Skip to main content
Environmental Epigenetics logoLink to Environmental Epigenetics
. 2022 Apr 22;8(1):dvac007. doi: 10.1093/eep/dvac007

Evolution of anticipatory effects mediated by epigenetic changes

Ilkka Kronholm 1,*
PMCID: PMC9031056  PMID: 35475265

Abstract

Anticipatory effects mediated by epigenetic changes occur when parents modify the phenotype of their offspring by making epigenetic changes in their gametes, guided by information from an environmental cue. To investigate when do anticipatory effects mediated by epigenetic changes evolve in a fluctuating environment, I use an individual-based simulation model with explicit genetic architecture. The model allows for the population to respond to environmental changes by evolving plasticity, bet hedging, or by tracking the environment with genetic adaptation, in addition to the evolution of anticipatory effects. The results show that anticipatory effects evolve when the environmental cue provides reliable information about the environment and the environment changes at intermediate rates, provided that fitness costs of anticipatory effects are rather low. Moreover, evolution of anticipatory effects is quite robust to different genetic architectures when reliability of the environmental cue is high. Anticipatory effects always give smaller fitness benefits than within-generation plasticity, suggesting a possible reason for generally small observed anticipatory effects in empirical studies.

Keywords: between-generation plasticity, genetic architecture, phenotypic plasticity, transgenerational plasticity, intergenerational effects

Introduction

Some organisms are able to prime their offspring for future environmental challenges that the offspring are likely to face. These effects have been called anticipatory parental effects, between-generation plasticity, induced epigenetic changes, or transgenerational effects in the literature [1–3]. From now on, I will use the term anticipatory effects following [2], to focus on the phenotypic consequences and to remain agnostic about the molecular mechanism responsible for the effect. The clearest examples of anticipatory effects have been observed in plants; if parents have encountered pathogens or herbivores, they prime their offspring to have elevated levels of defense against pathogens [4–6] or herbivores [7]. Offspring can also be prepared to deal with abiotic factors such as drought and osmotic stress [8, 9] or shading [10]. Anticipatory effects are not restricted to plants, but have been observed in many animals as well. Particularly, there are examples of fish responding to temperature during development in subsequent generations [11–13], water fleas developing different morphs in response to predators [14], nematodes responding to viral infection and starvation in subsequent generations [15–19], and fruit flies responding to diet through metabolic changes in offspring [20]. A general principle in these examples is that the parents experience a particular environment, and as a result they change the phenotype of their offspring in a presumably adaptive manner, priming the offspring to better cope with this particular environmental challenge, even if the offspring have not yet encountered this environment themselves.

Anticipatory effects require a mechanism that makes it possible, for example, for offspring to upregulate the expression of possibly costly defense-related genes, even if they have not yet encountered a particular pathogen. There are different mechanisms that may accomplish this, such as provisioning of nutrients or hormones to eggs, transfer of antimicrobial peptides or antibodies to the offspring, or epigenetic inheritance. In some cases, the mechanistic basis of anticipatory effects based on epigenetic inheritance has been worked out in great detail. The specifics vary by taxon: in the plant Arabidopsis thaliana certain genes are methylated by the RNA-dependent methylation pathway as a response to osmotic stress, and these methylation patterns can be inherited via seed but not by pollen [8]. In the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans environmental stress induces the production of small RNAs that are inherited, and these RNAs subsequently direct chromatin conformation and gene expression changes in their target genes [16, 21]. In fruit flies epigenetic inheritance was dependent on the polycomb repressive complex silencing machinery [20], and small RNAs may be involved in fruit flies as well [22].

In ordinary phenotypic plasticity organisms use some sort of environmental cue to inform developmental decisions. For instance, changes in day length predict the change of seasons and thus temperature. Conceptually, anticipatory effects closely resemble ordinary phenotypic plasticity that happens within a generation, but the inducing environmental signal has been perceived by the parent rather than the focal organism. In order for anticipatory effects to occur, there needs to be a genetic pathway already present that can sense, transduce, and respond to an environmental signal, like in normal within-generation phenotypic plasticity. Intuitively, the evolution of the capability to use anticipatory effects seems to be contingent on the environmental cue having some predictive power on what the environment will be like in the next generation.

Several previous theoretical studies have investigated the evolution of maternal effects that are transmitted using models based on phenotypic memory, where the phenotype of the parent is partially passed on to the offspring [23–29]. Because genes influence the phenotype of the parent and the phenotype is passed partially to offspring, phenotypic memory can be considered a special case of indirect genetic effects, which can then cascade over multiple generations [25, 27]. Previous studies indicate that the evolution of anticipatory effects is favored in fluctuating environments [23,24,28,29]. Degree of environmental autocorrelation and predictive accuracy of environmental cues influence whether maternal information is used [29]. However, some models give different results as to what has to be the timescale of environmental fluctuations [30]. Moreover, the evolution of anticipatory effects has been studied in the context of population structure, and moderate levels of gene flow were found to favor the evolution of anticipatory effects [28,29,31].

While the previous models have shed light on the conditions where anticipatory effects should evolve, there are still some uncertainties. First, it has been suggested that organisms can use different types of strategies to adapt to environmental changes that happen at different timescales [32]. Environmental changes that happen within a generation should favor the evolution of within-generation plasticity, slightly longer fluctuations could favor anticipatory effects, and populations should adapt to environmental changes over long periods of time by genetic adaptation. Yet, the exact conditions that favor the evolution of anticipatory effects in fluctuating environments when alternative strategies are also possible are unclear at the moment. There could be regions of the parameter space where anticipatory effects would evolve only if investigated in isolation but not when alternative strategies are also possible, as they might have a higher fitness preventing the evolution of anticipatory effects. Therefore, it is important to investigate the evolution of anticipatory effects in combination with alternative strategies, as done for phenotypic plasticity by [33]. Furthermore, a recent meta-analysis found that anticipatory effects are mostly weak [34]. Whether it is the case that conditions for the evolution of anticipatory effects are restrictive such that they are rare in nature or that those taxa that live in conditions that are favorable for the evolution of anticipatory effects are underrepresented in studies or that anticipatory effects are generally to be expected to be low is not known. By having a clear picture of the conditions where anticipatory effects are expected to evolve, empirical studies can focus on those species that live in such environments.

Second, previous models have mainly investigated the evolution of maternal or anticipatory effects using greatly simplified genetic architectures, either based on a few loci with restricted allelic effects or a quantitative genetic framework, where the assumption is that traits are determined by infinitely many small-effect loci. While this is likely to be a reasonable assumption for many phenotypes, there are certainly examples of large genetic contributions of single loci to the phenotype for a number of different traits, and if genetic architecture is based on a few large effect loci, this can change the observed evolutionary dynamics [35]. There could be constraints on the evolution of anticipatory effects if, for example, mutational effects size and the number of loci are important factors for the evolution of the genetic traits that govern the induction of anticipatory effects.

Furthermore, while it is certainly possible that epigenetic changes and indirect genetic effects based on phenotypic memory will conceptually work in a similar manner, this is not completely clear. In the case of epigenetic inheritance, parents can pass on information about their current environment in the form of chemical modifications of DNA or associated histone proteins, and this information about the current environment can be different than the current phenotype of the parents.

I ask the question: in what kind of fluctuating environments do anticipatory effects evolve? I mainly focus on investigating how do the predictability of the environment and the frequency of environmental fluctuations affect the evolution of anticipatory effects and what is the effect of relaxing the assumption of infinitely many loci of small effects. Furthermore, I investigate what effects do costs of phenotypic plasticity and anticipatory effects have on their evolution, and does genetic architecture constrain the evolution of anticipatory effects? To answer these questions I use an individual-based simulation model adapted and modified from [33]. In the model, each individual has a linear reaction norm that is determined by an explicit genetic architecture with arbitrary mutational effects. The model allows the population to evolve either reversible phenotypic plasticity where phenotype can be adjusted multiple times per generation, developmental plasticity where phenotype is adjusted only once, increased developmental variation as a type of bet-hedging strategy, or simple genetic adaptation to the environment by tracking the environmental optimum by changing reaction norm intercept. Finally, the population can also evolve epigenetic modification, allowing anticipatory effects, where information from the parents is passed on to the offspring that can adjust their phenotype in early development.

Methods

Simulation model

I used an individual-based Wright–Fisher model with non-overlapping generations and random mating. The model keeps track of explicit genetics of each individual. Phenotype is determined by multiple loci and environmental effects. Environment changes at different rates and individuals can potentially use an environmental cue to respond by plasticity or by anticipatory parental effects mediated by epigenetic changes. Developmental variation is also partially under genetic control, which makes it possible to evolve increased variation as a bet-hedging-type strategy. The model is based on a model of phenotypic plasticity used by [33] with some modifications. The events in the model are shown in Fig. 1.

Fig 1.

Fig 1.

Events in the simulation model. Each individual lives for five timesteps. In the first timestep, individuals can use epigenetic changes inherited from their parents to modify their phenotype and pay a fitness cost of ke for this action. Epigenetic modifications are reset at the end of this step. In the next timestep, individuals can use developmental plasticity to adjust their phenotype based on an observed environmental cue, with a fitness cost of kd. In Timesteps 3–5, individuals can use reversible plasticity to further adjust their phenotype based on the observed cues, with a fitness of cost of ka for each adjustment. After Timestep 5, any potential epigenetic modifications are set based on an observed environmental cue from Timestep 5. Then, fitness is calculated for each individual, individuals produce offspring in proportion to their fitness, population regulation happens, and next generation is formed. Then, new mutations are produced in those individuals and the cycle starts again.

Environmental changes

In the simulation, there are L timesteps in each generation, amounting to a total of Inline graphic timesteps, where ng is the number of generations. The number of timesteps in each generation was set to 5. The environment, E, changes cyclically according to:

graphic file with name M0001-Latex.gif (1)

where R is a parameter that determines how fast the environment changes relative to generation time. Supplementary Fig. S1 shows how changing R affects the speed of environmental fluctuations. Small values of R cause the environment to fluctuate fast, and large values of R mean slow fluctuations. The values of R that were used in the simulations were 1, 3.2, 10, 31.6, 100, 316.2, 1000, 3 162.3, and 10 000. In subsequent plots, these values are plotted as their base 10 logarithms: 0, 0.5, 1, 1.5, 2, 2.5, 3, 3.5, and 4.

There is also an environmental cue, C, that individuals can potentially use to anticipate changes in the environment. The distribution of the cue is Gaussian: Inline graphic, where P is a parameter that determines the predictability of the environment. When P = 1 the cue predicts the environment perfectly, and when P = 0, cue is randomly distributed around 0 with standard deviation of Inline graphic. Simulations were run for values of P from 0 to 1 at 0.1 intervals. While the environment changes in a deterministic manner, the environment is not necessarily predictable from the point of view of the organism as the environmental cue that the organisms observe has randomness built in. Furthermore, the offspring do not necessarily experience the same environment as their parents did if the rate of environmental change is moderate relative to generation time. [33] also tried other functions for E than a sine wave and observed that similar results were obtained, as the ability to predict the environment based on the cue is more important than the form of fluctuations that E has.

Genetics, development, and phenotype

Individuals’ phenotype, Z, is determined as in a standard quantitative genetic model, but with explicit loci. As the model allows for plasticity, the phenotype for individual i at time point t is determined by a linear reaction norm

graphic file with name M0002-Latex.gif (2)

where Ga is the genotypic value for the intercept, Gb is the genotypic value for the slope by which an individual changes its phenotype as a reaction to the environmental cue, and ϵ which is a random developmental variation in the phenotype. The random variation in development is distributed as Inline graphic, where Inline graphic and σE is the environmental component of random variation and Gh the genotypic component of random developmental variation. Thus, the amount of random developmental variation can potentially evolve. There are la loci that determine the intercept, lb loci that determine the slope, ld loci that determine the probability of plastic adjustment of the phenotype, lh loci that determine the genotypic component of random developmental variation, and le loci that determine the probability of epigenetic modification of slope loci. Genotypic values are always the sum of individual allelic effects for each locus, for example Inline graphic. However, genotypic values for adjustment probability, Gd, and probability of epigenetic modification, Ge, were restricted between 0 and 1. The genotypic values for random developmental variation, Gh, were restricted to between 0 and 3. Restricted genotypic values were adjusted by setting values of G < 0 to 0 and G > 1 to 1. The underlying allelic effects could evolve freely and were not restricted—this means that a population could evolve a robust probability of either 0 or 1, which seems biologically reasonable for probabilities. For Gh the limit was placed so that biologically completely unrealistic values could not evolve, and I never observed values close to the upper limit in the simulations. There were no nonadditive allelic effects. The number of loci for each category was set to 10 unless otherwise stated. At the start of the simulation, a genetic map was randomly generated, with all loci distributed randomly and uniformly across nc linkage groups (chromosomes).

Individuals live for five timesteps (stages): during the first stage an individual has a chance to adjust its phenotype based on epigenetic modifications received from parents, but it cannot yet use environmental cues on its own. The anticipatory effect Inline graphic is calculated over slope loci if they are epigenetically modified; Inline graphic denotes an epigenetic slope effect. The phenotype of an individual that uses epigenetic information in its first life stage is Inline graphic. Modified alleles incorporate the allelic effect of slope and the cue experienced by the parents in Timestep t − 1. In other words, modified alleles store in cue information experienced by the parents in the previous timestep. For an individual that has no epigenetic modifications the phenotype at first life stage is Inline graphic. In the second life stage, individuals can use developmental plasticity according to Equation (2). In the third, fourth, and fifth life stages, individuals can use reversible plasticity to adjust their phenotype based on environmental cues with a probability of Gd at each step, so individuals phenotype follows Equation (2) if it adjusts and Inline graphic if it does not. In the fifth and final life stage, individuals epigenetically modify all of their slope alleles with a probability of Ge. For each slope allele two values are stored: its allelic effect and epigenetic modification. If an individual modifies its alleles epigenetically it modifies all of them, setting the epigenetic modification to the value of Ct. Then, events of the model—reproduction and mutation—happen as in Fig. 1. Then in the next generation at first life stage individuals have a chance to use the stored epigenetic information, which will be from Time Point Inline graphic, as explained above.

Fitness and selection

Fitness for each individual over its entire life span is calculated as the sum of phenotypic deviations from the environmental optimum at each life stage:

graphic file with name M0003-Latex.gif (3)

where τ is a parameter for intensity of selection which was set to 0.25, ke is the fitness cost of adjusting the phenotype using epigenetic modifications, Inline graphic is the number of such adjustments, kd is the fitness cost for adjusting the phenotype by developmental plasticity, Inline graphic is the number of such adjustments, ka is the fitness cost for adjusting the phenotype by reversible phenotypic plasticity, and Inline graphic is the number of such adjustments. Any negative values for fitness due to costs of phenotypic plasticity were set to 0.

Reproduction and population regulation

The number of offspring produced by individual i is drawn from a Poisson distribution with mean of Inline graphic, where NR is the number of offspring produced on average by a perfectly adapted individual (that has a relative fitness of 1). Since individuals are hermaphrodites that cannot self, offspring are generated for Individual i so that for each offspring the other parent is picked randomly with replacement from the population weighted by their fitness. Thus, fitness affects how many offspring an individual produces and the probability to participate in matings. Gametes are produced by meiosis, and the number of crossovers for each pair of homologous chromosomes has a distribution of Inline graphic, so that the minimum number of crossovers is always 1. The λ parameter was set to 0.56, which corresponds to 1.56 crossovers for each pair of homologous chromosomes in meiosis. This is the number of chiasmata per chromosome pair that on average is observed across eukaryotes [36]. Crossover positions are drawn from a uniform distribution along the chromosome, with no interference. If and only if the total number of offspring produced by all individuals was larger than the population carrying capacity, K, then offspring were removed randomly until K offspring remained. Generally carrying capacity was reached in the simulations. Preliminary tests were also done with a different method of density regulation, where fitness affects the probability that individuals get to reproduce and then how many individuals they produce according to a logistic growth model. Method of density regulation had no significant effect on the results.

Mutation

After the next generation of individuals has been produced, mutations are generated. Each locus has a per-locus mutation rate, µ. Since there are 2 Nl alleles in a population, the mean number of mutations per generation is Inline graphic. Each mutation generates a new allele, and the allelic effect is drawn from a Gaussian distribution, Inline graphic. Standard deviation of mutational effects, σα, was set to 1. Mutational variance, which describes how much genetic variation is contributed by mutation in each generation, can be calculated as Inline graphic, and mutational heritability can be calculated by standardizing mutational variance with environmental variance, Inline graphic [37]. Substituting values used in the simulation to these equations leads to a mutational heritability of 0.2 for the reaction norm intercept or slope. This value is one to two orders of magnitude higher than typical mutational heritabilities that have been measured experimentally [37–39], presumably because per locus mutation rate used in the simulation, Inline graphic, is much higher than realistic mutation rates. However, these values of mutational parameters were used because of the relatively small population sizes that could be used in the simulation without extending the computational time beyond feasibility. Increased mutation rate may cause the simulations to reach an evolutionary optimum faster than would naturally happen, but this is not certain as population size in the simulations is much lower than in natural populations. Furthermore, mutation rates that are relevant for adaptation also depend on mutational target sizes, which are difficult to estimate. A large mutational target will cause the per-locus mutation rate to be much higher than the per-base-pair rate. Robustness of the results to mutation rates will be examined below.

Simulations and data analysis

Simulations were written in R [40]. Replicate simulations were run in parallel on a computer supercluster utilizing the “snow” package. To examine in which conditions anticipatory effects evolve, I ran 100 replicate simulations for each combination of R and P. Costs of plasticity were set to Inline graphic and Inline graphic, which correspond to values used by [33] for the costs of developmental and reversible plasticity. Each replicate simulation lasted for 3000 generations. After these simulations, it was clear that 3000 generations was more than enough for the populations to adapt, so in subsequent simulations 1500 generations were used to reduce computational time. The simulations were initialized so that there was no genetic variation present at the start. Each simulation started with 2000 individuals. Results were reported as the values of the different variables at the end of the simulation. All simulation parameters are shown in Table 1. Then I tested how costs of plasticity affect the evolutionary outcomes. I ran simulations where there were no costs of phenotypic plasticity or anticipatory effects Inline graphic and simulations where the costs were twice as high as in the original simulations Inline graphic and Inline graphic.

Table 1.

Parameters used in the simulation model.

Parameter Explanation Value
L Number of timesteps in each generation 5
n g Number of generations 3000 or 1500
R Parameter determining rate of environmental change relative to generation time Varied
P Predictability of the environment Varied
σ E Standard deviation of random environmental variation 0.1
l a Number of loci for intercept effect 10
l b Number of loci for slope effect 10
l d Number of loci for probability of plastic adjustment 10
l e Number of loci for probability of epigenetic modification 10
l h Number of loci for genetic component of developmental variation 10
n c Number of linkage groups 5
τ Intensity of selection 0.25
k e Fitness cost of phenotypic adjustment by epigenetics Varied
k d Fitness cost of developmental plasticity Varied
k a Fitness cost of reversible plasticity Varied
N R Mean number of offspring produced by an individual 5
K Population carrying capacity 5000
n xo Mean number of crossovers between homologous chromosomes 1.56
µ Mutation rate per locus per generation 10−4
σ α Standard deviation of mutational effects 1

Sensitivity analysis

To examine if the results were robust to different genetic architectures, I ran a sensitivity analysis of the simulation. Hundred replicate simulations for each parameter combination of P and R were run. For each replicate, random values were drawn for parameters σE, number of different loci, linkage groups, mutation rate, and σα. Costs of plasticity were Inline graphic and Inline graphic. Other parameters were the same as in previous simulations (Table 1). Values were drawn from the following uniform distributions: Inline graphic, Inline graphic, Inline graphic; number of loci was randomly selected from Inline graphic and was the same for each type of loci, and number of linkage groups was selected from Inline graphic. Loci were distributed evenly across the linkage groups, and the remainder was always assigned to linkage group 1.

Results

Phenotypic plasticity

When costs of plasticity were Inline graphic and Inline graphic, some sort of plasticity consistently evolved when the parameter-determining rate of environmental change, R, was less than 100 (Inline graphic) and environmental predictability, P, was 0.3 or greater (Fig. 2). Reversible plasticity (Supplementary Fig. S2) evolves when rate of environmental change is fast, that is when R is 10 (Inline graphic) or smaller. Higher values of R favored the evolution of developmental plasticity (Supplementary Fig. S3), thus when environmental change is slow enough the costs of reversible plasticity outweigh its fitness benefits. Developmental plasticity evolved in some replicates when R was 100 (Inline graphic) and predictability was high, but slower environmental change did no longer favor the evolution of plasticity. Instead the population tracked the moving environmental optimum by changing the reaction norm intercept (Supplementary Fig. S4). When environmental change was unpredictable (P < 0.3) and fast (R = 1, Inline graphic), the reaction norm of the population remained flat with an intercept of 0 (Fig. 2). This represents a conservative strategy where individuals are just tolerating environmental changes. However, when environmental changes were slower (R = 10, Inline graphic) but still unpredictable, the population evolved increased developmental variation (Supplementary Fig. S5). This represents a bet-hedging strategy where individuals benefit from producing offspring with different phenotypes in order for some of them to be adapted to the unpredictable environment. Overall, the results regarding plasticity are very similar as those obtained by [33], despite some differences in the genetic architecture of the traits. Here, the conditions favoring the evolution of a diversifying bet-hedging strategy are more restrictive than in [33], but in their model it was possible to evolve a phenotype that produced two distinct morphs, rather than only increased developmental variation.

Fig 2.

Fig 2.

Simulation results for Inline graphic and Inline graphic. For each small panel, the environmental cue is on the x-axis and phenotype on the y-axis. Each reaction norm is the population mean for one replicate simulation, and 100 replicates were performed for each combination of P and R. Red color shows reaction norms, where the population evolved reversible plasticity (Inline graphic or Inline graphic, and Inline graphic). Orange color shows reaction norms where reversible and anticipatory effects evolved (in addition Inline graphic), blue color indicates that only developmental plasticity evolved (Inline graphic or Inline graphic, and Inline graphic), purple color indicates that both developmental plasticity and anticipatory effects evolved (in addition Inline graphic), green color indicates that increased developmental variation evolved (Inline graphic), and black color indicates that there was no major change in reaction norm slope (Inline graphic). Note the logarithmic scale for R.

Anticipatory effects

Anticipatory effects mediated by epigenetic changes evolved consistently when the environment did not change too fast or too slow, Inline graphic (Inline graphic), and environmental predictability was 0.4 or larger (Fig. 2). Very fast environmental changes do not favor the evolution of anticipatory effects, because the environment can change between when the effects are induced in the parents and when offspring develop. Very slow environmental changes also do not favor anticipatory effects as the fitness costs of anticipatory effects do not outweigh their benefits. Finally, the environment has to be rather predictable, which is understandable, since not being able to predict what the offspring environment will be can result in maladaptive anticipatory effects. Anticipatory effects evolved together with either reversible plasticity (Supplementary Fig. S6) when Inline graphic (Inline graphic), or together with developmental plasticity (Supplementary Fig. S7) when Inline graphic (Inline graphic).

The probability of individuals using epigenetic modifications to mediate anticipatory effects was highest when environmental predictability was more than 0.5 and when Inline graphic (Inline graphic) (Fig. 3). Lower values of P and R gave slightly lower probabilities of epigenetic modification, until their epigenetic modification no longer happened.

Fig 3.

Fig 3.

Heat map for mean probability of epigenetic modification from 100 replicate simulations with each combination of P and R. Costs of plasticity were Inline graphic and Inline graphic.

Anticipatory effects increased the final mean fitness of individuals within a population. In the simulation, fitness is defined as how close individual’s phenotype is to the environmental optimum, and as the environment changes population mean in fitness can change or oscillate. Therefore, I used the geometric mean of the last 500 generations of mean fitness. For example, when R = 31.6 (Inline graphic) and P = 0.8, the 95% quantiles for geometric mean fitness for the last 500 generations in the simulation was 0.72 Inline graphic for simulations where evolution of anticipatory effects was impossible, that is there were no loci that coded for epigenetic modification, and 0.76 Inline graphic for a simulation where anticipatory effects could freely evolve. This difference means a relative fitness of 1.05 in favor of anticipatory effects, which is more than enough to drive their evolution.

Costs of plasticity and anticipatory effects

Fitness costs of plasticity had a large effect on what strategy the population evolved in a particular environment. When fitness costs were assumed to be zero, reversible phenotypic plasticity with anticipatory effects was very common throughout the parameter space (Fig. 4A). Clearly if there is no cost of anticipatory effects or plasticity, they should be very common, even if environmental cues predict the environment poorly. In Fig. 4A there is a strip at R = 316.2 (Inline graphic) where no anticipatory effects evolve but they seem to evolve at higher values of R. This can actually be explained by dynamic alternation of plasticity and genetic assimilation. When R > 100 (Inline graphic) and P > 0.3 the environment changes slowly, so that the population tracks the environment mainly by genetic adaptation. However, when there are no costs of plasticity, the population evolves transient plasticity and anticipatory effects during those periods that the environment is moving to one direction. Then when the environmental change slows, the slope of the reaction norms evolves toward zero, only to depart from zero again when the environment starts moving again (Supplementary Fig. S8). These types of dynamics have been observed before in models of plasticity and maternal effects, where there is an extraordinary environmental change that the population has to adapt to [25, 41]. Plastic and anticipatory effects help speed up adaptation. So this region of parameter space is characterized by transient anticipatory effects and genetic assimilation. Evolutionary changes tend to happen in reaction norm slope; the probability of epigenetic modification changes occasionally, but evolves to moderate or high value. The fitness benefits of these transient changes in plasticity are small, as any costs of plasticity prevented this type of dynamic from occurring (Fig. 4). The strip at R = 316.2 (Inline graphic) is due to environmental fluctuation being in a phase where genetic assimilation has happened and slope is zero in these populations (Supplementary Fig. S8A).

Fig 4.

Fig 4.

Simulation results with different costs for phenotypic plasticity and anticipatory effects. For each parameter combination of P and R, 100 replicate simulations are represented as colored tiles, with colors corresponding to different strategies evolved in that replicate. Costs are (A) Inline graphic, (B) Inline graphic and Inline graphic, (C) Inline graphic and Inline graphic.

However, there was a region of parameter space where anticipatory effects did not evolve even without costs. If P = 0 anticipatory effects did not evolve, or if Inline graphic (Inline graphic) and Inline graphic no plasticity or anticipatory effects evolved (Fig. 4A). In this region of parameter space, anticipatory effects are likely to be either neutral or possibly deleterious as preparing offspring to a wrong environment can be maladaptive. Increasing fitness costs of plasticity and epigenetic modification reduces the range where anticipatory effects evolve (Fig. 4B) and when both plasticity and epigenetic modification have high enough costs, only reversible or developmental plasticity evolves (Fig. 4C).

Influence of genetic architecture

To test whether the results were robust to genetic architecture of the phenotype, I ran a sensitivity analysis where parameters describing the genetic architecture of the population were randomly selected. The results of sensitivity analysis show that genetic architecture does influence the outcome of the simulations, but similar broad patterns are still recovered as with the initial simulations (Fig. 5). Anticipatory effects do evolve, but now higher environmental predictability is generally required, Inline graphic. Moreover, anticipatory effects now do not evolve as certainly as in the original simulation even if plasticity evolves when Inline graphic (Inline graphic) (Fig. 5). Since the fitness benefits of anticipatory effects are smaller than within-generation plasticity, sometimes genetic architecture does not allow fine-tuning of the anticipatory effects, which is seen especially in lower values of environmental predictability where lower probability of epigenetic modification evolves (Fig. 3). In some cases the population goes extinct because there is not enough genetic variation for adaptation, but this was not common as only 51 extinction events were observed, which is 0.5% of the total simulations. In the sensitivity analyses, the parameter space where increased developmental variation or a bet-hedging-type strategy could evolve was actually larger than in the original simulations. If the environmental component of random variation was small or when other strategies were more restricted, there was more opportunity to sometimes evolve increased developmental variation (Fig. 5).

Fig 5.

Fig 5.

Sensitivity of results to genetic architecture. Hundred replicate simulations were run for each combination of R and P, where parameter values for the random environmental effects, number of loci, number of chromosomes, mutation rate, and mutational effects were drawn randomly. Costs of plasticity were Inline graphic and Inline graphic. Colored tiles show which strategy evolved in the population or if it went extinct.

To investigate further the influence of specific parameters of genetic architecture on the evolution of anticipatory effects, I ran simulations changing the number of loci that were responsible for the phenotypic traits or the standard deviation of mutational effects. The number of loci had the strongest effect on the evolution of reversible plasticity. When the slope of the reaction norm and probability of adjustment were both determined by only one locus, evolution of reversible plasticity was prevented in an environment that completed its cycle in every generation, R = 1 (Inline graphic). Anticipatory effects evolved when Inline graphic and Inline graphic (Inline graphic) (Fig. 6). Moreover, increased developmental variability evolved when environmental predictability was low, Inline graphic, and rate of environmental change was intermediate, Inline graphic (Inline graphic). When the number of loci was increased to 5, reversible plasticity evolved also in environments that fluctuated every generation (Fig. 6B). Further increasing the number of loci to 20 allowed reversible plasticity to evolve in more unpredictable environments (Fig. 6C). Increasing the number of loci did allow anticipatory effects to evolve when P = 0.5 in cases where developmental plasticity had evolved. In general, the number of loci had only small effect on the evolution of anticipatory effects.

Fig 6.

Fig 6.

Effects of number of loci (A–C) and standard deviation of mutational effects (D–F). Hundred replicate simulations were run for each combination of R and P, but only those values of R were used where I had previously observed the evolution of plasticity when plasticity had a cost. Cost of plasticity were Inline graphic and Inline graphic. Colored tiles show which strategy evolved in the population. (A) Inline graphic, (B) Inline graphic, (C) Inline graphic, (D) Inline graphic, (E) Inline graphic, (F) Inline graphic.

Next I investigated the role of mutational effects in the evolution of anticipatory effects. I ran simulations with different standard deviation for mutational effects. When Inline graphic no plasticity of any kind evolved (Fig. 6D). When this was increased to Inline graphic, anticipatory effects did evolve when Inline graphic and Inline graphic (Inline graphic) (Fig. 6E). However, no reversible plasticity evolved in simulation when R = 1 (Inline graphic). The reason plasticity did not evolve in these conditions was that rate of evolution was too slow relative to the rate of environmental change due to the small mutational effects. When Inline graphic and R is small there is no evolution of plasticity (Supplementary Fig. S9A), but when R is large, R = 3162.3 (Inline graphic), so that the population evolves a strategy to track the environmental optimum by genetic adaptation, there is enough time to reach a stable state for the genotypic values of the intercept (Supplementary Fig. S9B). Therefore, it is not the case that there is no time to reach equilibrium during the 1500 generations, but that selection pressure changes too fast relative to the slow rate of evolution that would be enough time for the small effects to build up. This is also seen for Inline graphic, but in less extreme form, as only R = 1 (Inline graphic) is too fast to prevent adaptation (Supplementary Fig. S9C), while R = 3.2 (Inline graphic) already allows stable states to be reached (Supplementary Fig. S9D). Furthermore, increased developmental variation evolved in up to Inline graphic and Inline graphic (Inline graphic) (Fig. 6E), which represents a much larger parameter space than with larger mutational effects (Fig. 6F). When mutational effects were double the default value, Inline graphic, reversible plasticity evolved as normal, and anticipatory effects evolved also when predictability of the environment was slightly lower, P = 0.5 in cases where developmental plasticity had evolved. In the case where anticipatory effects did not evolve, substituting the parameter values used in the simulation gives a mutational variance of Inline graphic and mutational heritability of Inline graphic. These values are orders of magnitude lower than the values observed empirically for many phenotypic traits. In summary, anticipatory effects evolved unless mutational effects were very small, and it seems that genetic architecture is unlikely to completely prevent evolution of anticipatory effects if they are ecologically favored.

Discussion

The results reported here largely agree with previous modeling results concerning maternal effects in phenotypic memory type of models, in that anticipatory effects can evolve as long as environmental cues can predict the environment of the offspring to some degree [25,27–29,42]. The predictive power of environmental cues does not have to be perfect as anticipatory effects still evolved when environmental predictability was rather low. The costs of anticipatory effects influence the threshold of minimum predictability, since when there were no costs for any type of plasticity, anticipatory effects evolved even with minimal predictability. As usefulness of environmental cues in predicting the offspring environment decreases, less and less fitness benefit can be gained from anticipatory effects, and therefore only such anticipatory effects that have very low costs or no costs at all can evolve in rather unpredictable environments. Only a predictability of zero meant that there was no evolution of any kind of anticipatory effects.

Anticipatory effects evolved only when frequency of environmental fluctuations was slow enough that cues perceived by the parents still meant something when the offspring were developing, but fast enough there could be differences between the parent and offspring environments. So, if environmental change was slow enough, there is little point in paying the potential costs of anticipatory effects if the fitness benefit gained from such an effect is small. In contrast to intermediate fluctuation frequencies favoring anticipatory effects, in certain models phenotypic memory is favored in very fast fluctuations [23, 24]. However, this can be explained regarding how these models were defined; in [23] and [24] the environment fluctuated between two discrete states. Thus, it makes sense that faster fluctuations might favor anticipatory effects in these models. Other models of environmental epigenetic induction have substituted temporal environmental fluctuations for spatial heterogeneity in the environment, and if one considers temporal and spatial variation as analogous, at least qualitatively similar results emerge that migration between populations has to be moderate for anticipatory effects to evolve [31].

The evolution of anticipatory effects is obviously dependent on the magnitude of the fitness benefit they give relative to their costs. In the simulation when anticipatory effects had high costs, Inline graphic, the evolution of anticipatory effects was completely prevented. Understandably so, as in this model anticipatory effects gave only a moderate fitness benefit overall and within-generation plasticity was much more beneficial. This phenomenon has also been observed by [27], as using direct information about the current environment is always going to be better than using incomplete information about future environment. Therefore, in real populations one could expect anticipatory effects to be smaller in magnitude or rarer than within-generation plastic effects; this smaller fitness benefit possibly explains why anticipatory effects seem to be generally weak [34]. Most theory about the evolution of plasticity in general assumes that there are some costs of plasticity [43]. However, while some studies have detected costs of plasticity [44, 45], in general costs of plasticity have been notoriously difficult to document in nature and we have a poor idea if we expect costs of anticipatory effects to be generally the same or somehow different. As fitness benefits of anticipatory effects are smaller than for within-generation plasticity, it may be that costs of anticipatory effects are an important component limiting their evolution.

Certain organisms seem to exhibit a lot of anticipatory effects, such as nematodes [46]. This may reflect an experimental bias, as many people are studying these model organisms. However, another explanation is that there is something in their ecology that favors the evolution of anticipatory effects. Environments change at different rates from the perspective of different organisms since generation times can be vastly different. What are fast environment changes for long-lived multicellular organisms can be rather slow changes from the perspective of unicellular microbes. It may be, perhaps somewhat counterintuitively, that anticipatory effects can be more common in smaller organisms that are short-lived. Moreover, the implication also is that organisms which have a long period between zygote formation and emergence of live offspring, such as mammals with long gestation periods, are expected to have less anticipatory effects than organisms where there is a shorter time between zygote formation and emergence. The longer the time between setting the epigenetic marks in gametes and offspring birth, the smaller the correlation between the environment of the offspring and the environment of the parent. In plants it is known that anticipatory effects are sometimes transmitted through seed but not by pollen [8, 47], presumably because pollen can disperse over much longer distances, diminishing the correlation between parent and offspring environment. Unequal resetting of epigenetic marks in male and female gametes should apply as well to species where the time of gamete production and the time of fertilization are decoupled, such as certain insects, reptiles, and birds that are capable of storing sperm for long periods of time [48, 49].

Another important ecological question is how predictable real environments are from the perspective of organisms. Are there environmental cues that organisms can potentially use to predict future environments? The most obvious cues are related to changes in day length which does reliably predict seasons in high and low latitudes. Many environmental variables can also function as cues themselves, such as temperature. For example, high temperatures today can predict that temperature will also be high in the short term. Furthermore, there are some indications that many environmental variables are autocorrelated in nature [50] and that this autocorrelation often tends to be positive [51, 52], meaning that temporally close observations tend to be similar. Moreover, there seem to be differences in terrestrial and marine ecosystems; autocorrelation in terrestrial systems seems to be closer to zero, while marine ecosystems tend to have more positive autocorrelations [51, 52]. It will be interesting to see if anticipatory effects are more prevalent in marine organisms. Nevertheless, environments in nature tend to be at least somewhat autocorrelated, so there should be some opportunity for evolution of anticipatory effects.

In some empirical studies of epigenetically mediated anticipatory effects, the effects induced by the environment last for several generations. This has been particularly observed in nematodes [46]. The question remains whether these multigenerational effects are adaptive in nature or rather a consequence of some developmental constraint? In the model used in this study, it is difficult to see how multigenerational effects could be adaptive, as this would require the environment of the grandparents to predict the environment of the offspring better than the environment of their parents. In the model, epigenetic modifications were reset at every generation, but relaxing this assumption would likely not change the results, as it makes sense for the parents to overwrite any previous epigenetic marks using the current environmental cue to maximize the correlation between their environment and the offspring environment. This assumption of course depends on the mechanistic details on how the epigenetic modification system works, and if there is asymmetry in the way the epigenetic mark can influence the phenotype this could potentially change the results. For example, if the epigenetic change induces some antipredator defenses and predation risk remains elevated for several generations even in the absence of the initial environmental cue about predator presence, then multigenerational effects that maintain antipredator defenses for several generations could be adaptive. A model investigating the incomplete resetting of epigenetic marks found that incomplete resetting was favored when the environment changed infrequently and environmental cues were unreliable [53]. In the model studied here, costs of anticipatory effects generally prevented their evolution in these kinds of conditions. Therefore, it seems that multigenerational effects will be dependent on very specific ecological conditions and are more likely to occur in organisms with very short generation times.

Conclusions

Simulations with a genetically explicit model generally support many of the results obtained previously: anticipatory effects can evolve when the environment changes at moderate speeds relative to generation time, and there is some environmental cue that can be used to predict the environment. However, if anticipatory effects have fitness costs, these costs limit their evolution only to situations where environmental cues are reliable. If fitness costs are very low, anticipatory effects also help genetic adaptation to large environmental changes, in a manner similar to within-generation plasticity and maternal effects. In terms of novel results, this model shows that the evolution of anticipatory effects is quite robust to different genetic architectures, although genetic architecture can cause the need for environmental cues to be very reliable. Unless mutational effects were very small, anticipatory effects could readily evolve. In order to test assumptions of the model, future empirical studies could benefit from characterizing the predictability of environmental cues or the autocorrelation structure of the environmental variable that induces the possible anticipatory effects. The fitness costs of anticipatory effects remain another puzzle. It has been difficult to determine fitness costs of within-generation plasticity, so getting estimates of costs of anticipatory effects will undoubtedly be challenging. The model presented here could be tested experimentally by evolving eukaryotic microbes in environments that fluctuate at different frequencies and investigating whether the populations evolve plastic or anticipatory responses mediated by epigenetic changes.

Supplementary Material

dvac007_Supp

Acknowledgement

I thank the Finnish CSC-IT Center for Science Ltd for providing computational resources.

Data availability

An R package implementing the simulations is available on github: https://github.com/ikron/indsim (last accessed 11 March).

Supplementary data

Supplementary data is available at EnvEpig online.

Funding

This study was funded by an Academy of Finland Research Fellowship (no. 321 584).

Conflict of interest statement

None declared.

References

  • 1. Jablonka E, Raz G.. Transgenerational epigenetic inheritance: prevalence, mechanisms, and implications for the study of heredity and evolution. Q Rev Biol 2009;84:131–76. doi: 10.1086/598822 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Marshall JD, Uller T.. When is a maternal effect adaptive? Oikos 2007;116:1957–63. doi: 10.1111/j.2007.0030-1299.16203.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Kronholm I. Handbook of Epigenetics: The New Molecular and Medical Genetics , 2nd edn. (Ed. Tollefsbol T.), Adaptive evolution and epigenetics, Academic Press, 2017, 427–38. [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Rasmann S, De Vos M, Casteel CL.. et al. Herbivory in the previous generation primes plants for enhanced insect resistance. Plant Physiol 2012;158:854–63. doi: 10.1104/pp.111.187831 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Slaughter A, Daniel X, Flors V.. et al. Descendants of primed Arabidopsis plants exhibit resistance to biotic stress. Plant Physiol 2012;158:835–43. doi: 10.1104/pp.111.191593 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Luna E, Bruce TJ, Roberts MR.. et al. Next-generation systemic acquired resistance. Plant Physiol 2012;158:844–53. doi: 10.1104/pp.111.187468 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Holeski LM. Within and between generation phenotypic plasticity in trichome density of Mimulus guttatus. J Evol Biol 2007;20:2092–100. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2007.01434.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Wibowo A, Becker C, Marconi G.. et al. Hyperosmotic stress memory in Arabidopsis is mediated by distinct epigenetically labile sites in the genome and is restricted in the male germline by DNA glycosylase activity. eLife 2016;5:e13546. doi: 10.7554/eLife.13546 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Herman JJ, Sultan SE, Horgan-Kobelski T.. et al. Adaptive transgenerational plasticity in an annual plant: grandparental and parental drought stress enhance performance of seedlings in dry soil. Integr Comp Biol 2012;52:77–88. doi: 10.1093/icb/ics041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Galloway LF, Etterson JR.. Transgenerational plasticity is adaptive in the wild. Science 2007;318:1134–6. doi: 10.1126/science.1148766 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Salinas S, Munch SB.. Thermal legacies: transgenerational effects of temperature on growth in a vertebrate. Ecol Lett 2012;15:159–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2011.01721.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Donelson JM, Munday PL, McCormick MI.. et al. Rapid transgenerational acclimation of a tropical reef fish to climate change. Nat Clim Change 2012;2:30–2. doi: 10.1038/nclimate1323 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Shama LNS, Wegner KM.. Grandparental effects in marine sticklebacks: transgenerational plasticity across multiple generations. J Evol Biol 2014;27:2297–307. doi: 10.1111/jeb.12490 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Agrawal AA, Laforsch C, Tollrian R.. Transgenerational induction of defences in animals and plants. Nature 1999;401:60–3. doi: 10.1038/43425 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Rechavi O, Minevich G, Hobert O.. Transgenerational inheritance of an acquired small RNA-based antiviral response in C. elegans. Cell 2011;147:1248–56. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.10.042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Rechavi O, Houri-Ze’evi L, Anava S.. et al. Starvation-induced transgenerational inheritance of small RNAs in C. elegans. Cell 2014;158:277–87. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.06.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Jobson MA, Jordan JM, Sandrof MA.. et al. Transgenerational effects of early life starvation on growth, reproduction and stress resistance in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 2015;201:201–12. doi: 10.1534/genetics.115.178699 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Kishimoto S, Uno M, Okabe E.. et al. Environmental stresses induce transgenerationally inheritable survival advantages via germline-to-soma communication in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nat Commun 2017;8:14031. doi: 10.1038/ncomms14031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Ivimey-Cook ER, Sales K, Carlsson H.. et al. Transgenerational fitness effects of lifespan extension by dietary restriction in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc R Soc B 2021;288:20210701. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Öst A, Lempradl A, Casas E.. et al. Paternal diet defines offspring chromatin state and intergenerational obesity. Cell 2014;159:1352–64. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.11.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Rechavi O, Lev I.. Principles of transgenerational small RNA inheritance in Caenorhabditis elegans. Curr Biol 2017;27:R720–30. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Duempelmann L, Skribbe M, Bühler M.. Small RNAs in the transgenerational inheritance of epigenetic information. Trends Genet 2020;36:203–14. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2019.12.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Jablonka E, Oborny B, Molnár I.. et al. The adaptive advantage of phenotypic memory in changing environments. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1995;350:133–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Lachmann M, Jablonka E.. The inheritance of phenotypes: an adaptation to fluctuating environments. J Theor Biol 1996;181:1–9. doi: 10.1006/jtbi.1996.0109 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Hoyle RB, Ezard THG.. The benefits of maternal effects in novel and in stable environments. J R Soc Interface 2012;9:2403–13. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2012.0183 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Prizak R, Ezard THG, Hoyle RB.. Fitness consequences of maternal and grandmaternal effects. Ecol Evol 2014;4:3139–45. doi: 10.1002/ece3.1150 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Kuijper B, Hoyle RB.. When to rely on maternal effects and when on phenotypic plasticity? Evolution 2015;69:950–68. doi: 10.1111/evo.12635 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Kuijper B, Johnstone RA.. Parental effects and the evolution of phenotypic memory. J Evol Biol 2016;29:265–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Leimar O, McNamara JM.. The evolution of transgenerational integration of information in heterogeneous environments. Am Nat 2015;185:E55–69. doi: 10.1086/679575 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Furrow RE, Feldman MW.. Genetic variation and the evolution of epigenetic regulation. Evolution 2014;68:673–83. doi: 10.1111/evo.12225 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Greenspoon PB, Spencer HG.. The evolution of epigenetically mediated adaptive transgenerational plasticity in a subdivided population. Evolution 2018;72:2773–80. doi: 10.1111/evo.13619 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Kristensen TN, Ketola T, Kronholm I.. Adaptation to environmental stress at different timescales. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2020;1476:5–22. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13974 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Botero CA, Weissing FJ, Wright J.. et al. Evolutionary tipping points in the capacity to adapt to environmental change. PNAS 2015;112:184–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1408589111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Uller T, Nakagawa S, English S.. Weak evidence for anticipatory parental effects in plants and animals. J Evol Biol 2013;26:2161–70. doi: 10.1111/jeb.12212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Oomen RA, Kuparinen A, Hutchings JA.. Consequences of single-locus and tightly linked genomic architectures for evolutionary responses to environmental change. J Hered 2020;111:19–332. Esaa020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Otto SP, Lenormand T.. Resolving the paradox of sex and recombination. Nat Rev Genet 2002;3:252–61. doi: 10.1038/nrg761 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Lynch M, Walsh B.. Genetics and Analysis of Quantitative Traits. Sunderland: Sinauer Associates, Inc., 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Halligan DL, Keightley PD.. Spontaneous mutation accumulation studies in evolutionary genetics. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst 2009;40:151–72. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.39.110707.173437 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Durand E, Tenaillon MI, Ridel C.. et al. Standing variation and new mutations both contribute to a fast response to selection for flowering time in maize inbreds. BMC Evol Biol 2010;10:2. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-10-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Core Team R. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2019http://www.R-project.org (29 March 2022, date last accessed).
  • 41. Lande R. Adaptation to an extraordinary environment by evolution of phenotypic plasticity and genetic assimilation. J Evol Biol 2009;22:1435–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Ezard THG, Prizak R, Hoyle RB.. The fitness costs of adaptation via phenotypic plasticity and maternal effects. Funct Ecol 2014;28:693–701. doi: 10.1111/1365-2435.12207 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Callahan HS, Maughan H, Steiner UK.. Phenotypic plasticity, costs of phenotypes, and costs of plasticity. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2008;1133:44–66. doi: 10.1196/annals.1438.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Relyea RA. Costs of phenotypic plasticity. Am Nat 2002;159:272–82. doi: 10.1086/338540 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Dechaine JM, Johnston JA, Brock MT.. et al. Constraints on the evolution of adaptive plasticity: costs of plasticity to density are expressed in segregating progenies. New Phytol 2007;176:874–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8137.2007.02210.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Baugh LR, Day T.. Nongenetic inheritance and multigenerational plasticity in the nematode C. elegans. eLife 2020;9:e58498. doi: 10.7554/eLife.58498 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Herman JJ, Spencer HG, Donohue K.. et al. How stable ‘should’ epigenetic modifications be? Insights from adaptive plasticity and bet-hedging. Evolution 2014;68:632–43. doi: 10.1111/evo.12324 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Boomsma JJ, Baer B, Heinze J.. The evolution of male traits in social insects. Annu Rev Entomol 2005;50:395–420. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ento.50.071803.130416 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Holt WV, Fazeli A.. Sperm storage in the female reproductive tract. Annu Rev Anim Biosci 2016;4:291–310. doi: 10.1146/annurev-animal-021815-111350 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Halley JM. Ecology, evolution and 1/f-noise. Trends Ecol Evol 1996;11:33–7. doi: 10.1016/0169-5347(96)81067-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Vasseur DA, Yodzis P.. The color of environmental noise. Ecology 2004;85:1146–52. doi: 10.1890/02-3122 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Ruokolainen L, Lindén A, Kaitala V.. et al. Ecological and evolutionary dynamics under coloured environmental variation. Trends Ecol Evol 2009;24:555–63. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2009.04.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Uller T, English S, Pen I.. When is incomplete epigenetic resetting in germ cells favoured by natural selection? Proc Biol Sci 2015;282:20150682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

dvac007_Supp

Data Availability Statement

An R package implementing the simulations is available on github: https://github.com/ikron/indsim (last accessed 11 March).


Articles from Environmental Epigenetics are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES