Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2022 Apr 12;17(4):e0266849. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0266849

How does handwashing behaviour change in response to a cholera outbreak? A qualitative case study in the Democratic Republic of the Congo

Sian White 1,*, Anna C Mutula 2, Modeste M Buroko 2, Thomas Heath 3, François K Mazimwe 2, Karl Blanchet 4, Val Curtis 1, Robert Dreibelbis 1
Editor: Alison Parker5
PMCID: PMC9004767  PMID: 35413080

Abstract

Background

Handwashing with soap has the potential to curb cholera transmission. This research explores how populations experienced and responded to the 2017 cholera outbreak in the Democratic Republic of the Congo and how this affected their handwashing behaviour.

Methods

Cholera cases were identified through local cholera treatment centre records. Comparison individuals were recruited from the same neighbourhoods by identifying households with no recent confirmed or suspected cholera cases. Multiple qualitative methods were employed to understand hand hygiene practices and their determinants, including unstructured observations, interviews and focus group discussions. The data collection tools and analysis were informed by the Behaviour Centred Design Framework. Comparisons were made between the experiences and practices of people from case households and participants from comparison households.

Results

Cholera was well understood by the population and viewed as a persistent and common health challenge. Handwashing with soap was generally observed to be rare during the outbreak despite self-reported increases in behaviour. Across case and comparison groups, individuals were unable to prioritise handwashing due to competing food-scarcity and livelihood challenges and there was little in the physical or social environments to cue handwashing or make it a convenient, rewarding or desirable to practice. The ability of people from case households to practice handwashing was further constrained by their exposure to cholera which in addition to illness, caused profound non-health impacts to household income, productivity, social status, and their sense of control.

Conclusions

Even though cholera outbreaks can cause disruptions to many determinants of behaviour, these shifts do not automatically facilitate an increase in preventative behaviours like handwashing with soap. Hygiene programmes targeting outbreaks within complex crises could be strengthened by acknowledging the emic experiences of the disease and adopting sustainable solutions which build upon local disease coping mechanisms.

Introduction

For centuries cholera has been a marker of social inequalities, affecting the most vulnerable members of society and commonly occurring amid social and economic upheaval or disaster [1]. Cholera cases remain underreported but it is estimated that there are 2.9 million cases and 91,000 deaths annually due to the disease [1, 2]. In 2017 major outbreaks occurred in Yemen, the Democratic Republic of the Congo (DRC), Nigeria, Somalia and South Sudan leading to the highest global numbers of cases in history [3].

In the past, cholera was viewed as a waterborne disease, with environment-to-human transmission of vibrio cholera believed to be responsible for the majority of transmission [4]. However, recent spatiotemporal analyses of cholera outbreaks have demonstrated how cases cluster among close contacts [58]. This human-to-human transmission is heightened in dense living environments, and where water access is limited or intermittent, causing hygiene to be compromised [8].

Handwashing with soap is frequently recommended by international response agencies as a key household-level cholera prevention behaviour [9]. A recent meta-analysis of case control studies conducted during cholera outbreaks found that self-reported good hygiene practices and the availability of handwashing materials had the highest protective affect of any of the water, sanitation and hygiene (WASH) factors assessed [10]. Another broader review of cholera risk factors also found that handwashing had smaller but still protective effect against symptomatic cholera [8]. The authors acknowledged that the included studies used inconsistent measures of self-reported behaviour, likely to result in overestimates of actual handwashing behaviour [11].

Another review assessing the impact of water, sanitation and hygiene (WASH) interventions on cholera control found that handwashing promotion programmes during outbreaks generally had a positive affect but were limited by the behavioural and health outcomes they used (e.g. self-reported symptoms rather than laboratory confirmation). A more recent study in Bangladesh was able to overcome measurement limitations, and demonstrated that case-targeted interventions to promote handwashing and water treatment were successful in increasing behaviour which consequently reduced secondary transmission to household contacts by almost half [12]. However the majority of handwashing interventions during cholera outbreaks continue to focus only on health education [13]. This is problematic because knowledge of the health benefits of handwashing is unlikely to be sufficient to realise sustained behaviour change [14, 15]. During outbreaks there is a tendency for both researchers and practitioners to overemphasise the effect of cognitive determinants such as health knowledge, risk perception and fear, rather than taking a more holistic view of the determinants that could influence handwashing behaviour [15]. Outbreaks may also cause theory-informed processes for designing behaviour change programmes to be compromised due to the perceived need to act right away, rather than consult and learn from populations [16].

There have been calls for more qualitative research into hygiene behaviour during cholera outbreaks and new, scalable approaches to doing community engagement to support preventative behaviours [1, 17, 18]. This research responds to these calls and draws on anthropology and behavioural science to understand how individuals and communities experience and respond to outbreaks and whether this affects their handwashing behaviour. We explore both the consequences of cholera on people’s lives and the determinants of handwashing behaviour during the 2017 cholera outbreak in the eastern part of DRC.

Methods

Study site

This study took place in South Kivu in the eastern part of the DRC at the height of the 2017 cholera outbreak (October and November). The region experiences both endemic and epidemic cholera and in 2017 the outbreak was the largest in recent decades with >53,000 reported cases and 1,145 deaths [19]. The research took place in a town on the shores of Lake Kivu (known to be an environmental reservoir for cholera) [20, 21], which is home to about 200,000 people. The region was purposively selected because it was described as a cholera ‘hot spot’[19]. It also hosts a large number of internally displaced persons (IDPs) who have fled armed conflict in neighbouring villages. At the time of this research, the government provided IDPs with a small plot of land (~3m2) within one of two informal camps in the town. IDPs were responsible for constructing their own makeshift shelters from tarps and branches. In addition to these camp-like settings, some IDPs rented homes from permanent residents. Host community members typically lived in brick or compacted mud houses with corrugated iron roofs. Both IDPs and host community members typically worked in agriculture or as small-scale market vendors, although IDPs would typically earn less than host community members on a daily basis. Water and sanitation access in the region was poor. Pit latrines were common but often in poor condition and shared by many households, particularly in the informal camps. Water was considered scarce and was intermittent. IDPs and host communities had access to the same water sources which included tap stands and boreholes or water collection from rivers and lakes.

Multiple non-government organisations (NGOs) had worked in the region as part of sporadic emergency response initiatives oriented towards health and WASH. At the time of the research a temporary Cholera Treatment Centre (CTC) had been established by an NGO and was providing free care for cholera cases. Handwashing promotion was widespread and predominantly consisted of health education delivered by volunteer relais communautaires (health volunteers from the community) who were trained by NGOs. Health awareness sessions focuses on cholera transmission and prevention behaviours. Exposure to hygiene promotion was similar among IDPs and host communities, with the exception that those living further outside of the town in were exposed less frequently.

Research framework

This research used unstructured observations, in-depth interviews (IDIs) and focus group discussions (FGDs) to explore a range of behavioural determinants. We used the Behaviour Centred Design (BCD) framework [22] to develop a list of determinant categories and to refine appropriate methods for exploring each. BCD draws on evolutionary and environmental psychology to define critical domains of behaviour including cognitive processes, individual characteristics, the settings where behaviours take place and the broader physical, social and contextual environment. S1 Table defines the 16 BCD determinant categories that were assessed within this research in relation to handwashing. It indicates which methods were used to explore them and how these methods were developed. All of the methods aimed to identify contextual associational relationships within these broader determinant categories. The methodology adopted in this research replicated a process used in Iraq to understand how behaviour was affected during post-conflict displacement [23].

Sampling

For the observation and IDIs participants were selected purposively based on their exposure to cholera. To do this we worked with health staff to identify cholera cases registered within the last three months. Using this sampling frame, we purposively selected for a diversity in age, gender, geography (rural or urban) type of residence (residing in a camp or residing in the community). Once case households had agreed to participate in the research, we also approached other households in the nearby vicinity to be part of the research. We sampled these ‘comparison households’ against the same criteria for diversity. Some households participated in more than one method. Sampling continued until a degree of saturation was met for each method. FGD participants were sampled purposively to be similar in terms of gender, geographical regions and type of residence.

Data collection methods

Unstructured observations

Unstructured observations were designed to provide contextual detail about handwashing. Observations took place in eight case households and eight comparison households. Observations were for 2–3 hours, typically beginning at 6am and finishing when the participant had to depart for work. Observers wrote down all actions that that were done by all household members and the time actions took place. Observers paid attention to ‘critical times’ for handwashing which were defined as handwashing after using the toilet, or cleaning a child’s bottom, and before preparing food, eating food or feeding a child. Observers noted whether hands were washed at these critical times and whether soap was used.

In-depth interviews

A total of 51 IDIs were completed, involving 24 people from case households and 27 people from comparison households. Seven of the participants who took part in the observation also took part in the IDIs. For case households, we selected the individual who had cholera if they were over 18 and well enough to participate. Alternatively, the person primarily responsible for caring for the case was selected. Participants of a similar age and gender were then invited to participate from neighbouring comparison households. A total of eight participatory activities were used within the IDIs to explore perceived hygiene challenges, the enabling environment, water use, roles, capabilities, routines, norms, social networks, and broader contextual determinants. See S2 Table for details on these activities.

Focus group discussions

43 people participated in the FGDs. Four FGDs were conducted with women, two of which comprised women residing in the IDP camps and two which were with host community members. Three FGDs were conducted with men, two of which were with IDPs and one with host community members. Six participatory activities were included in FGDs to explore the prioritisation of hygiene challenges, perceived risk of cholera, attitudes towards people who had cholera, preferences related to infrastructure and soap, and motivations of behaviour. See S3 Table for descriptions of these activities.

Data collection

The data was collected by four of the authors (SW, ACM, MMB, FKM). We were a team with mixed cultural backgrounds (British and Congolese). Two days of classroom-based training was provided on the research rationale and the methods. We then piloted the methods in a similar setting and adapted the tools as necessary. All IDIs and FGDs were conducted in Congolese Swahili and audio recorded. Observation notes were taken by hand. At the end of each day of data collection we reflected on our findings and captured this through written field notes [24].

Data management and analysis

Preliminary data analysis was done concurrently with data collection. This allowed us make theoretical and methodological notes [25] and decide when we had reached a point of saturation. All audio recordings from IDIs and FGDs were transcribed and translated. Methods with semi-quantitative data such as ranked or scaled information were summarised in spreadsheets. Visual data such as drawings, photos and videos were descriptively summarised. All data and the field notes were imported into NVivo 12 software. The data analysis followed the process outlined by Braun and Clarke [26]. Data were classified according to whether the participant was from a case household or a comparison household. An initial top-down coding framework was applied based on the determinants of the BCD checklist. A second phase of coding was then conducted based on emergent themes. Coding was conducted by the first author and then theme summaries were validated by ACM, MMB, FKM and TH.

Ethics and consent

Informed written consent was obtained from each participant. The research was approved by the ethics committees at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine (Submission ID: 13545) and the School of Public Health at the University of Kinshasa (Submission ID: ESP/CE/038/2017). Permission to undertake this work was given by the Departments of Health in North and South Kivu. Organisations working in the area were informed of our work and preliminary findings were shared immediately after data collection to enable utilisation within programmatic work. Further detail on research permissions and stakeholder engagement is provided in an Inclusivity Questionnaire in S4 Table.

Results

Participant characteristics

In total 104 people took part in this research with 40% of these coming from households with one or more cholera cases in the last 3 months. A higher proportion of women were included in the sample, this purposive selection reflected the fact that women in this region were more involved with hygiene-related tasks and caregiving. Almost half of the participants were illiterate and average family size was 6.5 people. The linguistic diversity of participants and the high levels of internal displacement and people returning post-displacement (62% experiencing displacement) are reflective of decades of conflict in this region. Table 1 summarises the socio-demographic characteristics of the sample.

Table 1. Socio-demographic characteristics of all participants across the two camps and two villages.

Socio-Demographic characteristics Total %
N = 104
Sex
Male 37 36%
Female 72 69%
Place of residence
Camp 22 21%
Community 82 79%
Location
Rural 34 33%
Urban 70 67%
Literacy
Not literate 49 47%
Literate / Some literacy 55 53%
Cultural diversity
Number of local languages spoken across participants 8
Household Size
Range 1–13 people
Average 6.5 people
Displacement status
Internally Displaced 51 49%
Returnee (a person who has been displaced in the last 5 years and has returned to their home) 13 13%
Host community 40 38%
Duration of displacement for IDPs
Range 1 month to 20 years
Average 4.5 years
Cholera exposure
Participants who had one or more cholera cases in their household in the last 3 months 42 40%
Households with no recent direct exposure to cholera 62 60%
Duration since cholera cases were discharged
Range 1–90 days
Average 17 days

Handwashing behaviour

Handwashing with soap (HWWS) and hand rinsing were uncommon at critical times (HWWS = 1%, hand rinsing = 11%) and there was no discernible difference between behaviour in case households and comparison households or between IDPs and host community hosueholds. When handwashing with soap did occur, it was typically performed following dirty household cleaning tasks (such as sweeping or cleaning the toilet). Hand rinsing was commonly practiced before eating and feeding children. While people knew that ash could be used as an alternative to soap, this practice was not seen during any of the observations.

Determinants of handwashing behaviour

We identified a range of context specific determinants of handwashing behaviour across the 16 BCD categories (See S5 Table for full list of determinants and their association with handwashing behaviour). Some of the determinants assessed appeared to have no impact on handwashing behaviour including ethnicity, religion, education level, sociality, access to ash, descriptive norms, knowledge about key handwashing moments, and the motives of comfort and affiliation. Below we describe determinants that had a reported positive or negative influence on behaviour.

Knowledge. All participants were familiar with cholera, its symptoms (e.g. mentioning cholera-specific symptoms like ‘rice water’ stools), and recommended prevention behaviours. However, cholera was often used as a catch-all term to describe a range of diarrhoeal diseases. Handwashing knowledge was high with all participants able to list critical moments for handwashing and explain how handwashing can interrupt disease transmission. Participants attributed their familiarity with cholera and preventative behaviours such as handwashing to frequent exposure to hygiene promotion activities.

Physical environment and behavioural settings. Across the various settings within study site, there was little in the physical environment to enable or cue handwashing at key times. In discussions with participants, they would often differentiate between handwashing being easy to do as a behaviour, but difficult for them to practice because they lacked the products (soap) and infrastructure (water and handwashing facilities) which could facilitate it. Most research participants were agricultural labourers and spent the majority of their days outside the home. During this time people typically had no access to soap or water, preventing handwashing from taking place. Within the camp settings, NGOs had constructed simple bucket-style handwashing facilities and in some of the rural settings tippy-taps had been promoted. However almost all of these were non-functional at the time of our research. Participants also admitted that even when these facilities were functional, water, soap and ash were not readily available at the stations. In FGDs participants agreed it was important to have “somewhere special” for handwashing. They felt handwashing stations acted as a reminder to wash hands at key times and helped to inculcate good habits in children. However, the basic handwashing facilities promoted by NGOs were seen as being “poor designs, for poor people”. They tended to break easily and therefore failed to have a lasting impact on behaviour. Participants felt handwashing facilities should symbolise hygiene, rather than just facilitating handwashing:

“It is essential to have a beautiful, an attractive hand washing facility so one is at ease when washing handsthe hand washing facility has to be always kept clean so that it does not disgust, and a facility has to be respected by the whole family and everyone. (Male FGD participant)

Almost all participants reported that water scarcity was a major barrier to hand hygiene and a source of stress within their lives. In general, participants did not think handwashing consumed much water. However, water access and usage was carefully calculated and prioritised for other household tasks like bathing, cooking, laundry and dishes. While water was often sectioned out for different purposes within the home, but no families felt they could easily put water aside for handwashing. Observations indicated people used a range of water for handwashing including washing their hands directly in nearby lakes and rivers and re-using grey water (e.g. water from dishwashing or laundry).

Soap was a valued and scarce commodity. Overall, 62% of households had no soap of any kind available at the time of our visit. Among those who did have soap, it was typically kept on a high shelf in the bedroom and therefore not conveniently available for handwashing. No one reported buying soap just for the purpose of handwashing. Rather, soap was typically purchased when laundry needed to be done and then a small leftover section may be used for bathing and handwashing. While laundry soap was affordable it was not desirable for handwashing:

“You know, this [laundry soap] is not a soap we want to use because it can damage hands, but we just use it because of poverty. (Female FGD participant)

As with water, soap use within the household involved conscious trade-offs and decision-making between family members:

“Getting soap is not easy…In a house of nine children, you understand that a piece of soap will not be prioritized for hand washing. If we manage to afford the piece of soap for 100 Congolese Francs only once a weekHow, and on what, can you use just a small piece of soap? Will you use for laundry? For bathing? Or for hand washing? Things become complicated. (Female FGD participant)

Participants explained that it was relatively common to ask neighbours for soap and water if needed but said people would laugh at you if you asked for these items for handwashing.

The social environment—norms, routines and social influence

Daily routines were unpredictable for most of the participants, with individuals searching for employment in the fields of others on a day-to-day basis. Daily routines were further complicated by intermittent water supplies and individuals could spend several hours per day searching for water they considered to be safe. Combined, these factors created time and financial pressure. The irregularity of routines, and the daily stressors that accompanied this, decreased the likelihood of handwashing habits forming. This was because there were few routine sequences of behaviour to cue handwashing and there was limited ability to make plans related to handwashing (e.g. to budget to have enough soap in the house).

Handwashing was seen as socially desirable and an injunctive norm. Hand hygiene norms were heightened by the outbreak, with participants estimating 65% of their community had increased their frequency of handwashing due to cholera concerns. Despite this, social sanctions or judgment related to not washing hands was low:

“I cannot judge people around me for not being clean because sometimes I am not clean tooI think only about 30% of people would judge me negatively [if they saw me not washing my hands] because people are not focused on hand washing behaviour; they can take it as normal that people sometimes forget to wash their hands. (Female IDI participant from a comparison household)

Participants explained that people easily forgave each other for not washing hands because of their difficult circumstances. Social support for handwashing was limited. Participants felt handwashing could be facilitated by family members reminding each other to wash hands at key times, but this rarely happened in practice. The importance of handwashing behaviour was consistently reinforced by NGOs and while some people felt the repetition of handwashing messages helped to remind people, others found it frustrating that these organisations were unable to realise changes to their broader circumstances which would allow them to practice handwashing more regularly.

Motives

In FGDs participants were asked about which motives were associated with handwashing. Motives of love, attractiveness and status (e.g. wealth, education and social respect) were thought to be strongly associated with handwashing:

“No one can fall in love with someone with dirty hands! (Female FGD participant)

“An attractive person is likely to remember to wash hands because she is used to looking nice, so, her hands have to look nice as well. (Female FGD participant)

“Highly educated people are always clean because they do work with white papers and with clean things, and so they also have to have clean hands. And again, these educated people teach others; they cannot go in front, teaching others when they are unclean. They have to have clean hands so that people consider and respect them. (Male FGD participant)

Handwashing was less strongly associated with nurture and was not seen to be associated with affiliation (fitting in with a group). Participants explained that even ‘good parents’ are unable to mind their children and encourage handwashing behaviour because everyone has to work long hours outside the home. Others explained that relationships were typically built on shared interests and needs and so handwashing didn’t necessarily help a person to fit in. Participants in IDIs and FGDs agreed that if a person was hungry, poor or upset they would be unable to prioritise handwashing:

“Hygiene and good nutrition work together. Cleanliness cannot be visible in a house where food is absent. I can’t think about handwashing when I am so hungry. Another thing is the kind of life I live, since I am IDP, it has brought me to trauma or psychological problems. Hygiene has become difficult because of the many thoughts crossing my mind like: How my children are going to eat? How am I going to get money? It’s difficult. (Female IDI participant from a comparison household)

Hunger emerged as a particularly prominent barrier to hand hygiene in this setting. At critical moments for handwashing, such as preparing or eating food, people’s hunger would override all else, causing people to forget handwashing. Hunger also caused prevented people from making plans that could facilitate handwashing. For example, participants explained that their limited daily earnings are entirely consumed by purchasing food:

“If I do not go to work in the farms all day, we shall not eat… But if you are only able to earn 2500 Congolese Francs, then all of this money will have to go on food. We ask ourselves whether to buy soap… but it is difficult to choose this rather than prioritizing food for our children. (Female FGD participant)

Demographic characteristics Certain personal characteristics influenced handwashing behaviour. Older adults and men who lived alone often were unable to collect sufficient water to meet their needs and therefore reported making handwashing compromises. Their reduced water availability was due to accessibility or cultural barriers associated with gender norms (i.e. generally it was only women who collected water). IDPs typically faced more challenging living conditions than host community members and often described feeling that they were living “like animal”‘ or “living a life that was not our own”. Consequently, many IDP participants reported that the ‘problem of handwashing’ was new for them and if their old lives could be restored their behaviour would also improve:

“I can tell you that I had a good life, I was a rich personbut my life changed with displacement…the situation changed to bad and today I am as you see me. take me back to my previous life and you will see my feelings and emotions will change and then I will have a high chance of washing his hands with soap. (Male FGD participant)

Variations in experiences of cholera and behavioural determinants between participants from case and comparison households

The determinants of handwashing behaviour among case households were consistent with those in comparison households across the determinant categories described above. However, there were substantial differences between the two groups on cholera-related risk perceptions and the perceived or actual consequences of cholera on people’s livelihoods and routines.

Participants from comparison households. Cholera was the main health concern of almost all participants, but among those who had not had a cholera case in their household, it was seen as a common, chronic challenge that was inseparable from other adversities they faced:

“Cholera is a major health problem here … because we work hard, earn less, eat less and rest less. As consequence, we lose weight and look pale and have poor nutrition… Then it is easy for the cholera disease to attack people. (Female FGD participant)

“It has been a long time since we do not have drinking water in this area and that is the reason why cholera disease attacked people… if you have bad food, unhealthy water and your hands are dirty then of course you will suffer from cholera. (Male FGD participant)

The high number of cases in the 2017 outbreak did heighten perceptions of risk and participants from comparison households reported realising cholera was serious within recent months. Participants from comparison households thought cholera generally affected children or people who were already ‘sickly’, ‘unclean’ or ‘pale’. Host community members thought that cholera was primarily a problem that affected IDPs. In terms of the social and economic impacts of cholera, participants from comparison households said they feared people who had cholera and would avoid them while infectious. Those without direct personal experiences of cholera thought its impacts on the lives of cases or their families would be relatively temporary, given that disease was seen to be easy to treat and participants felt that people tended to recover quickly. Participants were divided about whether getting cholera would have a longer-term impact on a person’s reputation:

“Someone who has had it [cholera] would not like people to know that he had it because cholera disease really affects a person’s dignity, nobody can wish to get it. It leaves you with no reputation at all. (Male FGD participant)

“Everyone understands…I mean most people know someone who has experienced cholera at some point… So the person who gets infected, he can still recover and get back to his normalcy. (Male FGD participant)

In many ways local explanatory disease models were aligned with ‘Western’ biomedical messaging about the cholera (presumably because of the history of health promotion in the region). However, misperceptions about cholera persisted within the community despite familiarity with the disease. Several participants said others in their community do not take cholera seriously because they believe “black people don’t die of germs”. Similarly, some people believed that a certain degree of exposure to “dirtiness” helps to protect you because being too clean may leave you vulnerable to infection. Others felt the continued presence of cholera in Eastern DRC played to the interests of humanitarian organisations:

“We know that when [organisations] come just sensitizing about hygiene and cholera, they are paid on this—it’s no use coming all the time disturbing us. They come for their own interest. (Male IDI participant from a comparison household)

Over decades people had realised that most humanitarian aid was provided during cholera outbreaks. These were short term projects which subsided as cholera cases decreased. People had also learned how to make the most of a system that didn’t always appear to have their ongoing interests at heart. For example, on several occasions during our research people from the community who were not participants, approached us with lists of ‘cholera cases’ who needed help.

Participants from case households. Participants from case households reported that they were easily able to recognise cholera symptoms. All of these households explained that cholera had affected them suddenly and unexpectedly. Some participants said this in a literal sense, reflecting that people commonly went from feeling healthy to suddenly experiencing vomiting and diarrhoea which lead them to become so weak that they were unable to do anything within hours. More commonly, when participants said cholera was “unexpected” they were expressing that they struggled comprehend how the disease had been able to launch a “surprise attack” on a family like them. Participants therefore attributed their illness to a one-off “mistake” in their behaviour.

When the disease initially “attacks”, family members reported being worried. Patients described feeling “empty”, “not of this world” and “seeing only death before them”. When patients returned home from the CTC, their cholera experience was not over. Cholera cases described feeling “stuck” and “destabilized” in multiple aspects of their life. Patients were unable to do agricultural work or household tasks for about a month because they felt “weak like paper”. Given the majority of households in this region survived day-to-day, earning less than 3000 Congolese Francs per day (1.5 USD), this inability to work rapidly put families in a state of economic crisis:

“I cannot say that my economy decreased—it was totally blocked! (Female IDI participant who had cholera)

“A huge economic impact was observed…during that period of our child’s sickness, things really got harder. Although we generally eat badly, that particular time my husband was staying with the child at the hospital, we then ate more badly than usual because of the little amount of money—everything went really bad. (Female IDI participant who had a child with cholera)

People also reported that cholera affected their roles, responsibilities, and sense of self. Participants felt cholera caused their attractiveness to “fade” and that they now “hated their outlook” due to the amount of weight they had lost. Parents who had been cholera cases felt worried that in the months following their discharge they had become unable to care and provide for their children:

“I became like a baby…I had to wait for somebody else to take care of me, like the neighbours, it is like I have lost my role of mother to all these children. (Female IDI participant who had cholera)

“Suffering from cholera reduced considerably my responsibility as a father, I could not feel respected, and I could not feel myself as a father of the family because I was half-dead. (Male IDI participant who had cholera)

Another woman explained that she relied on her neighbour to breastfeed her new born baby for several months while she was sick with cholera. Children and older members of the household often had to stand in for parents to do the household chores. Relationships with neighbours and friends also changed. Some participants explained neighbours were integral in helping them through their recovery and that they often gave them food, money and water (although it was expected the household would find a way to pay this back):

“I am still feeling weak and have no money to buy food because I am not working, so some neighbours give me food to eat, but you understand how difficult it is to depend on someone else’s kitchen. (Female IDI participant who had cholera)

Others explained that neighbours and friends stayed away following their illness and that they felt isolated and stigmatised:

“The relationship with neighbours does change because of gossip, they start saying that it is because of your uncleanness…they end up avoiding you” (Man IDI participant who’s elderly brother had cholera)

“I lived an isolated life during those hard periods of cholera cases. (Female IDI participant who had 3 children who had cholera)

However, participants from case households reported being more motivated to wash their hands, explaining this was because they recognised their vulnerability to disease. They also used handwashing as a way of countering any misperceptions from neighbours about their cleanliness. The latter concern seemed to prompt a range of demonstrative action around handwashing. For example, several participants said they have actively encouraged others to practice handwashing:

“Though we got infected, we cannot feel discouraged from doing hygiene practices…we even try to improve it and we are telling our neighbours that they should keep on practicing hygienic behaviour because that is the only way to prevent cholera…And other people around here, when they see how clean you are, even if you got sick with cholera, they can decide to take you as their good example of cleanliness” (Male IDI participant who had 2 children with cholera)

One participant built a dedicated place for handwashing near the toilet after his daughter was admitted to hospital with cholera:

“I realise that fighting this cholera disease is very serious these days and so I thought that once we are practicing hand washing, we will be able to prevent our family from this disease…So I decided I had to make the place for handwashing, a place that would be respectedNow the neighbours are just appreciating [the facility] and I am telling them to do the same as I did, but whether they agree or not, I will never give up with the practice. (Male IDI participant whose daughter had cholera)

However, households with cholera cases also felt their circumstances following their exposure to the disease made it more challenging to practice handwashing. This was due to their reduced physical health, their inability to collect sufficient water (most households reported being able to access half as much water in the period post discharge as compared to their normal circumstances), increased hunger and malnutrition (due to loss of income), and difficulties affording other basic daily necessities, such as soap. A minority of participants were given a small bar of soap and six water purification tables upon discharge from the CTC. Participants who received these distributions, reported trying to use these sparingly to make them last as long as possible.

Discussion

Our research found that in the Eastern region of DRC, cholera is generally conceptualised as a persistent and commonplace health challenge but also one that is easily treatable. Frequent hygiene promotion sessions in this region have led to high levels of knowledge about the health impacts of cholera, its symptoms and recommended preventative behaviours. However, handwashing with soap was observed to be rare in this setting. By using theory-driven qualitative methods we were able to identify that this was because the psychological, social, and environmental behavioural determinants affecting handwashing in this context combined to limit the ability of individuals to improve their handwashing behaviour. Major barriers to handwashing related to the physical environment or behavioural setting included the absence of handwashing facilities, water scarcity, the unaffordability of soap, the small make-shift houses where displaced populations lived, the use of shared sanitation facilities, and the extended periods people spent working outside of the home. Handwashing behaviour was also hampered by broader experiences of living in poverty and within in a dynamic conflict-prone region with high rates of displacement and livelihood fragility. This was because handwashing was often deprioritised because of hunger, mental health challenges, the unpredictability of routines, and the lack of social support and sanctions around handwashing. The experiences of participants from case households indicated that in complex crises, cholera can have profound non-health impacts on a household’s income, productivity, social status, and sense of control–factors which in turn create additional barriers to handwashing.

Despite low rates of actual handwashing practice, our research participants reported handwashing had increased as concerns about cholera were heightened. Many participants felt these changes in behaviour might be sustained beyond the outbreak. Prior literature has indicated self-reported handwashing behaviour tends to increase during outbreaks [15, 2731], however, studies which use observational measures of behaviour show much lower rates of practice even during outbreaks [32]. Such findings act as a reminder that research exploring handwashing behaviour should prioritise including observational methods to gauge actual practice given that self-reported behaviour is commonly affected by social desirability bias, and that this bias may be heightened in outbreaks [33]. However, it may also be indicative how behaviour may fluctuate over the course of outbreaks. For example, initial gains in the frequency of handwashing behaviour at the onset of an outbreak seem to decline or vary over time as fear associated with the disease subsides or the disease is normalised [27, 34, 35]. One handwashing study published during the COVID-19 pandemic suggested that such patterns in behaviour may be explained through Terror Management Theory (TMT) [35]. This theory suggests that when the threat to our mortality from a disease is made more salient, we are more likely to adopt health behaviours, like handwashing, that can remove this threat from our focal attention [3537]. This theory also explains that when the disease threat is no longer the focus of our attention, protective behaviours may start to decline. In our study participants were aware of the proximal threat of cholera in their region but adopted other psychological defences (such as perceiving others to be at greater risk than them and believing “black people don’t die of germs”) which avoided the threat and made this reality easier to cope with on a day-to-day basis. TMT might also explain why case households were more driven to take demonstrative action around handwashing following their recent brush with death. In contrast, comparison households in our study site were pre-occupied by more salient threats to their mortality such as hunger and conflict. As such their daily behaviour was geared to the reduction of these threats rather than cholera prevention behaviours. There are few studies which explore how stress or external threats may affect the prioritisation of handwashing behaviour, however, consistent with our results, one study among health care workers in a high-income setting indicated that stress, cognitive load and threats to mortality that appear more urgent or proximal, may impair a person’s ability to practice handwashing [38]. Our findings challenge the common belief that if people understand the benefits of handwashing they will act ‘rationally’ during an outbreak and wash their hands more frequently to protect themselves and others [16, 39].

In our study, households with cholera cases experienced the disease as an exogenous shock to their already vulnerable state which plunged their household into a state of acute socio-economic crisis. This household-level crisis was characterised by a sudden but extended loss of income, increased hunger, isolation from social support systems, feeling unable to provide for family members, and feeling that their exposure to cholera may tarnish their social standing in the long term. These lived experiences of cholera are consistent with existing, but limited, literature from other settings where cholera outbreaks occur during complex crises or within fragile states and among populations with high levels of poverty [40, 41]. Our study found that exposure to cholera decreased the household’s access to food and made it hard to prioritise handwashing due to a reduced ability to access water purchase soap in the wake of their illness. This presents a critical challenge for cholera control given that hand hygiene is likely to be key to interrupting transmission during the 10 days when cases are hyper-infective following infection [42] and v. cholerae continues to be shed in their faeces. There is also some evidence that pre-existing and continued malnutrition during this period may prolong shedding [6]. Our findings support the likely effectiveness of targeted WASH interventions distributions of hygiene kits [12, 43] and suggests that these could be complemented by the distribution of food items in some settings.

Prior research in this region of DRC has highlighted that hygiene programming may be met with reduced acceptability if it is inadequately resourced, poorly contextualised, fails to acknowledge other priorities of the population, or does not address social and environmental factors that may constrain behaviour [44, 45]. Broader research has also indicated that during complex crises, experiences and responses to cholera outbreaks are associated with, and amplified by, structural and social vulnerabilities such as extreme poverty, conflict and displacement [4648]. Our findings are consistent with this body of research and indicate that hygiene programming aimed at mitigating cholera transmission amid complex crises is likely to be more effective if it is integrated into longer-term initiatives that focus on these larger vulnerabilities, such as food security, livelihoods and psychosocial support initiatives. In contexts where cholera is endemic, handwashing programmers must move beyond health-education and work with communities to build enabling environments through investment in handwashing facilities and reliable water supply systems, and supportive social structures. Participants in our study highlighted the importance of conveniently located, desirable and durable facilities in cueing behaviour at key times and this is supported by broader literature [15]. Our research also identified examples of adaptive coping strategies utilised by the population to facilitate handwashing behaviour, reduce vulnerability, and increase their sense of control over the unpredictability of their circumstances. Coping strategies included the use of surface water or grey water for handwashing, the use of ash when soap was unavailable, the pooling or water and soap resources within compounds, the careful calculation of water and soap use to facilitate all necessary household tasks and encouraging neighbours to remind all children within a compound about handwashing. While these actions were taken by a minority of households in our study, they could easily be shared and adopted by others by utilising a positive deviance approach [49]. Research in previous outbreaks has highlighted the importance of understanding whether local coping mechanisms are aligned with, juxtaposed to, or are able to fill gaps in government and organisation-led disease prevention strategies [5052]. Experiences during prior outbreaks has also emphasised that an overreliance on biomedical explanations of disease can be met with resistance from populations [16, 53, 54]. If health promotion fails to acknowledge emic perspectives and experiences it has the tendency to isolate the disease from its human host and the social experiences that facilitate transmission [55]. Our findings suggest that handwashing programmes should aim to change the public health discourse around cholera-related risk by focusing on local constructions of disease, the experiences of populations, and by communicating the non-health impacts of the disease. This may allow populations to adjust their decision-making and coping mechanisms towards prioritising behaviours like handwashing—particularly if it is seen to have health, social and economic benefits in the long term. Lastly, our research found that case households were more motivated to practice handwashing after their exposure and were better able to act upon their behavioural intentions to encourage the behaviour in others and create an enabling physical environment for handwashing. Humanitarians could build upon this by inviting cholera cases to share their experiences with others in the community. There is some evidence that this may be an effective way to motivate health behaviour, challenge misperceptions around diseases and to heighten perceived vulnerability in a way that is more sustainable than focusing on fear alone [56].

Limitations

Our research was primarily interested in exploring how the determinants of handwashing behaviour were affected by a cholera outbreak. While observed and self-reported behaviours are described qualitatively in this study, the methods were not designed to be representative and therefore this data could usefully be complemented by further research which measures actual behaviour before, during and after outbreaks in regions that are prone to them.

Where possible we used participatory activities that have been used in prior research however some new approaches were developed to explore motives, water prioritisation and experiences of conflict. Replication of these methods would be useful to demonstrate their validity and reliability.

Our sampling was guided by case lists from the CTC, however in this region cholera case admission is not always laboratory confirmed. Other research from DRC has shown that only a minority of those admitted to CTCs actually had cholera [57] and therefore this may skew some of our research findings in relation to experiences of the disease. As noted, cholera was often used by research participants to be a catch all term for diarrhoeal diseases, this emic construction may have therefore also distorted the way people described their experiences and perceptions in relation to the disease.

This research was conducted in partnership with Action Contre la Faim and for security reasons our research team were required to wear a branded vest throughout data collection and travelled in a branded vehicle. Given that the organisation have a history of working on WASH projects in this region and that participants had been exposed to decades of humanitarian response programmes, this may have increased willingness to participate and resulted in more socially desirable answers. The research team tried to reflect on this during daily research discussions and consider how our individual and collective positionalities may have shaped our findings.

Conclusion

In addition to having severe health implications, outbreaks have the potential to disrupt people’s social, psychological, and economic lives. By focusing on the lived experiences of cholera, our research highlighted that even when substantial shifts in behavioural determinants occur, it is not always enough to substantially influence the uptake of preventive behaviours like handwashing with soap. In this case, handwashing behaviour remained low during the outbreak due to the absence of enabling physical and social environments and the competing priorities and vulnerabilities of the population. Handwashing programmes targeting areas with endemic cholera or outbreaks within complex crises could be strengthened by acknowledging the underlying circumstances that create and perpetuate outbreaks, addressing the health and non-health impacts of diseases like cholera, investing in sustainable handwashing infrastructure, and identifying and sharing local disease coping mechanisms that facilitate the practice of preventative behaviours.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Handwashing determinant definitions adapted from on the BCD checklist of determinants (1, 2) and accompanied by method selections.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Description and sample size for all methods done at a household or individual level.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Purpose, description and sample size for each of the methods done within group discussions.

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Inclusivity questionnaire.

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Identified determinants and their associated influence on handwashing behaviour in Eastern DRC.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the following people for helping to facilitate the research and contributing to ongoing reflections about emergent insights: Isiaka Hemedi, Justine Badhera Habamungu, Léon Ngwasi, Batian Arthur Benao, Marie-Paul Chirimwami, Karine Le Roch, Jean Lapegue and representatives of the Goma WASH Cluster. We would particularly like to thank the individuals who gave of their time to participate in this research and who welcomed us into their homes and shared their personal experiences so openly.

This research was undertaken as part of the Wash’Em Project which aims to improve handwashing promotion in humanitarian crises. The contents are the responsibility of the authors of the paper and do not necessarily reflect the views of our donors, USAID or the United States Government.

Data Availability

The supporting dataset for this work is available via Figshare at the following link at the following citation: White, Sian (2022): Interviews and group discussions with crisis and outbreak affected populations in the Democratic Republic of the Congo on handwashing determinants. figshare. Dataset. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.19469270.v1 Please note that this includes transcripts in English of all interviews and focus group discussions which have been redacted to remove any identifiable information. Interested researchers may also contact the corresponding author or the Research Governance and Integrity Office at LSHTM: RGIO@lshtm.ac.uk to access additional data.

Funding Statement

SW and TH recieved the funding from the United States Agency for International Development’s Bureau of Humanitarian Assistance (Grant number: AID-OFDA-G-16-00270). Donor website: https://www.usaid.gov/who-we-are/organization/bureaus/bureau-humanitarian-assistance The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Global Task Force on Cholera Control. Ending Cholera: A Global Roadmap to 2030. 2017.
  • 2.Ali M, Nelson AR, Lopez AL, Sack DA. Updated global burden of cholera in endemic countries. PLoS neglected tropical diseases. 2015;9(6):e0003832–e. doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0003832 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.World Health Organisaiton. Weekly Epidemiological Record (WER)—September 2018. Weekly Epidemiological Record. 2018;93(38):489–500. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Phelps MD, Azman AS, Lewnard JA, Antillón M, Simonsen L, Andreasen V, et al. The importance of thinking beyond the water-supply in cholera epidemics: A historical urban case-study. PLOS Neglected Tropical Diseases. 2017;11(11):e0006103. doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0006103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Debes AK, Ali M, Azman AS, Yunus M, Sack DA. Cholera cases cluster in time and space in Matlab, Bangladesh: implications for targeted preventive interventions. International journal of epidemiology. 2016;45(6):2134–9. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyw267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Weil AA, Begum Y, Chowdhury F, Khan AI, Leung DT, LaRocque RC, et al. Bacterial shedding in household contacts of cholera patients in Dhaka, Bangladesh. The American journal of tropical medicine and hygiene. 2014;91(4):738–42. doi: 10.4269/ajtmh.14-0095 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Blackburn JK, Diamond U, Kracalik IT, Widmer J, Brown W, Morrissey BD, et al. Household-level spatiotemporal patterns of incidence of cholera, Haiti, 2011. Emerg Infect Dis. 2014;20(9):1516–9. doi: 10.3201/eid2009.131882 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Richterman A, Sainvilien DR, Eberly L, Ivers LC. Individual and Household Risk Factors for Symptomatic Cholera Infection: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. J Infect Dis. 2018;218(suppl_3):S154–s64. doi: 10.1093/infdis/jiy444 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.D’Mello-Guyett L, Gallandat K, Van den Bergh R, Taylor D, Bulit G, Legros D, et al. Prevention and control of cholera with household and community water, sanitation and hygiene (WASH) interventions: A scoping review of current international guidelines. PLOS ONE. 2020;15(1):e0226549. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0226549 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Wolfe M, Kaur M, Yates T, Woodin M, Lantagne D. A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of the Association between Water, Sanitation, and Hygiene Exposures and Cholera in Case-Control Studies. The American journal of tropical medicine and hygiene. 2018;99(2):534–45. doi: 10.4269/ajtmh.17-0897 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ram P. Practical Guidance for Measuring Handwashing Behavior. WSP website: Water and Sanitation Program 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.George CM, Monira S, Sack DA, Rashid M-u, Saif-Ur-Rahman KM, Mahmud T, et al. Randomized Controlled Trial of Hospital-Based Hygiene and Water Treatment Intervention (CHoBI7) to Reduce Cholera. Emerging Infectious Diseases. 2016;22(2):233–41. doi: 10.3201/eid2202.151175 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Taylor DL, Kahawita TM, Cairncross S, Ensink JHJ. The Impact of Water, Sanitation and Hygiene Interventions to Control Cholera: A Systematic Review. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(8):e0135676. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0135676 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.De Buck E, Van Remoortel H, Hannes K, Govender T, Naidoo S, Avau B, et al. Approaches to promote handwashing and sanitation behaviour change in low-and middle income countries: a mixed method systematic review. Campbell Systematic Reviews. 2017;7:1–447. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.White S, Thorseth AH, Dreibelbis R, Curtis V. The determinants of handwashing behaviour in domestic settings: An integrative systematic review. International Journal of Hygiene and Environmental Health. 2020;227:113512. doi: 10.1016/j.ijheh.2020.113512 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Czerniewska A, White S. Hygiene programming during outbreaks: a qualitative case study of the humanitarian response during the Ebola outbreak in Liberia. BMC public health. 2020;20(1):154. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-8240-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Luby SP, Davis J, Brown RR, Gorelick SM, Wong THF. Broad approaches to cholera control in Asia: Water, sanitation and handwashing. Vaccine. 2020;38:A110–A7. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2019.07.084 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.D’Mello-Guyett L, Yates T, Bastable A, Dahab M, Deola C, Dorea C, et al. Setting priorities for humanitarian water, sanitation and hygiene research: a meeting report. Conflict and Health. 2018;12(1):22. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Ingelbeen B, Hendrickx D, Miwanda B, van der Sande MAB, Mossoko M, Vochten H, et al. Recurrent Cholera Outbreaks, Democratic Republic of the Congo, 2008–2017. Emerging infectious diseases. 2019;25(5):856–64. doi: 10.3201/eid2505.181141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bompangue D, Giraudoux P, Handschumacher P, Piarroux M, Sudre B, Ekwanzala M, et al. Lakes as Source of Cholera Outbreaks, Democratic Republic of Congo. Emerging Infectious Diseases. 2008;14(5):798–800. doi: 10.3201/eid1405.071260 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Bompangue D, Giraudoux P, Piarroux M, Mutombo G, Shamavu R, Sudre B, et al. Cholera epidemics, war and disasters around Goma and Lake Kivu: an eight-year survey. PLoS Negl Trop Dis. 2009;3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Aunger R, Curtis V. Behaviour Centred Design: Toward and applied science of behaviour change. Health Psychology Review 2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.White S, Heath T, Ibrahim WK, Ihsan D, Blanchet K, Curtis V, et al. How does hygiene behaviour change over the course of displacement? A qualitative case study in Iraq and Kurdistan. PloS One. In Press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.McMahon SA, Winch PJ. Systematic debriefing after qualitative encounters: an essential analysis step in applied qualitative research. BMJ Global Health. 2018;3(5):e000837. doi: 10.1136/bmjgh-2018-000837 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Schatzman L, Strauss AL. Field research: Strategies for a natural sociology: Prentice Hall; 1973. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Braun V, Clarke V. Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology. 2006;3(2):pp. 77–101. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Babalola S, Krenn S, Rimal R, Serlemitsos E, Shaivitz M, Shattuck D, et al. KAP COVID Dashboard. In: Johns Hopkins Center for Communication Programs GOAaRN, Facebook Data for Good., editor. Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Jones JH, Salathe M. Early assessment of anxiety and behavioral response to novel swine-origin influenza A(H1N1). PLoS ONE [Electronic Resource]. 2009;4(12):e8032. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.SteelFisher GK, Blendon RJ, Kang M, Ward JR, Kahn EB, Maddox KE, et al. Adoption of preventive behaviors in response to the 2009 H1N1 influenza pandemic: a multiethnic perspective. Influenza & Other Respiratory Viruses. 2015;9(3):131–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Timpka T, Spreco A, Gursky E, Eriksson O, Dahlstrom O, Stromgren M, et al. Intentions to perform non-pharmaceutical protective behaviors during influenza outbreaks in Sweden: a cross-sectional study following a mass vaccination campaign. PLoS ONE [Electronic Resource]. 2014;9(3):e91060. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0091060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Lau JT, Kim JH, Tsui HY, Griffiths S. Anticipated and current preventive behaviors in response to an anticipated human-to-human H5N1 epidemic in the Hong Kong Chinese general population. BMC Infectious Diseases. 2007;7:18. doi: 10.1186/1471-2334-7-18 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Zohura F, Bhuyian SI, Monira S, Begum F, Biswas SK, Parvin T, et al. Observed Handwashing with Soap Practices Among Cholera Patients and Accompanying Household Members in a Hospital Setting (CHoBI7 Trial). The American journal of tropical medicine and hygiene. 2016;95(6):1314–8. doi: 10.4269/ajtmh.16-0379 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Contzen N, De Pasquale S, Mosler H-J. Over-Reporting in Handwashing Self-Reports: Potential Explanatory Factors and Alternative Measurements. PLOS ONE. 2015;10(8):e0136445. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0136445 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Phillips RM, Vujcic J, Boscoe A, Handzel T, Aninyasi M, Cookson ST, et al. Soap is not enough: handwashing practices and knowledge in refugee camps, Maban County, South Sudan. Conflict and Health. 2015;9:39. doi: 10.1186/s13031-015-0065-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Szczuka Z, Abraham C, Baban A, Brooks S, Cipolletta S, Danso E, et al. The trajectory of COVID-19 pandemic and handwashing adherence: findings from 14 countries. BMC public health. 2021;21(1):1791. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-11822-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Pyszczynski T, Solomon S, Greenberg J. Chapter One—Thirty Years of Terror Management Theory: From Genesis to Revelation. In: Olson JM, Zanna MP, editors. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 52: Academic Press; 2015. p. 1–70. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Goldenberg JL, Arndt J. The implications of death for health: a terror management health model for behavioral health promotion. Psychol Rev. 2008;115(4):1032–53. doi: 10.1037/a0013326 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Sands M, Aunger R. Determinants of hand hygiene compliance among nurses in US hospitals: A formative research study. PLOS ONE. 2020;15(4):e0230573. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0230573 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Wang X, Gao D, Wang J. Influence of human behavior on cholera dynamics. Mathematical Biosciences. 2015;267(Supplement C):41–52. doi: 10.1016/j.mbs.2015.06.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.‘People Were Dying Like Flies’: The Social Contours of Cholera in Harare’s High-Density Townships. In: Chigudu S, editor. The Political Life of an Epidemic: Cholera, Crisis and Citizenship in Zimbabwe. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2020. p. 155–83. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(20)30249-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Guillaume Y, Ternier R, Vissieres K, Casseus A, Chery MJ, Ivers LC. Responding to Cholera in Haiti: Implications for the National Plan to Eliminate Cholera by 2022. The Journal of Infectious Diseases. 2018;218(suppl_3):S167–S70. doi: 10.1093/infdis/jiy491 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.World Health Organisaiton. Cholera Fact Sheet WHO Website: WHO; 2021. [Available from: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/cholera]. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.D’Mello-Guyett L, Cumming O, Bonneville S, D’hondt R, Mashako M, Nakoka B, et al. Effectiveness of hygiene kit distribution to reduce cholera transmission in Kasaï-Oriental, Democratic Republic of Congo, 2018: a prospective cohort study. BMJ open. 2021;11(10):e050943. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2021-050943 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Blum LS, Yemweni A, Trinies V, Kambere M, Tolani F, Allen JV, et al. Programmatic implications for promotion of handwashing behavior in an internally displaced persons camp in North Kivu, Democratic Republic of Congo. Conflict and Health. 2019;13(1):54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Claude KM, Serge MS, Alexis KK, Hawkes MT. Prevention of COVID-19 in Internally Displaced Persons Camps in War-Torn North Kivu, Democratic Republic of the Congo: A Mixed-Methods Study. Global Health: Science and Practice. 2020;8(4):638. doi: 10.9745/GHSP-D-20-00272 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Talavera A, Perez EM. Is cholera disease associated with poverty? The Journal of Infection in Developing Countries. 2009;3(06). doi: 10.3855/jidc.410 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Collins AE, Lucas ME, Islam MS, Williams LE. Socio‐economic and environmental origins of cholera epidemics in Mozambique: guidelines for tackling uncertainty in infectious disease prevention and control. International Journal of Environmental Studies. 2006;63(5):537–49. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Sim C. Control and Intervention of Cholera Outbreaks in Refugee Camps. Global Societies Journal. 2013;1:65–80. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Sternin J. Positive Deviance: A New Paradigm for Addressing Today’s Problems Today. The Journal of Corporate Citizenship. 2002(5):57–62. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Nalin DR, Haque Z. Part three: Folk beliefs about cholera among Bengali Muslims and Mogh Buddhists in Chittagong, Bangladesh. Medical Anthropology. 1977;1(3):55–68. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Richards P, Mokuwa GA, Vandi A, Mayhew SH, Ebola Gbalo Research T. Re-analysing Ebola spread in Sierra Leone: The importance of local social dynamics. PLOS ONE. 2020;15(11):e0234823. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0234823 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Parker M, Hanson TM, Vandi A, Babawo LS, Allen T. Ebola and Public Authority: Saving Loved Ones in Sierra Leone. Medical Anthropology. 2019;38(5):440–54. doi: 10.1080/01459740.2019.1609472 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.de Vries DH, Rwemisisi JT, Musinguzi LK, Benoni TE, Muhangi D, de Groot M, et al. The first mile: community experience of outbreak control during an Ebola outbreak in Luwero District, Uganda. BMC public health. 2016;16(1):161. doi: 10.1186/s12889-016-2852-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Cohn S, Kutalek R. Historical Parallels, Ebola Virus Disease and Cholera: Understanding Community Distrust and Social Violence with Epidemics. PLoS currents. 2016;8:ecurrents.outbreaks.aa1f2b60e8d43939b43fbd93e1a63a94. doi: 10.1371/currents.outbreaks.aa1f2b60e8d43939b43fbd93e1a63a94 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Abdalla M, editor Beyond Biomedicine: Sub-Saharan Africa and the struggle for HIV/AIDS discourse2014.
  • 56.Ziebland S, Wyke S. Health and Illness in a Connected World: How Might Sharing Experiences on the Internet Affect People’s Health? The Milbank Quarterly. 2012;90(2):219–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0009.2012.00662.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Williams C, Cumming O, Grignard L, Rumedeka BB, Saidi JM, Grint D, et al. Prevalence and diversity of enteric pathogens among cholera treatment centre patients with acute diarrhea in Uvira, Democratic Republic of Congo. BMC Infectious Diseases. 2020;20(1):741. doi: 10.1186/s12879-020-05454-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Alison Parker

2 Feb 2022

PONE-D-21-38155How does handwashing behaviour change in response to a cholera outbreak? A qualitative case study in the Democratic Republic of the Congo.PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. White,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. The reviewers are generally impressed with the paper but have some specific comments for improvement which I hope you are able to follow carefully and respond to.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Mar 19 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Alison Parker

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

1. When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2.  Please include a complete copy of PLOS’ questionnaire on inclusivity in global research in your revised manuscript. Our policy for research in this area aims to improve transparency in the reporting of research performed outside of researchers’ own country or community. The policy applies to researchers who have travelled to a different country to conduct research, research with Indigenous populations or their lands, and research on cultural artefacts. The questionnaire can also be requested at the journal’s discretion for any other submissions, even if these conditions are not met.  Please find more information on the policy and a link to download a blank copy of the questionnaire here: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/best-practices-in-research-reporting. Please upload a completed version of your questionnaire as Supporting Information when you resubmit your manuscript.

3. We note that you have stated that you will provide repository information for your data at acceptance. Should your manuscript be accepted for publication, we will hold it until you provide the relevant accession numbers or DOIs necessary to access your data. If you wish to make changes to your Data Availability statement, please describe these changes in your cover letter and we will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide.

4. Please amend your authorship list in your manuscript file to include author  Sian White, Anna C Mutula, Modeste M Buroko, Thomas Heath, François K Mazimwe, Karl Blanchet, Val Curtis, Robert Dreibelbis. 

5. We note you have included a table to which you do not refer in the text of your manuscript. Please ensure that you refer to Table 1 in your text; if accepted, production will need this reference to link the reader to the Table.

6. Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: N/A

Reviewer #2: N/A

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Overall, this was well designed matched case-control study and the matching criteria were clearly laid out. However, there following would need some clarification.

Methods

1. Line 40-42 states the objective was to find out how the people responded to the cholera outbreak and how the outbreak affected their handwashing behavior. Is it right to assume that you wanted to find out if they washed their hands more often after the outbreak? How was this done/measured? was there a measurement of handwashing behavior before the outbreak to compare with that after the outbreak?

Results

From table 1, 21% lived in camps while the rest were lived in the community, was there a difference in water supply between the camps and community? Did this difference have any impact on the handwashing behavior?

Summary and general comments

Thank you for the privilege to review this well written paper. The authors carried out a matched case-control study to examine the hygienic behavior of the people of Kivu during the 2017 cholera outbreak. These results will be very useful for the cholera outbreak literature. I suggest revising for key elements in the methods and results which need a bit more clarification. Again I commend the authors for clarity of thoughts which allowed me to only focus on the science.

Reviewer #2: Referee review for: "How does handwashing behavior change in response to a cholera outbreak? A qualitative case study in the Democratic Republic of Congo”

This study uses a mixed methods approach to examine perceptions and behaviors associated with handwashing with soap among households that had experienced cholera and households that had not experienced cholera during a cholera outbreak. The outbreak took place in 2017 in a resource poor community in the eastern DR Congo where endemic and epidemic cholera exists. Health education messages involving biomedical information about the health benefits of handwashing had been disseminated over time in the study community. Study results suggest the handwashing with soap was rare during the outbreak among both case and control households despite self-reports of handwashing. The findings highlight that competing demands related to food consumption and livelihoods constrained people living in both case and comparison households from practicing handwashing. The authors conclude that severe psychological, social, and economic impacts that occurred in case households increased barriers to handwashing. The authors underline that handwashing strategies in contexts with endemic and outbreaks of cholera should consider emic understandings of disease and promote sustainable strategies that take into account social and physical environmental factors, including access to handwashing materials, that influence handwashing behavior.

Comments

1. Little information is provided on the cholera outbreak. It would be helpful to include a description of the outbreak including how long it lasted and the number of cases and deaths.

2. The manuscript would benefit from a description of the messages provided to community members, including how messages were disseminated, the content of messages and how this changed during the epidemic, the frequency of dissemination, and the target audience. Were the messages focused on diarrhea prevention or were cholera-specific messages disseminated?

3. It is not clear how focus group discussion participants were selected. Also, a description of the composition of the focus groups is needed.

4. While the authors mention that some households participated in more than one method, there is no indication regarding the extent to which IDIs were carried out in households where structured observations took place. The methods section should provide a description indicating when more than one method was carried out in one household.

5. Little is said about the differences between the IDP and local populations and how their living environments and daily routines affected exposure to interventions, perceptions of cholera, and handwashing practices. It would be helpful to include a description of the different social and physical environments, as well as exposure to messaging, to understand how this may have improved or constrained handwashing. Also, a description of differences in the way the outbreak impacted IDPs and the local population would be helpful.

6. The study results suggest the cholera is a general term used to describe a range of diarrheal illness. However, there is no mention of how this may have influenced emic views and the cholera explanatory disease model related to disease severity, causal explanations, and transmission, as well as the perceived need to wash hands. In other words, to what extent were informants able to distinguish disease signs, symptoms, severity, causation, and transmission from other types of diarrheal illness.

7. The authors rightly advocate for incorporating emic views and local constructions of disease into messaging and intervention strategies. However, the manuscript lacks some key information on the local explanatory disease models that could potentially be useful when informing strategies. I wonder whether some of this information was collected during the participatory methods that were not presented in the results. If so, it would be useful to include more details on the local explanatory model of cholera and how this may have affected handwashing behaviors. In that regard, what were the results of the participatory methods and how were the findings used.

8. In the discussion, the authors cite literature suggesting that behaviors may change over the course of an outbreak. It would be helpful to know whether this research detected any behavioral changes over time as perceptions related to the disease and risk of exposure may have changed.

Other comments

Recent research carried out in an IDP camp in North Kivu, which highlights similar findings related to the prioritization of competing needs over handwashing, should be mentioned.

Some statements made in the first paragraph of the discussion section do not necessarily reflect the study findings. Examples include:

- Cholera is conceptualized as a health challenge that is easily treatable

- Handwashing was hampered due to the fact that the work took place in a conflict zone

- Health promotion has led to high levels of knowledge related to the health impacts of cholera

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Afoumbom Mildred Tita

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Attachment

Submitted filename: review of DRC handwashing mansucript.docx

PLoS One. 2022 Apr 12;17(4):e0266849. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0266849.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


25 Feb 2022

We thank both reviewers for their encouraging words of support and their constructive ideas for strengthening this manuscript. We have explained how we have taken on their feedback below:

Reviewer 1 Comments:

7. Methods: Line 40-42 states the objective was to find out how the people responded to the cholera outbreak and how the outbreak affected their handwashing behavior. Is it right to assume that you wanted to find out if they washed their hands more often after the outbreak? How was this done/measured? was there a measurement of handwashing behavior before the outbreak to compare with that after the outbreak?

Response: No this is an incorrect assumption. The objective is broader than what reviewer 1 assumes. Given that this was an exploratory study we did not start with a priori assumptions about how behaviour may change during an outbreak. The study focuses primarily on the determinants of behaviour (rather than behaviour itself) and how these may have been shifted by the outbreak. While observational measures of handwashing are included, they were only on a small sample and were designed to be qualitatively analysed rather than to indicate handwashing prevalence or frequency. Similarly participants self-reported their behaviour during many of the methods but we know that self-reported handwashing behaviour tends to overestimate practice. We have not adjusted the objective but rather have added some text in the limitations to better explain this.

8. Results: From table 1, 21% lived in camps while the rest were lived in the community, was there a difference in water supply between the camps and community? Did this difference have any impact on the handwashing behavior?

Response: This information has been added to the text. In summary there were no substantial differences.

9: Summary and general comments: Thank you for the privilege to review this well written paper. The authors carried out a matched case-control study to examine the hygienic behavior of the people of Kivu during the 2017 cholera outbreak. These results will be very useful for the cholera outbreak literature. I suggest revising for key elements in the methods and results which need a bit more clarification. Again I commend the authors for clarity of thoughts which allowed me to only focus on the science.

Response: We thank Reviewer 1 for their kind words of encouragement.

Reviewer 2 Comments

10. Little information is provided on the cholera outbreak. It would be helpful to include a description of the outbreak including how long it lasted and the number of cases and deaths.

Response: While in DRC data on cases and deaths were shared with us by individual health centers, humanitarian organizations and the district government. However this data was inconsistent and hard to verify. Total case numbers and mortality from the national outbreak are available and have been included in the text but unfortunately due to these data issues we have not been able to include anything more specific for the region. ‘Outbreaks’ of cholera in DRC are, sadly unending, with this region reporting endemic cases every week of the year. Therefore and outbreak within the DRC context refers to a dramatic rise in cases as is typically experienced most years between about August and January. We have not given this detail in the text as we have already mentioned that our study took place during the peak of the outbreak during October and November.

11. The manuscript would benefit from a description of the messages provided to community members, including how messages were disseminated, the content of messages and how this changed during the epidemic, the frequency of dissemination, and the target audience. Were the messages focused on diarrhea prevention or were cholera-specific messages disseminated?

Response: This was not assessed formally as part of this study. We have provided some additional description of this based on what participants told us. However we did conduct some parallel research with humanitarians about their programming throughout the Eastern region of DRC. This work has been submitted separately to Conflict and Health.

12. It is not clear how focus group discussion participants were selected. Also, a description of the composition of the focus groups is needed.

Response: Information on sampling is provided under the heading ‘sampling’ in the methods section. We have expanded on this information about the composition of the FGDs under this section of the methods.

13. While the authors mention that some households participated in more than one method, there is no indication regarding the extent to which IDIs were carried out in households where structured observations took place. The methods section should provide a description indicating when more than one method was carried out in one household.

Response: We have added this information under the IDI section.

14. Little is said about the differences between the IDP and local populations and how their living environments and daily routines affected exposure to interventions, perceptions of cholera, and handwashing practices. It would be helpful to include a description of the different social and physical environments, as well as exposure to messaging, to understand how this may have improved or constrained handwashing. Also, a description of differences in the way the outbreak impacted IDPs and the local population would be helpful.

Response: As noted above we have added some information on the similarity of WASH circumstances between IDPs and host communities. In the text we had tried to describe the different housing of both populations. Since these camps were informal and located within the heart of the communities there was little difference in exposure to hygiene promotion, however those outside the center of town were slightly less exposed as is now noted in the text. We have also included more detail on employment.

15. The study results suggest the cholera is a general term used to describe a range of diarrheal illness. However, there is no mention of how this may have influenced emic views and the cholera explanatory disease model related to disease severity, causal explanations, and transmission, as well as the perceived need to wash hands. In other words, to what extent were informants able to distinguish disease signs, symptoms, severity, causation, and transmission from other types of diarrheal illness.

Response: We have added content to the limitations section of the paper to address this point. We agree that this may have biased or distorted their reporting of experiences and perceptions towards the disease, however we don’t feel that this would have had a substantial bearing on understandings of disease causation and transmission given that this would be similar to many other diarrheal diseases (at least at a lay population level). Under the knowledge heading in the text I have added that while people viewed it as a catch all term people were able to easily identify cholera-specific symptoms such as rice-water stools.

16. The authors rightly advocate for incorporating emic views and local constructions of disease into messaging and intervention strategies. However, the manuscript lacks some key information on the local explanatory disease models that could potentially be useful when informing strategies. I wonder whether some of this information was collected during the participatory methods that were not presented in the results. If so, it would be useful to include more details on the local explanatory model of cholera and how this may have affected handwashing behaviors. In that regard, what were the results of the participatory methods and how were the findings used.

Response: As Reviewer 2 suspected the research aims and methods did not specifically seek to comprehensively understand local explanatory disease models. However quite a lot was learned about this during some of the participatory methods used in the IDIs and FGDs (e.g. personal histories, risk scaling, and cholera case descriptions). Most of these findings we have tried to capture in our results already. Specifically the following points are linked to this:

- Cholera is seen as a challenge closely associated with poverty and hunger

- Cholera is associated with people who are thin, pale and sickly (ie these individuals are considered more vulnerable).

- Understandings of cholera were aligned with western biomedical models of understanding disease (this point has been added in text) given the history of health promotion in the region.

- A minority of people felt that Congolese people are resilient to cholera or that if they got infected it wouldn’t cause them serious illness.

- A minority of people felt that if you are too clean you would actually be more vulnerable to cholera.

- A minority of people felt that it was in humanitarian and government interests for cholera outbreaks to continue.

- People who had had cholera tended to be more likely to see cholera as being random.

In terms of the second part of your question, participatory methods were used in throughout each of the IDIs and FGDs (see supplementary materials for full descriptions). Therefore all the content included in the results relates to these methods.

17. In the discussion, the authors cite literature suggesting that behaviors may change over the course of an outbreak. It would be helpful to know whether this research detected any behavioral changes over time as perceptions related to the disease and risk of exposure may have changed.

Response: It is perhaps worth noting that the literature cited in relation to this point is limited by its quality. As noted in text that is because the majority of these studies rely on self-reported behaviour. As noted in the text the consensus across such texts is that behaviour seems to fluctuate over the course of many outbreaks and that one of the many factors driving this appears to be perceptions about the disease and the fear associated with it. This indeed is the premise behind Terror Management Theory which is referenced in the text. Given that we feel we have already explained this literature and its implications we have not added anything to this section.

18. Recent research carried out in an IDP camp in North Kivu, which highlights similar findings related to the prioritization of competing needs over handwashing, should be mentioned.

Response: We have added in some additional content into the discussion section based on the work of Blum et al 2019 and Claude et al 2020.

19. Some statements made in the first paragraph of the discussion section do not necessarily reflect the study findings. Examples include: a) Cholera is conceptualized as a health challenge that is easily treatable b) Handwashing was hampered due to the fact that the work took place in a conflict zone, c) Health promotion has led to high levels of knowledge related to the health impacts of cholera.

Response: In relation to point a) this was mentioned in our results but has now been amended so this is a bit more explicit. In relation to point b) our discussion doesn’t quite claim that handwashing is hampered because of the conflict but rather the social and environmental factors that are associated with poverty, and the circumstances crated by conflict and displacement. We have tried to further clarify this nuance in the text. In relation to point c) We realize that while mentioned the high levels of knowledge in the results section we left out information on where participants felt this knowledge came from. This has now been added to the results section so this link is clearer.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Responses to reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Alison Parker

29 Mar 2022

How does handwashing behaviour change in response to a cholera outbreak? A qualitative case study in the Democratic Republic of the Congo.

PONE-D-21-38155R1

Dear Dr. White,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Alison Parker

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: N/A

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Afoumbom Mildred Tita

Acceptance letter

Alison Parker

1 Apr 2022

PONE-D-21-38155R1

How does handwashing behaviour change in response to a cholera outbreak? A qualitative case study in the Democratic Republic of the Congo.

Dear Dr. White:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Alison Parker

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. Handwashing determinant definitions adapted from on the BCD checklist of determinants (1, 2) and accompanied by method selections.

    (DOCX)

    S2 Table. Description and sample size for all methods done at a household or individual level.

    (DOCX)

    S3 Table. Purpose, description and sample size for each of the methods done within group discussions.

    (DOCX)

    S4 Table. Inclusivity questionnaire.

    (DOCX)

    S5 Table. Identified determinants and their associated influence on handwashing behaviour in Eastern DRC.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: review of DRC handwashing mansucript.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Responses to reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    The supporting dataset for this work is available via Figshare at the following link at the following citation: White, Sian (2022): Interviews and group discussions with crisis and outbreak affected populations in the Democratic Republic of the Congo on handwashing determinants. figshare. Dataset. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.19469270.v1 Please note that this includes transcripts in English of all interviews and focus group discussions which have been redacted to remove any identifiable information. Interested researchers may also contact the corresponding author or the Research Governance and Integrity Office at LSHTM: RGIO@lshtm.ac.uk to access additional data.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES