Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2022 Apr 26;17(4):e0267191. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0267191

Do family and maternal background matter? A multilevel approach to modelling mental health status of Australian youth using longitudinal data

Rubayyat Hashmi 1,2,*, Khorshed Alam 2,#, Jeff Gow 2,3,#, Sonja March 4,#
Editor: Petri Böckerman5
PMCID: PMC9041811  PMID: 35472126

Abstract

Purpose

Most previous research place great importance on the influence of family and maternal background on child and adolescents’ mental health. However, age of onset studies indicates that the majority of the mental health disease prevalence occurs during the youth years. This study investigates the relationship of family and maternal background, as well as individual circumstance on youth mental health status.

Method

Data from 975 participants and 4632 observations of aged cohort 15 to 19 years in the Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) longitudinal study were followed for 10 years (2007–2017). Multilevel logistic regression models were used to analyse the impact of youth circumstances on mental health status.

Results

The findings suggests that not all dimensions of family and maternal background (especially maternal education) have impacts on youth mental health. We found low household income (AOR: 1.572, 95% CI: 1.017–2.43) and adverse living arrangement (AOR: 1.586, 95% CI: 1.097–2.294) significantly increases mental disorder odds whereas maternal education or occupation fixed effects were not significant. Individual level circumstances have much stronger impact on youth mental health. We found financial shock (AOR: 1.412, 95% CI: 1.277–1.561), life event shock (AOR: 1.157, 95% CI: 1.01–1.326), long term health conditions (AOR: 2.855, 95% CI: 2.042–3.99), smoking (AOR: 1.676, 95% CI: 1.162–2.416), drinking (AOR: 1.649, 95% CI: 1.286–2.114) and being female (AOR: 2.021, 95% CI: 1.431–2.851) have significant deteriorating effects on youth mental health.

Conclusions

Our finding is in contrast to the majority of studies in the literature which give a preeminent role to maternal characteristics in child and youth mental health status. Mental health interventions should consider heterogeneity of adverse youth circumstances and health-related behaviours.

Introduction

Social gradients in physical and mental health status exist and social justice demand that mental health inequality is minimised [13]. Thus, understanding the determinants of socioeconomic inequality is important for policy makers and researchers alike. While socioeconomic inequalities in adult mental health dominates current research, a growing body of literature currently also suggests that lower socioeconomic status (SES) is an important determinant of mental health problems in children and adolescents [46]. Furthermore, since childhood and adolescence are the critical ages for the onset of mental illnesses, mounting evidence also suggests that maternal background plays an important role in the social determinants of children and adolescents’ mental health [79].

Although child and adolescent periods appear to be emerging points for mental disorders, age of onset (AOO) studies have identified that the majority of mental disorder incidence occurs at the early stages of youth, particularly when young people transition to adulthood [10, 11]. The problem in the existing literature is that the age bands in these studies are broad, obscuring the stages of youth by either younger youths being included with ‘children and adolescents’ (e.g. age 1–18 years) or older youths being included with ‘adults’ (e.g. 15–64 years) [5, 12, 13]. The circumstances (e.g., life chances, opportunities and adverse events) experienced by individuals in their childhood and adolescent period are certainly much different than the period when they are transitioning to youth and adulthood. Thus, the impact of family and maternal background on this transitioning phase (15–19 years) on an individual’s mental health outcome is not clear and may very well be different.

In this paper, we tried to address this issue by selecting a 15–19 years age cohort and following the cohort for ten years (up to six measurement points) to investigate the impact of youth circumstances (adverse life events experiences, family and maternal background characteristics, household characteristics etc.) on mental health outcomes. Although significant advances have been made in our understanding of the impact of family and maternal background on childhood mental health status, considerable knowledge gaps still exist. For instance, how different attributes that constitutes family and maternal social class variations (such as mothers’ education, income or occupational status) contributes to the variation in youth mental status or how such mental health inequalities evolve over time are not well understood in the literature [14, 15]. Little is known about the variability of individual level and social class level characteristics on mental health outcome inequalities for youth and young people. In summary, we answer two research questions here i) Which individual and background characteristics of youth circumstances impact youth mental health outcomes? And how much? and ii) Whether family and maternal background contribute to substantial variation in youth mental health status.

Thus, the primary goal of this paper is to improve this knowledge gap and attempt to provide a link between prior studies on childhood and adult mental health inequalities. In addition, the focus on Australian youth complements existing US, UK or European studies on youth mental health inequalities. Our study extends the literature to another developed country with different social welfare system and norms that provide different perspectives on mental health equity issues.

Methods

Data source

All our analyses are based on sample data from the Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) panel survey [16]. This nationally representative household survey has been carried out annually from 2001 through 2018 (waves 1–18). It interviews and subsequently reinterviews all members aged 15 years and over of the same selected household every year. More than 30,000 individuals (40,000+ enumerated) have participated in the survey over the years and on average 15,000 individuals have been interviewed every year. A 90% wave on wave response rates of HILDA survey are comparable with other large longitudinal surveys like the British Household Panel Study (BHPS) or Panel Study of Income Dynamics (PSID) [17]. Details of HILDA sample design, survey response rates and attrition rates can be found elsewhere [17].

Inclusion criteria of the samples

For the purpose of this study, we limit the sample to young Australians aged 15–19 years (late adolescent period) at the baseline wave (wave 7) and then followed the participants for 10 years (up to six measurement points) which covers youth (20–24 years) and transition to adulthood phase (25–29 years) in the follow up. We chose to start from wave 7, because HILDA survey did not start to collect Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K10) scores (our main outcome of interest) in earlier waves and it provides the score subsequently in every odd wave (every two years) thereafter. Thus, we constructed an unbalanced panel data using wave 7, 9, 11, 13, 15 and 17. To be included in the analyses, the participants had to be interviewed in the baseline wave 7 and has to appear in at least one of the follow-up waves. Our final sample contains 975 participants across the six waves with a total of 4,632 observations. The 15–19 age cohort was thus followed up to 25–29 years with an average of 5.18 observations per person. The participant flow into the sample is shown in Fig 1.

Fig 1. Participants flow into the sample.

Fig 1

Outcome variable, exposure variables and other co-variates

This study uses the Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K10) as the measure of mental health outcomes and is the main dependent variable for analyses [18]. In clinical practice, the scale is used to assess the likelihood of having a mental disorder; for example, a person with a score of 10–15 has a low risk of having a mental disorder whereas a person with a score of 20–24 is likely to have a mild mental disorder, a score of 25–30 would indicate a likely moderate mental disorder and a person with a score of 30–50 is likely to have a severe mental disorder [19]. In the analyses, we use a dichotomous K10 variable (where a score of greater than 20 depict the likelihood of a mental disorder) as measures of our dependent variable for mental health performance [20].

Following Roemer’s equality of opportunity theory [21, 22] we classify all our exposure variables into two types: i) circumstances category and ii) effort category. The theory of equality of opportunity revolves around the goal of compensating for ‘negative’ circumstances (such as parental background) on health outcomes while controlling the health inequalities generated by effort category variables (such as lifestyle or health habits) that can be attributed to the behaviour of an individual. We use the biological mothers’ education level and occupational status, household income and family living arrangements (whether the participant lived with both parents at the age of 14 years old) to determine the family and maternal background status as a group level characteristic of the circumstances category. We define maternal education level as low if the highest qualification level obtained by the mother is secondary level or lower. We use the Australian Socioeconomic Index 2006 (AUSEI06) occupational status scale as the measure of the occupational status of mother [23]. We assign occupational status as low if the value range falls in the lowest quintile. Similarly, we assign household income as low if the equivalised household income range falls in the lowest quintile. Using household income, family living arrangement, maternal education and occupational status we have constructed 16 (2x2x2x2) different types of family and maternal background history groups for the multilevel analyses.

We use the number of financial shocks, number of life event shocks and long-term health conditions in the individual level circumstances category [12]. The number of financial shock variable shows the number of adverse financial events the study participant has experienced (for example: went without meals or asked for financial help from friends or family). Similarly, life event shock variable shows the number of life events related to grief, loss or injury the study participant has suffered (for example: death of a family member or serious personal injury). The list of events that constitutes financial and life event shocks are given in the S1 Appendix. We use negative health habits such as being obese (as a proxy of unhealthy eating and lack of exercising), being a daily smoker and regular drinker (drinks more than four standard drink/day), and positive health habits such as being an active member of a sporting/hobby/community-based club or association as an effort type of variables. This study also included gender and rural residency as demographic covariates in the analyses based on past literature [24]. In addition, we construct our time variable by setting zero at the baseline wave 7 and subsequently adding two for each additional measurement point (since between wave time is two years and there are up to six measurement points) to get a ten-year follow-up at wave 17 (t = 0,2,4,6,8, and 10).

Statistical analyses

The authors constructed an unbalanced longitudinal data set of the youth cohort by linking an individual’s record who participated in the baseline (wave 7) at age 15–19 years and in one of the follow-up waves (9, 11, 13, 15 and 17). Descriptive statistics and mental health opportunity profile were summarized to understand the impact of family and maternal background group characteristics on youth mental health. Visual trends of psychological distress scale were analysed for group level characteristics. Traditional single level regression analysis such as logistic regression model only assumes fixed-effect impacts of dependent variables and does not allow for random effects of intercepts and slopes for individual and group level characteristics. However, data structure can be nested or clustered by some observable characteristics that creates similarity between individuals and ignoring these phenomena can violate the independence assumption of regression analysis. Multi-level models allow for a nested data structure (i.e., repeated measures) and make it possible to study sources of variance at different levels of an outcome variable [25]. The nested data structure is illustrated in Fig 2. In our analyses, we used both single level logistic regression and multilevel logistic regression models. we have nested our data structure into three levels: i) time, ii) individual, and iii) family and maternal background history types (a total of 16 different background history types; for example a background history type could be: household income- high; from two types: ‘high’ and ‘low’, mothers education- low; from two types: ‘high’ and ‘low’, mothers occupation- low; from two types: ‘high’ and ‘low’ and family living arrangement—whether not lived with both biological parents- yes; from two types: ‘yes’ and ‘no’. Thus, we have 2x2x2x2 = 16 types. A full combination of 16 types can be seen in Table 2’s opportunity profile). We assigned unique identifiers (From 1 to 16, see Table 2’s opportunity profile’s rank number for identifiers) for each group for the analysis. We control for individual fixed effects characteristics like circumstances and effort covariates in level 2 and group level fixed effects characteristics like various family and maternal background group characteristics in level 3. All statistical analyses were conducted using Stata 15.

Fig 2. Repeated measure three level data structure.

Fig 2

Ethics approval

The HILDA study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of Melbourne. The study used only de-identified existing unit record data from the HILDA survey. The authors completed and signed a confidentiality agreement with NCLD (ncldresearch@dss.gov.au) and obtained database access from the Australian Data Archive (ada@anu.edu.au) following application acceptance. Thus, the dataset studied during this work were subject to the signed confidentiality agreement.

Results

Describing the sample

Table 1 displays the socio-demographic characteristics of the study population by mental health status. It can be seen that age groups do not vary substantially in mean K10 score both in the baseline wave and in all waves average. However, in our sample, males have lower average K10 score than females in both baseline wave and all waves average. Richer household income group has on average two-point lower K10 scores at baseline and approximately three points in all waves average. Maternal education level does not indicate any significant difference in average scores between education groups. However, mothers with lower occupational status have approximately one point higher average scores. Those youth, who did not live with both biological parents at age 14, have two-point higher average K10 scores both in baseline and all waves average. The standard deviation of the K10 score for the socio-demographic characteristic variables (gender, age, household income groups, mothers’ education and occupation, living arrangement) ranges between five and eight points for both the baseline and all waves. This indicates considerable variability of the K10 score at the individual level.

Table 1. Socio-demographic characteristics of the study population by mental health status.

Baseline (wave 7) All waves
N (%) K10 score Mean (std) N (%) K10 score Mean (std)
Gender
 Male 465 (47.69) 15.76 (5.87) 2,109 (45.53) 16.39 (6.51)
 Female 510 (52.31) 17.78 (6.92) 2,523 (54.47) 17.77 (7.25)
Age
 15 years 197 (20.21) 16.62 (6.84) 197 (4.25) 16.62 (6.84)
 16 years 240 (24.62) 16.60 (6.29) 240 (5.18) 16.60 (6.29)
 17 years 184 (18.87) 17.38 (6.43) 363 (7.84) 17.22 (6.74)
 18 years 195 (20) 16.8 (6.26) 399 (8.61) 16.84 (6.43)
 19 years 159 (16.31) 16.77 (6.89) 466 (10.06) 16.85 (6.79)
HH Income group (Lowest quintile)
 Low 222 (22.77) 18.37 (7.65) 931 (19.78) 19.34 (8.32)
 High 753 (77.23) 16.36 (6.07) 3716 (80.22) 16.59 (6.46)
Mother’s Education (Low = secondary or lower)
 Low 204 (20.92) 16.80 (6.67) 1759 (37.97) 17.71 (7.27)
 High 771 (79.08) 16.80 (6.48) 2873 (62.03) 17.00 (6.87)
Mother’s occupational status (Lowest quintile)
 Low 216 (22.15) 17.43 (7.12) 943 (20.36) 18.46 (7.88)
 High 759 (77.85) 16.64 (6.33) 3689 (79.64) 16.80 (6.66)
Did not live with both parents
 No 652 (66.87) 16.03 (5.69) 3169 (68.42) 16.56 (6.46)
 Yes 323 (33.13) 18.41 (7.7) 1463 (31.58) 18.40 (7.79)

For a deeper understanding of family and maternal background, the mental health opportunity profile of the study participants is provided in Table 2. Depending upon household income, maternal education, maternal occupation and living history arrangement of the participant, 16 types of background groups are identified. The groups are ranked in ascending order according to the average K10 score (lower score implies better mental health). Out of 16 groups, there are three groups with high risk level of developing a mental disorder. Three more groups also show a K10 average of more than 19 and sightly avoid entering into the high-risk group. In addition, the high household income attribute has been clustered into lower rankings and vice versa. To further investigate, we plot the temporal evolution by the 16 family and maternal background types in Fig 3. The thick line (trend values varies between 15 and 25) shows that there also exist a lot of group level variability overtime in the average K10 scores. The trend analysis thus indicates both individual and group level variability and justifies analysing the data through a multi-level modelling approach.

Table 2. Mental health opportunity profile.

Rank HH income Mother’s education Mother’s occupational status Did not live with both parents Group sample size (n) Average k10 score of the participant Risk level
1 High Low High No 328 16.1 Low
2 High High High No 2032 16.25 Low
3 High Low Low Yes 68 16.53 Low
4 High High Low No 208 16.62 Low
5 High High High Yes 731 17.12 Low
6 Low High High No 231 17.17 Low
7 Low High Low No 100 17.48 Low
8 High Low High Yes 97 17.52 Low
9 High Low Low No 150 17.69 Low
10 High High Low Yes 87 18.72 Low
11 Low Low Low No 81 19.26 Low
12 Low Low High Yes 46 19.28 Low
13 Low Low High No 39 19.97 Low
14 Low High High Yes 185 20.7 High
15 Low Low Low Yes 107 20.89 High
16 Low High Low Yes 142 21.15 High

Fig 3. Temporal evolution of mental health status (K10 score) by background.

Fig 3

Regression analysis

The results of the regression models are in Table 3. Since, a single point change in the average K10 score might not mean anything unless it drives up into other risk categories Table 3 considers a dichotomous dependent variable (K10 ≥ 20 implies a higher risk of mental disorder) which measures risks through nonlinear estimation of odds ratios. The ‘null’ model results are shown in the first column. The ‘null’ model considers no explanatory variable and focuses just between and within individual variability. The random effect variances estimate for both family and maternal background level (level 3 σ2v0 is 0.423 and SE is lower at 0.202) and individual level (level 2 σ2u0 is 4.101 and SE is also much lower at 0.422) of the null model justifies the use of the multi-level approach. The second model in Table 3 shows the fixed effect logit estimates for comparison purpose. Unlike multilevel (ML) models, the logit does not have a random component and only shows fixed effects of the variables. To understand the family and maternal background variability, we do not consider the fixed effect of family and maternal background in the third model (Mixed 1 multilevel model). However, the final multilevel model (mixed 2) considers family and maternal background fixed effects. Individual fixed effects are considered in all models.

Table 3. Parameter estimates of different logit regression models.

(Depedent variable: Whether likely to have mental disorder- i.e. K10 ≥ 20).

Null Logit Mixed 1 Mixed 2
Fixed effects Estimate (OR) Std error 95% CI Estimate (AOR) Std error 95% CI Estimate (AOR) Std error 95% CI Estimate (AOR) Std error 95% CI
Intercept 0.296*** 0.059 (0.2–0.437) 0.168*** 0.017 (0.138–0.204) 0.075*** 0.018 (0.047–0.12) 0.05*** 0.011 (0.322–0.078)
Wave (time) 1.019 0.011 (0.998–1.04) 0.999 0.022 (0.957–1.042) 1.00 0.022 (0.961–1.047)
Individual characteristics
Gender—Female (Ref.: Male) 1.484*** 0.108 (1.286–1.712) 2.063*** 0.363 (1.461–2.913) 2.021*** 0.355 (1.431–2.851)
Rural resident—Yes (Ref.: No) 0.759* 0.095 (0.593–0.97) 0.89 0.195 (0.579–1.366) 0.899 0.197 (0.586–1.383)
Number of financial shock 1.321*** 0.041 (1.243–1.404) 1.436*** 0.074 (1.298–1.589) 1.412*** 0.072 (1.277–1.561)
Number of life event shock 1.156*** 0.052 (1.059–1.262) 1.161* 0.081 (1.013–1.331) 1.157* 0.08 (1.01–1.326)
Long term health condition—Yes (Ref.: No) 2.232*** 0.212 (1.853–2.688) 2.934*** 0.502 (2.098–4.103) 2.855*** 0.488 (2.042–3.99)
Club activities—Yes (Ref.: No) 0.651*** 0.05 (0.559–0.758) 0.623*** 0.078 (0.487–0.797) 0.635*** 0.08 (0.496–0.812)
Daily smoker—Yes (Ref.: No) 1.241* 0.125 (1.018–1.512) 1.801** 0.339 (1.246–2.604) 1.676** 0.313 (1.162–2.416)
Heavy drinker—Yes (Ref.: No) 1.344*** 0.099 (1.163–1.554) 1.651*** 0.209 (1.288–2.117) 1.649*** 0.209 (1.286–2.114)
Obese—Yes (Ref.: No) 1.131 0.11 (0.935–1.367) 1.372 0.269 (0.935–2.014) 1.311 0.256 (0.895–1.921)
Background characteristics
Household Income—Low (Ref.: High) 1.258* 0.116 (1.05–1.506) 1.572* 0.349 (1.017–2.43)
Did not live with both parents—Yes (Ref.: High) 1.183* 0.091 (1.017–1.376) 1.586* 0.298 (1.097–2.294)
Mothers Education—Low (Ref.: High) 0.972 0.088 (0.814–1.162) 0.921 0.203 (0.597–1.421)
Mothers’ occupation—Low (Ref.: High) 1.188 0.109 (0.992–1.423) 1.314 0.296 (0.845–2.043)
Random effects
Background (level 3)
Intercept variance σ2v0 0.423 0.202 (0.166–1.08) 0.078 0.082 (0.01–0.608) 7.14e-32 3.89e-17
Individual (level 2)
Intercept variance σ2u0 4.101 0.422 (3.353–5.017) 4.068 0.718 (2.878–5.749) 4.116 0.720 (2.921–5.8)
Wave variance σ2u1 0.062 0.015 (0.039–0.098) 0.062 0.015 (0.039–0.099)
Covariance σ2v0v1 -0.091 0.071 (-0.231–0.048) -0.098 0.071 (-0.238–0.041)
ICC
rho background 0.054 0.024 (0.022–0.127) 0.011 0.011 (0.001–0.076) 9.64e-33 5.26e-18
rho individual | background 0.579 0.026 (0.527–0.629) 0.558 0.043 (0.472–0.64) 0.556 0.043 (0.47–0.063)

Notes:

*** p < 0.001,

** p < 0.01, and

* p < 0.05.

The individual level circumstances category variables are highly significant in all models. For example, exposure to an additional financial shock has a 1.4 times higher risk of having a mental illness than individuals who do not experience a shock (logit Adjusted Odds Ratio [AOR]: 1.321, 95% CI: 1.243–1.404; Mixed 1 AOR: 1.436, 95% CI: 1.298–1.589 and Mixed 2 AOR: 1.412, 95% CI: 1.277–1.561). Similarly, a single life event shock increases the risk of having mental disorder by 1.15 times higher (logit AOR: 1.156, 95% CI: 1.059–1.262; Mixed 1 AOR: 1.161, 95% CI: 1.013–1.331 and Mixed 2 AOR: 1.157, 95% CI: 1.01–1.326). This is considerable if you consider the possibility of experiencing multiple life events and financial shocks in a period. In addition, the study results also show that individuals who have long term health conditions are approximately 2.9 times highly likely to have a mental condition (logit AOR: 2.232, 95% CI: 1.853–2.688; Mixed 1 AOR: 2.934, 95% CI: 2.098–4.103 and Mixed 2 AOR: 2.855, 95% CI: 2.042–3.99).

The individual effort or lifestyle category variables such as ‘daily smoker’, ‘heavy drinker’ and ‘active membership of club or sporting activities’ are also significant in all models. Club activities have a positive effect on mental health (logit AOR: 0.651, 95% CI: 0.559–0.758; Mixed 1 AOR: 0.623, 95% CI: 0.487–0.797 and Mixed 2 AOR: 0.635, 95% CI: 0.496–0.812). On the contrary, negative habits such as smoking (logit AOR: 1.241, 95% CI: 1.018–1.512; Mixed 1 AOR: 1.801, 95% CI: 1.246–2.604 and Mixed 2 AOR: 1.676, 95% CI:1.162–2.416) and drinking (logit AOR: 1.344, 95% CI: 1.163–1.554; Mixed 1 AOR: 1.651, 95% CI: 1.288–2.117 and Mixed 2 AOR: 1.649, 95% CI: 1.286–2.114) have deteriorating effects on mental health. This study, however, did not find any significant association of being obese and mental health for the study cohort in all our models. In the case of demographic variables, the study found that women are twice as likely as men to have a mental disorder (logit AOR: 1.484, 95% CI: 1.286–1.712; Mixed 1 AOR: 2.063, 95% CI: 1.461–2.913 and Mixed 2 AOR: 2.021, 95% CI: 1.431–2.851). However, the ‘rural resident’ variable was found to be significant in only the logit estimate (AOR: 0.759, 95% CI: 0.593–0.97). In addition, the study found not significant association between the sample period (time variable) and mental disorder of the study cohort.

In our findings, individual-level fixed effects have much stronger impacts on mental health than family and maternal background characteristics. We found that only household income and parental living arrangement (whether participants did not have the opportunity to live with both biological parents) were significant. Individuals who grew up in a poor household have approximately 1.6 times more likely to have mental disorder compared to youth who grew up in an affluent family (logit AOR: 1.258, 95% CI: 1.05–1.506; Mixed 2 AOR: 1.572, 95% CI: 1.017–2.43). Similarly, individuals who did not grow up with both biological parents in their childhood have approximately 1.6 times more likely to have mental disorder compared to the youths who grew up with both parents (logit AOR: 1.183, 95% CI: 1.017–1.376; Mixed 2 AOR: 1.586, 95% CI: 1.097–2.294). However, in our study, both mother’s education and occupational status were not significant in any model. In addition, the random variances of family and maternal background in multilevel models were much lower compared to the null model (Null σ2v0: 0.423, 95% CI: 0.166–1.08 and Mixed 1 σ2v0: 0.078, 95% CI: 0.01–0.608). Indeed, the background variance disappears if fixed effect background group characteristics are considered. Contrary to background random effects, individual level intercept variances are much larger (Null σ2u0: 4.101, 95% CI: 3.353–5.017, Mixed 1 σ2u0: 4.068 95% CI: 2.878–5.749 and Mixed 2 σ2u0: 4.116, 95% CI: 2.921–5.8). In summary, rather than the group level family and maternal backgrounds, the driving forces in mental health outcomes of the youths are the individual-level characteristics.

Discussion

The present study aimed to investigate the influence of group level family and maternal background characteristics and individual level circumstances-effort characteristics on the performance of youth mental health over time in Australia. Past research amassed substantial evidence in linking maternal education and occupation, with child’s health outcomes [6, 7, 9]. However, contrary to this, we did not find any evidence linking youths’ mental health with mother’s education in any of our regression results. Perhaps, the thesis examined by Patrick West in earlier research plays a role in this context [26]. West argued that youth, in contrast to childhood, possess a process of equalisation which removes the influences of certain dimensions of family background differences (such as maternal education in our case) in youth mental health outcomes. Few studies have explored this area, and further work is needed for the youth age groups. It is possible that as youth become more independent that the influence of mothers’ education becomes less important. We did, however, find significant impact of household income and family living arrangement on mental health performance of the youth. This impact is supported by other empirical literature [6, 7, 9, 27].

In order to investigate the underlying value judgement of individual effects, we followed equality of opportunity theory and categorised our variables into circumstances and effort groups [21, 22]. Our estimated results are consistent with the theory. We found that financial shocks, life event shocks and long-term health conditions significantly deteriorate youth mental health condition. These findings are consistent with the adverse event literature [12, 28, 29]. In addition, we found that negative health habits such as smoking and drinking worsen mental health where as positive social habits such a club or sporting activities favours mental health, which is also in line with existing research [30]. Certainly, as youth become independent, the role of social relationships with those outside of families become particularly important in bolstering mental health.

One of the major contributions of this study is that we considered individual and group level variability through a multilevel modelling technique that other studies in the literature ignore. We found that there exists significant variability in individual level characteristics. In addition, individual level slope and intercepts also varied across time. However, compared with individual effects, the group level impact of family and maternal background characteristics did not vary. The implication of our finding is that, even though, some background dimensions (i.e., household income and living arrangements) have significant influences, the impact of maternal background is much smaller than the individual effects such as financial and adverse life events, long-term health conditions, and health behaviour related activities (smoking and drinking habits).

Our results and findings have some interesting implications. Our findings stimulate discussion about the mechanism of maternal background linking the mental health childhood and adult cohorts. The findings suggest, more research is needed both in childhood and adult cohorts to further our understanding as to the impact of maternal background. Whilst maternal background may shape health in early childhood, its role in shaping youth health and mental health may not be so clear. On the other hand, there are number of factors that are clearly linked to youth mental health trajectories, including their physical health during ages 15–19. For example, smoking and drinking have clearly negative consequences on youth mental health, whereas club activities have positive effects. Policy makers might therefore be interested in implementing health related behavioural interventions to promote both physical and mental health. Another observation of this study also suggests the importance of providing ongoing support to youth who have experienced financial and adverse life events in order to prevent long-term mental illness. This may include financial, care coordination and emotional support to manage the consequences of the adverse events in the short-term and trauma-informed psychological care in the long-term. Detailed research in the methodology and design of such interventions as well as estimation of the associated delivery costs of such program are needed.

A few limitations rise when interpreting the findings of this study. First, the primary outcome of this study is a dichotomized variable that serves as a proxy measure to identify people with mental disorder. This procedure overlooks the fact that an increase in the K10 score indicates an increase in suffering and an increased risk of serious mental problems. As a result, mental illness severity categories were excluded from our study. Second, we have used K10, as an instrument of psychological distress to measure the likelihood of having a mental disorder. Although this measure has the strength of reducing the self-reported bias of patient reported mental health outcome, it is still an instrument that primarily used to measure psychological distresses. Thus, a bit of caution is necessary when interpreting outcomes with such instruments. Future research is necessary to further explore our research questions with other valid instruments. Lastly, we do not completely rule out the potential of ecological fallacy in relation to maternal background variation. Given the paucity of evidence about the influence of maternal background variables in our study, caution is advised when interpreting this finding. Additional research is required to validate this issue.

Conclusions

In summary, our findings contribute to current knowledge by drawing attention to the lack of impact of maternal background on youth mental health. Our study findings suggest that the influence of maternal background is significantly less than the individual impacts of adverse life events, chronic health disorders, and health behaviour-related activities. We also extend the scope of our research by using improved modelling techniques, for example, utilising multi-level modelling to assess mental health outcomes, which is another major contribution of this study.

It is imperative that future research examines further the link of maternal background between younger and older age cohorts. The main strength of our study is the use of an equality of opportunity framework and multilevel modelling techniques to address critical questions on youth mental health in Australia. Policy-wise, mental health interventions should consider heterogeneity of adverse youth circumstances and health-related behaviours. This research will provide essential insights into how to improve such interventions.

Supporting information

S1 Appendix

(DOCX)

Data Availability

Melbourne Institute of Applied Economic and Social Research (MIAESR) is the data custodian of the HILDA survey. Access to this data is restricted, and it is not publicly available. The data is available from the National Centre for Longitudinal Data (NCLD) of Department of Social Services (DSS) for researchers of approved organisations who meet the criteria for access to confidential data. Those interested in accessing this data should contact the NCLD and Australian Data Archive (ADA). Those who apply to access the data need to complete the confidentiality deed poll and send the copy to NCLD (ncldresearch@dss.gov.au) and ADA (ada@anu.edu.au). The following web link has the guideline for the application: https://dataverse.ada.edu.au/dataverse.xhtml. To contact the national office: 71 Athllon Drive, Greenway ACT 2900, GPO Box 9820, Canberra ACT 2601. DSS national number: 1300 653 227, For International Callers: + 61 2 6146 0001. To contact MIAESR: Melbourne Institute of Applied Economic and Social Research, The University of Melbourne, VIC 3010, Australia (Tel: +61 3 8344 2073 or Fax: +61 3 9347 6739).

Funding Statement

The authors received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Fryers T, Melzer D, Jenkins R. Social inequalities and the common mental disorders—A systematic review of the evidence. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2003;38(5):229–37. doi: 10.1007/s00127-003-0627-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Marmot M, Bell R. Fair society, healthy lives. Public Health. 2012;126:S4–S10. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2012.05.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hashmi R, Alam K, Gow J, March S. Prevalence of Mental Disorders by Socioeconomic Status in Australia: A Cross-Sectional Epidemiological Study. American Journal of Health Promotion. 2020:0890117120968656. doi: 10.1177/0890117120968656 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Koivusilta LK, Rimpelä AH, Kautiainen SM. Health inequality in adolescence. Does stratification occur by familial social background, family affluence, or personal social position? BMC Public Health. 2006;6:110-. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-6-110 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.McLaughlin KA, Breslau J, Green JG, Lakoma MD, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, et al. Childhood socio-economic status and the onset, persistence, and severity of DSM-IV mental disorders in a US national sample. Social Science & Medicine. 2011;73(7):1088–96. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.06.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Reiss F. Socioeconomic inequalities and mental health problems in children and adolescents: A systematic review. Social Science & Medicine. 2013;90:24–31. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.04.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Arroyo-Borrell E, Renart G, Saurina C, Saez M. Influence maternal background has on children’s mental health. International Journal for Equity in Health. 2017;16(1):63. doi: 10.1186/s12939-017-0559-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Cui Y, Liu H, Zhao L. Mother’s education and child development: Evidence from the compulsory school reform in China. Journal of Comparative Economics. 2019;47(3):669–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jce.2019.04.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Meyrose A-K, Klasen F, Otto C, Gniewosz G, Lampert T, Ravens-Sieberer U. Benefits of maternal education for mental health trajectories across childhood and adolescence. Social Science & Medicine. 2018;202:170–8. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.02.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Girolamo Gd, Dagani J, Purcell R, Cocchi A, McGorry PD. Age of onset of mental disorders and use of mental health services: needs, opportunities and obstacles. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences. 2012;21(1):47–57. Epub 2011/12/13. doi: 10.1017/s2045796011000746 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kessler RC, Amminger GP, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Alonso J, Lee S, Üstün TB. Age of onset of mental disorders: a review of recent literature. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 2007;20(4). doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e32816ebc8c [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hashmi R, Alam K, Gow J. Socioeconomic inequalities in mental health in Australia: Explaining life shock exposure. Health Policy. 2020;124(1):97–105. doi: 10.1016/j.healthpol.2019.10.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Reiss F, Meyrose A-K, Otto C, Lampert T, Klasen F, Ravens-Sieberer U. Socioeconomic status, stressful life situations and mental health problems in children and adolescents: Results of the German BELLA cohort-study. PLOS ONE. 2019;14(3):e0213700. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0213700 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.World Health Organization. Improving early childhood development: WHO guideline. Geneva: World Health Organization,; 2020. 80 p. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.World Health Organization. Guidelines on mental health promotive and preventive interventions for adolescents: helping adolescents thrive. Geneva: WHO; 2020. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.The Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) Survey, RESTRICTED RELEASE 18 (Waves 1–18) [Internet]. ADA Dataverse. 2020. doi: 10.26193/BBOTSM [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Summerfield M, Bright S, Hahn M, La N, Macalalad N, Watson N, et al. HILDA User Manual—Release 18. Australia: Melbourne Institute: Applied Economics and Social Research, University of Melbourne; 2019.
  • 18.Kessler RC, Andrews G, Colpe LJ, Hiripi E, Mroczek DK, Normand SLT, et al. Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychological Medicine. 2002;32(6):959–76. Epub 2002/09/26. doi: 10.1017/s0033291702006074 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Wooden M. Use of the Kessler Psychological Distress Scale in the HILDA Survey. Melbourne: Melbourne Institute of Applied Economic and Social Research, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Andrews G, Slade T. Interpreting scores on the Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K10). Australian and New Zealand Journal of Public Health. 2001;25(6):494–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-842x.2001.tb00310.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Roemer JE. Equality of Opportunity. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Roemer JE, Trannoy A. Equality of Opportunity: Theory and Measurement. Journal of Economic Literature. 2016;54(4):1288–332. doi: 10.1257/jel.20151206 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.McMillan J, Beavis A, Jones FL. The AUSEI06: A new socioeconomic index for Australia. Journal of Sociology. 2009;45(2):123–49. doi: 10.1177/1440783309103342 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Silva M, Loureiro A, Cardoso G. Social determinants of mental health: A review of the evidence. Eur J Psychiatry. 2016;30(4):259–92 [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Evans CR, Williams DR, Onnela J-P, Subramanian SV. A multilevel approach to modeling health inequalities at the intersection of multiple social identities. Social Science & Medicine. 2018;203:64–73. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.11.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.West P. Health inequalities in the early years: Is there equalisation in youth? Social Science & Medicine. 1997;44(6):833–58. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(96)00188-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Perales F, Johnson SE, Baxter J, Lawrence D, Zubrick SR. Family structure and childhood mental disorders: new findings from Australia. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2017;52(4):423–33. doi: 10.1007/s00127-016-1328-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Dalgard OS, Bjork S, Tambs K. Social support, negative life events and mental health. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1995;166(JAN.):29–34. doi: 10.1192/bjp.166.1.29 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Schilling EA, Aseltine RH Jr., Gore S. Adverse childhood experiences and mental health in young adults: a longitudinal survey. BMC Public Health. 2007;7:30-. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-7-30 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Buttery AK, Mensink GBM, Busch MA. Healthy behaviours and mental health: findings from the German Health Update (GEDA). Eur J Public Health. 2014;25(2):219–25. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/cku094 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Petri Böckerman

16 Jun 2021

PONE-D-21-14569

Does maternal background matter? A multilevel approach to modelling mental health status of Australian youth using longitudinal data

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Hashmi,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

The revised version should address all comments.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jul 31 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Petri Böckerman

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2.  Thank you for stating the following financial disclosure:

"NO - The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript."

At this time, please address the following queries:

  1. Please clarify the sources of funding (financial or material support) for your study. List the grants or organizations that supported your study, including funding received from your institution.

  2. State what role the funders took in the study. If the funders had no role in your study, please state: “The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.”

  3. If any authors received a salary from any of your funders, please state which authors and which funders.

  4. If you did not receive any funding for this study, please state: “The authors received no specific funding for this work.”

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

3. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

4. Your ethics statement should only appear in the Methods section of your manuscript. If your ethics statement is written in any section besides the Methods, please move it to the Methods section and delete it from any other section. Please ensure that your ethics statement is included in your manuscript, as the ethics statement entered into the online submission form will not be published alongside your manuscript.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: I Don't Know

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: More references to be added to support the introduction, purpose and discussions. Additional comments are attached in the reviewed manuscript. Conclusion should be more specific and can't be generalized.

Reviewer #2: Mental illness is a leading cause of disability worldwide among young people (aged 10–24 years). Whilst there is a good body of work that has generated substantial evidence about the influence of maternal circumstances on child and adolescent mental health, a key methodological issue in most of them being their exclusive reliance on cross-sectional designs. Longitudinal analyses using repeated measures offer a richer understanding of the natural history as one progress in the life trajectory and can identify risk factors associated with transitions. In this context, the present manuscript on youth mental health using longitudinal data has potential scope for adding further evidence to the existing literature on child and adolescent mental health and will contribute towards life course perspective on mental health problems.

However, for better understanding and clarity, I have the following concerns to be addressed:

English language and grammar used throughout the manuscript needs thorough revision and editing

Purpose & Abstract: The method in the Abstract section should go beyond the description of data and provide the method (model, Analysis) used for answering the research question.

Keywords can be better refined to match the appropriate controlled vocabulary

Wherever possible, a better technical term should be used. For example, “ great emphasize or great importance” instead of “great store”; “important determinant/risk factor” instead of socioeconomic status (SES) is an “important marker.”

Introduction: As the authors have stated that “circumstances experienced by individuals in their childhood and adolescent period are certainly much different than the period when they are transitioning to youth and adulthood” it is important that how they have addressed or parametrized the time-varying covariates in their model. Currently, the manuscript falls short of providing the detail.

Also, it would be added value to show how the covariates have varied over time to substantiate the above statement.

In the term “to investigate the impact of youth circumstances”, circumstances here mislead one to assume those recent or current occurrences around youth period. They may not be appropriate to describe the maternal circumstances. The authors need to use better term to denote the maternal circumstances.

The specific objective or research question, or hypothesis must be mentioned explicitly besides the primary goal. Otherwise, it will allow readers to make their own assumptions.

Methods: The authors should have clarity and consistency in using the terms mental health problems, mental health distress and mental disorders. Mental disorders usually require a diagnostic tool. K10 is used to detect non-specific psychological distress, not mental disorders.

The authors do not specify the point in time at which the covariates for maternal background status are considered. Are these the data collected at baseline? If so, how would they take into account of time-varying components? Household income might change over time.

For other covariates, are baseline values used? Time-varying values? Values from the previous period?

For demographic covariates, there are many families that move from rural to urban, and some vice versa. How this was addressed in the analyses.

Authors need to provide brief details on how they have addressed repeated measures, multi-collinearity and interaction -effects in their model.

Probably, the author can bring more clarity to the nested structure through diagrammatic illustration. Further, instead of “maternal background history” the author should use “maternal background history groups”, as it allows one to assume it as an individual characteristic without the qualifier.

Results: The results section appears a little terse, and the language used to describe the results needs to be reconciled for better clarity. For example, one loses clarity on the sentence “All groups have approximately six to seven points of standard deviation which indicates considerable variability at the individual level”; it was unclear what the authors meant by “all groups” and how group value indicates individual variability. This information can be added in Table 1 for better clarity.

The authors can prefer to use the term “substantially” instead of significantly in the sentence “It can be seen that age groups do not vary significantly in mean K10 score both”, as it was not finding from statistical analyses.

Though not a statistical expert, it was not clear either from their description of their statistical analyses or from table 3 how they arrived at individual estimates for background characteristics if they were grouped to form level 3 with 16 groups. Suppose the background characteristic were entered at the individual level (2), then how they can be a component of level 3. For example, if maternal education is one component that contributed to form level 3 group, then how one can derive estimates for maternal education. The authors need to specify which variables corresponded to random slope and random intercept.

The components like household income, family living arrangement, maternal education and occupational status are very much a characteristic at individual level. However, the authors have used these components and formed 16 subjective groups. In that sense, I presume that there was no natural nesting. The authors need to provide strong rational for artificially creating a group and its practical relevance.

Discussion: Overall, the discussion needs strengthening by relating the results with the available literature and include strengths and limitations.

The authors need to discuss the limitation of dichotomizing the categorical outcome especially in the context that increase in K10 score signifies increase in distress.

The way the group (level 3) was conceptualized has a significant impact on the study finding; there are evidences linking maternal characteristics with some individual characteristics which were significantly associated with mental health of young. Without looking at moderation/interaction effects, any conclusion on maternal characteristic would be unconvincing.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Racha Abi Hana

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Attachment

Submitted filename: PONE-D-21-14569_reviewer-RAH.pdf

Decision Letter 1

Petri Böckerman

25 Jan 2022

PONE-D-21-14569R1Does maternal background matter? A multilevel approach to modelling mental health status of Australian youth using longitudinal dataPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Hashmi,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. The revised version should address the remaining comments.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Mar 11 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Petri Böckerman

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #2: I Don't Know

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #2: No

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #2: Overall, the authors have addressed most of the concerns. However, based on the response by the authors, I have the following concerns:

The authors should remove the term “group” from “maternal background group” in all the places except in the statistical analyses section. It would not be appropriate and easily comprehensible to use the term “maternal background group” in all the places.

As the grouping involves HH income and family living arrangement, which are more of a family background characteristic, it would be misleading and erroneous to include and label it as maternal background characteristics. The face validity of the construct of maternal background becomes debatable when HH income and family living arrangement, which are more of a family background characteristic, were included to define the maternal background characteristics. The authors need to use a more valid and appropriate term as it has implications for the conclusions. This is particularly important when this paper makes a strong claims and conclusions about the lack of impact of maternal background on youth mental health.

In this context, the authors need to refine the title to reflect the content and context more accurately.

From the author's response, it became evident that the grouping was forced artificially to answer the research question, and the model did not follow natural nesting. The very nature of grouping and the subsequent modelling and conclusions does not rule out the possibility of ecological fallacy completely. This will be a serious concern, and the authors need to be cautious in making strong conclusions on the lack of impact of maternal background on youth mental health. Accordingly, the abstract and discussion section should be refined and revised further to account for the limitations of the grouping and modelling.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 2

Petri Böckerman

5 Apr 2022

Do family and maternal background matter? A multilevel approach to modelling mental health status of Australian youth using longitudinal data

PONE-D-21-14569R2

Dear Dr. Hashmi,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Petri Böckerman

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #2: I Don't Know

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #2: No

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #2: I thank the authors for their careful responses and revisions to their paper in response to my comments

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #2: Yes: Senthil Amudhan

Acceptance letter

Petri Böckerman

13 Apr 2022

PONE-D-21-14569R2

Do family and maternal background matter? A multilevel approach to modelling mental health status of Australian youth using longitudinal data

Dear Dr. Hashmi:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Professor Petri Böckerman

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Appendix

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: PONE-D-21-14569_reviewer-RAH.pdf

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: response2_rc_pone_v1_221121.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: response_rc_plos_300322.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    Melbourne Institute of Applied Economic and Social Research (MIAESR) is the data custodian of the HILDA survey. Access to this data is restricted, and it is not publicly available. The data is available from the National Centre for Longitudinal Data (NCLD) of Department of Social Services (DSS) for researchers of approved organisations who meet the criteria for access to confidential data. Those interested in accessing this data should contact the NCLD and Australian Data Archive (ADA). Those who apply to access the data need to complete the confidentiality deed poll and send the copy to NCLD (ncldresearch@dss.gov.au) and ADA (ada@anu.edu.au). The following web link has the guideline for the application: https://dataverse.ada.edu.au/dataverse.xhtml. To contact the national office: 71 Athllon Drive, Greenway ACT 2900, GPO Box 9820, Canberra ACT 2601. DSS national number: 1300 653 227, For International Callers: + 61 2 6146 0001. To contact MIAESR: Melbourne Institute of Applied Economic and Social Research, The University of Melbourne, VIC 3010, Australia (Tel: +61 3 8344 2073 or Fax: +61 3 9347 6739).


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES