Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2022 Apr 14;17(4):e0263769. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0263769

Cross-disorder and disorder-specific deficits in social functioning among schizophrenia and alzheimer’s disease patients

Ilja M J Saris 1,*, Moji Aghajani 1,2, Niels Jongs 3, Lianne M Reus 4, Nic J A van der Wee 5,6, Amy C Bilderbeck 7, Inge Winter van Rossum 8, Celso Arango 9,10, Alejandro de la Torre-Luque 10,11, Asad Malik 7, Andreea Raslescu 7, Gerard R Dawson 7, José L Ayuso-Mateos 10,12, Martien J Kas 3, Brenda W J H Penninx 1
Editor: Alejandra Mondragón-Maya13
PMCID: PMC9009658  PMID: 35421108

Abstract

Background

Social functioning is often impaired in schizophrenia (SZ) and Alzheimer’s disease (AD). However, commonalities and differences in social dysfunction among these patient groups remain elusive.

Materials and methods

Using data from the PRISM study, behavioral (all subscales and total score of the Social Functioning Scale) and affective (perceived social disability and loneliness) indicators of social functioning were measured in patients with SZ (N = 56), probable AD (N = 50) and age-matched healthy controls groups (HC, N = 29 and N = 28). We examined to what extent social functioning differed between disease and age-matched HC groups, as well as between patient groups. Furthermore, we examined how severity of disease and mood were correlated with social functioning, irrespective of diagnosis.

Results

As compared to HC, both behavioral and affective social functioning seemed impaired in SZ patients (Cohen’s d’s 0.81–1.69), whereas AD patients mainly showed impaired behavioral social function (Cohen’s d’s 0.65–1.14). While behavioral indices of social functioning were similar across patient groups, SZ patients reported more perceived social disability than AD patients (Cohen’s d’s 0.65). Across patient groups, positive mood, lower depression and anxiety levels were strong determinants of better social functioning (p’s <0.001), even more so than severity of disease.

Conclusions

AD and SZ patients both exhibit poor social functioning in comparison to age- and sex matched HC participants. Social dysfunction in SZ patients may be more severe than in AD patients, though this may be due to underreporting by AD patients. Across patients, social functioning appeared as more influenced by mood states than by severity of disease.

Introduction

Social functioning is crucial for human survival and it has been established that ‘loneliness kills’ [1, 2]. Social functioning entails a collection of intricate and multi-factorial behavioral repertoires, which seem to be facilitated by many complex brain network processes [35]. Social dysfunction could be broadly defined as inability of the individual to “integrate behavioral, cognitive and affective skills to flexibly adapt to diverse social context and demands” [6]. Because of its complexity, social dysfunction often arises as one of the first symptoms in neuropsychiatric disorders such as Schizophrenia (SZ) and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [3]. However, the origins of social dysfunction in these disorders remain poorly understood [7, 8]. Specifically, while SZ and AD patients are characterized by distinctive psychopathologies, their psychosocial deficits are deemed partly overlapping. However, empirical data on behavioral and neurobiological commonalities and differences is practically lacking.

The definition of ‘social dysfunction’ has specific focal points dependent on the disorder being studied, with a broad range in study methods and outcome measures as a consequence. The neurobiological underpinnings of the subjective, affective evaluation of social interactions (i.e. loneliness, self-perceived social capacities) are thought to differ from the ones underlying the more objective, behavioral aspects of social interactions (i.e. frequency of participating in social activities, hours spend alone) [4, 9, 10]. We therefore differentiate between behavioral and affective indicators of social functioning, in line with prior work by our group and other studies [9, 1113]. Social dysfunction in SZ is described in a large and growing body of literature that has identified severe impairments in mentalizing, interpersonal interaction, regulating emotions and emotion decoding in social situations [1417]. It has been argued that higher order affective social processes such as mentalizing are more prone to deficits in SZ patients than processes which require less mental effort, such as emotion recognition and mirroring [14, 18]. In addition, the behavioral pattern of social withdrawal is described as a prodromal symptom of SZ, with an onset up to several years before the first psychotic episode [19]. In contrast to SZ, empirical data on social dysfunction in AD is arguably more sparse. Social dysfunction in AD typically starts with initially subtle impairments in social and affective cognition, which worsen as the disorder progresses [20, 21]. Social impairment has been described as a distinct constellation of AD symptoms, possibly explained by the degenerative processes differentially affecting brain regions [22]. More specifically, in AD social dysfunction seems to be associated with impairments in interpreting cues to others’ emotional states, and thus identifying others’ emotions, i.e. affective aspects of social dysfunction [21]. In addition, it has been described that the judgments of AD patients about their own social functioning is more positive than according to the perception of the caregiver [20, 23, 24]. In sum, social functioning in both SZ and AD seems most affected in understanding others [8].

The notion of highly overlapping social functioning deficits in neuropsychiatric disorders has gained more support and interest in recent years, especially since the launch of the RDoC framework and the EU-ROAMER initiatives, which advocate a deeper understanding of trans-diagnostic clinical phenomena and their neurobiobehavioral underpinnings [25, 26]. Currently, a clear understanding of what transdiagnostic research exactly entails, is still under debate and clear guidelines are lacking [27, 28]. One of the proposed, stringent guidelines for transdiagnostic research are the four Mansell criteria: (1) It describes both a clinical, and (2) a non-clinical sample, (3) it must be present in at least four disorders and (4) the transdiagnostic construct must be demonstrated in all mental disorders investigated [29]. However, before being able to examine such underpinnings, it is necessary to more fully grasp the differences and commonalities in a complex phenomenon such as social dysfunction–including both behavioral and affective aspects—across neuropsychiatric disorders.

OeHoThe OeHomain aim of the present study is to examine differences and commonalities in affective and behavioral indicators of social functioning among SZ and AD patients, as compared to age and sex-matched healthy controls (HC), as well as compared to each other (SZ versus AD). Ultimately this could guide future neurobiological research on the transdiagnostic domain of social functioning, however, not fulfilling the Mansell criteria in this study, we will use cross-disorder instead as preferred terminology. The current study is part of the larger EU-funded PRISM Project (Psychiatric Ratings using Intermediate Stratified Markers), which examines the neurobiobehavioral underpinnings of social dysfunction in order to advance and formulate more effective treatment strategies [30]. Whereas SZ and AD differ in core symptoms, genetic profile, obvious age difference and underlying neurobiology, they importantly seem to overlap considerable in social functioning deficits (i.e., social withdrawal, interpersonal functioning, loneliness) [30, 31]. A uniform assessment of social functioning is essential to progress the field of transdiagnostic analyses. Studying patients with distinctively different neuropsychiatric disorders also aligns nicely with the RDoC perspective that clinical psychological problems are best defined along functional domains with shared neurobiological substrates, regardless of diagnostic nosologies, to attain novel insights and advance treatment [25]. We aimed to include patients with a relatively recent disease onset to capture as much as possible the underlying neurobiology of social dysfunction rather than long-term consequences of psychopathology or neurodegeneration. A transdiagnostic approach of investigating social (dys)function with these two discrete clinical entities may thus elucidate both disorder-specific and cross-disorder deficits.

Material and methods

Participants

Data for the current study were derived from the PRISM study, which examines social dysfunction as a transdiagnostic symptom in individuals with SZ (N = 56), probable AD (N = 52), and age-matched HC participants (two groups, N = 29 for younger HC (age 18–45) and N = 28 for older HC (age 50–80) [30]. Two participants (AD patients) did not complete social functioning questionnaires, leaving 163 participants for analyses.

Participants were recruited between July 2017 and March 2019 from five different recruiting sites across Spain (Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón and Hospital Universitario de La Princesa) and the Netherlands (University Medical Center Utrecht, VU University Medical Center Amsterdam and Leiden University Medical Center). The study was approved by the Ethics Review Board of all participating centers: University Medical Center Utrecht, VU University Medical Center Amsterdam, Leiden University Medical Center, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón and Hospital Universitario de La Princesa. All participants provided verbal and written informed consent. Rationale and clinical implementation for the PRISM study is described in depth elsewhere [4, 30, 32].

In- and exclusion criteria

SZ patients were eligible if they had a) a diagnosis of schizophrenia (confirmed using DSM-based Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) assessment), b) had a maximum of 15-year disease duration since diagnosis, c) an age between 18–45 years, and d) a score of ≤22 on the 7-item positive subscale of the positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) [33] to rule out an active psychotic episode hampering adequate study participation [32]. SZ patients were excluded when they were, in the clinician’s judgment, a danger to themselves or others. AD patients were eligible if they had: a) a diagnosis of probable AD (meeting the National Institute on Aging and the Alzheimer’s Association criteria), b) a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [34] score between 20–26 (indicating mild AD pathology), c) an aged between 50–80 years. Multiple strokes, either based on clinical judgement, medical history or imaging results were exclusion criteria for the AD patient group.

Because AD is a progressive disorder with–after diagnosis- a quite variable disease course, we have included AD patients based on their cognitive abilities as measured by the MMSE [34]. Since social impairments may also stem from altered social interactions during ‘sick years’, SZ patients were included with a maximum of 15-year disease duration since diagnosis.

For both the SZ and AD patient groups, we had additional exclusion criteria: a) diagnosis of a severe, current Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) DSM-IV diagnosis (as assessed with the MINI) [35] and with a Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology, Self-Rated (QIDS-SR) [36] ≥16), b) diagnosis of any other primary psychiatric diagnosis that requires intervention; c) alcohol or drug abuse/dependence within previous 3 years (as assessed on the MINI), d) severe Parkinsonism as a consequence of antipsychotic medication (as assessed with a score ≥4 on the Extrapyramidal Symptom Rating Scale) [37], e) unstable comorbid somatic disorders potentially affecting the central nervous system (CNS), f) unstable use of medication that could affect CNS (e.g. start of or changed dosage within last 8 weeks).

We included two HC groups, matching on sex and age with the SZ (between 18–45 years) and AD (between 50–80 years) groups. Scores on the MMSE for the older HC participants should be comparable to normative data according to age and years of education. Exclusion criteria for the HC groups were: a) history of psychiatric Axis-I disorder (as confirmed by the MINI) or neurological disease associated with cognitive impairment; b) mild or more severe depression (score >5 on the QIDS-SR); c) current or prior use of antidepressant or anxiolytic medication including benzodiazepines, or other prescribed medication in the last 6 weeks that may affect the CNS.

Behavioral and affective social functioning indicators

Behavioral indicators of social functioning—The Social Functioning Scale (SFS) consists of seven subscales; social withdrawal, interpersonal functioning, competence and performance independence, recreational and prosocial activities, and employment. The subscale ‘employment’ was not used for total scale analyses, since most participants were retired in the older HC and AD group introducing a bias as confirmed by the significant association with age and in line with reporting in a previous study [38]. We conducted Principal Component Analyses with oblique rotation as the variables are correlated to test if the different subscales represent one factor (i.e. behavioral social functioning) or two (i.e. affective and behavioral aspects of social functioning). In line with other studies using the SFS [3840], the factor analysis confirmed that the six SFS subscales reflect one component (see S1 Table). The social withdrawal subscale (higher score indicates less social withdrawal) focuses on time spend alone. The interpersonal functioning subscale includes ability to have rational conversation and difficulty talking to people. The subscale independence-competence and independence-performance are two identical lists of activities (e.g. buying items from shops alone) where participants judge whether they think they are able to do these independently (competence) and following what they actually independently did in the past three months (performance). The subscales, recreational and pro-social activities, consist of a list with several activities such as visiting relatives or playing sports, and the frequency in which participants engage in that activity. We have followed the original scoring guidelines [39], which advocate using Social Functioning Scale total scores (Cronbach’s alpha 0.80), as these better captures underlying social functions/behaviors, and also safeguards reproducibility across studies. However, since the different subscales might reflect differences in impairments, we opted to also show the various subscales in subsequent supplementary analyses (S2 Table).

Affective indicators of social functioning—Loneliness was assessed with the 11-item de Jong Gierveld loneliness scale [41], which examines feelings of loneliness (Cronbach’s alpha 0.8). Perceived social disability was measured with an adjusted 5-item ‘getting along’ subdomain from the WHODAS 2.0 (WHODAS 2.0: World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule 2.0 [42]), which includes questions about difficulties in maintaining friendships in the last 30 days (Cronbach’s alpha for adjusted ‘getting along’ domain 0.9). Our division of social dysfunction (behavioral versus affective) is supported by higher inter-domain than cross-domain correlations between behavioral and affective social indicators (Table 2).

For patients, perceived social disability (WHO-DAS 2.0) was also assessed by the caregiver when available (most often parent or partner) and by the research staff conducting the assessment. Consistent with our earlier observations [43], correlations between the caregiver and researcher rated scores were high (r = .79 for SZ group; r = .79 for AD group, both p’s<0.001, S3I and S3II Table) and there were some missing values (not all patients participated in the study with a caregiver). In cases where both scores were available, we computed a ‘rater-perceived’ social disability score by calculating a mean score from both the caregiver and researcher rated score. In other cases, we used the available score (either caregiver or researcher rated). Correlations were strong for the SZ and HC groups between their self-rated perceived social disability score and the rater score (r = .84 for SZ; r = .90 for HC, p’s<0.001). For AD patients the correlation between the self-rated and rater-score was lower (r = .36). Corresponding to our earlier study on the self- and proxy-rated WHODAS scores [43], caregiver/researcher rated perceived social disability was higher than the rating by the AD patients themselves. We included the rater-perceived social disability in our analyses comparing patient groups.

All instruments are validated for both SZ and AD and were chosen after careful consideration [4].

Severity of disease

Cognitive dysfunction was estimated in AD patients using the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale–Cognitive subscale (ADAS-cog) [44] which includes 13 tasks involving both subject-completed test and observer-based assessments such as word recall, naming objects, and orientation. Current states of positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia were measured using the PANSS (positive and negative syndrome scale) [33].

Mood characteristics

All participants (patients and controls) were asked to complete three mood questionnaires. The 16-item Quick inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (QIDS-SR) [36], the 20-item State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [45] and the 20-item Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS) [46] examined depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms and current positive and negative affect states, respectively.

Statistical analyses

Demographic and clinical characteristics were described using χ2 for dichotomous variables and t-tests for continuous variables. The Mann-Whitney test was used as nonparametric test when assumptions parametric testing were not met. Pearson correlations described associations between social functioning indicators and continuous demographics (age, years of education), point-biserial correlation coefficient described associations for binary demographics (sex, partner status and country). Analyses of covariance (ANCOVA’s) with post-hoc tests, all Bonferroni adjusted, compared social functioning indicators among SZ and AD patients, as well as their age-matched HC groups, while adjusting for age, sex, years of education, partner status and country. Effect sizes were calculated following Cohen’s formula for estimated differences [47]. To examine the association of disease severity and mood (the dependent variables) with behavioral and affective indicators of social functioning (independent variables), linear regression analyses were conducted, again while adjusting for age, sex, years of education, partner status and country. Statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS (IBM, version 24.0, IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA), and a two-tailed significance level of P < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

HC participants had most years of education (17.2 and 16.7 for younger and older HC respectively), whilst both of the patient groups had on average 15.0 years of education (Table 1). SZ patients were less frequently with a partner (21.4%) as compared to younger HC (51.7%), AD patients (84.0%) and older HC (82.1%). As expected, psychotropic medication use was high in SZ patients (e.g. 89.3% used antipsychotics) and 43.8% of AD patients used acetylcholinesterase inhibitor and/or a NDMA receptor antagonist.

Table 1. Baseline characteristics (N = 163) across patient and control groups.

Schizophrenia patients N = 56 Younger healthy controls N = 29 p-value Alzheimer’s disease patients N = 50 Older healthy controls N = 28 p-value
Demographics
Age, mean years (SD) 30.8 (6.4) 28.7 (7.4) 0.13 68.6 (7.2) 67.1 (7.0) 0.32
Sex (% female) 28.6% 41.4% 0.23 44.0% 46.4% 0.84
Education, mean years (SD) 15.0 (3.8) 17.2 (2.6) 0.001 15.0 (5.6) 16.7 (4.9) 0.21
Partner status (% with partner) 21.4% 51.7% 0.004 84.0% 82.1% 0.83
Country (% Spain) 39.1% 48.3% 0.43 42.0% 25.0% 0.13
Specific disorder characteristics
Psychotropic medication
Antipsychotic (%) 89.3% 0% 4.0% 0%
Antidepressant (%) 19.6% 0% 16.0% 0%
Acetylcholinesterase inhibitor or NDMA receptor antagonist (%) 0% 0% 43.8% 0%
Benzodiazepines (%) 10.7% 0% 6.3% 7.1%
Other psychotropics (%) 14.3% 0% 2.1% 3.6%
Severity of disorder
Schizophrenia severity
 Positive symptoms, mean PANSS (SD) 11.0 (3.4) NA NA NA
 Negative symptoms, mean PANSS (SD) 14.6 (6.2) NA NA NA
AD severity, mean ADAS-Cog (SD) NA NA 26.9 (7.2) NA
Mood characteristics
Depression severity, mean QIDS-SR (SD) 8.0 (5.3) 2.1 (1.5) <0.001 4.1 (2.8) 2.0 (1.3) <0.001
Anxiety severity, mean STAI (SD) 43.5 (11.2) 30.4 (5.9) <0.001 30.7 (8.2) 27.5 (5.8) 0.08
Mood state, PANAS
 Positive Affect, mean (SD) 29.5 (6.6) 37.5 (5.9) <0.001 32.5 (5.1) 38.2 (6.1) <0.001
 Negative Affect, mean (SD) 18.7 (6.5) 13.9 (3.0) <0.001 13.9 (4.6) 12.7 (3.0) 0.24

Current positive and negative symptoms among SZ patients were 11.0 (SD ±3.4) and 14.6 (SD ±6.2) respectively on the PANSS (mean total score 51, SD ±13.1). Mean cognitive dysfunction of AD was 26.9 (SD ±7.2) on the ADAS-COG. SZ participants had more negative mood symptoms (i.e. more depressed mood, more anxiety, less positive affect and more negative affect) than all other groups (AD and both HC groups). Anxiety and negative affect were comparable between AD and their matched HC group, but AD patients had more depressive symptomatology and less positive affect (Table 1).

Correlation analyses (Table 2) across all participants show that age, educational level and partner status had significant associations with almost all social indices. However, sex and country had much less consistent associations with social indices (up to three associations were significant but without consistent direction of association).

Table 2. Pearson and point biserial correlations between social indicators and demographics in the overall sample (N = 163).

Behavioral social indicators Affective social indicators
Social withdrawal Interpersonal functioning Independence competence Independence performance Recreational activities Prosocial activities Total SFS score Perceived social disability Rater-perceived social disability Loneliness
Demographics
Age .273*** .272** -.162* -.060 .376** .119 .229** -.305** -.135 -.217**
Sex (female = 1; male = 0) .160* .036 -.009 .187* .108 .028 .117 -.032 -.060 -.057
Education level .148 .189* .157* .247** .176* .289** .272** -.071 -.261** -.125
Partner status (yes = 1; no = 0) .413** .372** .055 .012 .268** .151 .332** -.382** -.342** -.356**
Country (Spain = 1; Netherlands = 0) -.189* -.120 -.161* -.039 -.140 .027 -.160* -.013 .048 .201*
Behavioral social indicators
Social withdrawal 1
Interpersonal functioning .541** 1
Independence-competence .256** .320** 1
Independence-performance .283** .361** .605** 1
Recreational activities .437** .389** .359** .440** 1
Prosocial activities .465** .482** .343** .492** .564** 1
Total SFS score .715** .763** .595** .671** .749** .755** 1
Affective social indicators
Perceived social disability -.572** -.681** -.357** -.307** -.414** -.504** -.662** 1
Rater-perceived social disability -.560** -.598** -.434** -.483** -.496** -.616** -.730** .785** 1
Loneliness -.556** -.615** -.296** -.278** -.357** -.393** -.594** .615** .536** 1

*p-value < 0.05,

** p-value<0.01,

*** p-value<0.001.

Comparison of patient groups with healthy controls

Fig 1 shows unadjusted means for the total SFS score as behavioral indicator, and self-rated perceived social disability and loneliness as affective social indicators per group with comparisons for SZ and AD with their age-matched controls.

Fig 1. Unadjusted mean scores for total SFS, loneliness and perceived social disability across psychopathology.

Fig 1

Error bars represent the standard error. Y axis depicts the different scores. *p-value < 0.05, ** p-value<0.01, *** p-value<0.001.

It is clear in Table 3 that the least favorable social functioning outcomes on all measures were found for the SZ group with large effect sizes (Cohen’s d’s = 0.81–1.69) particularly pronounced for the affective indicators. AD patients showed less favorable outcomes for most behavioral social functioning indicators as compared to their matched HC group: independence-competence and -performance, recreational and prosocial activities and the total SFS score with medium to large effect sizes (d’s = 0.65–1.14). For both SZ and AD patients the total SFS was significantly lower from their age-matched controls with large effect sizes (d = 1.80 and 1.14 respectively). Affective social functional indicators of perceived social disability and loneliness, however, were not significantly different (d’s = 0.17 and d = 0.05 respectively) between AD and older HC participants. In contrast, the (mean score of caregiver and research staff) rater-perceived social disability was significantly worse in AD as compared to the older HC group (p<0.001, d = 1.29).

Table 3. Mean adjusted social functioning scores across disorders compared with healthy controls.

Schizophrenia patients N = 56 Younger healthy controls N = 29 p-value Effect size Cohen’s d Alzheimer’s disease patients N = 50 Older healthy controls N = 28 p-value Effect size Cohen’s d
Behavioral social functioning indicators
Social withdrawal, mean (SE) 10.4 (0.5) 13.3 (0.6) <0.001 1.30 12.3 (0.5) 13.1 (0.6) 0.74 0.35
Interpersonal functioning, mean (SE) 6.4 (0.4) 8.5 (0.4) <0.001 1.33 8.1 (0.4) 8.8 (0.4) 0.43 0.66
Independence-competence, mean (SE) 36.2 (0.7) 38.7 (0.8) 0.002 0.81 33.9 (0.8) 37.0 (0.8) <0.001 1.06
Independence-performance, mean (SE) 29.4 (1.1) 34.6 (1.3) <0.001 1.04 28.1 (1.2) 32.4 (1.3) <0.001 0.96
Recreation activities, mean (SE) 19.3 (1.4) 24.5 (1.7) 0.002 0.81 23.2 (1.6) 28.3 (1.7) 0.002 0.83
Prosocial activities, mean (SE) 24.8 (2.1) 36.8 (2.6) <0.001 1.25 28.6 (2.4) 34.5 (2.5) 0.030 0.65
Total SFS score, mean (SE) 111.2 (1.7) 125.3 (2.1) <0.001 1.80 116.5 (1.9) 124.9 (2.0) <0.001 1.14
Affective social functioning indicators
Perceived social disability, mean (SE) 11.3 (0.8) 6.1 (1.0) <0.001 1.39 6.1 (0.9) 5.5 (1.0) 1.00 0.17
Rater-perceived social disability, mean (SE) 12.2 (0.8) 5.7 (1.0) <0.001 1.69 10.5 (1.0) 6.0 (1.0) <0.001 1.29
Loneliness, mean (SE) 4.6 (0.6) 0.4 (0.8) <0.001 1.45 2.0 (0.7) 1.8 (0.8) 1.00 0.05

Adjusted for age, sex, years of education, partner status and country. Bonferroni adjustment for multiple comparisons.

Comparisons across/within patient groups

In the cross-disorder comparison between SZ and AD patients, (Table 4), all behavioral social indicators were comparable, as well as the total SFS score (p = 0.70, d = 0.31). Perceived social disability showed a significantly poorer outcome for SZ patients as compared to AD participants (Cohen’s d = 0.65, p = 0.008). The self-reported perceived social disability was higher in SZ compared to AD patients, but was not significantly different for the rater-perceived social disability.

Table 4. Mean adjusted social functioning scores across disorders with effect sizes (N = 106).

Schizophrenia patients N = 56 Alzheimer’s disease patients N = 50 p-value Effect size d
Behavioral social indicators
Social withdrawal, mean (SE) 10.4 (0.5) 12.3 (0.5) 0.27 0.40
Interpersonal functioning, mean (SE) 6.4 (0.4) 8.1 (0.4) 0.08 0.28
Independence-competence, mean (SE) 36.2 (0.7) 33.9 (0.8) 0.54 0.33
Independence-performance, mean (SE) 29.4 (1.1) 28.1 (1.2) 1.00 0.13
Recreational activities, mean (SE) 19.3 (1.4) 23.2 (1.6) 0.98 0.27
Pro-social activities, mean (SE) 24.8 (2.1) 28.6 (2.4) 1.00 0.18
Total SFS score, mean (SE) 111.2 (1.7) 116.5 (1.9) 0.70 0.31
Affective social indicators
Perceived social disability, mean (SE) 11.3 (0.8) 6.1 (0.9) 0.008 0.65
Rater-perceived social disability, mean (SE) 12.2 (0.8) 10.5 (1.0) 1.00 0.20
Loneliness, mean (SE) 4.6 (0.6) 2.0 (0.7) 0.21 0.41

Adjusted for age, sex, years of education, partner status and country. Bonferroni adjustment for multiple comparisons.

Linear regression analyses (Table 5) examined the association between clinical characteristics (severity of disorder, mood, positive/negative affect) and social functioning indices among SZ and AD patients adjusted for aforementioned covariates. Overall, only a few predictors were found to associate strongly with overall disease severity. More AD symptomatology was associated with fewer prosocial activities (β = -0.400, p = 0.014). More negative SZ symptomatology was associated with less interpersonal functioning (β = -0.330, p = 0.010), and more positive SZ symptoms related to more loneliness (β = 0.278, p = 0.049). Disease severity was not associated with the total SFS.

Table 5. Adjusted associations of clinical characteristics and various social functioning indicators within the group of patients (AD N = 50, SZ = 56).

ADAS-Cog AD only β PANSS positive symptoms SZ only β PANSS negative symptoms SZ only β Depression severity AD and SZ β Anxiety severity AD and SZ β PANAS positive affect AD and SZ β PANAS negative affect AD and SZ β
Behavioral social indicators
Social withdrawal 0.173 -0.224 -0.053 -0.555*** -0.413*** 0.414*** -0.355**
Interpersonal functioning -0.059 -0.072 -0.330* -0.522*** -0.424*** 0.468*** -0.309**
Independence-competence -0.166 0.009 -0.224 -0.151 -0.132 0.158 -0.175
Independence-performance -0.158 -0.039 -0.151 -0.102 -0.121 0.166 -0.091
Recreational activities -0.263 -0.124 -0.021 -0.166 -0.169 0.395*** -0.158
Pro-social activities -0.400* -0.054 -0.077 -0.312** -0.165 0.366*** -0.043
Total SFS score -0.242 -0.111 -0.205 -0.498*** -0.399*** 0.561*** -0.310**
Affective social indicators
Perceived social disability 0.194 -0.082 0.238 0.650*** 0.423*** -0.516*** 0.274**
Rater-perceived social disability 0.322 -0.274 0.177 0.424*** 0.232* -0.336** 0.125
Loneliness -0.163 0.278* 0.227 0.653*** 0.495*** -0.370** 0.445***

adjusted for sex, age, educational years, partner status and country.

*p-value < 0.05,

** p-value<0.01,

*** p-value<0.001.

More consistent associations were found for mood state indicators (Table 5). Across both diagnostic groups, positive affect was associated with all behavioral and affective social functioning indicators, except for independence-competence and -performance. More favorable social functioning was found for those with less depressive symptomatology, less anxiety and better mood state. Across patient groups, positive mood and lower depression and anxiety levels were thus strongly associated with better social functioning, even more so than severity of disease per patient group.

Discussion

The current study presents novel findings from the pan-European PRISM project on cross-disorder and disorder-specific deficits in social functioning among SZ and AD patients. As compared to HC, both behavioral and affective social functioning are clearly poorer in SZ patients (Cohens d’s 0.81–1.69), whereas AD patients have mostly poorer behavioral social function (Cohen’s d’s 0.65–1.14). Behavioral indices of social functioning were fairly similar across patient groups, SZ patients have more feelings socially disability than AD patients (Cohen’s d 0.65). Across patient groups, positive mood, lower depression and anxiety levels were strong determinants of better social functioning (p’s <0.001), even more so than severity of disease. Overall, this indicates that SZ and AD patients have rather similar social functioning levels in terms of behavior, but affective social functioning is different.

Consistent with our earlier observations [43], we observed a putative lack of insight among AD patients, who perceive their social disability on the same level as healthy controls, whereas AD informants and research staff perceive their social disability as significantly more impaired. By contrast the current data suggest that SZ patients are more aware of their own social disabilities than AD patients, an observation that warrants further investigation in future studies [48]. Findings are in line with previous research in AD patients where it was shown that a strong self-concept was associated with larger social dysfunction discrepancy comparing self-rating with caregiver rating [24]. Interestingly, social functioning in both diagnostic groups was found to be more strongly associated with mood than by current state of the disorders.

For SZ patients impairments in behavioral social functioning are consistently described [14, 49, 50], as are impairments in affective social functioning, such as feeling lonely and socially impaired [5153]. Our results seem consistent with and replicate this previous SZ research. Findings for AD patients are less unequivocal. Impairments in behavioral social functioning for AD patients are described as rather subtle in an early stage of the disease [21, 23]. In our study, however, AD patients differed from their matched HC group on most behavioral social functioning indices. It is possible that broad assessment of social functioning as implemented here could easier reveal subtle differences in specific aspects of social behavior. Among affective social indicators, feelings of loneliness were similar between AD and their matched healthy controls, as was found before [54].

When comparing SZ and AD patients on behavioral social functioning, a highly similar pattern of (dys)functioning emerged. This was especially true for the combined SFS outcomes. The largest differences between the patient groups emerged for the affective social indicators. For instance, patients with SZ evaluated themselves as less capable in social contact than AD patients. This suggests that patients have quite similar social functioning behavior but may evaluate this differently. It has been shown that feelings of loneliness are not necessarily reflected by objective social isolation [51, 55]. AD patients evaluated their perceived social disability on the same level as their matched healthy controls, whereas an informant evaluated their social abilities as strongly impaired. This suggest that AD patients may have an overly positive view of their own social functioning, possibly leading to underreporting of their impairments. This overestimation of their own social functioning, as compared to informants, has been described before [24, 43] and has been linked to loss of awareness due to AD [23]. Lack of awareness of social dysfunction is related to greater distress among the caregivers of AD patients [20, 23]. Mood influenced social functioning to a large extent, especially the affective indicators, with better mood associated with more favorable affective social outcomes. The large impact of anxiety and depressive disorders on social functioning has been described before, although the underlying pathophysiological mechanisms remain largely unknown. One hypothesis argues that imbalanced brain network function (especially in the brain’s default mode network [56]) not only impedes adaptive social functioning, but also interferes with maintenance of stable (positive) mood [9, 57, 58]. Current severity of disease in SZ and AD had only minor influence on social functioning indicators, as was previously found for AD [22] and SZ [49], although the reverse has been described as well [50, 54].

The PRISM study described here is unique in its comparison of two patient groups with distinctive pathologies and age-ranges, in relation to various indicators of social functioning. Transdiagnostic research is limited by the fact that similar, uniform instruments are often not used in different disease populations. We argue that such standardization of specific outcomes is necessary to progress the transdiagnostic research field. It could be that underlying contributing factors to social functioning are not perse similar across different disease conditions, but this does not mean that certain outcomes, like social functioning, cannot be measured in a uniform way. Some limitations inherent to the study should be mentioned. First, our data are cross-sectional, thereby not allowing for causal interpretations. Second, data is based on self- or informer-based evaluation, which may have resulted in biased responding. Passive monitoring of social behavior is a promising development to objectively measure ambulatory behavior and tackle possible self-report bias. Smartphone technology such as the BEHAPP-app [43, 59, 60] will allow the comparison of perceived social functioning with objective measurements. Third, we excluded the employment subscale of the SFS, since most AD patients and older HC’s were retired or not working, in line with other research [38]. However, inclusion of the employment subscale would arguably increase differences in social functioning between younger SZ patients and younger HC’s. Fourth, we made a distinction between behavioral (i.e. more quantitative) and affective (i.e. more perceptions of own social dysfunction) social indicators, which bears some clear benefits and is supported by prior work, but may ultimately pose an oversimplification of a complex phenomenon. Future work is therefore warranted to further explore and validate the findings and interpretations of the current study.

In summary, the present study reveals that behavioral aspects of social functioning are rather similarly affected in SZ and AD, whilst the affective perception of this behavior seems partly different in the two patient groups. Patients diagnosed with AD evaluate their interpersonal relations as only mildly impaired, whereas patients diagnosed with SZ have a more negative perspective of their own social functioning. AD patients may underreport their impairments in social functioning. Future research on social functioning should make a distinction between behavioral and affective indicators of social functioning, as these may associate differentially with pathophysiology. Findings further suggest that mood is highly associated with social functioning. This study may serve as a point of departure for future cross-disorder studies into the neurobiological underpinnings of social impairments in SZ and AD.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Factor analyses of the six subscales of the social functioning scale.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Mean scaled social functioning scale scores (N = 163).

(DOCX)

S3 Table

I. Perceived social disability (WHO-DAS score) availability across patient groups. II. Perceived social disability correlations across groups.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank the editor and the reviewers for their constructive feedback to improve the quality and understanding of the study.

Data Availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from PRISM (www.prism-project.eu). Restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under license for the current study, and are not publicly available. Data are available with the permission of PRISM. Necessary data variables for replication are the baseline characteristics (age, sex, education, partner status, country); specific disorder characteristics (medication, PANSS, ADAS-COG), mood characteristics (QIDS-SR, STAI, PANAS) and the social functioning indices (SFS, loneliness questionnaire, self-rated and external rated WHODAS).

Funding Statement

The project leading to this application has received funding from the Innovative Medicines Initiative 2 Joint Undertaking under grant agreement No 115916. This Joint Undertaking receives support from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme and EFPIA. This publication reflects only the author’s views and neither the IMI 2JU nor EFPIA nor the European Commission are liable for any use that may be made of the information contained therein.

References

  • 1.Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Layton JB. Social relationships and mortality risk: A meta-analytic review. PLoS Med. 2010;7. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness Matters: A Theoretical and Empirical Review of Consequences and Mechanisms. Ann Behav Med. 2010;40. doi: 10.1007/s12160-010-9210-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Adolphs R. The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60: 693–716. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.van der Wee NJA, Bilderbeck AC, Cabello M, Ayuso-Mateos JL, Saris IMJ, Giltay EJ, et al. Working definitions, subjective and objective assessments and experimental paradigms in a study exploring social withdrawal in schizophrenia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 1 Feb 2019. 97: 38–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.06.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Cacioppo JT, Cacioppo S, Capitanio JP, Cole SW. The Neuroendocrinology of Social Isolation. Annu Rev Psychol. 2015;66: 733–67. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015240 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bierman KL, Welsh JA. Assessing Social Dysfunction: the Contributions of Laboratory and Performance-Based Measure. J Clin Child Psychol. 2000;29: 526–539. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP2904_6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Cullen KR, Westlund MK, Klimes-Dougan B, Mueller BA, Houri A, Eberly LE, et al. Abnormal amygdala resting-state functional connectivity in adolescent depression. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71: 1138–1147. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Porcelli S, Van Der Wee N, van der Werff S, Aghajani M, Glennon JC, van Heukelum S, et al. Social brain, social dysfunction and social withdrawal. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;97: 10–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.09.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Saris IMJ, Aghajani M, Werff SJA, Wee NJA, Penninx BWJH. Social functioning in patients with depressive and anxiety disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2017;136: 352–361. doi: 10.1111/acps.12774 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Cacioppo S, Grippo AJ, London S, Goossens L, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness: Clinical Import and Interventions. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10: 238–249. doi: 10.1177/1745691615570616 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Dickens A.; Richards S.; Greaves C. & Campbell J. Interventions targeting social isolation in older people: a systematic review. BMC Public Health. 2011;11: 647. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-647 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Cacioppo JT, Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Thisted RA. Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Psychol Aging. 2006;21: 140–151. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.140 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Rico-Uribe LA, Caballero FF, Olaya B, Tobiasz-Adamczyk B, Koskinen S, Leonardi M, et al. Loneliness, social networks, and health: A cross-sectional study in three countries. PLoS One. 2016. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0145264 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Green MF, Horan WP, Lee J. Social cognition in schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16: 620–631. doi: 10.1038/nrn4005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Galderisi S, Mucci A, Buchanan RW, Arango C. Negative symptoms of schizophrenia: new developments and unanswered research questions. The lancet Psychiatry. 2018;5: 664–677. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30050-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Yang C, Qi A, Yu H, Guan X, Wang J, Liu N, et al. Different levels of facial expression recognition in patients with firstepisode schizophrenia: A functional MRI study. Gen Psychiatry. 2018;31: 1–6. doi: 10.1136/gpsych-2018-000014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Maat A, van Haren NEM, Bartholomeusz CF, Kahn RS, Cahn W. Emotion recognition and theory of mind are related to gray matter volume of the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2016;26: 255–264. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.12.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Addington J, Addington D. Social and cognitive functioning in psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2008;99: 176–181. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.07.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Owen MJ, Sawa A, Mortensen PB. Schizophrenia. Lancet. 2016;388: 86–97. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)01121-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Belfort T, Simões P, de Sousa MFB, Santos RL, Barbeito I, Torres B, et al. The Relationship Between Social Cognition and Awareness in Alzheimer Disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2018;31: 27–33. doi: 10.1177/0891988717743587 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Henry JD, Von Hippel W, Molenberghs P, Lee T, Sachdev PS. Clinical assessment of social cognitive function in neurological disorders. Nat Rev Neurol. 2016;12: 28–39. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2015.229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Cosentino S, Zahodne LB, Brandt J, Blacker D, Albert M, Dubois B, et al. Social cognition in Alzheimer’s disease: A separate construct contributing to dependence. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2014;10: 818–826. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2013.12.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Nelis SM, Clare L, Martyr A, Markova I, Roth I, Woods RT, et al. Awareness of social and emotional functioning in people with early-stage dementia and implications for carers. Aging Ment Heal. 2011;15: 961–969. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2011.575350 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Clare L, Nelis SM, Martyr A, Roberts J, Whitaker CJ, Markova IS, et al. The influence of psychological, social and contextual factors on the expression and measurement of awareness in early-stage dementia: Testing a biopsychosocial model. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012;27: 167–177. doi: 10.1002/gps.2705 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, Heinssen R, Pine D, Quinn K, et al. Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): Toward a New Classification Framework for Research on Mental Disorders. Am J Psychiatry Online. 2010;167: 748–751. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Haro JM, Ayuso-Mateos JL, Bitter I, Demotes-Mainard J, Leboyer M, Lewis SW, et al. ROAMER: Roadmap for mental health research in Europe. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2014;23: 1–14. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1406 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Fusar-Poli P, Solmi M, Brondino N, Davies C, Chae C, Politi P, et al. Transdiagnostic psychiatry: a systematic review. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 28.Sauer-Zavala S, Gutner CA, Farchione TJ, Boettcher HT, Bullis JR, Barlow DH. Current Definitions of “Transdiagnostic” in Treatment Development: A Search for Consensus. Behav Ther. 2017;48: 128–138. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2016.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Mansell M, Harvey A, Watkins E, Shafran R. Conceptual foundations of the transdiagnostic approach to CBT. J Cogn Psychother. 2009;23: 6–19. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Kas MJ, Penninx B, Sommer B, Serretti A, Arango C, Marston H. A quantitative approach to neuropsychiatry: The why and the how. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;97: 3–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.12.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Cuthbert BN. The PRISM project: Social withdrawal from an RDoC perspective. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;97: 34–37. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.08.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bilderbeck AC, Penninx BWJH, Arango C, van der Wee N, Kahn R, Winter-van Rossum I, et al. Overview of the clinical implementation of a study exploring social withdrawal in patients with schizophrenia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;97: 87–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.06.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for Schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13: 261–275. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Tombaugh TN, McIntyre NJ. The Mini-Mental State Examination: A Comprehensive Review. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1992;40: 922–935. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1992.tb01992.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Sheehan D V., Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59: 22–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Rush AJ, Gullion CM, Basco MR, Jarrett RB, Trivedi MH. The Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (IDS): psychometric properties. Psychol Med. 1996;26: 477–86. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700035558 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Chouinard G, Margolese HC. Manual for the Extrapyramidal Symptom Rating Scale (ESRS). Schizophr Res. 2005;76: 247–265. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.02.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Vázquez Morejón AJ, Jiménez G-Bóveda R. Social Functioning Scale: New contributions concerning its psychometric characteristics in a Spanish adaptation. Psychiatry Res. 2000;93: 247–256. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(99)00123-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Birchwood M, Smith J, Cochrane R, Wetton S, Copestake S. The Social Functioning Scale. The development and validation of a new scale of social adjustment for use in family intervention programmes with schizophrenic patients. Br J Psychiatry. 1990;157: 853–859. doi: 10.1192/bjp.157.6.853 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Iffland JR, Lockhofen D, Gruppe H, Gallhofer B, Sammer G, Hanewald B. Validation of the German version of the Social Functioning Scale (SFS) for schizophrenia. PLoS One. 2015;10: 1–18. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121807 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.de Jong-Gierveld J, Kamphuis F, Kamphuls F. The Development of a Rasch-Type Loneliness Scale. Appl Psychol Meas. 1985;9: 289–299. doi: 10.1177/014662168500900307 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Üstün TB, Chatterji S, Kostanjsek N, Rehm J, Kennedy C, Jordan JE, et al. Developing the World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule 2.0. Bull World Health Organ. 2010;88: 815–823. doi: 10.2471/BLT.09.067231 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Jongs N, Penninx B, Arango C, Ayuso-Mateos JL, van der Wee N, van Rossum IW, et al. Effect of disease related biases on the subjective assessment of social functioning in Alzheimer’s disease and schizophrenia patients. J Psychiatr Res. 2020. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.11.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Rosen WG, Mohs RC, Davis KL. A new rating scale for Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry. 1984;141: 1356–1364. doi: 10.1176/ajp.141.11.1356 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and Validation of Brief Measures of Positive and Negative Affect: The PANAS Scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54: 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Parellada M, Boada L, Fraguas D, Reig S, Castro-Fornieles J, Moreno D, et al. Trait and state attributes of insight in first episodes of early-onset schizophrenia and other psychoses: A 2-year longitudinal study. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37: 38–51. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Lysaker PH, McCormick BP, Snethen G, Buck KD, Hamm JA, Grant M, et al. Metacognition and social function in schizophrenia: Associations of mastery with functional skills competence. Schizophr Res. 2011;131: 214–218. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.06.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Simons CJP, Bartels-Velthuis AA, Pijnenborg GHM. Cognitive Performance and Long-Term Social Functioning in Psychotic Disorder: A Three-Year Follow-Up Study. PLoS One. 2016;11: e0151299. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0151299 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Trémeau F, Antonius D, Malaspina D, Goff DC, Javitt DC. Loneliness in schizophrenia and its possible correlates. An exploratory study. Psychiatry Res. 2016;246: 211–217. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.09.043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Eglit GML, Palmer BW, Martin AS, Tu X, Jeste D V. Loneliness in schizophrenia: Construct clarification, measurement, and clinical relevance. PLoS One. 2018;13: 1–20. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0194021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Green MF, Horan WP, Lee J, McCleery A, Reddy LF, Wynn JK. Social Disconnection in Schizophrenia and the General Community. Schizophr Bull. 2018;44: 242–249. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx082 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Balouch S, Rifaat E, Chen HL, Tabet N. Social networks and loneliness in people with Alzheimer’s dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2019;34: 666–673. doi: 10.1002/gps.5065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Holwerda TJ, Deeg DJH, Beekman ATF, Van Tilburg TG, Stek ML, Jonker C, et al. Feelings of loneliness, but not social isolation, predict dementia onset: Results from the Amsterdam Study of the Elderly (AMSTEL). J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2014;85: 135–142. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2012-302755 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Spreng RN, Andrews-Hanna JR. The Default Network and Social Cognition. Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference. Elsevier Inc.; 2015. pp. 165–169. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Kupferberg A, Bicks L, Hasler G. Social functioning in major depressive disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;69: 313–332. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Santini ZI, Koyanagi A, Tyrovolas S, Mason C, Haro JM. The association between social relationships and depression: A systematic review. J Affect Disord. 2015;175: 53–65. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.12.049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Eskes P, Spruit M, Brinkkemper S, Vorstman J, Kas MJ. The sociability score: App-based social profiling from a healthcare perspective. Comput Human Behav. 2016;59: 39–48. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Jongs N, Jagesar R, Van Haren NEM, Penninx BWJH, Reus L, Visser PJ, et al. A framework for assessing neuropsychiatric phenotypes by using smartphone-based location data. Transl Psychiatry. 2020;10. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-00893-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 Table. Factor analyses of the six subscales of the social functioning scale.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Mean scaled social functioning scale scores (N = 163).

(DOCX)

S3 Table

I. Perceived social disability (WHO-DAS score) availability across patient groups. II. Perceived social disability correlations across groups.

(DOCX)

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from PRISM (www.prism-project.eu). Restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under license for the current study, and are not publicly available. Data are available with the permission of PRISM. Necessary data variables for replication are the baseline characteristics (age, sex, education, partner status, country); specific disorder characteristics (medication, PANSS, ADAS-COG), mood characteristics (QIDS-SR, STAI, PANAS) and the social functioning indices (SFS, loneliness questionnaire, self-rated and external rated WHODAS).


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES