Abstract
The neurophysiological bases of mind wandering (MW)—an experiential state wherein attention is disengaged from the external environment in favour of internal thoughts—and state meta‐awareness are poorly understood. In parallel, the relationship between introspection confidence in experiential state judgements and neural representations remains unclear. Here, we recorded EEG while participants completed a listening task within which they made experiential state judgements and rated their confidence. Alpha power was reliably greater during MW episodes, with unaware MW further associated with greater delta and theta power. Multivariate pattern classification analysis revealed that MW and meta‐awareness can be decoded from the distribution of power in these three frequency bands. Critically, we show that individual decoding accuracies positively correlate with introspection confidence. Our results reaffirm the role of alpha oscillations in MW, implicate lower frequencies in meta‐awareness, and are consistent with the proposal that introspection confidence indexes neurophysiological discriminability of representational states.
Keywords: confidence, EEG, meta‐awareness, mind wandering, support vector machine
The neurophysiological bases of mind wandering (MW) and state meta‐awareness are poorly understood. We recorded EEG while participants completed a listening task within which they made experiential state judgments and rated their confidence. Multivariate pattern classification analysis revealed that MW and meta‐awareness can be decoded from the distribution of power in delta, theta, and alpha frequency bands. Critically, we show that individual decoding accuracies positively correlate with introspection confidence. Our results reaffirm the role of alpha oscillations in MW, implicate lower frequencies in meta‐awareness, and are consistent with the proposal that introspection confidence indexes neurophysiological discriminability of representational states.
1. INTRODUCTION
Our brains are constantly bombarded by dynamic sensory input, yet we frequently shift away from the external environment toward thoughts, emotions, and images that do not emerge from ongoing perceptual processes, are self‐generated and unrelated to one's current task (mind wandering; (Antrobus, Singer, Goldstein, & Fortgang, 1970; Christoff, Gordon, Smallwood, Smith, & Schooler, 2009; Smallwood & Schooler, 2015). Research suggests that mind wandering (MW) occurs in ~30–50% of our waking hours (Killingsworth & Gilbert, 2010; McVay, Kane, & Kwapil, 2009) and has deleterious effects on sensory and cognitive processing with corresponding reductions in event‐related potentials (ERP) in response to external stimuli (Baird, Smallwood, Lutz, & Schooler, 2014; Barron, Riby, Greer, & Smallwood, 2011; Kam et al., 2011; Smallwood, Beach, Schooler, & Handy, 2008). These effects produce concomitant negative effects in a variety of tasks from driving (Geden, Staicu, & Feng, 2018) to reading (Feng, D'Mello, & Graesser, 2013; Hollis & Was, 2016; Schooler, Reichle, & Halpern, 2004).
Converging findings implicate default mode network (DMN; Fox et al., 2005; Raichle et al., 2001) in mind wandering (Christoff et al., 2009) and its associated cognitive operations such as self‐related processing (Fingelkurts & Fingelkurts, 2011), autobiographical memory, theory of mind, and future planning (Andrews‐Hanna, 2012; Andrews‐Hanna, Smallwood, & Spreng, 2014; Spreng & Grady, 2010). Mind wandering episodes can occur with (tuning‐out) and without (zoning‐out) meta‐awareness (Schooler et al., 2004, 2011; Smallwood, McSpadden, & Schooler, 2007) with the latter suggested to reflect a more pronounced form of mind wandering characterised by poorer performance (Smallwood, McSpadden, & Schooler, 2007) and greater recruitment of DMN and executive control network (Christoff et al., 2009).
Despite advances in its network architecture, the patterns of oscillatory activity underpinning mind wandering are poorly understood (Martinon, Smallwood, McGann, Hamilton, & Riby, 2019). Multiple studies indicate that mind wandering states (Boudewyn & Carter, 2018; Compton, Gearinger, & Wild, 2019; Groot et al., 2021; Macdonald, Mathan, & Yeung, 2011), and particularly zoning‐out (unaware mind wandering) (Boudewyn & Carter, 2018), are characterised by elevated alpha (~8–12 Hz) power. Alpha oscillations are suggested to support inhibition‐related processes (Klimesch, 2012; Palva & Palva, 2007) and attentional suppression (Foxe & Snyder, 2011), and are further implicated during working memory and mental imagery tasks (Von Stein & Sarnthein, 2000), internally oriented brain states (Cooper, Croft, Dominey, Burgess, & Gruzelier, 2003; Hanslmayr, Gross, Klimesch, & Shapiro, 2011), and inner speech (Villena‐González, López, & Rodríguez, 2016), all of which figure prominently in the experience of mind wandering. However, at least two studies failed to replicate these effects (Braboszcz & Delorme, 2011; van Son et al., 2019) and observed greater delta (~2–3 Hz) and theta (~4–7 Hz) power during mind wandering. Activity in these frequency bands may reflect lapses in sustained attention, redirection of attention from external stimuli to internal representations and, or the maintenance of these representations in awareness. For example, slow‐wave brain oscillations are typically associated with decreased sustained task‐related attention (Klimesch, 1999). Delta frequency contributions have also been shown during increased focus on internal processing and pertinent inhibition of interference (Harmony, 2013) whereas theta activity has been consistently shown to relate to maintenance of information in working memory (Klimesch, 1999; Mitchell, McNaughton, Flanagan, & Kirk, 2008). Discrepancies in the observed association between alpha and mind wandering are plausibly attributed to the tasks and methods used in the aforementioned studies (Braboszcz & Delorme, 2011; van Son et al., 2019). That is, lower alpha activity during mind wandering episodes in some studies might be due to the concurrent task (breath‐counting) involving internally‐focused attention and counting (Palva, Palva, & Kaila, 2005; Sauseng et al., 2005). In parallel, it is difficult to compare findings between self‐reports that are prompted (probe‐caught) and the above studies due to the latter using self‐caught measures (participants are asked to indicate when they catch themselves mind wandering), which likely capture shifts from internal to external focus that probably involve different mechanisms to the occurrence of mind wandering.
Measuring the neurophysiology of self‐generated thoughts requires experience sampling methods in which participants report on aspects of their experience, thereby affording a prominent role to introspective abilities in the assessment of mind wandering (Smallwood & Schooler, 2006). A neglected feature of these abilities within the context of mind wandering is confidence in these introspective reports. Emerging evidence suggests that confidence reflects variability in access to experiential states (Fleming & Lau, 2014; Seli, Jonker, Cheyne, Cortes, & Smilek, 2015) and thus is likely to be highly informative in elucidating variability in the phenomenology and neurophysiology of mind wandering episodes. Confidence in perceptual judgements (Fleming & Lau, 2014) positively correlates with decision accuracy (Gherman & Philiastides, 2018; Kunimoto, Miller, & Pashler, 2001; Morgan, Mason, & Solomon, 1997) and reliably tracks ERP dynamics related to error (Boldt & Yeung, 2015) and sensory processing (e.g., Zakrzewski, Wisniewski, Iyer, & Simpson, 2019). Confidence in mind wandering reports has to date been neglected but preliminary work has shown that it varies greatly within and between individuals and moderates the relationship between response time variability and self‐reports of mind wandering (Seli et al., 2015) (but see Meier, 2018). Nevertheless, the neurophysiology of these effects is unknown.
One possibility is that if confidence reflects superior access to experiential states, high confidence would be associated with more clearly dissociable neural representations. Multivariate pattern classification analysis (MVPC) has been successfully used to identify the mapping between distributed patterns of neural activity and corresponding mental states (Bae & Luck, 2018; Haxby et al., 2001; Haynes & Rees, 2006; Jin, Borst, & van Vugt, 2019; Mittner et al., 2014). An advantage of MVPC is that it allows researchers to assess whether shared information across multiple features (e.g., channels, frequency, time points) encodes class‐related information (Haxby et al., 2001). Using this method, recent research has revealed associations between decoding accuracy and individual differences in perceptual discrimination (Kim et al., 2015) and intra‐individual variability in confidence (Weaver, Fahrenfort, Belopolsky, & Van Gaal, 2019). The extent to which experiential states can be decoded, reflecting multivariate dissimilarity of neural representations, may thus underlie confidence in the corresponding mental representations.
The present study investigated the oscillatory dynamics of experiential states using an ecological task lacking performance indicators (Smallwood, Nind, & O'Connor, 2009) in order to examine the neurophysiological basis of mind wandering, dissociate meta‐awareness of mind wandering (henceforth state meta‐awareness) and investigate the neurophysiological implications of participants' confidence in self‐reports. During concurrent EEG recording, participants listened to an audiobook and were intermittently probed regarding their experiential state and state meta‐awareness and rated their confidence in both judgements. We expected that mind wandering would be characterised by elevated alpha power (Compton et al., 2019) whereas unaware mind wandering would additionally be associated with differential power in slow oscillatory bands (delta, theta). Motivated by previous research showing that joint activity patterns across different features (e.g., frequency bands) can be more informative of mental representations than univariate information (Allefeld & Haynes, 2015), we then assessed whether distributed information across patterns of EEG spectral features could be used to decode different experiential states using MVPC. These analyses were further guided by our aim to evaluate the hypothesis that introspection confidence in experiential states reflects higher dissimilarity of the underlying neural representations.
2. MATERIALS AND METHODS
2.1. Participants
Forty‐six right‐handed participants (28 females, age range: 18–43, M Age = 25.9, SD = 5.7; years of education [post‐secondary school]: M Yoe = 4.3, SD = 2.2) with normal or corrected‐to‐normal vision provided written informed consent to volunteer in the study and were compensated £10 per hour. A sample size of 40 allowed us to detect paired‐samples effects of d ≥ .45 (α = .05, 1‐β = .80, two‐tailed). We recruited 46 participants due to potential attrition and loss of participants because of insufficient numbers of trials for the different state responses. All participants self‐reported proficiency in English (1 = no proficiency, to 10 = native speaker; M = 9.2, SD = 1.04). Seven participants were excluded due to technical issues during EEG data recording (n = 1), or insufficient number of response types in the task (n = 6; see section 2.5), resulting in a final sample of 39 participants (25 females, age range: 18–43, M Age = 25.6, SD = 5.8, English language skills: [M = 9.2%, SD = 1.0]). The study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Department of Psychology at Goldsmiths, University of London.
2.2. Materials
2.2.1. Audiobook listening task
This task consisted of participants listening to an audio version of Bill Bryson's A Short History of Nearly Everything (Bryson, 2004), a general science book that has previously been used in mind wandering research (e.g., Smallwood, Nind, et al. 2009). Participants focused on a central white fixation cross on a grey background at a distance of approximately 90 cm and listened (through speakers) to the audiobook in three 20 min blocks (corresponding to chapters 7, 24, and 30 in counterbalanced order). During the task, participants were prompted via on‐screen thought probes at pseudorandom intervals (30, 40, or 50s) to report on their experiential state: “Just before the probe, were you mind wandering?” (ES Judgement, response options: yes, no). If a participant responded in the affirmative, they were next prompted regarding state meta‐awareness: “Just before the probe, were you zoning‐out or tuning‐out?” (MA Judgement, response options: tuning‐out, zoning‐out). Participants responded to both probes using a continuous visual analogue scale in which they made their binary judgement combined with an estimate of confidence in their response (ranging from completely not confident, to completely confident). Response options for both probes alternated sides randomly to control for response biases.
2.2.2. Audiobook listening assessment
A sequence of 20 true/false questions (corresponding to the content of the preceding block) were administered to participants after each block. Question order followed the presentation order of the information in the audiobook with each question corresponding to approximately 1 min of content.
2.3. Procedure
After EEG preparation and general instructions, participants completed a battery of psychometric measures (to be reported elsewhere). Participants sat in a dimly lit room and first underwent a 5‐min eyes‐open resting state condition in which they focused on a central white fixation cross (1 cm2) while their EEG was recorded and subsequently completed a self‐report resting state measure (to be reported elsewhere).
Prior to completing the task, mind wandering was defined to participants as any thoughts that are not related to the material being presented (Christoff et al., 2009; Lindquist & McLean, 2011), and are usually internally focused. Participants were provided with examples of mind wandering, such as thoughts about past events, friends or significant others or concerns about an upcoming exam (Varao Sousa, Carriere, & Smilek, 2013). Tuning‐out was defined as a state in which one mind wanders and is aware while they are doing so whereas zoning‐out was defined as a state in which one mind wanders and is unaware that they are doing so until they “catch” themselves.
The experimenter introduced the audiobook listening task and defined the response options in the ES and MA judgements and ensured that participants understood these options and the response scale. Participants completed a 3 min training block followed by three 20 min experimental blocks (30 probes per block), resulting in 90 probes in total. After each block, participants completed the audiobook listening assessment. Blocks took approximately 25 min to complete with the entire experiment lasting approximately 2.5 hr. The experiment was programmed and implemented in MATLAB 2018a (The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA), using the Psychophysics Toolbox extensions (Kleiner et al., 2007).
2.4. Behavioural analyses
Participants' data were segregated at the probe‐level according to self‐report in two ways for separate analyses: dichotomously (on‐task vs. mind wandering) and trichotomously (on‐task vs. tuning‐out vs. zoning‐out). Frequency (%) of each state report and performance on the audiobook listening assessment (accuracy [%]) were additionally computed at the block‐level.
2.5. Electrophysiological data acquisition and analyses
EEG signals were recorded using a 64‐Ag‐AgCl electrode Biosemi ActiveTwo system. Electrodes were placed according to the International 10‐20 system. Two electrodes placed on the participants' earlobes were used as reference. Additional electrodes recorded right side vertical (VEOG) and bilateral horizontal (HEOG) electro‐oculogram signals to be used for artefact detection and rejection. The recording was sampled at 512 Hz for all participants.
Data preprocessing was implemented using the EEGlab toolbox in MATLAB (Delorme & Makeig, 2004). The average of the two earlobe electrodes was used as reference and the data were subsequently filtered with a high‐pass filter at 0.5 Hz and a notch‐filter between 48 and 52 Hz. We used the pop_eegfiltnew function in EEGlab, which applies a finite impulse response (FIR) filter to the data using an automatic filter order (3,380 and 846, respectively). Bad electrodes (range: 0–2 across participants) detected during the recording, or via visual inspection of the raw data, were removed. Next, independent component analysis (ICA) was performed on the continuous data to detect eye‐movement artefacts. IC scalp maps, spectra and raw activity were visually inspected to reject further artefacts (range: 1–5, M = 3.15, SD = 1.48) such as eye movements, further channel noise, and prominent muscle movements. Next, data from removed electrodes were replaced using spherical interpolation and all data were re‐referenced using the average of the 64 channels.
For each participant, continuous data were next segmented into 14 s epochs: −12 to 2 s relative to probe onset. The time window of interest extended from −10 to 0 s, but an additional 2 s were included on each side to avoid edge artefacts in subsequent analyses; these data were omitted after time‐frequency transformation. Further epoch exclusion was conducted via manual rejection based on visual inspection. Data were subsequently segregated into two conditions corresponding to the experiential states reported by the participants: on‐task vs. mind wandering. Mind wandering states were further partitioned into two meta‐awareness states: tuning‐out vs. zoning‐out. A 700‐ms epoch during the interstimulus interval after the probe response phase and start of the next trial was used as a baseline. A time‐frequency transformation of the data was implemented by applying a Hanning window at 50 ms steps to each 14 s‐long epoch and corresponding baseline segment for frequencies of 1–45 Hz. Window length varied along the frequency dimension, with 7,000 ms at the lowest frequency (1 Hz) decreasing linearly (time window = 7/frequency) at each frequency bin, and 150 ms for 45 Hz. Trial‐wise spectral power was averaged and then normalised by division of the baseline level (gain model; Grandchamp & Delorme, 2011).
Participants varied in their mind wandering and state meta‐awareness reports, resulting in different sample sizes and respective numbers of trials per state for each comparison. State‐specific data that included fewer than 10% of probes (9 probes) were excluded from any analyses involving the respective state. Four main contrasts were implemented with variable trials (M ± SD) and sample sizes (N). After artefact removal and participant exclusions the number of trials per state for each of the four contrasts were: (i) on‐task (39.7 ± 15.0) vs. mind wandering (27.4 ± 12.0) [N = 39]; (ii) on‐task (36.0 ± 13.0) vs. tuning‐out (20.5 ± 9.8) [N = 27]; (iii) on‐task (35.5 ± 13.7) vs. zoning‐out (14.1 ± 5.4) [N = 25]; and (iv) tuning‐out (22.0 ± 13.4) vs. zoning‐out (13.3 ± 4.0) [N = 21]. Insofar as the number of trials differed across states, we performed a series of control analyses in which significant contrasts were repeated with closer matching of the number of trials; these yielded similar results (see Control Analyses in Data S1, Supporting Information).
2.6. Multivariate pattern classification analysis
2.6.1. Time‐varying MVPC
Complementing the univariate analysis of spectral power differences, we implemented a two‐class MVPC for the four aforementioned two‐state contrasts. Here, we hypothesised that information about the different experiential states would be shared across different frequency bands and electrodes. Insofar as this information could unfold over time, we used time‐varying MVPC to investigate whether subjective reports about experiential states could be decoded from trial‐wise EEG patterns of oscillatory activity across different frequency bands, and separately for different time points.
The features used for the decoding analyses were the trial‐wise measures of spectral power for delta, theta, and alpha frequency bands for each of the 64 EEG channels. After preprocessing the data (see section 2.5), a time‐frequency transformation was implemented in the same way as in spectral analysis however data were neither baselined nor collapsed across trials. The spectral power values were then averaged separately for each frequency band (delta [2–3 Hz], theta [4–7 Hz], alpha [8–13 Hz]). For the time‐varying MVPC, these measures were additionally averaged in bins of 500 ms within 10s epochs, resulting in 20 time bins. A linear support vector machine (SVM library for MATLAB; Lotte, Congedo, Lécuyer, Lamarche, & Arnaldi, 2007; Chang & Lin, 2011) was trained to distinguish between classes (states) at each time bin. In order to examine whether single‐frequency classification was superior or comparable to cross‐frequency classification (multifrequency model), we further performed MVPC analyses separately for each frequency band (reduced feature space). The multifrequency model used data with 192 (64 electrodes × 3 frequency bands) dimensions for each trial, while the three single‐frequency models (delta, theta, alpha) were comprised of 64‐dimensional data per trial. Each of the implemented two‐class MVPC analyses were balanced by matching the number of trials in each class using random trial selection. The matching of trials resulted in the following number of trials (M ± SD) per class for each two‐class decoding: (i) on‐task vs. mind wandering (23.4 ± 9.0, N = 39); (ii) on‐task vs. tuning‐out (18.8 ± 8.2, N = 27); (iii) on‐task vs. zoning‐out (13.9 ± 5.1, N = 25); and (iv) tuning‐out vs. zoning‐out (12.6 ± 3.6, N = 21). MVPC was implemented using a threefold leave‐one‐out cross‐validation procedure to assess the effectiveness of the model. The individual empirical decoding accuracy was the average accuracy across the three folds. Details on the participant‐ and group‐level statistical analyses on time‐varying MVPC decoding accuracies are described in section 2.7.3.
2.6.2. Time‐averaged MVPC
An important consideration is that, although mind wandering processes could unfold over time, they are inherently not time‐locked, and indeed our experimental design was not event‐related. To account for the case that there would not be a reliable temporal representation of mind wandering states across trials, we further complemented the time‐varying MVPC with a time‐averaged MVPC approach. We collapsed the spectral power measures across time to assess whether we could still decode classes from the same features. An additional advantage of this second approach is that using time‐averaged measures of spectral power in MVPC reduces the number of multiple comparisons. Spectral power at each time point was averaged across the whole epoch (−10 to 0 s before probe onset). Similar to the time‐varying analyses, the time‐averaged MVPC was performed using the spectral power values for each frequency band and for the set of 64 channels as features. This was performed separately for the multifrequency model (192 dimensions) and for each frequency band of interest (single‐frequency models: 64 dimensions). This was carried out 11 times after partitioning the data into balanced classes 11 times, which aimed to obtain accuracy estimates more representative of the total trial sample. Classification parameters and trial amounts per class used for each two‐class time‐averaged MVPC were the same with time‐varying MVPC (see section 2.6.1). The individual empirical decoding accuracy was the average accuracy across the three folds and 11 analyses. Details on the participant‐ and group‐level statistical analyses on time‐averaged MVPC decoding accuracies are described in section 2.7.3).
The general time‐averaged MVPC was complemented with a method that allows assessing the activation patterns that describe the contribution of each channel or frequency band to the decoding of class‐related information (Haufe et al., 2014). In the case of linear classifiers, we can obtain an estimate of the activation patterns A by projecting the extraction filters of the classifier, W, onto the channels as follows: A ∝ ∑xW, where ∑x is the covariance matrix of the data. This calculation can be simplified by computing the covariance between the data ( x ) and hidden factors ( y , vector of labels of each class): A ∝ ∑xW = Cov[ x (n), y (n)], where n is the number of trials (Haufe et al., 2014). The covariance Cov[ x (n), y (n)] is computed for each pattern dimension (i.e., each channel or frequency band) across the total number of training trials n, and separately for each fold (followed up by an average across folds). The amplitude and sign of the covariance matrix Cov[ x (n), y (n)] can be interpreted as the strength and polarity with which class‐related information is reflected in each pattern dimension (channel or frequency band). The interpretation of the polarity depends on the order in which data from each class is concatenated for MVPC analysis and pattern extraction: a positive polarity in a pattern dimension reflects that the activity (e.g., theta power) is greater in class 1 than in class 2 for that specific dimension (channel). In the results, we specify the order in which the class‐related data were concatenated for each analysis. Importantly, MVPC is a multivariate analysis method, and thus drawing inferences from the pattern amplitude (strength) to describe univariate effects (e.g., in individual channels) should be avoided. For multifrequency models, we averaged the activation maps across frequency bands, to obtain a single topographic representation across 64 channels. The activation patterns associated with each frequency range were assessed using the single‐frequency models.
Finally, we conducted correlation analyses between decoding accuracies derived from our time‐averaged models and participants' confidence in their respective reports. Correlation analyses were performed exclusively in a subset of the models, based on our EEG findings and previous research (see section 2.7.3).
2.7. Statistical analyses
2.7.1. Behavioural data
Confidence ratings between states were compared with paired‐samples t tests (2‐tailed) and assessment performance was compared to 50% using a one‐sample t test. Associations between task‐level mind wandering frequency and accuracy in the task were assessed by correlational analyses following automatic bivariate outlier (boxplot method) removal using the Robust Correlation toolbox in MATLAB (Pernet, Wilcox, & Rousselet, 2013) in the computation of skipped correlations (Rousseeuw, 1984; Rousseeuw & Van Driessen, 1999; Verboven & Hubert, 2005). We report Spearman's r s for data that violated parametric test assumptions.
2.7.2. Spectral analysis
Significant differences in the spectral power between different states were assessed by means of cluster‐based paired permutation tests across participants (Maris & Oostenveld, 2007). Nonparametric cluster permutation tests were used separately for prespecified oscillatory frequency bins (delta [2–3 Hz], theta [4–7 Hz], alpha [8–13 Hz]). These analyses were undertaken in two phases. First, we calculated the observed test statistic for the respective contrast by: (i) conducting paired‐samples t tests comparing the two states at each data sample (frequency × channel × time); (ii) samples whose t‐values were below threshold (α < .05) were selected and clustered in sets based on feature adjacency (spectral, spatial, and temporal); and (iii) t‐values were summed to compute cluster‐level statistics whose maximum served as the test statistic to evaluate state differences. The second phase entailed the same steps but this time the test statistic was computed for 500 permutations of randomly partitioned data in two subsets (Monte Carlo permutation test). These test statistics were compared to the observed test statistic (Maris & Oostenveld, 2007). The cluster‐level significance value was set at two‐tailed α < .025 with a minimum of two neighbouring channels constituting a cluster. This method controls for multiple comparisons by controlling the family‐wise error rate at α = .05. We interpret effects in the range of .025 < p < .030 as reflecting trends and p‐values greater than .030 as reflecting nonsignificant effects. For nonsignificant findings, we report p‐values for the most prominent cluster. The analyses were conducted with the Fieldtrip toolbox in MATLAB (Maris & Oostenveld, 2007). Effect sizes were estimated using Hedges's g and bootstrap 95% confidence intervals (CI, bias‐corrected and accelerated method, 10,000 samples; Efron, 1987), on power averages across frequency, time window, and electrode sites identified by cluster analyses, using the Measures of Effect Size Toolbox in MATLAB (Hentschke & Stüttgen, 2011).
2.7.3. MVPC analyses
Statistical inference for the time‐varying MVPC analyses was initially performed at the participant level, followed by group‐level analysis. At the participant level, the null distribution for accuracy was generated by performing MVPC 500 times after randomly shuffling the class labels in the data. P‐values were computed at each time bin as the proportions (%) of permutation accuracies that are greater than or equal to the observed decoding accuracy (mean accuracy of threefold cross‐validation), yielding one p‐value per time bin. We report the proportion of participants for which we observed significant decoding in at least one time bin. Next, at the group level, we aimed to identify the time bins showing statistically significant above‐chance decoding accuracy, using a pairwise permutation test (Monte Carlo permutation test, 5,000 iterations). Both at the participant‐ and group‐ levels, we corrected for multiple comparisons by controlling the false discovery rate (FDR) at .05 by using an adaptive two‐stage linear step‐up procedure (Benjamini, Krieger, & Yekutieli, 2006). At the group‐level, the corrected threshold p‐value obtained from this procedure, pth, is given when multiple comparisons were performed.
Time‐averaged MVPC analyses were performed in the same manner but were limited to a single averaged time bin and repeated 11 times. At the participant level, the null distribution for accuracy was computed by performing the analysis 500 times after randomly shuffling the class labels in the data. For each of the 11 repetitions, p‐values were computed as the proportions (%) of permutation accuracies that are greater than or equal to the observed decoding accuracy (mean accuracy of threefold cross‐validation), yielding one p‐value per participant for each repetition. We report the range and mean percentage of participants across the 11 repetitions for which we observed significant (p < .05) decoding for each model. We then obtained the average empirical decoding accuracy per participant across the 11 analyses and used the results to conduct statistical analyses at the group level. Here we used as chance level the average accuracy of the permutation distributions (11 analyses) in each participant. Statistical assessment with a permutation test was performed by comparing these group mean accuracies and the estimated null distribution (Monte Carlo permutation test, 5,000 permutations) to identify statistically significant decoding accuracy across participants at p < .05. Different models were compared using the Monte Carlo approach described above (paired permutation test).
Finally, we assessed associations between mean participant‐level decoding accuracies (averaged across the 11 repetitions) and participants' corresponding mean confidence ratings. Toward this end, confidence in ES and MA Judgements were averaged within participants for each state in order to obtain the average confidence for each class. These were then correlated at the group‐level with decoding accuracies of the respective classification. To minimise the family‐wise error rate, we selected a small number of models for this analysis based on the previous literature and our EEG data. For decoding of mind wandering vs. on‐task states and tuning‐out vs. on‐task states, we examined the associations between decoding accuracies in the multifrequency model and confidence ratings. For zoning‐out vs. on‐task states and zoning‐out vs. tuning‐out decoding we examined correlations between confidence ratings and the delta and theta models' decoding accuracies. As described above, bivariate outliers were removed in the computation of skipped Pearson correlations (Rousseeuw, 1984; Rousseeuw & Van Driessen, 1999; Verboven & Hubert, 2005). For discrepancies between 95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) and p‐values, correlations are interpreted as nonsignificant.
3. RESULTS
3.1. Characteristics of mind wandering, state meta‐awareness, and introspection confidence
During the audiobook listening task, participants (N = 39) reported mind wandering (M% ± SD) on 39.7 ± 17.9 of the probes, with stable rates across blocks (block 1: 41.2 ± 20.6; block 2: 40.9 ± 21.6; block 3: 36.9 ± 19.0). Among mind wandering states, participants reported tuning‐out (56.6 ± 19.0) more often than zoning‐out (43.4 ± 19.0). Participants varied (range, M% ± SD) in their confidence for ES judgements (14.8–91.4, 61.7 ± 7.7), and displayed less confidence in mind wandering reports (11.3–90.0, 53.2 ± 19.9), than in on‐task reports (8.6–94.2, 64.4 ± 19.0), t(38) = 4.78, p < .001, g = .57, [0.32 0.90]. Specifically, on‐task reports were rated with significantly higher confidence than both tune‐out (t(38) = 2.39, p = .02, g = .34, [0.08 0.67]) and zone‐out reports (t(38) = 3.80, p < .001, g = .58, [0.28 0.96]). Participants were moderately confident in their MA judgements during mind wandering states (16.7–92.0, 58.1 ± 6.3), with numerically, albeit nonsignificantly, greater confidence in tune‐out (17.2–92.3, 58.0 ± 18.8) than in zone‐out (6.9–90.5, 52.3 ± 21.8) reports, t(38) = 1.55, p = .13, g = .27, [−0.06 0.64]. These rates are similar to previous research (Christoff et al., 2009; Seli et al., 2015; Varao Sousa et al., 2013) and demonstrate variability in experiential states, state meta‐awareness, and introspection confidence during the task.
3.2. Audiobook listening assessment and mind wandering frequency
Accuracy on the assessment averaged across blocks (M% ± SD: 74.5 ± 10.0) was above chance performance (50%, one sample t‐test: t(38) = 15.3, p < .001, g = 2.44, [1.84, 3.59]). Performance was comparable across blocks (block 1: 72.3 ± 12.0, block 2: 74.6 ± 14.4, block 3: 76.5 ± 12.1), suggesting stable motivation throughout the task. Mind wandering frequency reliably significantly correlated negatively with assessment accuracy in the last two blocks (block 1: r = −.29 [95% CI: −0.53, 0.02], p = .079 (p = .017, with outliers), block 2: r s = −.43 [−0.68, −0.10], p = .009 (p < .001, with outliers), block 3: r = −.41 [−0.68, −0.11], p = .011 (p = .011, with outliers), thereby providing an indirect behavioural validation of participants' self‐reports and corroborating previous research (Boudewyn & Carter, 2018; Schooler et al., 2004).
3.3. Oscillatory characteristics of mind wandering and state meta‐awareness
As expected, the cluster‐based permutation test revealed greater alpha power during mind wandering than on‐task states (Figure 1). The analysis revealed two temporally‐adjacent clusters just prior to probe onset, p = .004, g = 0.56 [0.32, 0.92]; p = .024, g = 0.50 [0.29, 0.80]. Both effects were topographically diffuse and most pronounced over bilateral fronto‐central and right posterior sites. Similarly, we observed greater alpha power during tuning‐out than on‐task states in a single cluster, p = .008, g = 0.66 [0.37, 1.05] (Figure S1). This effect was also close to probe onset and was primarily observed over fronto‐central and parieto‐occipital regions. There were no other significant differences between states in the other frequency bands (Figure S2): for on‐task vs. mind wandering, the analysis did not yield any significant clusters for delta, p = .24, or theta power, p = .40. Similarly, delta and theta power did not significantly differ between tuning‐out and on‐task states (Figure S2, delta: p = .23; theta: p = .20).
FIGURE 1.
Oscillatory differences between states (MW – on‐task, N = 39) as a function of time relative to probe onset (0 s). (a) Time‐frequency decomposition averaged across electrode sites. Broken black rectangles denote spectrotemporal clusters reflecting significant state differences (p < .025, two‐sided cluster‐based permutation test). (b) Alpha (8–13 Hz) spectral power averaged over the electrode sites of the two clusters (significance denoted by black bars on the x‐axis). (c) Topography of the clusters at different 500/550 ms subwindows (black markers denote electrodes that were present on at least 50% of samples in each time window). MW = mind wandering
In line with the foregoing results, zoning‐out (unaware mind wandering) states were characterised by greater power than on‐task states in delta, theta, and alpha bands in two distinct time windows (Figure 2). Alpha power was greater for zoning‐out than on‐task states in a short interval early in the epoch, p = .004, g = 0.75 [0.51, 1.06], and a long interval just prior to probe onset, p = .002, g = 0.86 [0.57, 1.28]. Both effects were larger in magnitude than the comparisons between on‐task and mind wandering and tuning‐out states and topographically diffuse but strongest over bilateral frontal and posterior sites. Similarly, theta power was greater in zoning‐out than on‐task states in a single window overlapping with the late alpha effect, p = .010, g = 0.83 [0.54, 1.24]; this effect was larger in right frontal electrodes but over time shifted to left temporo‐parietal sites. A second theta band cluster overlapped in time with the early alpha cluster but did not achieve significance despite a large effect size, p = .028, g = 0.76 [0.50, 1.14]. Zoning‐out states were also associated with greater delta power than on‐task states in two clusters that were temporally coincident with the foregoing effects but with substantially larger effect sizes, p = .018, g = 1.43 [1.04, 2.16]; p = .018, g = 1.16 [0.74 1.73]. These effects were topographically more focal and largely restricted to midline central electrodes.
FIGURE 2.
Oscillatory differences between states (zoning‐out – on‐task, N = 25) as a function of time relative to probe onset (0 s). (a) Time‐frequency decomposition averaged across electrode sites. Broken black rectangles indicate spectrotemporal clusters reflecting a significant difference (p < .025, two‐sided cluster‐based permutation test) and the grey rectangle indicates a trend‐level (.025 < p < .05) cluster. (b–d) Alpha (8–13 Hz), theta (4–7 Hz) and delta (2–3 Hz) spectral power averaged over the electrode sites of the clusters (black bars = significant, grey bar = trend). (e) Topography of the clusters at different subwindows within the cluster (black markers denote electrodes that were present on at least 50% of samples in each time window, white electrodes mark topography of the trend‐level effect)
Zoning‐out states were also characterised by greater theta power than tuning‐out states in a single cluster close to probe onset at a trend‐level of significance, p = .026, g = 0.86 [0.45 1.41] (Figure S3). This effect was most pronounced in parieto‐central electrodes. Failure to reach significance is plausibly due to the reduced sample for this analysis (N = 21) due to MW trial partitioning. There were no other significant effects in the other frequency bands (Figure S2). Specifically, tuning‐out and zoning‐out states did not differ in delta (p = .05) or alpha power (p = .16). Similarly, exploratory analyses of beta power did not yield significant differences between any ES or MA states (see Figure S4).
3.4. Multivariate pattern classification analyses
The univariate analyses of the EEG signals managed to identify spectral and spatiotemporal differences between states. This analysis, however, failed to demonstrate a robust and significant difference between aware and unaware mind wandering. In the following, we present the results for two sets of analyses: the time‐varying MVPC analyses performed in the epoch before probe onset (−10 to 0 s; 20 bins of 500 ms) and the time‐averaged MVPC analyses performed after collapsing temporal information for decoding across the entire epoch.
3.4.1. Time‐varying MVPC
For each two‐class (state) comparison, we tested four models that used as features the spectral power in the 64 channels in the delta, theta and alpha frequency bands (multifrequency model) and in these three bands separately (single‐frequency band models). At the participant‐level, we report the percentage of participants for which significant decoding was obtained in at least one bin.
The multifrequency model (trials per class: 23.4 ± 9.0) significantly decoded mind wandering from on‐task states in 26% of participants, with similar proportions in single‐frequency models: delta: 23%; theta: 15%; and alpha: 26% (Figure S5a). At the group level, both the multifrequency and the single‐frequency models were able to decode mind wandering and on‐task states with significant accuracies observed at various bins in the window (Figure 3a). Similarly, the multifrequency model (trials per class: 18.8 ± 8.2) significantly decoded on‐task from tuning‐out states in 22% of participants although the single‐frequency models displayed superior decoding: delta: 44%; theta: 30%; alpha: 33% (Figure S5b). Group‐level classification was significant in four bins in the multifrequency model with similar results for theta and alpha, but not delta (Figure 3b). Significant decoding accuracy between zoning‐out and on‐task states (trials per class: 13.9 ± 5.1) was observed in 24% of participants for the multifrequency model, with comparable or superior accuracy in the single‐frequency models (delta: 44%; theta: 36%; alpha: 24%; Figure S5c). Group‐level significant decoding was found for the multifrequency model and the single‐frequency models (Figure 3c). The theta and delta effects were spread throughout the window with the delta model exhibiting significant decoding accuracy across the full window. Tuning‐out and zoning‐out states (trials per class: 12.6 ± 3.6) were decoded in 14% of participants in the multifrequency model, with superior decoding in the single‐frequency models (delta: 24%; theta: 19%; alpha: 29%, Figure S5d). At the group‐level, the multifrequency and single‐frequency models showed significant decoding although this was restricted to one bin in the former and generalised throughout the window in the latter (Figure 3d). These results indicate that decoding for all two‐class comparisons was significant in several time bins for models using both the multifrequency and the reduced feature space (single‐frequency) suggesting that the individual frequency bands alone are sufficient to decode experiential states and state meta‐awareness (a histogram demonstrating the frequency associated with each number of significant bins is shown in Figure S6 for each analysis. The latencies of the significant bins per participant are illustrated in Figures S7–S10).
FIGURE 3.
Time‐varying MVPC (Multivariate Pattern Classification analysis): Group decoding accuracy across participants in multifrequency (top panels) and single‐frequency (bottom panels) models for four different state contrasts. The null distribution was obtained by 5,000 random permutations after shuffling the labels (black lines). Above‐chance decoding (relative to the permutation distribution) is denoted by horizontal x‐axis coloured markers (based on an FDR correction; see Table S1). MW = mind wandering=
3.4.2. Time‐averaged MVPC
At the participant‐level, the multifrequency model significantly decoded mind wandering from on‐task states in (M ± SD) 19 ± 6% (range: 10–28%) of participants (Figure 4a). Individual‐frequency models decoded states in similar proportions of participants: delta: 13 ± 3% (10–21%); theta: 15 ± 5% (8–26%); alpha: 22 ± 4% (15–26%), and did not significantly differ, p > pth (Figure 5e). At the group‐level (Figure 5a), the multifrequency model displayed significant decoding accuracy (M% ± SE: 54.72 ± 1.47, p = 0), as did single‐frequency models: delta (52.85 ± 1.11, p < .001), theta (53.23 ± 1.18, p < .001), and alpha (55.11 ± 1.31, p = 0). The multifrequency model decoded tuning‐out from on‐task states in 14 ± 7% (4–30%) of participants (delta: 12 ± 4% [4–19%]; theta: 14 ± 6% [11–30%]; alpha: 12 ± 6% [4–22%]), with no significant differences between models, p > pth (Figure 5f). At the group‐level (Figure 5b), the multifrequency model displayed significant decoding accuracy (53.93 ± 1.51, p < .001), as did the single‐frequency models (delta: 52.52 ± 1.30, p < .001, theta: 54.77 ± 1.26, p = 0, alpha: 52.61 ± 1.30, p < .001).
FIGURE 4.
Time‐averaged MVPC: (a, b) Decoding accuracy in individual participants relative to permutation distributions: chance level obtained by 500 random permutations after shuffling the class labels (upper panel). Participants for which above‐chance decoding was observed (relative to the permutation distribution) are denoted by horizontal x‐axis coloured markers. (c, d) Scatterplots of participant‐level mean decoding accuracies and mean judgement confidence ratings. Note that the empirical decoding accuracies in a few participants were below their respective chance level, which is often the case in the type of MVPC analysis we conducted (using the same two classes for training and testing data, Allefeld, Görgen, & Haynes, 2016). What cannot be below chance level is the true single‐subject accuracy, as it measures the amount of information (Allefeld et al., 2016). Red markers denote bivariate outliers; square brackets denote Bootstrap 95% CIs. MW = mind wandering. *p < .05
FIGURE 5.
Time‐averaged MVPC: (a, d) Group decoding accuracy across participants in multifrequency and single‐frequency models for four different state contrasts. (e, f) Participants (count and percentage) with significant (p < .05) decoding for each of the 11 MVPC repetitions. Black lines indicate the mean across the 11 repetitions. MW = mind wandering
Zoning‐out from on‐task states were significantly decoded in 28 ± 6% (20–44%) of participants. Performance varied across single‐frequency models (delta: 18 ± 11% [4–40%]; theta: 19 ± 5% [8–28%], Figure 4b; alpha: 28 ± 9% [12–44%]). Both the multifrequency and alpha models were significantly better than the delta, p = .004, p = .003 and theta models, p = .049, p = .035 (Figure 5g). At the group‐level (Figure 5c), the alpha model showed the greatest decoding accuracy (58.44 ± 1.83, p = 0), albeit with comparable accuracy in the multifrequency model (57.32 ± 2.30, p = 0). The delta (53.18 ± 1.75, p = .002) and theta (54.64 ± 2.01, p = .001) models were also significant.
Meta‐awareness of mind wandering (zoning‐out [unaware] vs. tuning‐out [aware]) was significantly decoded in 20 ± 6% (14–29%) of participants by the multifrequency model. Single‐frequency models decoded states in similar proportions of participants (delta: 17 ± 5% [10–24%]; theta: 18 ± 7% [10–33%]; alpha: 18 ± 10% [5–38%]), and these differences were not significant, p > pth (Figure 5h). At the group‐level (Figure 5d), all models showed comparable decoding accuracies (multifrequency [55.46 ± 2.27, p = .002], delta [54.47 ± 1.91, p < .001], theta [54.02 ± 2.56, p = .016], alpha [56.06 ± 2.22, p = 0]). Owing to the fluctuating nature of mind wandering states, we expected that time‐varying MVPC would reveal less consistent individual and group‐level effects than time‐averaged MVPC. However, the results did not suggest any substantial differences between the two approaches.
To further uncover the activation patterns that represent the expression of class‐decoding information in each channel, we visualised the topography of the activation patterns reconstructed from the classification filters, following Haufe et al. (2014). The polarity of the values in the activation map denotes the effect direction of the class‐related information represented in each channel, which depends on the order in which the data are concatenated. For decoding on‐task and mind wandering states (Figure 6), we conducted the analysis by concatenating the data in the following order: (1) mind wandering and (2) on‐task. This analysis revealed that, for the theta model, class‐related information was strongly present at frontal electrodes across participants and was associated with negative activation values, which reflects reduced theta power during mind wandering than on‐task states in those electrodes. In the delta model, posterior electrodes represented class‐related information more strongly, also in association with negative values (lower delta power for mind wandering states in those channels). By contrast, pronounced positive values were observed for the multifrequency and alpha models at left parietal and fronto‐central regions, suggesting that these electrode regions represented class‐related information more strongly (with a positive direction of the effect: elevated total/alpha power in mind wandering relative to on‐task states in the multifrequency/alpha model). The topography of the reconstructed activation maps for the additional MVPC analyses was partially different from the patterns obtained for on‐task vs. mind wandering decoding (see Supplementary Results in Data S1 and Figure S11).
FIGURE 6.
Activation patterns indicative of the strength and polarity with which class‐related information is reflected in each pattern dimension (electrode site) for the time‐averaged MVPC. Each column displays activation patterns for each model (multifrequency, delta, theta, alpha) and for mind wandering vs. on‐task classification. Activation values have arbitrary units. Positive values in the activation patterns denote greater activity (power) for mind wandering relative to on‐task states
3.4.3. Association between decoding accuracy and introspection confidence
Our final set of analyses were motivated by the hypothesis that introspection confidence reflects conscious accessibility to experiential states. Toward this end, we evaluated the predictions that individual decoding accuracies in select models would positively correlate with confidence in experiential state and state meta‐awareness judgements. We computed correlations between decoding accuracies of the multifrequency model for on‐task vs. MW and on‐task vs. tuning‐out states and decoding accuracies of theta and delta models for zoning‐out vs. on‐task and zoning‐out vs. tuning‐out, with judgement confidence in the respective state.
In support of our overarching prediction, decoding accuracies for MW vs. on‐task (multifrequency model) positively correlated with ES judgement confidence ratings of mind wandering, p = .034 [0.07, 0.59] (Figure 4c, [p = .011, with outliers]), but not with on‐task ES judgement confidence, r = .23, p = .18 [95% CI: −0.09, 0.52] (p = .49 with outliers). Accuracies for the multifrequency model decoding between on‐task and tuning‐out states did not correlate significantly with ES judgement confidence (on‐task): r = .004, p = .99 [−0.34, 0.38] (p = .81 with outliers) or MA confidence (tuning‐out): r = −.02, p = .92 [−0.48, 0.34]. Accuracies for the theta model decoding between on‐task and zoning‐out states did not correlate significantly with ES judgement confidence (on‐task) r = .01, p = .95 [−0.30, 0.30] (p = .48 with outliers), but did correlate with MA confidence (zoning‐out), p = .034 (Figure 4d, [p = .067 with outliers]). We also found that delta model decoding accuracies between on‐task and zoning‐out states did not correlate with ES confidence (on‐task) r = −.09, p = .67 [−0.63, 0.04] (p = .39 with outliers) and MA confidence (zoning‐out), r = .32, p = .12 [0.04, 0.57]. Accuracies for the theta model decoding state meta‐awareness did not correlate significantly with MA confidence for tuning‐out states, r = .08, p = .73 [−0.49, 0.44], nor with MA confidence for zoning‐out states, r = .02, p = .95 [−0.40, 0.40]. Delta model decoding accuracies between tuning‐out and zoning‐out states did not correlate with MA confidence (tuning‐out: r = .10, p = .66 [−0.27, 0.40], zoning‐out, r = .35 p = .12 [−0.13, 0.66]).
4. DISCUSSION
Using EEG and an ecological listening task, this study investigated the neural oscillatory dynamics of mind wandering and meta‐awareness states. Mind wandering was reliably characterised by greater alpha power than on‐task states with more prominent effects in this band and both delta and theta bands for unaware mind wandering. Consistent with the notion that mind wandering is more pronounced when one lacks meta‐awareness (Christoff et al., 2009), moment‐to‐moment variations between unaware mind wandering and on‐task states were the most reliably decoded via multivariate pattern classification. Critically, we found that decoding accuracy in the classification of different experiential states predicted confidence in the corresponding state judgements. This is consistent with the proposal that confidence indexes metacognitive access to experiential states.
4.1. Mind wandering as external inattention and internal focus
As in previous research demonstrating the impact of mind wandering on comprehension (e.g., Boudewyn & Carter, 2018), self‐reported mind wandering was associated with poorer recall in the listening task. One of the principal results of this study was that power in the alpha frequency band was greater during mind wandering than on‐task states, replicating previous work (Baldwin et al., 2017; Boudewyn & Carter, 2018; Compton et al., 2019; Macdonald et al., 2011). This effect was specific to the alpha band and generalised across meta‐awareness states suggesting that elevated alpha power is a frequency‐specific but generalised neurophysiological characteristic of mind wandering. Alpha power differences had a relatively diffuse distribution and were concentrated in frontal and posterior sites. The broad distribution of alpha power is consistent with a previous study on mind wandering during a listening task and commensurate with alpha activity during auditory language comprehension (Boudewyn & Carter, 2018, see also Compton et al., 2019). Posterior alpha has been extensively linked to mind wandering (Baldwin et al., 2017; Boudewyn & Carter, 2018; Compton et al., 2019; Macdonald et al., 2011), as well as internally oriented states more generally (Hanslmayr et al., 2011). Our findings are in line with studies showing elevated alpha power when attention is focused internally (Cooper et al., 2003) as well as multiple lines of evidence suggesting that mind wandering is associated with decay of perceptual processing (Barron et al., 2011; Smallwood et al., 2008). These results suggest that greater alpha power reflects detachment from the external world and a shift toward internal processing (Smallwood & Schooler, 2015).
Our findings suggest that unaware mind wandering is more divergent from on‐task states than episodes of mind wandering with awareness (Christoff et al., 2009). Although both were characterised by higher alpha power compared to on‐task states (Boudewyn & Carter, 2018), unaware mind wandering was additionally associated with greater delta and theta power, particularly in right frontal and parieto‐central sites, respectively. Aware and unaware mind wandering had suggestively distinct oscillatory features thereby implying that state meta‐awareness represents a dimension of attention that is orthogonal to the direction of attention. These findings may help to explain previous reports of elevated delta and theta during mind wandering in self‐caught paradigms (Braboszcz & Delorme, 2011; van Son et al., 2019), which in our view almost exclusively index unaware mind wandering, since they investigate EEG correlates seconds before participants realise they are mind wandering.
The observation of elevated delta and theta power during unaware mind wandering, which was validated in the activation patterns of the corresponding decoding analysis, aligns with previous research on the cognitive correlates of oscillatory activity in these bands. Theta oscillations are suggested to be involved in cognitive control (Cavanagh & Frank, 2014), working memory (Klimesch, 1999; Mitchell et al., 2008) and conflict detection (Cohen, 2014), processes that could be implicated in appropriate selection between multiple simultaneous thoughts related to the processing of current concerns during unaware mind wandering. This aligns with hypothesised parallels between mind wandering and meditative states related to moment‐to‐moment navigation through mental objects (Vago & Zeidan, 2016). In particular, our finding of higher theta power during unaware than aware mind wandering states is potentially congruent with higher frontal midline and temporo‐parietal theta during meditative states characterised by deeper absorption (DeLosAngeles et al., 2016) and thus suggest potential links between absorption and zone‐outs. Delta frequency contributions have been revealed during increased focus on internal processing and pertinent inhibition of interference (Harmony, 2013). Delta and theta activity could therefore reflect the involvement of memory in self‐related processing during self‐generated thoughts in the context of unaware mind wandering episodes. The complementary role of delta activity might be to preserve internal processing during unaware mind wandering by inhibiting external interference (Harmony, 2013; Harmony et al., 1996). Accordingly, our findings may align with the proposal that mind wandering recruits processes to ensure that one's internal train of thought is maintained (Smallwood, Brown, Baird, & Schooler, 2012; Smallwood, Fishman, et al., 2007).
4.2. Decoding of experiential states
Although our data were not event‐related, our time‐varying multivariate classifier was able to trace mind wandering in several temporal segments within the 10 s time window preceding experiential state probes. Moreover, MVPC allowed us to decode experiential states from oscillatory activity at both participant‐ and group‐levels, highlighting the utility of spectral measures coupled with machine learning in decoding mind wandering (Groot et al., 2021; Jin et al., 2019) and state meta‐awareness. The time‐varying and time‐averaged analyses did not reveal substantially different results. In both analyses, all models decoded mind wandering from on‐task states, including in approximately one‐quarter of participants (multifrequency, alpha) and with comparable classification accuracies. Similarly, aware mind wandering was decoded from on‐task states in both analyses, with models using power in the delta and alpha bands for classification showing the weakest performance. It is potentially notable that the time‐varying analyses yielded significant decoding between unaware mind wandering and on‐task states in multiple time windows, particularly for the delta and theta models, which decoded the two experiential states across almost the entire epoch. By contrast, the time‐averaged MVPC delta and theta models showed a weaker classification performance compared to the multifrequency and alpha models which achieved the highest decoding accuracy at the group‐ and participant‐levels. Although insofar as all models achieved significant decoding, our analyses do not suggest that decoding between these two states is frequency specific per se. Finally, the time‐averaged multifrequency MVPC decoded aware from unaware mind wandering states in 20% of participants on average with comparable decoding accuracy values across all models. Taken together, these results suggest that information pertaining to state meta‐awareness might be distributed across different frequency bands including slow oscillations—as shown in the higher prevalence of significant decoding in the sample (relative to other decoding analyses). However, our analyses did not reveal any robust evidence for frequency specificity and thus it seems that there is not a specific oscillatory pattern that contains more information about mind wandering and state meta‐awareness.
Assessment of the topography of the decoding results (Haufe et al., 2014) highlighted that the single‐frequency models comprise differential activation patterns representing class‐related information regarding experiential states and meta‐awareness. Insofar as these analyses are purely descriptive, we should exercise caution in interpreting the topographic results, however these analyses suggested that class‐related information tended to be strongly represented in fronto‐central and posterior regions which broadly aligns with the effects observed in the cluster‐based analysis. Combined EEG‐fMRI studies investigating the topography in different frequency bands associated with activation of resting state networks can shed light on the neural correlates of mind wandering through the observed activation patterns (e.g., Jann, Kottlow, Dierks, Boesch, & Koenig, 2010). One interpretation of the activation maps associated with each single‐frequency model is co‐activation of the DMN and executive control (fronto‐parietal) network (Jann et al., 2010) during mind wandering states which aligns with a wealth of research (Christoff et al., 2009; see also Fox, Spreng, Ellamil, Andrews‐Hanna, & Christoff, 2015). In addition, previous evidence has shown inverse associations between DMN activity and delta (Hlinka, Alexakis, Diukova, Liddle, & Auer, 2010) and frontal midline theta power (Scheeringa et al., 2008), while activity in the alpha band is positively associated with DMN recruitment (Mantini, Perrucci, Del Gratta, Romani, & Corbetta, 2007; Marino, Arcara, Porcaro, & Mantini, 2019; see also Knyazev, 2013). A complementary interpretation is that the activation maps for the delta and theta frequency bands are also similar to the topography associated with network activity supporting working memory (Jann et al., 2010) and thus may point to its recruitment during mind wandering episodes (Marcusson‐Clavertz, Cardeña, & Terhune, 2016). Finally, the observed activation patterns may reflect auditory suppression during mind wandering, compared to on‐task states (Jann et al., 2010) which is consistent with poorer performance on the listening task during mind wandering.
We further corroborate that unaware mind wandering is more dissimilar at the neural level to external attention (on‐task states) (Christoff et al., 2009). Combined with our MVPC results decoding aware from unaware mind wandering, we confirm that state meta‐awareness should be considered as an important dimension of mind wandering in future studies, with evidence for disparate neural substrates for aware and unaware mind wandering. Collectively, MVPC trained on EEG‐extracted features reliably decoded different experiential states both at participant and group levels. Discrepancies between group‐level and participant‐level classification is in line with research showing the utility of using individualised markers in decoding mind wandering (Dhindsa et al., 2019). Even though our task involved passive listening, one possibility is that residual eye movements could have contributed to the decoding. Eye movements can have confounding effects in neural decoding, particularly within visual tasks that involve active viewing (Thielen, Bosch, van Leeuwen, van Gerven, & van Lier, 2019). An additional limitation of our study is that we used a small fold number (3) for leave‐one‐out cross‐validation, which may lead to an overestimation of the model's skills (Kohavi, 1995). An inherent feature of the phenomenon we are investigating is that some participants only report very few instances of mind wandering, which results in small trial numbers. This limited the use of higher folds for cross‐validation, which is preferable (e.g., 10‐folds). On the other hand, we estimated the chance level separately in each participant using a permutation distribution. Accordingly, our decoding analyses overcome the previously reported caveat of using theoretical chance levels (e.g., 50% for two‐class classification), which are not accurate for small data sets (Combrisson & Jerbi, 2015).
4.3. Introspection confidence
Previous research investigating confidence suggests that self‐reports of mind wandering characterised by higher confidence constitute more accurate evaluations of one's experiential states (Seli et al., 2015). Our results build upon this, showing that confidence in experiential state judgements tended to map onto state meta‐awareness: participants reported the greatest certainty for on‐task episodes, were less confident in aware mind wandering, and the least for mind wandering episodes without awareness, although confidence ratings between the latter two were not significantly different. Future work could investigate how confidence relates to other prominent dimensions of mind wandering, such as intentionality (Seli et al., 2017; Seli, Risko, Smilek, & Schacter, 2016). Participant‐level decoding of experiential states allowed us to evaluate the prediction that introspection confidence would be positively associated with decoding accuracy. Indeed, confidence in different experiential state and state meta‐awareness judgements reliably correlated with individual differences in MVPC decoding accuracies. In particular, confidence correlated with cross‐frequency decoding accuracy in classifying mind wandering from on‐task states. In addition, consistent with findings implicating theta activity in metacognition (Wokke, Cleeremans, & Ridderinkhof, 2017), we also found that confidence was associated with decoding of unaware mind wandering from on‐task states accuracy in the theta model. Collectively, these results demonstrate that confidence in one's experiential states is positively related to the multivariate decodability of these states with implications for the neural bases of experiential state confidence.
Our results align with previous findings (Seli et al., 2015) suggesting that high confidence levels reflect a more accurate assessment of one's experiential state reflected in stronger coupling between mind wandering reports and well‐established impacts on behaviour. We extend this notion and provide evidence that higher confidence may reflect greater dissociation of experiential states at the neurophysiological level. In MVPC, higher decoding accuracy denotes better discriminability or separation between EEG patterns associated with each experiential state class. Although this discriminability refers only to pattern analysis, it is possible that more dissociable or distinct neural patterns are metacognitively represented and therefore reflected in individuals' confidence ratings. One interpretation of these findings thus is that states accompanied by high confidence are quantitatively more intense or salient and thus characterised by superior conscious access that is grounded in or related to underlying neurophysiological discriminability. Alternatively, this relationship might be attributable to participants with low confidence displaying weak experiential state discriminability, that is, they have relatively poor metacognitive access in their experiential states, associated with lower multivariate decoding. However, whether and to what extent confidence judgements about internal states reflect a readout of discriminability between neural patterns remains an unresolved issue. Recent work demonstrated that confidence judgements and behavioural accuracy are dissociated during decision making and these dissociations can be explained by differences in neural computations (Peters et al., 2017). One limitation in this analysis is that due to the small number of trials per certain classes in certain participants, our analyses were limited to mean confidence ratings. Future research on mind wandering could utilise confidence ratings on a trial‐by‐trial basis to provide a more precise estimate of the relationship between confidence and classification accuracy (Weaver et al., 2019).
5. CONCLUSIONS
Our findings expand upon research linking elevated alpha power with mind wandering episodes and reveal distinct electrophysiological characteristics of state meta‐awareness. Unaware mind wandering was consistently more dissimilar from on‐task states than aware mind wandering, as evidenced by superior decoding and greater neurophysiological differences. These results highlight a clear distinction between unaware and aware mind wandering states and confirm the utility of introspective methods in the study of transient fluctuations in conscious experience. The observed effects demonstrate the potential of using EEG machine learning classifiers to capture mind wandering and state meta‐awareness during an ecological task without performance indicators. We found that confidence in experiential state and state meta‐awareness reports correlated with the decoding of the respective states, suggesting that introspection confidence scales with neurophysiological dissimilarity. These effects suggest that introspection confidence taps into variability in metacognitive access to, and differential phenomenological characteristics, of experiential states.
Supporting information
Figure S1 Oscillatory differences between states (tuning‐out – on‐task, N = 27) as a function of time relative to probe onset (0 s). (a) Time frequency decomposition averaged across all electrode sites. The broken black rectangle indicates the spectrotemporal cluster reflecting significant state differences (p < .025, two‐sided cluster‐based permutation test). (b) Alpha (8–13 Hz) spectral power averaged over the electrode sites of the cluster (significance denoted by a black bar on the x‐axis). (c) Topography of the cluster at different 400/450 ms subwindows (black markers denote electrodes that were present in at least 50% of samples in each time window).
Figure S2 Spectral power averaged over all 64 electrode sites as a function of frequency band and condition for contrasts that were nonsignificant. We report the strongest, albeit nonsignificant, effects. Top panel: the on‐task vs. MW analysis did not yield any significant clusters for delta (p = .24, at −4.500 to −3.750 s) or theta power (p = .40, at −3.550 to −3.200, negative cluster). Middle panel: theta and delta power also did not significantly differ between tuning‐out and on‐task states (theta: p = .20, at −7.500 to −7.050 s, delta: p = .23 at −8.250 to −7.750 s, negative cluster). Bottom panel: tuning‐out and zoning‐out states did not significantly differ in alpha (p = .16, at −7.500 to 6.850 s) or delta power (p = .05, at −8.400 to −7.050). MW = mind wandering.
Figure S3 Oscillatory differences between states (zoning‐out − tuning‐out, N = 21) as a function of time relative to probe onset (0 s). (a) Time frequency decomposition averaged across electrode sites. The broken grey rectangle denotes a spectrotemporal cluster reflecting the largest identified state differences (p = .026, two‐sided cluster‐based permutation test). (b) Theta (4–7 Hz) spectral power averaged over the electrode sites of the cluster (trend effect denoted by grey bar on the x‐axis). (c) Topography of the cluster at two 600 ms subwindows (white markers denote electrodes that were present at least 50% of samples in time window).
Figure S4 Beta (14–30 Hz) spectral power averaged over all 64 electrode sites: (a) MW vs. on‐task (N = 39): p = .41 (at −4.400 to −4.300 s), (b) On‐task vs. tuning‐out (N = 27): p = .35 (at −7.250 to −7.100), (c) Zoning out vs. on‐task (N = 25): p = .16 (at −3.300 to −3.100), (d) Zoning‐out vs. tuning‐out (N = 21): p = .15 (at 7.150 to −6.850). P‐values correspond to the most prominent nonsignificant clusters. MW = mind wandering.
Figure S5 Time‐varying MVPC: Participants (count and percentage) with significant (p < pth) decoding in at least one time bin. MW = mind wandering.
Figure S6 Time‐varying MVPC: Histogram of the frequency (number of participants) for each number of significant bins. MW = mind wandering
Figure S7 Time‐varying MVPC of mind wandering vs. on‐task: Time bins of significant above‐chance decoding accuracy (relative to the permutation distribution) for each participant are denoted by horizontal bars (p ≤ pth after control of FDR at level .05).
Figure S8 Time‐varying MVPC of tuning‐out vs. on‐task: Time bins of significant above‐chance decoding accuracy (relative to the permutation distribution) for each participant are denoted by horizontal bars (p ≤ pth after control of FDR at level .05).
Figure S9 Time‐varying MVPC of zoning‐out vs. on‐task: Time bins of significant above‐chance decoding accuracy (relative to the permutation distribution) for each participant are denoted by horizontal bars (p ≤ pth after control of FDR at level .05).
Figure S10 Time‐varying MVPC of zoning‐out vs. tuning‐out: Time bins of significant above‐chance decoding accuracy (relative to the permutation distribution) for each participant are denoted by horizontal bars (p ≤ pth after control of FDR at level .05).
Figure S11 Activation patterns indicative of the strength and polarity with which class‐related information is reflected in each pattern dimension (electrode site) for the time‐averaged MVPC. Each column displays activation patterns for each model (multifrequency, delta, theta, alpha), whereas each row corresponds to a different two‐class comparison. Activation values have arbitrary units. Positive values in the activation patterns denote greater activity (power) for tuning‐out relative to on‐task states (first row) and zoning‐out relative to on‐task states (second row). In the bottom panel, positive values denote greater activity (power) for zoning‐out relative to tuning‐out states. Note the close alignment of the activation patterns in the MVPC analysis and the univariate analysis presented in the main manuscript: see Figures S1, 2, and S3.
Appendix S1 Supporting Information.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
MHR was partially funded by the Basic Research Program of the National Research University Higher School of Economics (Russian Federation). The authors would like to thank Stefan Haufe for engaging in useful discussions about activation patterns of MVPC analyses.
Polychroni, N. , Herrojo Ruiz, M. , & Terhune, D. B. (2022). Introspection confidence predicts EEG decoding of self‐generated thoughts and meta‐awareness. Human Brain Mapping, 43(7), 2311–2327. 10.1002/hbm.25789
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.
REFERENCES
- Allefeld, C. , Görgen, K. , & Haynes, J.‐D. (2016). Valid population inference for information‐based imaging: From the second‐level t‐test to prevalence inference. NeuroImage, 141, 378–392. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allefeld, C. , & Haynes, J.‐D. (2015). Multi‐voxel Pattern Analysis. In A. W. Toga (Ed.). Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference (pp. 641–646). Waltham, MA: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Andrews‐Hanna, J. R. (2012). The Brain's default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation. Neuroscientist, 18(3), 251–270. 10.1177/1073858411403316 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrews‐Hanna, J. R. , Smallwood, J. , & Spreng, R. N. (2014). The default network and self‐generated thought: Component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1316(1), 29–52. 10.1111/nyas.12360 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antrobus, J. S. , Singer, J. L. , Goldstein, S. , & Fortgang, M. (1970). Section of psychology: Mind wandering and cognitive structure. Transactions of the New York Academy of Sciences, 32, 242–252. 10.1111/j.2164-0947.1970.tb02056.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bae, G. Y. , & Luck, S. J. (2018). Dissociable decoding of spatial attention and working memory from EEG oscillations and sustained potentials. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(2), 409–422. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2860-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baird, B. , Smallwood, J. , Lutz, A. , & Schooler, J. W. (2014). The decoupled mind: Mind‐wandering disrupts cortical phase‐locking to perceptual events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(11), 2596–2607. 10.1162/jocn_a_00656 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baldwin, C. L. , Roberts, D. M. , Barragan, D. , Lee, J. D. , Lerner, N. , & Higgins, J. S. (2017). Detecting and quantifying mind wandering during simulated driving. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 1–15. 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00406 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barron, E. , Riby, L. M. , Greer, J. , & Smallwood, J. (2011). Absorbed in thought. Psychological Science, 22(5), 596–601. 10.1177/0956797611404083 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benjamini, Y. , Krieger, A. M. , & Yekutieli, D. (2006). Adaptive linear step‐up procedures that control the false discovery rate. Biometrika, 93(3), 491–507. 10.1093/biomet/93.3.491 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Boldt, A. , & Yeung, N. (2015). Shared neural markers of decision confidence and error detection. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(8), 3478–3484. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0797-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boudewyn, M. A. , & Carter, C. S. (2018). I must have missed that: Alpha‐band oscillations track attention to spoken language. Neuropsychologia, 117, 148–155. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.05.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braboszcz, C. , & Delorme, A. (2011). Lost in thoughts: Neural markers of low alertness during mind wandering. NeuroImage, 54(4), 3040–3047. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bryson, B. (2004). A short history of nearly everything. London, England: Black Swan. [Google Scholar]
- Cavanagh, J. F. , & Frank, M. J. (2014). Frontal theta as a mechanism for affective and effective control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(8), 414–421. 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012.Frontal [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang, C. C. , & Lin, C. J. (2011). LIBSVM: A library for support vector machines. ACM Transactions on Intelligent Systems and Technology, 2, 1–27. 10.1145/1961189.1961199 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Christoff, K. , Gordon, A. M. , Smallwood, J. , Smith, R. , & Schooler, J. W. (2009). Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(21), 8719–8724. 10.1073/pnas.0900234106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, M. X. (2014). A neural microcircuit for cognitive conflict detection and signaling. Trends in Neurosciences, 37(9), 480–490. 10.1016/j.tins.2014.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Combrisson, E. , & Jerbi, K. (2015). Exceeding chance level by chance: The caveat of theoretical chance levels in brain signal classification and statistical assessment of decoding accuracy. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 250, 126–136. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.01.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Compton, R. J. , Gearinger, D. , & Wild, H. (2019). The wandering mind oscillates: EEG alpha power is enhanced during moments of mind‐wandering. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 19(5), 1184–1191. 10.3758/s13415-019-00745-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper, N. R. , Croft, R. J. , Dominey, S. J. J. , Burgess, A. P. , & Gruzelier, J. H. (2003). Paradox lost? Exploring the role of alpha oscillations during externally vs. internally directed attention and the implications for idling and inhibition hypotheses. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 47(1), 65–74. 10.1016/S0167-8760(02)00107-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delorme, A. , & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single‐trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134(1), 9–21. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeLosAngeles, D. , Williams, G. , Burston, J. , Fitzgibbon, S. P. , Lewis, T. W. , Grummett, T. S. , … Willoughby, J. O. (2016). Electroencephalographic correlates of states of concentrative meditation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 110, 27–39. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.09.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dhindsa, K. , Acai, A. , Wagner, N. , Bosynak, D. , Kelly, S. , Bhandari, M. , … Sonnadara, R. R. (2019). Individualized pattern recognition for detecting mind wandering from EEG during live lectures. PLoS One, 14(9), 1–30. 10.1371/journal.pone.0222276 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Efron, B. (1987). Better bootstrap confidence intervals. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 82(397), 171–185. 10.1080/01621459.1987.10478410 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Feng, S. , D'Mello, S. , & Graesser, A. C. (2013). Mind wandering while reading easy and difficult texts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(3), 586–592. 10.3758/s13423-012-0367-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fingelkurts, A. A. , & Fingelkurts, A. A. (2011). Persistent operational synchrony within brain default‐mode network and self‐processing operations in healthy subjects. Brain and Cognition, 75(2), 79–90. 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.11.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleming, S. M. , & Lau, H. C. (2014). How to measure metacognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 443. 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00443 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox, K. C. R. , Spreng, R. N. , Ellamil, M. , Andrews‐Hanna, J. R. , & Christoff, K. (2015). The wandering brain: Meta‐analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of mind‐wandering and related spontaneous thought processes. NeuroImage, 111, 611–621. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox, M. D. , Snyder, A. Z. , Vincent, J. L. , Corbetta, M. , Van Essen, D. C. , & Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(27), 9673–9678. 10.1073/pnas.0504136102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foxe, J. J. , & Snyder, A. C. (2011). The role of alpha‐band brain oscillations as a sensory suppression mechanism during selective attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–13. 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geden, M. , Staicu, A.‐M. , & Feng, J. (2018). The impacts of perceptual load and driving duration on mind wandering in driving. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 57, 75–83. 10.1016/j.trf.2017.07.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gherman, S. , & Philiastides, M. G. (2018). Human VMPFC encodes early signatures of confidence in perceptual decisions. eLife, 7, 1–28. 10.7554/eLife.38293 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grandchamp, R. , & Delorme, A. (2011). Single‐trial normalization for event‐related spectral decomposition reduces sensitivity to noisy trials. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–14. 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00236 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groot, J. M. , Boayue, N. M. , Csifcsák, G. , Boekel, W. , Huster, R. , Forstmann, B. U. , & Mittner, M. (2021). Probing the neural signature of mind wandering with simultaneous fMRI‐EEG and pupillometry. NeuroImage, 224, 117412. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117412 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanslmayr, S. , Gross, J. , Klimesch, W. , & Shapiro, K. L. (2011). The role of alpha oscillations in temporal attention. Brain Research Reviews, 67(1–2), 331–343. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2011.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harmony, T. (2013). The functional significance of delta oscillations in cognitive processing. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 7, 1–10. 10.3389/fnint.2013.00083 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harmony, T. , Fernández, T. , Silva, J. , Bernal, J. , Díaz‐Comas, L. , Reyes, A. , … Rodríguez, M. (1996). EEG delta activity: An indicator of attention to internal processing during performance of mental tasks. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 24(1–2), 161–171. 10.1016/S0167-8760(96)00053-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haufe, S. , Meinecke, F. , Görgen, K. , Dähne, S. , Haynes, J. D. , Blankertz, B. , & Bießmann, F. (2014). On the interpretation of weight vectors of linear models in multivariate neuroimaging. NeuroImage, 87, 96–110. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haxby, J. V. , Gobbini, M. I. , Furey, M. L. , Ishai, A. , Schouten, J. L. , & Pietrini, P. (2001). Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science, 293(5539), 2425–2430. 10.1126/science.1063736 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haynes, J. D. , & Rees, G. (2006). Decoding mental states from brain activity in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(7), 523–534. 10.1038/nrn1931 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hentschke, H. , & Stüttgen, M. C. (2011). Computation of measures of effect size for neuroscience data sets. European Journal of Neuroscience, 34(12), 1887–1894. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07902.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hlinka, J. , Alexakis, C. , Diukova, A. , Liddle, P. F. , & Auer, D. P. (2010). Slow EEG pattern predicts reduced intrinsic functional connectivity in the default mode network: An inter‐subject analysis. NeuroImage, 53(1), 239–246. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hollis, R. B. , & Was, C. A. (2016). Mind wandering, control failures, and social media distractions in online learning. Learning and Instruction, 42, 104–112. 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2016.01.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Jann, K. , Kottlow, M. , Dierks, T. , Boesch, C. , & Koenig, T. (2010). Topographic electrophysiological signatures of fMRI resting state networks. PLoS One, 5(9), 1–10. 10.1371/journal.pone.0012945 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jin, C. Y. , Borst, J. P. , & van Vugt, M. K. (2019). Predicting task‐general mind‐wandering with EEG. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 19(4), 1059–1073. 10.3758/s13415-019-00707-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kam, J. W. Y. , Dao, E. , Farley, J. , Fitzpatrick, K. , Smallwood, J. , Schooler, J. W. , & Handy, T. C. (2011). Slow fluctuations in attentional control of sensory cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(2), 460–470. 10.1162/jocn.2010.21443 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Killingsworth, M. A. , & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science, 330(6006), 932. 10.1126/science.1192439 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim, J. , Chung, Y. G. , Park, J. Y. , Chung, S. C. , Wallraven, C. , Bülthoff, H. H. , & Kim, S. P. (2015). Decoding accuracy in supplementary motor cortex correlates with perceptual sensitivity to tactile roughness. PLoS One, 10(6), 1–17. 10.1371/journal.pone.0129777 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleiner, M. , Brainard, D. , Pelli, D. , Ingling, A. , Murray, R. , & Broussard, C. (2007). What's new in psychtoolbox‐3. Perception, 36(14), 1–16. [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: A review and analysis. Brain Research Reviews, 29(2–3), 169–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch, W. (2012). Alpha‐band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(12), 606–617. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knyazev, G. G. (2013). EEG correlates of self‐referential processing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 1–14. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00264 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohavi, R. (1995). A study of cross‐validation and bootstrap for accuracy estimation and model selection. International Joint Conference of Artificial Intelligence, 2, 1137–1143. [Google Scholar]
- Kunimoto, C. , Miller, J. , & Pashler, H. (2001). Confidence and accuracy of near‐threshold discrimination responses. Consciousness and Cognition, 10(3), 294–340. 10.1006/ccog.2000.0494 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindquist, S. I. , & McLean, J. P. (2011). Daydreaming and its correlates in an educational environment. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(2), 158–167. 10.1016/j.lindif.2010.12.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lotte, F. , Congedo, M. , Lécuyer, A. , Lamarche, F. , & Arnaldi, B. (2007). A review of classification algorithms for EEG‐based brain‐computer interfaces. Journal of Neural Engineering, 4(2), R1–R13. 10.1088/1741-2560/4/2/R01 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macdonald, J. S. P. , Mathan, S. , & Yeung, N. (2011). Trial‐by‐trial variations in subjective attentional state are reflected in ongoing prestimulus EEG alpha oscillations. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 82. 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00082 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mantini, D. , Perrucci, M. G. , Del Gratta, C. , Romani, G. L. , & Corbetta, M. (2007). Electrophysiological signatures of resting state networks in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(32), 13170–13175. 10.1073/pnas.0700668104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marcusson‐Clavertz, D. , Cardeña, E. , & Terhune, D. B. (2016). Daydreaming style moderates the relation between working memory and mind wandering: Integrating two hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(3), 451–464. 10.1037/xlm0000180 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marino, M. , Arcara, G. , Porcaro, C. , & Mantini, D. (2019). Hemodynamic correlates of electrophysiological activity in the default mode network. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13, 1060. 10.3389/fnins.2019.01060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maris, E. , & Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG‐ and MEG‐data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 164(1), 177–190. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinon, L. M. , Smallwood, J. , McGann, D. , Hamilton, C. , & Riby, L. M. (2019). The disentanglement of the neural and experiential complexity of self‐generated thoughts: A users guide to combining experience sampling with neuroimaging data. NeuroImage, 192, 15–25. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McVay, J. C. , Kane, M. J. , & Kwapil, T. R. (2009). Tracking the train of thought from the laboratory into everyday life: An experience‐sampling study of mind wandering across controlled and ecological contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(5), 857–863. 10.3758/PBR.16.5.857 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meier, M. E. (2018). Can research participants comment authoritatively on the validity of their self‐reports of mind wandering and task engagement? A replication and extension of Seli, Jonker, Cheyne, Cortes, and Smilek (2015). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(10), 1567–1585. 10.1037/xhp0000556 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell, D. J. , McNaughton, N. , Flanagan, D. , & Kirk, I. J. (2008). Frontal‐midline theta from the perspective of hippocampal “theta”. Progress in Neurobiology, 86(3), 156–185. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mittner, M. , Boekel, W. , Tucker, A. M. , Turner, B. M. , Heathcote, A. , & Forstmann, B. U. (2014). When the brain takes a break: A model‐based analysis of mind wandering. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(49), 16286–16295. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2062-14.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan, M. J. , Mason, A. J. S. , & Solomon, J. A. (1997). Blindsight in normal subjects? Nature, 385(6615), 401–402. 10.1038/385401b0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palva, J. M. , Palva, S. , & Kaila, K. (2005). Phase synchrony among neuronal oscillations in the human cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(15), 3962–3972. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4250-04.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palva, S. , & Palva, J. M. (2007). New vistas for α‐frequency band oscillations. Trends in Neurosciences, 30(4), 150–158. 10.1016/j.tins.2007.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pernet, C. R. , Wilcox, R. , & Rousselet, G. A. (2013). Robust correlation analyses: False positive and power validation using a new open source matlab toolbox. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 606. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00606 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters, M. A. K. , Thesen, T. , Ko, Y. D. , Maniscalco, B. , Carlson, C. , Davidson, M. , … Lau, H. (2017). Perceptual confidence neglects decision‐incongruent evidence in the brain. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(7), 1–8. 10.1038/s41562-017-0139 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raichle, M. E. , MacLeod, A. M. , Snyder, A. Z. , Powers, W. J. , Gusnard, D. A. , & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(2), 676–682. 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rousseeuw, P. J. (1984). Least median of squares regression. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 79(388), 871–880. 10.1080/01621459.1984.10477105 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rousseeuw, P. J. , & Van Driessen, K. (1999). A fast algorithm for the minimum covariance determinant estimator. Technometrics, 41(3), 212–223. 10.1080/00401706.1999.10485670 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sauseng, P. , Klimesch, W. , Doppelmayr, M. , Pecherstorfer, T. , Freunberger, R. , & Hanslmayr, S. (2005). EEG alpha synchronization and functional coupling during top‐down processing in a working memory task. Human Brain Mapping, 26(2), 148–155. 10.1002/hbm.20150 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scheeringa, R. , Bastiaansen, M. C. M. , Petersson, K. M. , Oostenveld, R. , Norris, D. G. , & Hagoort, P. (2008). Frontal theta EEG activity correlates negatively with the default mode network in resting state. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 67(3), 242–251. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.05.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schooler, J. W. , Reichle, E. D. , & Halpern, D. V. (2004). Zoning out while reading: Evidence for dissociations between experience and Metaconsciousness. In Thinking and seeing: Visual metacognition in adults and children (pp. 203–226). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Schooler, J. W. , Smallwood, J. , Christoff, K. , Handy, T. C. , Reichle, E. D. , Sayette, M. A. (2011). Meta‐awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(7), 319–326. 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seli, P. , Jonker, T. R. , Cheyne, J. A. , Cortes, K. , & Smilek, D. (2015). Can research participants comment authoritatively on the validity of their self‐reports of mind wandering and task engagement? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(3), 703–709. 10.1037/xhp0000029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seli, P. , Ralph, B. C. W. , Risko, E. F. , Schooler, W. , Schacter, D. L. , & Smilek, D. (2017). Intentionality and meta‐awareness of mind wandering: Are they one and the same, or distinct dimensions? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 24(6), 1808–1818. 10.3758/s13423-017-1249-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seli, P. , Risko, E. F. , Smilek, D. , & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Mind‐wandering with and without intention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(8), 605–617. 10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smallwood, J. , Beach, E. , Schooler, J. W. , & Handy, T. C. (2008). Going AWOL in the brain: Mind wandering reduces cortical analysis of external events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(3), 458–469. 10.1162/jocn.2008.20037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smallwood, J. , Brown, K. , Baird, B. , & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Cooperation between the default mode network and the frontal‐parietal network in the production of an internal train of thought. Brain Research, 1428, 60–70. 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.03.072 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smallwood, J. , Fishman, D. J. , & Schooler, J. W. (2007). Counting the cost of an absent mind: Mind wandering as an underrecognized influence on educational performance. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(2), 230–236. 10.3758/BF03194057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smallwood, J. , McSpadden, M. , & Schooler, J. W. (2007). The lights are on but no one's home: Meta‐awareness and the decoupling of attention when the mind wanders. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(3), 527–533. 10.3758/BF03194102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smallwood, J. , Nind, L. , & O'Connor, R. C. (2009). When is your head at? An exploration of the factors associated with the temporal focus of the wandering mind. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(1), 118–125. 10.1016/j.concog.2008.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smallwood, J. , & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946–958. 10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smallwood, J. , & Schooler, J. W. (2015). The science of mind wandering: Empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 487–518. 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015331 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreng, R. N. , & Grady, C. L. (2010). Patterns of brain activity supporting autobiographical memory, prospection, and theory of mind, and their relationship to the default mode network. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(6), 1112–1123. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21282 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thielen, J. , Bosch, S. E. , van Leeuwen, T. M. , van Gerven, M. A. J. , & van Lier, R. (2019). Evidence for confounding eye movements under attempted fixation and active viewing in cognitive neuroscience. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 1–8. 10.1038/s41598-019-54018-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vago, D. R. , & Zeidan, F. (2016). The brain on silent: Mind wandering, mindful awareness, and states of mental tranquility. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1373(1), 96–113. 10.1111/nyas.13171 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Son, D. , De Blasio, F. M. , Fogarty, J. S. , Angelidis, A. , Barry, R. J. , & Putman, P. (2019). Frontal EEG theta/beta ratio during mind wandering episodes. Biological Psychology, 140, 19–27. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varao Sousa, T. L. , Carriere, J. S. A. , & Smilek, D. (2013). The way we encounter reading material influences how frequently we mind wander. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 892. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00892 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verboven, S. , & Hubert, M. (2005). LIBRA: A MATLAB library for robust analysis. Chemometrics and Intelligent Laboratory Systems, 75(2), 127–136. 10.1016/j.chemolab.2004.06.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Villena‐González, M. , López, V. , & Rodríguez, E. (2016). Orienting attention to visual or verbal/auditory imagery differentially impairs the processing of visual stimuli. NeuroImage, 132, 71–78. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Von Stein, A. , & Sarnthein, J. (2000). Different frequencies for different scales of cortical integration: From local gamma to long range alpha/theta synchronization. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 38(3), 301–313. 10.1016/S0167-8760(00)00172-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weaver, M. D. , Fahrenfort, J. J. , Belopolsky, A. , & Van Gaal, S. (2019). Independent neural activity patterns for sensory‐and confidence‐based information maintenance during category‐selective visual processing. ENeuro, 6(1), ENEURO.0268–ENEU18.2018. 10.1523/ENEURO.0268-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wokke, M. E. , Cleeremans, A. , & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2017). Sure i'm sure: Prefrontal oscillations support metacognitive monitoring of decision making. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(4), 781–789. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1612-16.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zakrzewski, A. C. , Wisniewski, M. G. , Iyer, N. , & Simpson, B. D. (2019). Confidence tracks sensory‐ and decision‐related ERP dynamics during auditory detection. Brain and Cognition, 129, 49–58. 10.1016/j.bandc.2018.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Figure S1 Oscillatory differences between states (tuning‐out – on‐task, N = 27) as a function of time relative to probe onset (0 s). (a) Time frequency decomposition averaged across all electrode sites. The broken black rectangle indicates the spectrotemporal cluster reflecting significant state differences (p < .025, two‐sided cluster‐based permutation test). (b) Alpha (8–13 Hz) spectral power averaged over the electrode sites of the cluster (significance denoted by a black bar on the x‐axis). (c) Topography of the cluster at different 400/450 ms subwindows (black markers denote electrodes that were present in at least 50% of samples in each time window).
Figure S2 Spectral power averaged over all 64 electrode sites as a function of frequency band and condition for contrasts that were nonsignificant. We report the strongest, albeit nonsignificant, effects. Top panel: the on‐task vs. MW analysis did not yield any significant clusters for delta (p = .24, at −4.500 to −3.750 s) or theta power (p = .40, at −3.550 to −3.200, negative cluster). Middle panel: theta and delta power also did not significantly differ between tuning‐out and on‐task states (theta: p = .20, at −7.500 to −7.050 s, delta: p = .23 at −8.250 to −7.750 s, negative cluster). Bottom panel: tuning‐out and zoning‐out states did not significantly differ in alpha (p = .16, at −7.500 to 6.850 s) or delta power (p = .05, at −8.400 to −7.050). MW = mind wandering.
Figure S3 Oscillatory differences between states (zoning‐out − tuning‐out, N = 21) as a function of time relative to probe onset (0 s). (a) Time frequency decomposition averaged across electrode sites. The broken grey rectangle denotes a spectrotemporal cluster reflecting the largest identified state differences (p = .026, two‐sided cluster‐based permutation test). (b) Theta (4–7 Hz) spectral power averaged over the electrode sites of the cluster (trend effect denoted by grey bar on the x‐axis). (c) Topography of the cluster at two 600 ms subwindows (white markers denote electrodes that were present at least 50% of samples in time window).
Figure S4 Beta (14–30 Hz) spectral power averaged over all 64 electrode sites: (a) MW vs. on‐task (N = 39): p = .41 (at −4.400 to −4.300 s), (b) On‐task vs. tuning‐out (N = 27): p = .35 (at −7.250 to −7.100), (c) Zoning out vs. on‐task (N = 25): p = .16 (at −3.300 to −3.100), (d) Zoning‐out vs. tuning‐out (N = 21): p = .15 (at 7.150 to −6.850). P‐values correspond to the most prominent nonsignificant clusters. MW = mind wandering.
Figure S5 Time‐varying MVPC: Participants (count and percentage) with significant (p < pth) decoding in at least one time bin. MW = mind wandering.
Figure S6 Time‐varying MVPC: Histogram of the frequency (number of participants) for each number of significant bins. MW = mind wandering
Figure S7 Time‐varying MVPC of mind wandering vs. on‐task: Time bins of significant above‐chance decoding accuracy (relative to the permutation distribution) for each participant are denoted by horizontal bars (p ≤ pth after control of FDR at level .05).
Figure S8 Time‐varying MVPC of tuning‐out vs. on‐task: Time bins of significant above‐chance decoding accuracy (relative to the permutation distribution) for each participant are denoted by horizontal bars (p ≤ pth after control of FDR at level .05).
Figure S9 Time‐varying MVPC of zoning‐out vs. on‐task: Time bins of significant above‐chance decoding accuracy (relative to the permutation distribution) for each participant are denoted by horizontal bars (p ≤ pth after control of FDR at level .05).
Figure S10 Time‐varying MVPC of zoning‐out vs. tuning‐out: Time bins of significant above‐chance decoding accuracy (relative to the permutation distribution) for each participant are denoted by horizontal bars (p ≤ pth after control of FDR at level .05).
Figure S11 Activation patterns indicative of the strength and polarity with which class‐related information is reflected in each pattern dimension (electrode site) for the time‐averaged MVPC. Each column displays activation patterns for each model (multifrequency, delta, theta, alpha), whereas each row corresponds to a different two‐class comparison. Activation values have arbitrary units. Positive values in the activation patterns denote greater activity (power) for tuning‐out relative to on‐task states (first row) and zoning‐out relative to on‐task states (second row). In the bottom panel, positive values denote greater activity (power) for zoning‐out relative to tuning‐out states. Note the close alignment of the activation patterns in the MVPC analysis and the univariate analysis presented in the main manuscript: see Figures S1, 2, and S3.
Appendix S1 Supporting Information.
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.