Abstract
Background
Long-term survivors of pediatric acute lymphoblastic leukemia are at elevated risk for neurocognitive deficits and corresponding brain dysfunction. This study examined sex-based differences in functional neuroimaging outcomes in acute lymphoblastic leukemia survivors treated with chemotherapy alone.
Methods
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and neurocognitive testing were obtained in 123 survivors (46% male; median [min-max] age = 14.2 years [8.3-26.5 years]; time since diagnosis = 7.7 years [5.1-12.5 years]) treated on the St. Jude Total XV treatment protocol. Participants performed the n-back working memory task in a 3 T scanner. Functional neuroimaging data were processed (realigned, slice time corrected, normalized, smoothed) and analyzed using statistical parametric mapping with contrasts for 1-back and 2-back conditions, which reflect varying degrees of working memory and task load. Group-level fMRI contrasts were stratified by sex and adjusted for age and methotrexate exposure. Statistical tests were 2-sided (P < .05 statistical significance threshold).
Results
Relative to males, female survivors exhibited less activation (ie, reduced blood oxygen dependent–level signals) in the right parietal operculum, supramarginal gyrus and inferior occipital gyrus, and bilateral superior frontal medial gyrus during increased working memory load (family-wise error–corrected P = .004 to .008, adjusting for age and methotrexate dose). Female survivors were slower to correctly respond to the 2-back condition than males (P < .05), though there were no differences in overall accuracy. Performance accuracy was negatively correlated with fMRI activity in female survivors (Pearson’s r = −0.39 to −0.29, P = .001 to .02), but not in males.
Conclusions
These results suggest the working memory network is more impaired in female survivors than male survivors, which may contribute to ongoing functional deficits.
Leukemia is the most common pediatric cancer, with the majority of children being diagnosed between the ages of 2 and 5 years (1). Survival rates of childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia (ALL) exceed 90% on contemporary treatment protocols that have replaced prophylactic cranial irradiation with risk-stratified intrathecal therapy, high-dose intravenous methotrexate, and dexamethasone (2,3). Although survival rates have improved, leukemia patients treated on contemporary chemotherapy protocols remain at elevated risk for long-term neurocognitive impairment and altered patterns of brain function (4,5). Given the young age at diagnosis and high rate of survival, it is critical to examine the long-term impact of cancer and its treatment on brain function and neurocognitive abilities to mitigate the risk for potential neurocognitive late effects (ie, cognitive deficits that emerge in the years following therapy). Long-term survivors of childhood ALL (≥5 years from diagnosis) most commonly exhibit deficits in attention, executive function, and processing speed (5,6). These neurocognitive impairments can contribute to poor quality of life, reduced academic achievement, and increased unemployment among survivors, demonstrating a strong burden to the individual (7,8).
The degree of neurocognitive impairment in long-term ALL survivors is known to be associated with certain treatment exposures (ie, intrathecal therapy, high-dose methotrexate, dexamethasone, etc) and age at diagnosis, but recent evidence suggests that these interactions may be sex specific (9-11). A higher risk of neurocognitive impairment has been observed in female survivors compared with males, with research studies noting statistically significant deficits in attention and executive function (12-15). Neuroimaging investigations have also reported sex-specific differences in brain connectivity patterns in long-term ALL survivors, with females displaying greater brain alterations compared to males (10,16). For example, females exhibited widespread cortical thinning that corresponded with younger age at diagnosis and higher dexamethasone exposure, a pattern not observed among males. Additionally, female survivors displayed disrupted brain connectivity within the cerebello-thalamo-cortical network, which is involved in higher-ordered cognitive processes (ie, executive function, working memory) and rich in glucocorticoid receptors. These findings suggest that females may be particularly vulnerable to the neurotoxic effects of leukemia and its treatment, especially regarding corticosteroid exposure. To advance understanding about sex-based differences among childhood ALL survivors treated with chemotherapy, we undertook a large-scale neuroimaging investigation using task-based functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine sex-specific differences in brain activity during a working memory task.
Methods
Study Population
A total of 408 children with newly diagnosed ALL were treated on the Total XV protocol at St. Jude Children’s Research Hospital between 2000 and 2010 (ClinicalTrials.gov, NCT0013711) (2). Survivors were eligible for the separate prospective neuroimaging study if they were 5 years and more from diagnosis of ALL, English speaking, and 8 years and older at the time of study enrollment. Survivors were ineligible for the study if they had received cranial radiation therapy for central nervous system (CNS) relapse, had relapse or secondary neoplasms, were diagnosed with a genetic disorder associated with cognitive impairments (ie, Trisomy 21), or had a history of head trauma or neurological conditions unrelated to cancer therapy. Of the 302 eligible survivors, 213 (71%) completed neurocognitive testing, and 123 (58%) had evaluable functional neuroimaging data. Figure 1 summarizes the flow of study recruitment procedures. Demographic data (ie, sex) and clinical characteristics were obtained from the survivors’ medical records. Sex refers to the biological characteristics of males and females. This study was approved by the institutional review board, and written informed consent was obtained by the participants and/or their legal guardians.
Figure 1.
Flow chart of recruitment procedures. fMRI = functional magnetic resonance imaging; M = male; F = female.
Total Therapy XV Treatment
Children diagnosed with ALL were stratified and treated in low-risk or standard/high-risk treatment arms (17). Compared with patients treated in the standard/high-risk arm, those treated in the low-risk arm received fewer doses of triple intrathecal treatment with methotrexate, hydrocortisone and cytarabine (13-18 vs 16-25), lower dosage of intravenous high-dose methotrexate (2.5 vs 5.0 gm/m2) for every other week for 4 courses as consolidation treatment, and lower dosage of dexamethasone pulses at 8 vs 12 mg/m2 per day for 4 days per course for 25 courses. All long-term survivors in this study were treated using these protocols. Serum concentrations of methotrexate were obtained at 3 different time periods following intravenous administration (ie, 6, 23, and 42 hours post administration), and an average area under the curve (AUC) was computed. Dexamethasone AUC was also calculated based on serum samples, which were collected during therapy (ie, before, 1, 2, 4, 8 hours post oral administration).
Neurocognitive Evaluation
Neurocognitive evaluations were administered by certified examiners under the supervision of a board-certified clinical neuropsychologist on ALL survivors who were 5 to 10 years from their initial cancer diagnosis. Standardized clinical guidelines for cognitive evaluations were implemented to reduce interference, test order effects, and fatigue. The neurocognitive battery included standardized measures of intelligence (18), executive function (19), attention (20), processing speed (21), memory (22), and fine motor dexterity (23). The neurocognitive tests relevant to the aim of this study included metrics of working memory (ie, Weschler Digit Span Backwards), verbal fluency (ie, Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System Verbal Fluency), and cognitive flexibility (ie, Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System Trail Making), because all of these neurocognitive functions are related to the n-back task (24). Neurocognitive impairment was defined as age-adjusted normative scores below the 10th percentile.
N-Back Task
The n-back task is a measure of working memory load and capacity (25,26). The task involved 3 separate conditions: the 0-back, 1-back, and 2-back conditions, which reflect varying degrees of working memory and task load. Participants completed the n-back task while undergoing a fMRI exam. An illustration of the task paradigm and parameters is presented in Figure 2. A 2-second instruction screen was presented between each block with 5 seconds of silence after the instructions. Performance accuracy was calculated separately for each condition using the following formula: accuracy = (hits + correct rejections)/total stimuli; correct rejections = number of distractors − commission errors; hits = number of targets − omission errors. We applied performance cutoffs for the n-back task, based on previous literature, to ensure that participants were actively engaged in the task and had an appropriate understanding of the task instructions (24). For the 1-back and 2-back conditions, participants were excluded if they did not achieve at least 30% target accuracy (ie, with 25% target accuracy representing random selection). Given that the 0-back task is easier than the other 2 conditions, a more stringent performance threshold was applied (ie, cut off <75% target accuracy) (25). Participants who did not meet the performance threshold (ie, validity screening) were not included in the behavioral or fMRI analysis.
Figure 2.
Illustration of the n-back task paradigm used in the fMRI. fMRI = functional magnetic resonance imaging; ms = milliseconds; s = seconds.
Functional MRI
fMRI was performed on 3 T scanners (Trio or Skyra, Siemens, Malvern, PA, USA) using a 14-channel standard head coil. The functional images were acquired using a single-shot T2*-weighted echo-planar imaging sequence (Repetition Time [TR] = 2.06 seconds, Time to Echo [TE] = 30 ms, Feild-of-view [FOV] = 192 mm, matrix = 64 × 64, slice thickness = 5 mm) during the n-back task. Functional neuroimaging data were preprocessed (realigned, slice time corrected, normalized, and smoothed) and analyzed using statistical parametric mapping (SPM) (27) with contrasts developed for the 1-back vs 0-back, 2-back vs 0-back, and 2-back vs 1-back conditions. Group-level fMRI contrasts were stratified by sex and adjusted for age at evaluation and methotrexate dosage (ie, methotrexate AUC).
Statistical Analysis
Normative age-standardized scores were calculated among survivors to determine neurocognitive impairment in domains of working memory, verbal fluency, and cognitive flexibility (ie, scores <10th percentile) and compared by sex while adjusting for methotrexate AUC. The behavioral performance on the n-back task was analyzed using general linear models that adjusted for age at evaluation and methotrexate AUC. Functional neuroimages were preprocessed and analyzed using SPM (27). The SPM preprocessing steps included correction for interscan head motion using realignment techniques, normalization of images to the Montreal Neurological Institute brain template, and image smoothing with a 6-mm full width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel. The preprocessed functional images were then resliced to 2 mm isotropic resolution for analysis using general linear models. Statistical calculations were computed using SPM based on an initial voxel-level analysis, which included a family-wise error correction for multiple comparisons, with a minimum cluster size of 5 voxels (T threshold = 3.5). Differences in brain activity were considered statistically significant for corrected P values of less than .05. Cluster-level analyses was also conducted with a voxel uncorrected threshold P value of less than .001 and minimum cluster size of 5 voxels. Anatomical labels for the clusters were determined with SPM, and the location of clusters was crosschecked by visual comparison with the Talairach atlas. Associations between fMRI activity, neurocognitive performance, treatment exposures, patient demographics, and n-back task performance were examined using multiple linear regressions and Pearson correlations in XLSTAT 22.5.1. All statistical tests were 2-sided with a P < .05 threshold for statistical significance.
Results
fMRI and neurocognitive testing were obtained in 123 survivors (46% male, 54% female; median [min-max] age = 14.2 years [8.3-26.5 years]; time since diagnosis = 7.7 years [5.1-12.5 years]). The demographic information and clinical characteristics of this cohort are presented in Table 1. Neurocognitive and n-back task performance are presented in Table 2. No behavioral differences in performance accuracy during the n-back task were observed between male and female survivors. However, female survivors had statistically significant slower reaction times during the 2-back condition (P = .048). Standardized neurocognitive performance (ie, working memory, verbal fluency, and cognitive flexibility tasks) was not associated with fMRI brain activity during the n-back task. However, male survivors demonstrated greater deficits in cognitive flexibility than females (P = .003). There were no other differences between females and males for the other neurocognitive outcomes relevant to this study (ie, working memory or verbal fluency; P > .05).
Table 1.
Demographic and clinical characteristics of acute lymphoblastic leukemia survivors by sexa
Category | Overall | Females | Males | P |
---|---|---|---|---|
No. (%) | ||||
Sex | ||||
Male | 56 (46) | 0 | 56 | — |
Female | 67 (54) | 67 | 0 | — |
Treatment risk stratum | ||||
Low risk | 52 (42.3) | 46 (68.7) | 25 (32.6) | .007 |
Standard/high risk | 71 (57.7) | 21 (31.3) | 31 (67.4) | .007 |
Mean (SD) | ||||
Age at evaluation, y | 15.1 (4.7) | 15.9 (4.9) | 14.3 (4.5) | .07 |
Age at diagnosis, y | 7.2 (4.5) | 6.6 (4.3) | 7.8 (4.7) | .15 |
Methotrexate AUC | 32.8 (11.6) | 30.2 (10.1) | 35.9 (12.5) | .003 |
Dexamethasone AUC | 542.7 (285.5) | 531.3 (255.9) | 556.2 (316.6) | .84 |
Total IT injections, No. | 14.4 (3.9) | 13.5 (3.0) | 15.5 (4.6) | .005 |
AUC = area under the curve; IT = intrathecal injection of methotrexate. Student t test comparisons with P values less than .05 represent statistical significance.
Table 2.
Neurocognitive evaluations, n-back task performance, and fMRI outcomes among acute lymphoblastic leukemia survivors by sexa
Cognitive and fMRI Outcomes | Males | Females | P |
---|---|---|---|
(n = 56) M (SD) | (n = 67) M (SD) | ||
Wechsler digit span backwards, Z score | −0.31 (1.11) | −0.20 (0.89) | .64 |
D-KEFS trail-making task, Z score | −0.70 (1.19) | −0.15 (0.98) | .003 |
D-KEFS verbal fluency, Z score | −0.51 (0.96) | −0.26 (0.85) | .37 |
0-back task accuracy, % | 95.7 (0.06) | 95.1 (0.06) | .88 |
1-back task accuracy, % | 91.0 (0.11) | 90.1 (0.13) | .83 |
2-back task accuracy, % | 78.7 (0.16) | 79.6 (0.12) | .49 |
0-back hits reaction time, ms | 512.9 (74.2) | 540.0 (88.2) | .21 |
1-back hits reaction time, ms | 591.2 (136.3) | 637.9 (141.2) | .31 |
2-back hits reaction time, ms | 652.3 (140.7) | 723.3 (179.3) | .048 |
R. Inferior occipital gyrus BOLD signal | 0.133 (0.36) | −0.124 (0.38) | <.001 |
R. Supramarginal gyrus BOLD signal | 0.289 (0.41) | −0.061 (0.45) | <.001 |
R. Parietal operculum BOLD signal | 0.091 (0.22) | –0.104 (0.27) | <.001 |
R. Superior medial frontal gyrus BOLD signal | 0.268 (0.52) | −0.046 (0.46) | <.001 |
L. Superior medial frontal gyrus BOLD signal | 0.254 (0.57) | −0.072 (0.53) | <.001 |
BOLD = blood oxygen level–dependent; D-KEFS = Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System; fMRI = functional magnetic resonance imaging; L = left; R = right. Statistical analyses were conducted using general linear models adjusting for age at evaluation and methotrexate area under the curve, with P values less than .05 representing statistical significance. fMRI outcomes were corrected for multiple comparisons using family-wise error. BOLD signal values represent peak voxel activation within the region of interest. Z scores were age standardized and thus were only adjusted for methotrexate area under the curve in the general linear models.
For fMRI, females had reduced brain activity (ie, reduced blood oxygen level–dependent [BOLD] signal) in the right parietal operculum, supramarginal gyrus and inferior occipital gyrus, and bilateral superior medial frontal gyrus during increased working memory task load (ie, 2-back vs 1-back contrast) compared with males (family-wise error–corrected P = .004 to .008, adjusting for age at evaluation and methotrexate dose) (see Figure 3). Brain activation patterns were not associated with treatment risk group, age at diagnosis, executive dysfunction status, or total number of intrathecal injections. Multiple linear regression models revealed that performance accuracy on the n-back task accounted for 11.8% to 17.5% of the variation in BOLD signaling in the bilateral superior medial frontal gyrus during increased working memory load in female survivors, but not in male survivors (P < .05). Performance accuracy on the n-back task was negatively correlated with BOLD activation in this brain region during the 1-back (left medial frontal gyrus: Pearson r = −0.394, P = .001; right medial frontal gyrus: Pearson r = −0.322, P = .008) and 2-back conditions (left medial frontal gyrus: Pearson r = −0.358, P = .003, right medial frontal gyrus: Pearson r = −0.290, P = .02) in female survivors. Performance accuracy was not associated with fMRI activity in male survivors. Additionally, dexamethasone AUC accounted for 8.2% to 18.7% of the variation in BOLD signaling in the right parietal operculum, supramarginal gyrus, and inferior occipital gyrus among females during increased working memory load (P < .05). Dexamethasone AUC was negatively correlated with the amplitude of BOLD signals in these brain regions (parietal operculum: Pearson r = −0.433, P = .001; supramarginal gyrus: Pearson r = −0.286, P = .03; inferior occipital gyrus: Pearson r = −0.347, P = .008).
Figure 3.
Sex-specific variations in blood oxygen level–dependent signaling during increased working memory task load (ie, 2-back vs 1-back condition) presented in radiological convention. Female survivors display reduced activity in the (a) right inferior occipital gyrus, (b) supramarginal gyrus and parietal operculum, and (c) bilateral superior medial frontal gyrus, compared to males. L = left; R = right. T value threshold statistically significant if greater than 3.5.
Discussion
The findings from this study suggest that female sex is associated with brain dysfunction in the working memory networks of ALL survivors. Despite the lack of difference in behavioral performance accuracy between female and male survivors on the n-back task, there were sex-specific variations in reaction time (ie, processing speed) and brain activation patterns. Compared with males, female survivors were statistically significantly slower (P < .05) at correctly identifying target stimuli during the n-back task condition that required the greatest level of working memory load (ie, 2-back). Although reaction times typically become slower as a cognitive task becomes more difficult, these effects were exacerbated in female survivors. These findings suggest that the working memory network may become overloaded during complex tasks, with consequent impairment in processing speed and regional changes in functional brain activation. The current literature in noncancer populations does not support the notion of sex-specific differences in behavioral performance (ie, reaction time, accuracy) (28-30) or fMRI brain activity (31) during the verbal n-back task, suggesting that the differences observed in this study are unique to female ALL survivors who have undergone chemotherapy. These findings demonstrate that female survivors may be particularly sensitive to treatment exposures, which likely contribute to long-term neurocognitive late effects.
Previous neuroimaging investigations in noncancer populations have established that working memory is supported by activation of the fronto-parietal brain regions, including the cingulate, parietal, and prefrontal cortices (32). Interestingly, reduced BOLD activation was exclusively observed in these same brain regions among female survivors, suggesting a sex-specific vulnerability for impaired brain function in the years following chemotherapy. The reductions in brain activation among females may reflect emerging brain deficits that have yet to manifest into overt working memory impairments. Given that neurocognitive late effects are often subtle, functional neuroimaging techniques may be particularly useful for detecting discrete and domain-specific neurocognitive functions (6).
Performance accuracy on the 1-back and 2-back conditions was associated with BOLD activity during increased working memory load among female survivors. Considering that the fMRI contrast of interest was derived from the 2-back vs 1-back conditions, it is not surprising that accuracy on these trials accounted for some of the variation in BOLD signaling. In addition, dexamethasone exposure had a sex-specific impact on BOLD activation, with female survivors displaying a statistically significant inverse relationship between these 2 factors.
These findings are consistent with previous literature that observed sex-dependent variations on the impact of dexamethasone exposure on neuroanatomical brain structure in long-term ALL survivors (10). The increased density of glucocorticoid receptors in female survivors within these brain regions may increase the neurotoxicity of chemotherapy agents and steroids administered during ALL therapy. The neurotoxic agents used in chemotherapy are capable of penetrating and damaging the blood-brain barrier, which in turn can cause increased cytokine-induced inflammation (33), neural apoptosis (34), and decreased hippocampal neurogenesis (35). Additional longitudinal studies investigating neuroimaging and neurocognitive outcomes in relation to these treatment factors are warranted to help inform the validity of this proposed mechanism of neurocognitive impairment in long-term ALL survivors.
Research has indicated that the frontal lobes are involved in higher-ordered cognitive processes, including working memory (36), and are among the last brain regions to develop, with full maturation not met until the third decade of life (37-39). While caudal regions of the brain are more developed in children, rostral brain regions (ie, the prefronto-parietal cortex) are less developed and may be particularly susceptible to the neurotoxic effects associated with chemotherapy and steroid exposure. As previously mentioned, the peak incidence of ALL occurs between 2 and 5 years of age. Although rapid brain development occurs during this period, it is important to note that the child’s brain size is only 80%-90% of that achieved during adulthood and biologically significant changes in synaptic pruning, myelination, and reorganization have yet to occur (40). Given that children have yet to develop the myelination and dendritic arborization, their neurons need to be protected from chemotherapy-induced toxic insult to avoid cognitive deficits in the years following treatment. Given this trajectory of brain development in the frontal lobe, neurocognitive processes supported by the fronto-parietal regions (ie, working memory and attention) may be particularly vulnerable to the neurotoxicity associated with CNS-directed therapy in children, especially among females.
The findings from this study have limitations to consider, including the lack of a healthy control group. However, the large sample size permitted within group comparisons and clinically relevant outcomes was still observed. Different types of MRI scanners (trio and skyra) were used, though both MRIs were developed by the same manufacturer and had the same magnet strength of 3 T to ensure homogeneity in the data collection process. Variations in the type of scanner may introduce unwanted variance and reduce power to detect differences in BOLD signaling, but these factors would not account for the statistically significant differences observed. Other treatment variables that differed between male and female survivors, such as total number of intrathecal methotrexate injections or treatment risk stratification, were not adjusted for in the statistical models. Instead, methotrexate AUC dosage was adjusted for in the models because it highly correlates with the number of injections and treatment risk stratification but provides more information regarding methotrexate clearance and toxicity. Despite these limitations, this study demonstrated sex-based differences in brain activation patterns associated with working memory in a large cohort of long-term ALL survivors. These findings suggest that neural systems involved in working memory are detrimentally affected by contemporary chemotherapy protocols for pediatric ALL, and these effects may be exacerbated by female sex. Considering evidence indicating that sex hormones play varying roles in protecting the central nervous system from chemotherapy-related insult (41-44), future research investigations may benefit from including longitudinal assessments of sex hormones before, during, and following chemotherapy to determine the extent of their involvement in facilitating or reducing treatment-induced neurotoxicity.
Inclusion of structural and functional neuroimaging investigations (ie, task-based and resting-state fMRI, diffusion tensor imaging, etc) in long-term cancer survivors is essential to advance our understanding of how chemotherapy and other treatment factors can affect brain connectivity, network efficiency, and white matter integrity. Identification of risk factors associated with altered brain function and neurocognitive deficits is important to inform the development of interventions for current childhood cancer survivors and to improve treatment strategies for those who are newly diagnosed.
Funding
This work was supported by the National Cancer Institute at the National Institutes of Health T32 Institutional Research Training Grant (T32 CA225590 to KRK), National Institute of Mental Health (MH085849 to KRK), National Cancer Institute (CA195547 to MMH and LLR; CA021765), and by the American Lebanese Syrian Associated Charities (ALSAC).
Notes
Role of the funder: The funding source did not have a role in the study design, the collection, analysis, and interpretation of the data, the preparation and writing of the manuscript, or the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Disclosures: The authors report no conflict of interest or disclosures.
Author contributions: Conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, visualization, and writing original draft: KG, KRK; Methodology: KG, MAS, SF, KRK; Writing, review, and editing: KG, MAS, NP, EvdP, SF, LMJ, CP, MMH, WER, RS, KRK; Supervision and funding acquisition: KRK.
Acknowledgements: We thank the research participants for their involvement in the study.
Data Availability
The data supporting this article will be shared upon reasonable request to the corresponding author.
References
- 1. Ries LAG. Cancer Incidence and Survival Among Children and Adolescents: United States SEER Program, 1975-1995. National Cancer Institute; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Pui C-H, Campana D, Pei D, et al. Treating childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia without cranial irradiation. N Engl J Med. 2009;360(26):2730–2741. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Pui C-H, Carroll WL, Meshinchi S, Arceci RJ.. Biology, risk stratification, and therapy of pediatric acute leukemias: an update. J Clin Oncol. 2011;29(5):551–565. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Krull KR, Brinkman TM, Li C, et al. Neurocognitive outcomes decades after treatment for childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia: a report from the St Jude lifetime cohort study. J Clin Oncol. 2013;31(35):4407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Krull KR, Cheung YT, Liu W, et al. Chemotherapy pharmacodynamics and neuroimaging and neurocognitive outcomes in long-term survivors of childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia. J Clin Oncol. 2016;34(22):2644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Fellah S, Cheung YT, Scoggins MA, et al. Brain activity associated with attention deficits following chemotherapy for childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2019;111(2):201–209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Kirchhoff AC, Krull KR, Ness KK, et al. Physical, mental, and neurocognitive status and employment outcomes in the childhood cancer survivor study cohort. Cancer Epidemiol Prev Biomarkers. 2011;20(9):1838–1849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Kunin‐Batson A, Kadan‐Lottick N, Neglia JP.. The contribution of neurocognitive functioning to quality of life after childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia. Psychooncology. 2014;23(6):692–699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. van der Plas E, Qiu W, Nieman BJ, et al. Sex-specific associations between chemotherapy, chronic conditions, and neurocognitive impairment in acute lymphoblastic leukemia survivors: a report from the childhood cancer survivor study. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2021;113(5):588–596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Phillips NS, Cheung YT, Glass JO, et al. Neuroanatomical abnormalities related to dexamethasone exposure in survivors of childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2020;67(3):e27968. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Partanen M, Phipps S, Russell K, et al. Longitudinal trajectories of neurocognitive functioning in childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia. J Pediatr Psychol. 2021;46(2):168–178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Sherief LM, Sanad R, ElHaddad A, et al. A cross-sectional study of two chemotherapy protocols on long term neurocognitive functions in Egyptian children surviving acute lymphoblastic leukemia. Curr Pediatr Rev. 2018;14(4):253–260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Von der Weid N, Mosimann I, Hirt A, et al. Intellectual outcome in children and adolescents with acute lymphoblastic leukaemia treated with chemotherapy alone: age-and sex-related differences. Eur J Cancer. 2003;39(3):359–365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Jain N, Brouwers P, Okcu MF, Cirino PT, Krull KR.. Sex‐specific attention problems in long‐term survivors of pediatric acute lymphoblastic leukemia. Cancer. 2009;115(18):4238–4245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Jacola LM, Krull KR, Pui C-H, et al. Longitudinal assessment of neurocognitive outcomes in survivors of childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia treated on a contemporary chemotherapy protocol. J Clin Oncol. 2016;34(11):1239–1247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Phillips NS, Kesler SR, Scoggins MA, et al. Connectivity of the cerebello-thalamo-cortical pathway in survivors of childhood leukemia treated with chemotherapy only. JAMA Network Open. 2020;3(11):e2025839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Pui C, Relling M, Sandlund J, Downing J, Campana D, Evans W.. Rationale and design of Total Therapy Study XV for newly diagnosed childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia. Ann Hematol. 2004;83:S124–S126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1999; [Google Scholar]
- 19. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Holdnack J.. Reliability and validity of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System: an update. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(2):301–303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Conners C. Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II. Multi-Health Systems; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 21. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–4th ed. (WAIS–IV). San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson. 2008;22(498):1. [Google Scholar]
- 22. Meyers JE, Meyers KR.. Rey Complex Figure Test and Recognition Trial Supplemental Norms for Children and Adults. Psychological Assessment Resources; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- 23. Strauss E, Sherman E, Spreen O.. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 24. Van Der Plas E, Erdman L, Nieman BJ, et al. Characterizing neurocognitive late effects in childhood leukemia survivors using a combination of neuropsychological and cognitive neuroscience measures. Child Neuropsychol. 2018;24(8):999–1014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Owen AM, McMillan KM, Laird AR, Bullmore E.. N‐back working memory paradigm: a meta‐analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25(1):46–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Jacola LM, Willard VW, Ashford JM, et al. Clinical utility of the N-back task in functional neuroimaging studies of working memory. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2014;36(8):875–886. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002;15(1):273–289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Tende J, Eze E, Yusuf A, Malgwi I, Wilcox B.. Sex differences in the working memory of students in Ahmadu Bello University, Zaria, Nigeria using the n-back task. J Dental Med Sci. 2012;2(6):8–11. [Google Scholar]
- 29. Lejbak L, Crossley M, Vrbancic M.. A male advantage for spatial and object but not verbal working memory using the n-back task. Brain Cogn. 2011;76(1):191–196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Reed JL, Gallagher NM, Sullivan M, Callicott JH, Green AE.. Sex differences in verbal working memory performance emerge at very high loads of common neuroimaging tasks. Brain Cogn. 2017;113:56–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Schmidt H, Jogia J, Fast K, et al. No gender differences in brain activation during the N‐back task: an fMRI study in healthy individuals. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30(11):3609–3615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Chai WJ, Abd Hamid AI, Abdullah JM.. Working memory from the psychological and neurosciences perspectives: a review. Front Psychol. 2018;9:401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Pazdernik T. Neurotoxicology of chemotherapy in relation to cytokine release, the blood-brain barrier, and cognitive impairment. 2008;35(6):916–920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Kaufmann SH, Earnshaw WC.. Induction of apoptosis by cancer chemotherapy. Exp Cell Res. 2000;256(1):42–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Christie L-A, Acharya MM, Parihar VK, Nguyen A, Martirosian V, Limoli CL.. Impaired cognitive function and hippocampal neurogenesis following cancer chemotherapy. Clin Cancer Res. 2012;18(7):1954–1965. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Curtis CE, D'Esposito M.. Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003;7(9):415–423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Huttenlocher PR. Synaptic density in human frontal cortex-developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Res. 1979;163(2):195–205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Casey B, Giedd JN, Thomas KM.. Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development. Biol Psychol. 2000;54(1-3):241–257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Lenroot RK, Giedd JN.. Brain development in children and adolescents: insights from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30(6):718–729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Dekaban AS, Sadowsky D.. Changes in brain weights during the span of human life: relation of brain weights to body heights and body weights. Ann Neurol. 1978;4(4):345–356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Białek M, Zaremba P, Borowicz KK, Czuczwar SJ.. Neuroprotective role of testosterone in the nervous system. Pol J Pharmacol. 2004;56(5):509–518. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Dalle IA, Paranal R, Zarka J, et al. Impact of luteinizing hormone suppression on hematopoietic recovery after intensive chemotherapy in patients with leukemia. Haematologica. 2021;106(4):1097–1105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Krawczuk‐Rybak M, Płonowski M, Leszczyńska E, et al. The influence of different intensity of treatment on hormonal markers of gonadal function in acute lymphoblastic leukemia survivors. Hematol Oncol. 2019;37(5):609–616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Teixeira C, Reed JC, Pratt MC.. Estrogen promotes chemotherapeutic drug resistance by a mechanism involving Bcl-2 proto-oncogene expression in human breast cancer cells. Cancer Res. 1995;55(17):3902–3907. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The data supporting this article will be shared upon reasonable request to the corresponding author.