (2010). Influence and selection processes in weapon carrying during adolescence: The roles of status, aggression, and vulnerability. Criminology, 48, 187–220. [Google Scholar]
  15. Dukes RL, Stein JA, & Zane JI (2010). Gender differences in the relative impact of physical and relational bullying on adolescent injury and weapon carrying. Journal of School Psychology, 48, 511–532. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2010.08.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Eiden RD, Coles CD, Schuetze P, & Colder CR (2014). Externalizing behavior problems among polydrug cocaine-exposed children: indirect pathways via maternal harshness and self-regulation in early childhood. Psychology Of Addictive Behaviors, 28, 139–153. doi: 10.1037/a0032632 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Eiden RD, Foote A, & Schuetze P (2007). Maternal cocaine use and caregiving status: group differences in caregiver and infant risk variables. Addictive Behaviors, 32, 465–476. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.05.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Eiden RD, Godleski S, Schuetze P, & Colder CR (2015). Prenatal substance exposure and child self-regulation: pathways to risk and protection. Journal Of Experimental Child Psychology, 137, 12–29. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.02.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Eiden RD, Schuetze P, Colder CR, & Veira Y (2011). Maternal cocaine use and mother-toddler aggression. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 33, 360–369. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2011.03.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Esselmont C (2014). Carrying a weapon to school: The roles of bullying victimization and perceived safety. Deviant Behavior, 35, 215–32. doi: 10.1080/01639625.2013.834767 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Ettekal I, Eiden R, Nickerson AB, Molnar DS, & Schuetze P (2019). doi: 10.1017/S095457941800144X [DOI]
  22. Felix ED, Sharkey JD, Green JG, Furlong MJ, & Tanigawa D (2011). Getting precise and pragmatic about the assessment of bullying: The development of the California Bullying Victimization Scale. Aggressive Behavior, 37, 234–247. doi: 10.1002/ab.20389 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fite PJ, Schwartz S, & Hendrickson M (2012). Childhood proactive and reactive aggression: Different risk for substance use? Aggression and Violent Behavior, 17, 240–246. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2012.02.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fite PJ, Vitulano M, Wynn P, Wimsatt A, Gaertner A, & Rathert J (2010). Influence of perceived safety on proactive and reactive aggression. Journal of Community Psychology, 38, 757–768. doi: 10.1002/jcop.20393 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Foster H, & Brooks-Gunn J (2009). Toward a stress process model of children’s exposure to physical family and community violence. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 12, 71–94. doi: 10.1007/s10567-009-0049-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Goldstein SE, Young A, & Boyd C (2008). Relational aggression at school: associations with school safety and social climate. Journal Of Youth and Adolescence, 37, 641–654. doi: 10.1007/s10964-007-9192-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Grych JH, & Swan SC (2012). Toward a more comprehensive understanding of interpersonal violence: Introduction to the special issue on interconnections among different types of violence. Psychology of Violence, 2, 105–110. doi: 10.1037/a0027616 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hart JL, O’Toole SK, Price-Sharps JL, & Shaffer TW (2007). Risk and protective factors of violent juvenile offending: An examination of gender differences. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 5, 367–384. doi: 10.1177/1541204006297367 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hase CN, Goldberg SB, Smith D, Stuck A, & Campain J (2015). Impacts of traditional bullying and cyberbullying on the mental health of middle school and high school students. Psychology in the Schools, 52, 607–617. doi: 10.1002/pits [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Houston J, & Grych J, (2016). Maternal attachment buffers the association between exposure to violence and youth attitudes about aggression. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 45, 605–613. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2014.987380 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Huizinga D, Esbensen F, & Weiher AW (1991). Are There Multiple Paths to Delinquency? The Journal Of Criminal Law & Criminology, 82, 83–118. [Google Scholar]
  32. Institute of Medicine and National Research Council (2013). Priorities for research to reduce the threat of firearm-related violence Washington, DC: Author. [Google Scholar]
  33. Linares TJ, Singer LT, Kirchner L, Short EJ, Min MO, Hussey P, & Minnes S (2006). Mental health outcomes of cocaine-exposed children at 6 years of age. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 31, 85–97. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsj020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lochman JE, Powell NP, Boxmeyer CL, & Jimenez-Camargo L (2011). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for externalizing disorders in children and adolescents. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 20, 305–318. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2011.01.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Loeber R, & Farrington DP (2000). Young children who commit crime: Epidemiology, development origins, risk factors, early interventions, and policy implications. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 737–762. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400004107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Mayes LC (2002). A behavioral teratogenic model of the impact of prenatal cocaine exposure on arousal regulatory systems. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 24, 385–395. doi: 10.1016/S0892-0362(02)00200-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Menting A, Orobio de Castroa B, & Matthys B (2013). Effectiveness of the Incredible Years parent training to modify disruptive and prosocial child behavior: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 33, 901–913. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Minnes S, Singer LT, Kirchner HL, Short E, Lewis B, Satayathum S, & Queh D (2010). The effects of prenatal cocaine exposure on problem behavior in children 4–10 years. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 32, 443–451. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2010.03.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Moffitt TE (1993). “Life-course-persistent” and “adolescence-limited” antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100, 674–701. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.100.4.674 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Moran L, Lengua LJ, Zalewski M, Ruberry E, Klein M, Thompson S, & Kiff C (2016). Variable-and person-centered approaches to examining temperament vulnerability and resilience to the effects of contextual risk. Journal of Research in Personality doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2016.03.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  41. Muthén LK, & Muthén BO (1998–2017). Mplus User’s Guide (Eighth ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen. [Google Scholar]
  42. Myers TDW, Salcido A, Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Steinberg L, & Cauffman E (2018). Understanding the link between exposure to violence and aggression in justice-involved adolescents. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 593–603. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417001134 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2004). Trajectories of physical aggression from toddlerhood to middle childhood: Predictors, correlates, and outcomes. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69 (4), vii, 1–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Nickerson AB (2019). Preventing and intervening with bullying in schools: A framework for evidence-based practice. School Mental Health, 11, 15–28. doi: 10.1007/s12310-017-9221-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Ostrov JM, & Godleski SA (2013). Relational aggression, victimization, and adjustment during middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 801–815. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000187 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Perren S, Ettekal I, & Ladd G (2013). The impact of peer victimization on later maladjustment: Mediating and moderating effects of hostile and self-blaming attributions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 46–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02618.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Posthumus JA, Raaijmakers M, Maassen G, van Engeland H, & Matthys WJ (2012). Sustained effects of Incredible Years as a preventive intervention in preschool children with conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 487–500. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9580-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Reid JA, Richards TN, Loughran TA, & Mulvey EP (2017). The relationships among exposure to violence, psychological distress, and gun carrying among male adolescents found guilty of serious legal offenses. Annals of Internal Medicine, 166, 412–418. doi: 10.7326/M16-164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Reynolds CR, & Kamphaus RW (1992). The Behavior Assessment System for Children Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. [Google Scholar]
  50. Richters JE, & Saltzman W (1990). Survey of exposure to community violence-parent report version Rockville, MD: National Institute of Mental Health. [Google Scholar]
  51. Rodkin PC, Espelage DL, & Hanish LD (2015). A relational framework for understanding bullying: Developmental antecedents and outcomes. American Psychologist, 70, 311–321. doi: 10.1037/a0038658 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Schafer JL, & Graham JW (2002). Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods, 7, 147–177. doi: 10.1037//1082-989X.7.2.147 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Schwartz D, Lansford JE, Dodge KA, Pettit GS, & Bates JE (2018). Peer victimization during middle childhood as a marker of attenuated risk for adult arrest. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46, 57–65. doi: 10.1007/s10802-017-0354-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Shapiro JP (2000). Attitudes Toward Guns and Violence Questionnaire: Manual Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
  55. Shapiro JP, Dorman RL, Burkey WM, Welker CJ, & Clough JB (1997). Development and factor analysis of a measure of youth attitudes towards guns and violence. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 26, 311–320. doi: 10.1177/088626099014011001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Sobell LC, & Sobell MB (1996). Alcohol timeline followback (TLFB) users’ manual Toronto, Canada: Addiction Research Foundation. [Google Scholar]
  57. Spano R (2012). First time gun carrying and the primary prevention of youth gun violence for African American youth living in extreme poverty. Aggressive and Violent Behavior, 17, 83–88. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2011.10.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  58. Spano R, & Bolland J (2013). Disentangling the effects of violent victimization, violent behavior, and gun-carrying for minority inner-city youth living in extreme poverty. Crime and Delinquency, 59, 191–213. doi: 10.1177/0011128710372196 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  59. Sturge-Apple M, Davies PT, Cicchetti D, Hentges RF, & Coe JL (2017). Family instability and children’s effortful control in the context of poverty: Sometimes a bird in the hand is worth two in the bush. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 685–696. doi: 10.1017/S0954579416000407 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sukhodolsky DG, Kassinove H, & Gorman BS (2004). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for anger in children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 9, 247–269. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2003.08.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  61. Tracy M, Braga AA, & Papachristos AV (2016). The transmission of gun and other weapon-involved violence within social networks. Epidemiologic Reviews, 38, 70–86. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxv009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. van Geel M, Vedder P, & Tanilon J (2014). Bullying and weapon carrying: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 168, 714–720. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.213 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Vitaro F, Brendgen M, Barker ED (2006). Subtypes of aggressive behaviors: A developmental perspective. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 12–19. doi: 10.1177/0165025406059968 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  64. Vossekuil B, Fein RA, Reddy M, Borum R, & Modzeleski W (2002). The final report and findings of the safe school initiative Washington, DC: U.S. Secret Service and U.S. Department of Education. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES