Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Apr 27.
Published in final edited form as: Genes Brain Behav. 2021 Oct 21;20(8):e12773. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12773

Heritable variation in locomotion, reward sensitivity and impulsive behaviors in a genetically diverse inbred mouse panel

Lauren S Bailey 1, Jared R Bagley 1, Rainy Dodd 2, Ashley Olson 2, Mikayla Bolduc 2, Vivek M Philip 2, Laura G Reinholdt 2, Stacey J Sukoff Rizzo 2,3, Lisa Tarantino 2,4, Leona Gagnon 2, Elissa J Chesler 2, J David Jentsch 1,2,*
PMCID: PMC9044817  NIHMSID: NIHMS1794536  PMID: 34672075

Abstract

Drugs of abuse, including alcohol and stimulants like cocaine, produce effects that are subject to individual variability, and genetic variation accounts for at least a portion of those differences. Notably, research in both animal models and human subjects point towards reward sensitivity and impulsivity as being trait characteristics that predict relatively greater positive subjective responses to stimulant drugs. Here we describe use of the eight Collaborative Cross (CC) founder strains and thirty-eight (reversal learning) or ten (all other tests) CC strains to examine the heritability of reward sensitivity and impulsivity traits, as well as genetic correlations between these measures and existing addiction-related phenotypes. Methods. Strains were all tested for activity in an open field and reward sensitivity (intake of chocolate BOOST®). Mice were then divided into two counterbalanced groups and underwent reversal learning (impulsive action and waiting impulsivity) or delay discounting (impulsive choice). Results. CC and founder mice demonstrate significant heritability for impulsive action, impulsive choice, waiting impulsivity, locomotor activity, and reward sensitivity, with each impulsive phenotype determined to be non-correlating, independent traits. This research was conducted within the broader, inter-laboratory effort of the Center for Systems Neurogenetics of Addiction (CSNA) to characterize CC and DO mice for multiple, cocaine abuse related traits. These data will facilitate the discovery of genetic correlations between predictive traits, which will then guide discovery of genes and genetic variants that contribute to addictive behaviors.

Introduction

Not all individuals who initiate drug or alcohol use will eventually lose control over their use and/or meet diagnostic criteria for Substance Use Disorder (SUD). This progression is moderated by a myriad of genetic and environmental factors that interact with one another in a developmentally- and sex-dependent manner (Bevilacqua & Goldman, 2013; Bezdijan, Baker, & Tuvblad, 201; Kreek et al., 2005; Piazza & Deroche-Gamonet, 2013). Evidence from twin studies supports the idea that the majority of risk for developing an SUD relates to a single substance-nonspecific genetic factor, with lesser influences of common and unique environmental influences (Kendler et al., 2003), but more recent genome-wide association studies indicate a multitude of common and distinct genetic factors (Crist, Reiner, & Berrettini, 2019; Hancock et al., 2018). A major focus of current addiction studies is therefore to discover potential genetic and neurobiological factors that moderate individual SUD risk. Multiple heritable phenotypes have been shown to be predictive for heightened likelihood to seek out and use drugs in both humans and laboratory animals, including novelty preference and seeking (Belin et al., 2011; Molander et al., 2011; Wingo et al., 2015), locomotor response to novelty (Nadal, Armario, & Janak, 2002; Piazza & Deroche-Gamonet, 2013), anxiety-related behaviors (Giles, Turk, & Fresco, 2006; Spanagel et al., 1995; Stathopoulou et al., 2021), altered stress responses (Enoch, 2010; Kreek & Koob, 1998; Sinha, 2008), circadian phenotypes (Logan, Williams, & McClung, 2014; Rosenwasser, 2010), and impulsivity. These behaviors may have overlapping neurogenetic components, and by studying them in tandem, common and unique genetic factors may be identified and investigated for their relationship to SUD traits.

Impulsivity is the trait-like proclivity to engage in excessive, uncontrolled, or rash reward pursuit and consumption (Dalley, Everitt, & Robbins, 2011; Evenden, 1999; Jentsch et al., 2014). These behaviors are considered pathological when they are intrusive, disrupt normal life routines, cause clinical distress, or lead to harmful outcomes (Moeller et al., 2001). Impulsivity is furthermore identified as having multiple dimensions, each of which can be separately measured and may have a unique relationship to addiction vulnerability. This is exemplified in the widely used Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11), which measures self-reported cognitive, motor, and non-planning impulsiveness (Barratt, 1959). Behavioral tasks have also been developed to quantify impulsive phenotypes in humans, such as the Go/No-Go task, delay discounting, reversal learning, Five-Choice Serial Reaction Time Task (5-CSRTT), and stop signal reaction time task, which have analogs for use in animal models. Research on animal models exposed a unique relationship between types of impulsivity and various pharmacological interventions, in that drug treatment did not uniformly increase, decrease, or maintain different impulsive phenotypes (Evenden, 1998; Evenden, 1999; Winstanley et al., 2004). No significant bivariate correlation between impulsive action (five-choice serial reaction time task) and impulsive choice (delay discounting) has been found in either lab rats or humans; and furthermore, three factors reflecting statistically orthogonal measures of impulsivity were identified in human subjects: self-report, impulsive action, and impulsive choice (Broos et al., 2012; MacKillop et al., 2016). These findings may very likely be attributed to different underlying neurogenetic mechanisms regulating each type of impulsivity.

Delay discounting, a common test of impulsive choice, was initially created to assess rats and pigeons (Evenden & Ryan, 1996), though it is now used in both human and animal subjects with variations to the methodology. Delay discounting is a paradigm established to assess an individual’s tendency to reduce (discount) the subjective value of a reward if it must wait to receive it. The delay discounting procedure aims to establish how the subject therefore discounts the delayed reward, either by altering the volume of reward or the length of delay. A fundamental aspect of delay discounting is that the subject makes an action to choose one of two options and then must wait for the consequence, thus differing from other similar procedures such as the differential reinforcement task (Evenden, 1999), and measuring impulsive choice (frequent selection of the small immediate lever) versus self-control (frequent selection of the large, delayed option) (Odum, 2013).

Reversal learning, a measure hypothetically linked with impulsive action, revolves around changing reinforcement contingencies: one of an array of actions (e.g. pressing the left most lever) is paired with an outcome (e.g. receiving a food reward), and the subject learns to discriminate between those contingencies (Izquierdo & Jentsch, 2011). After reaching an accuracy criterion, the contingencies are reversed (e.g., only pressing the right lever, not the left, now leads to the food reward). Subjects must demonstrate cognitive and behavioral flexibility by constraining their previous responses and discarding the initially learned rule in order to maximize reward. Greater difficulty with stopping or updating behavior during reversal learning has been suggested to reflect greater impulsive action. Studies have shown that this behavioral inflexibility is genetically linked to impulsivity (Franken et al., 2008; Izquierdo & Jentsch, 2011). In addition to responding after reversal of response contingency, premature (inter-trial interval) responding within reversal learning can be measured. This measure is analogous to premature responding in the 5-CSRTT. Both measures of anticipatory responses are considered to be waiting impulsivity, or the inability to withhold response in anticipation of a reward-related cue (Dalley, Everitt, & Robbins, 2011).

Inbred mouse panels permit estimates of heritability of these traits, which is operationally defined as the proportion of phenotypic variation that is explained by genetic variation. In inbred lines, heritability is estimated as the proportion of phenotypic variance accounted for by strain. Past genetic reference population studies have considered this to be an effective estimate of heritability, considering each mouse from each strain is, to the extent maximally possible, genetically identical to one another (Hegmann & Possidente, 198; Philip et al., 2009). Environmental and technical sources of variance are reduced within these panels, further increasing the ability to detect heritability of a trait over external influence and providing an advantage over human twin studies (Williams et al., 2004).

Past efforts have utilized recombinant inbred (RI) panels to identify the heritability and genetic architecture of impulsive phenotypes. Laughin et al. (2011) conducted reversal learning in 51 BXD mouse strains and identified heritable strain variation in impulsivity, as well as a genome-wide significant quantitative trait locus (QTL) on chromosome 10. Positional candidate genes, including Syn3 – encoding the synaptic phosphoprotein Synapsin III - expressed from this QTL were selected based upon expression phenotypes that were genetically correlated with the behavioral phenotype. A subsequent study using the BXD strains that exhibited extreme differences in reversal learning found that the poor reversal learning strains more rapidly acquired cocaine self-administration and administer cocaine at greater rates than do good reversal learning strains (Cervantes, Laughlin & Jentsch, 2013).

Utilizing mouse populations with greater genetic diversity may expand observed phenotypic ranges and lead to new insights in neurogenetics and neurobiology of impulsivity and its relationship to SUDs. The Collaborative Cross is a genetically diverse, multi-parental RI panel. It alleviates genetic bottleneck limitations through the intercross of five classical inbred strains (A/J, C57BL/6J, 129S1/SvImJ, NOD/LtJ, NZO/HlLtJ), and three wild-derived inbred strains (CAST/EiJ, PWK/PhJ, WSB/EiJ), to capture >90% of the known genetic variation present in the laboratory mouse (Odet et al., 2015). In addition to having a large amount of genetic diversity, the Collaborative Cross also has generally balanced allele frequencies and evenly distributed recombination sites (Aylor et al., 2011; Philip et al., 2011), resulting in greater statistical power to detect genetic correlations among phenotypes gathered in different laboratories.

The present study aims to examine heritability and genetic correlations between locomotor response to novelty, palatable food consumption (a measure of reward sensitivity), reinforcement learning, two tests of impulsivity (delay discounting and reversal learning), and other catalogued addiction-related behaviors measured by others in the eight CC founder strains and ten CC strains. The tests of impulsivity are designed to measure three types of impulsivity: impulsive action (total trials to criteria in reversal learning), waiting impulsivity (anticipatory responses measured during reversal learning) and impulsive choice (indices of preference for an immediate reward). We hypothesized that the three measures of impulsivity are heritable traits, though not necessarily coinherited (genetically correlated). Locomotor activity and palatable food consumption are similarly anticipated to be heritable traits, each of which has shown conflicting evidence in the literature of being positively correlated with impulsivity (Isles et al., 2004; Anastasio et al., 2019; Velázquez-Sánchez et al., 2014). These studies were conducted as part of a multi-lab collaboration in the Center for Systems Neurogenetics of Addiction. A set of strains were tested across different laboratories for impulsivity traits as well as SUD-related traits. Results from these efforts will provide insights into the inter-lab replicability of impulsivity traits as well as the genetic relationships among a large number of traits that may be predictive of SUDs.

Material and Methods

Subjects

All inbred and recombinant inbred mice involved in this study were born at The Jackson Laboratory (JAX; Bar Harbor ME). A subset was subsequently shipped to Binghamton University, while the remainder were phenotyped at JAX, in the facilities of the Behavioral Phenotyping Core of the Center for Systems Neurogenetics of Addiction. All procedures were performed according to the “Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals” (National Research Council, 2011) in the AAALAC accredited programs at Binghamton University or JAX, after approval by the relevant Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees.

Binghamton University Site

Subjects were shipped from JAX to Binghamton University between 35–49 days of age. A total of 6 cohorts were shipped between 8/2016 and 8/2019. Mice from the eight founder strains were delivered in the first three cohorts, each of which was balanced to include representatives from all 8 strains and both sexes. Ten strains of CC mice were also studied; they were first included in cohort three. Details on mouse strains/cohorts are provided in Table 1. A total of 286 mice were tested at the Binghamton site.

Table 1.

Strains tested at the Binghamton University site.

Mouse ID JAX Stock No. MGI ID N Cohorts
NOD/ShiLtJ 001976 2162056 18 1–3
129S1/SvImJ 002448 2160041 18 1–3
A/J 000646 2159747 18 1–3
C57BL/6J 000664 3028467 18 1–3
NZO/HlLtJ 002105 2668669 18 1–3
CAST/EiJ 000928 2159793 18 1–3
WSB/EiJ 001145 2160667 18 1–3
PWK/PhJ 003715 2160654 18 1–3
CC002/UncJ 021236 5649080 12 4–6
CC004/TauUncJ 020944 5649082 23 3–6
CC006/TauUncJ 022869 5649237 12 4–6
CC011/UncJ 018854 5649240 12 4–6
CC012/GeniUncJ 028409 5694080 12 4–6
CC025/GeniUncJ 018857 5649246 11 4–6
CC028/GeniUncJ 025126 5659485 12 4–6
CC032/GeniUncJ 020946 5649248 12 4–6
CC041/TauUncJ 021893 5649251 24 3–6
CC061/GeniUncJ 023826 5649258 12 4–6

Upon arrival, mice were housed in the same groups in which they were shipped, with three mice of the same strain and sex being grouped together in a cage. The colony room was maintained on a 12h light/dark cycle (lights on at 0615 h) and at an average temperature of 69–70°F. During this initial acclimation period, food (Lab Diet 5001, ScottPharma Solutions) and water was available ad libitum. A nestlet and a translucent red acrylic tube (9.75cm long, 5cm diameter, approximately 65g) were placed in each cage. For removal from the cage, the mice were briefly handled by their tails using a gloved hand. Wild-derived inbred strains and some CC strains (CC004/TauUncJ and CC011/UncJ) were handled by the tail with forceps.

All three wild-derived strains (CAST/EiJ, WSB/EiJ, and PWK/PhJ) exhibit high levels of aggression and are at high risk of injury when group-housed in adulthood. To avoid injury, all mice of both sexes and of all strains, including the wild and non-wild derived lines, were singly housed in identical caging conditions starting at PND 60. Thus, all animals tested in this study experienced a uniform environment, and single-housing was only instituted in adulthood. Mice were acclimated to these conditions for 10 days until PND 70.

Prior to the initiation of the operant conditioning protocols described below, mice were introduced to a schedule of limited access to chow. Mice were weighed daily during food restriction and percent of free-feeding body weight was calculated by dividing the current weight by the free-feeding weight. During the limited access to food period, mice were fed once a day between 3pm and 5pm; chow quantity provided per day was titrated until mice reach 80–85% or 83–88% (CAST/EiJ, PWK/PhJ, WSB/EiJ) of their free feeding weights. Wild-derived inbred strains were maintained at a higher free-feeding percentage due to observed lethargy and dehydration when their weights approached 80%. Once mice reached their target weights, operant testing began (see Table 2 for reversal learning mouse weights; Table 3 for delay discounting mouse weights). If, at any point during the testing period, a mouse dropped below 80% of its free feeding weight, the quantity of chow provided was increased. If increased food availability did not lead to a recovery of body weight to ≥ 80% within a day, it was temporarily returned to ad libitum food access until its weight had recovered.

Table 2.

Average of strain’s body weight during each stage of reversal learning testing, represented as a percentage of their initial free-feeding weight. SEM is represented as ± the mean.

Mouse ID Start Training Acquisition Reversal
NOD/ShiLtJ 27.2 ± 0.99g 85.8 ± 0.76% 84.2 ± 0.71% 85.8 ± 0.74%
129S1/SvImJ 23.7 ± 0.64g 88.0 ± 1.04% 86.6 ± 1.09% 85.6 ± 1.02%
A/J 21.8 ± 0.61g 87.8 ± 0.62% 84.7 ± 0.65% 84.8 ± 0.52%
C57BL/6J 23.6 ± 0.51g 85.6 ± 0.47% 84.7 ± 0.61% 85.1 ± 0.54%
NZO/HlLtJ 41.0 ± 1.24g 88.4 ± 1.30% 83.7 ± 0.92% 83.6 ± 0.64%
CAST/EiJ 15.5 ± 0.38g 86.0 ± 0.92% 85.3 ± 0.86% 84.4 ± 0.65%
WSB/EiJ 15.7 ± 0.40g 86.7 ± 0.92% 86.3 ± 0.77% 87.6 ± 0.74%
PWK/PhJ 17.2 ± 0.40g 85.5 ± 0.43% 85.7 ± 0.67% 85.1 ± 0.61%
CC001/UncJ 24.3 ± 0.90g 84.1 ± 0.94% 83.1 ± 0.88% 82.8 ± 0.92%
CC002/UncJ 24.2 ± 2.07g 86.7 ± 0.66% 85.5 ± 0.56% 84.8 ± 0.86%
CC003//UncJ 27.7 ± 0.59g 84.0 ± 1.04% 84.8 ± 0.98% 84.9 ± 1.15%
CC004/TauUncJ 28.6 ± 1.44g 86.8 ± 1.76% 84.7 ± 0.94% 85.6 ± 1.45%
CC005/TauUncJ 19.9 ± 0.92g 87.1 ± 0.89% 85.8 ± 0.86% 85.9 ± 0.57%
CC006/TauUncJ 23.8 ± 0.98g 86.0 ± 1.04% 87.0 ± 0.71% 86.9 ± 0.57%
CC008/GeniUncJ 32.4 ± 0.73g 84.7 ± 1.01% 86.5 ± 0.86% 85.7 ± 0.85%
CC011/UncJ 25.7 ± 1.01g 87.6 ± 0.88% 85.5 ± 1.09% 85.4 ± 0.73%
CC012/GeniUncJ 20.8 ± 1.38g 87.3 ± 1.21% 86.6 ± 1.17% 87.9 ± 0.49%
CC013/GeniUncJ 27.2 ± 1.01g 83.8 ±0.88% 86.0 ± 0.98% 86.8 ± 0.77%
CC015/UncJ 21.0 ± 1.36g 86.1 ± 0.78% 83.5 ± 0.77% 84.8 ± 0.90%
CC016/GeniUncJ 24.6 ± 1.87g 85.0 ± 0.33% 83.2 ± 0.49% 82.7 ± 0.36%
CC017/UncJ 21.0 ± .59g 85.3 ± 0.67% 83.9 ± 0.93% 84.4 ± 0.90%
CC019/TauUncJ 19.1 ± 2.91g 86.3 ± 0.27% 84.7 ± 2.25% 83.8 ± 2.17%
CC023/GeniUncJ 22.5 ± 1.23g 84.4 ± 85.1% 85.1 ± 2.48% 82.2 ± 1.59%
CC025/GeniUncJ 23.6 ± 1.43g 86.1 ± 0.32% 83.1 ± 1.83% 83.1 ± 0.50%
CC026/GeniUncJ 25.3 ± 1.79g 84.8 ± 0.50% 84.5 ± 0.83% 82.9 ± 0.65%
CC027/GeniUncJ 24.4 ± 1.40g 86.9 ± 0.50% 85.1 ± 1.11% 83.7 ± 1.13%
CC028/GeniUncJ 29.4 ± 2.03g 86.3 ± 0.57% 84.5 ± 0.27% 83.8 ± 1.18%
CC030/GeniUncJ 23.9 ± 1.23g 86.4 ± 0.50% 85.2 ± 0.20% 85.7 ± 0.44%
CC032/GeniUncJ 25.7 ± 1.60g 90.7 ± 1.55% 86.0 ± 0.75% 86.0 ± 0.00%
CC033/GeniUncJ 21.7 ± 1.77g 82.8 ± 0.59% 85.4 ± 1.27% 84.7 ± 0.86%
CC036/UncJ 25.7 ± 1.75g 85.4 ± 0.66% 85.2 ± 1.87% 84.5 ± 1.57%
CC037/TauUncJ 22.8 ± 0.80g 84.7 ± 2.15% 84.8 ± 0.15% 85.3 ± 0.90%
CC040/TauUncJ 32.9 ± 3.60g 86.1 ± 0.10% 81.7 ± 1.35% 79.4 ± 0.25%
CC041/TauUncJ 24.9 ± 1.42g 88.1 ± 0.84% 85.3 ± 0.92% 85.8 ± 0.13%
CC043/GeniUncJ 20.1 ± 0.00g 84.5 ± 1.05% 84.4 ± 2.85% 86.1 ± 2.45%
CC051/TauUncJ 32.9 ± 3.27g 85.2 ± 0.49% 83.5 ± 0.88% 82.9 ± 0.32%
CC057/UncJ 28.3 ± 2.25g 83.0 ± 0.70% 82.4 ± 0.05% 84.0 ± 0.30%
CC059/TauUncJ 35.1 ± 2.60g 83.9 ± 1.00% 82.0 ± 0.15% 82.6 ± 0.50%
CC061/GeniUncJ 21.8 ± 0.00g 87.4 ± 0.00% 85.1 ± 0.00% 87.2 ± 0.00%
CC075/UncJ 25.9 ± 1.70g 85.8 ± 0.15% 81.4 ± 0.20% 81.7 ± 0.20%
CC078/TauUncJ 25.7 ± 2.05g 87.7 ± 1.25% 87.7 ± 3.00% 86.1 ± 1.25%
CC079/TauUncJ 21.2 ± 3.70g 85.3 ± 0.60% 83.8 ± 0.75% 84.2 ± 0.40%
CC080/TauUncJ 20.1 ± 5.05g 84.0 ± 1.05% 86.5 ± 0.05% 84.0 ± 0.10%
CC084/TauJ 28.7 ± 2.55g 83.7 ± 0.15% 83.0 ± 0.65% 83.7 ± 0.60%
Table 3.

Average of strain’s body weight during each stage of delay discounting testing, represented as a percentage of their initial starting weight (grams). No significant differences between strains were observed. SEM is represented as ± the mean.

Mouse ID Start 0s 3s 6s 9s
NOD/ShiLtJ 27.4 ± 1.98g 89.0 ± 1.13% 89.3 ± 1.10% 88.7 ± 0.75% 88.9 ± 1.07%
129S1/SvImJ 22.5 ± 0.78g 89.8 ± 1.42% 87.7 ± 1.17% 90.1 ± 1.39% 89.8 ± 1.61%
A/J 21.7 ± 0.54g 88.9 ± 1.32% 90.8 ± 1.19% 91.3 ± 0.61% 91.0 ± 0.96%
C57BL/6J 24.2 ± 1.20g 90.2 ± 1.23% 85.5 ± 0.86% 89.5 ± 1.32% 88.7 ± 0.95%
NZO/H1LtJ 39.7 ± 1.15g 88.6 ± 1.07% 89.2 ± 1.16% 89.8 ± 0.89% 87.8 ± 0.95%
CAST/EiJ 15.5 ± 0.56g 90.0 ± 1.32% 89.8 ± 0.77% 91.5 ± 1.06% 90.5 ± 0.88%
WSB/EiJ 17.2 ± 0.58g 89.1 ± 1.15% 89.3 ± 1.13% 88.2 ± 1.04% 90.3 ± 1.35%
PWK/PhJ 16.0 ± 0.57g 91.4 ± 0.94% 86.8 ± 0.75% 89.7 ± 0.79% 88.2 ± 1.31%
CC002/UncJ 26.0 ± 0.79g 87.8 ± 0.85% 90.5 ± 1.25% 88.2 ± 1.26% 88.5 ± 0.93%
CC004/TauUncJ 26.9 ± 1.14g 87.8 ± 0.96% 87.6 ± 1.03% 88.3 ± 1.26% 87.8 ± 1.23%
CC006/TauUncJ 24.0 ± 0.77g 88.7 ± 1.65% 90.7 ± 1.51% 90.0 ± 0.76% 87.0 ± 1.41%
CC011/UncJ 27.7 ± 1.55g 88.7 ± 1.05% 89.7 ± 1.88% 88.5 ± 1.87% 88.5 ± 1.83%
CC012/GeniUncJ 21.0 ± 1.05g 90.5 ± 1.19% 89.0 ± 1.75% 91.5 ± 0.98% 87.5 ± 1.38%
CC025/GeniUncJ 25.1 ± 1.13g 88.4 ± 1.66% 87.0 ± 2.17% 92.4 ± 1.63% 88.6 ± 1.44%
CC028/GeniUncJ 27.5 ± 1.20g 89.7 ± 1.33% 88.0 ± 1.82% 89.7 ± 2.35% 85.3 ± 0.91%
CC032/GeniUncJ 21.0 ± 0.39g 92.3 ± 0.99% 88.0 ± 2.01% 88.8 ± 1.29% 86.8 ± 1.37%
CC041/TauUncJ 25.9 ± 1.09g 86.4 ± 2.01% 89.1 ± 1.37% 87.5 ± 0.95% 89.7 ± 1.09%
CC061/GeniUncJ 21.2 ± 0.93g 92.2 ± 1.69% 87.8 ± 1.05% 88.5 ± 1.34% 90.2 ± 1.54%

The JAX Site

With the following exceptions, maintenance and feeding of the mice at JAX followed the same details outlined above. The eight CC founder strains were bred and maintained within the Behavioral Phenotyping Core of the CSNA at JAX; all CC strains were born in the commercial Production facility at JAX and were relocated to the Behavioral Phenotyping Core at four to six weeks of age. Mouse home cages contained a disposable dome-shaped shack (Shepherd Specialty Papers, Inc., Watertown, TN, USA), instead of a red tube. A total of eight founder strains and thirty-six CC strains were studied (Table 4), with thirty-six CC strains tested at the JAX site, and ten of those strains tested simultaneously at the Binghamton site. A total of 272 mice were tested at the JAX site.

Table 4.

Strains tested at the Jackson Laboratory site.

Mouse ID JAX Stock No. MGI ID N Source
NOD/ShiLtJ 001976 2162056 18 Bred in-house
129S1/SvImJ 002448 2160041 7 Bred in-house
A/J 000646 2159747 10 Bred in-house
C57BL/6J 000664 3028467 27 Bred in-house
NZO/HlLtJ 002105 2668669 7 Bred in-house
CAST/EiJ 000928 2159793 19 Bred in-house
WSB/EiJ 001145 2160667 15 Bred in-house
PWK/PhJ 003715 2160654 17 Bred in-house
CC001/UncJ 021238 5649079 4 Shipped
CC002/UncJ 021236 5649080 6 Shipped
CC003//UncJ 021237 5649081 4 Shipped
CC004/TauUncJ 020944 5649082 3 Shipped
CC005/TauUncJ 020945 5649083 4 Shipped
CC006/TauUncJ 022869 5649237 4 Shipped
CC008/GeniUncJ 026971 5659476 3 Shipped
CC011/UncJ 018854 5649240 6 Shipped
CC012/GeniUncJ 028409 5694080 6 Shipped
CC013/GeniUncJ 021892 5649241 4 Shipped
CC015/UncJ 018859 5659478 2 Shipped
CC016/GeniUncJ 021237 5659479 8 Shipped
CC017/UncJ 022870 5649242 4 Shipped
CC019/TauUncJ 021894 5649244 10 Shipped
CC023/GeniUncJ 025131 5659483 1 Shipped
CC025/GeniUncJ 018857 5649246 8 Shipped
CC026/GeniUncJ 024685 5649247 2 Shipped
CC027/GeniUncJ 025130 5659484 5 Shipped
CC028/GeniUncJ 025126 5659485 7 Shipped
CC030/GeniUncJ 025426 5659487 7 Shipped
CC032/GeniUncJ 020946 5649248 2 Shipped
CC033/GeniUncJ 025910 5659488 2 Shipped
CC036/UncJ 025127 5659490 6 Shipped
CC037/TauUncJ 025423 5649249 3 Shipped
CC040/TauUncJ 023831 5649250 3 Shipped
CC041/TauUncJ 021893 5649251 3 Shipped
CC043/GeniUncJ 023828 5649256 2 Shipped
CC051/TauUncJ 021897 5649257 2 Shipped
CC057/UncJ 024683 5659646 2 Shipped
CC059/TauUncJ 025125 5659650 10 Shipped
CC061/GeniUncJ 023826 5649258 6 Shipped
CC075/UncJ 027293 5659668 3 Shipped
CC078/TauUncJ 025989 5796475 2 Shipped
CC079/TauUncJ 025990 5796476 2 Shipped
CC080/TauUncJ 025988 5796471 3 Shipped
CC084/TauJ 028923 6159255 3 Shipped

Phenotyping Protocols

The Binghamton University cohort was sequentially phenotyped using a set of protocols described below. They were evaluated first for Locomotor Response to Novelty and Habituation (PND 70–71), followed by testing for Palatable Food Consumption (PND 72–78) and then one of two operant conditioning procedures: either Reversal Learning or Delay Discounting. The cohort evaluated at JAX were sequentially phenotyped in open field, light/dark, hole board, and novelty place preference assays prior to Reversal Learning. At both sites, behavioral tests were conducted during the light phase of the animal’s light cycle.

Locomotor Response to Novelty and Habituation (Binghamton University pipeline)

At PND 70, mice were assessed for locomotor response in a novel open field environment. Subjects were transported to a testing room on a cart. Each mouse was individually placed in a 17 “ L × 17” W × 12” H (43.2 × 43.2 × 30.5 cm) open field chamber fitted with infrared beams (Med-Associates MED-OFAS-RSU; St Albans VT). All open field chambers were within sound attenuating cubicles measuring 26” W × 22” H × 20.5” D (66 × 52.7 × 55.9 cm) at the interior with walls 0.75” (1.9 cm) thick. Activity was recorded for 40-min. The primary dependent measure examined was total distance traveled in cm, as well as the duration of time (in s) spent: ambulating, resting, performing stereotypy, or rearing. After the session, mice were immediately removed and returned to their home cage. Between subjects, the apparati were cleaned with a mixture of 10% Alconox detergent in water.

The following day at the same time, mice were placed back in the same open field chamber, and activity parameters were again recorded for 40-min. Locomotor habituation was assessed by calculating the difference in total distance traveled between day 1 and 2.

Palatable Food Consumption

Beginning on PND 72 (one day after the conclusion of locomotor assessments), mice experienced access to a highly palatable chocolate-flavored Boost solution (Nestle) in their home cage. Boost was made available using a plastic petri dish that was placed on top of the bedding. The solution was available continuously for a 48-h period, with the solution being refreshed at the 24-h time point.

From PND 74 to 80, mice were evaluated daily for Boost (and water) consumption in 2-h lickometry sessions. All testing took place inside dual lickometer Scurry boxes (Model 80822S, activity wheel removed; Lafayette Instruments, Lafayette IN). Each lickometer box is 35.3 × 23.5 × 20c m and is fitted with a food hopper and two 50 mL sipper bottles, with pine chip bedding on the chamber floor. Mice were transported to the testing room on a cart. Room lights were on during testing and a room dehumidifier provided ambient background noise. Mice were placed, individually, into the lickometer boxes and allowed to freely consume Boost and water (no chow provided). One bottle was filled with Boost solution and the other was filled with water; the position (left or right) of the two solutions relative to one another was counterbalanced pseudorandomly across the testing days. Licks on each spout were counted by a computer. The number of licks was divided by the body weight of the animal to account for behavioral variability that might be attributable to body weight differences.

Operant Conditioning – Reversal Learning

Following Boost consumption testing, mice were maintained on food-restriction and transitioned to operant testing when a stable target weight was achieved. Half of the mice from each sex and each strain were randomly designated for evaluation using an operant discrimination/reversal learning procedure. All operant testing took place in 8.5” L × 7” W × 5” H (21.6 × 17.8 × 12.7 cm) operant modular chambers (Model ENV-307W, Med Associates Inc.) that were fitted with stainless-steel grid floors (Model ENV-307W-GFW, Med Associates Inc.) and located in sound attenuating cubicles. The operant box contains a horizontal array of five nose poke apertures on one side of the box, and a central food magazine, flanked on the left and right by two retractable, ultrasensitive levers on the other. A house light and white noise maker are positioned within the cubicle above the operant box. Mice were removed from their home cage by their tail and placed inside the operant box. To remove the CAST/EiJ, PWK/PhJ, and WSB/EiJ wild-derived strains and the CC004/TauUncJ and CC011/UncJ from the operant chamber, the red tube from their home cage was placed into the box; once the mouse entered the tube, it was transferred into their home cage.

Each mouse was sequentially tested in a series of programs; mice transitioned from program to program individually, as they met criterion performance (see below). Mice underwent the following programs:

  • Stage 1: Box habituation. House light and white noise were active. No reinforcements were provided. Box habituation comprised of one session that lasted 1-h.

  • Stage 2: Magazine training. House light and white noise were active for the duration of the test. During this test, 20–21μl Boost was dispensed into the food magazine every 30 s. The session ended after 1-h or after the mouse received and retrieved 50 rewards, whichever came first. A mouse progressed to Stage 3 when it earned 30 or more rewards within a session.

  • Stage 3: Initial operant (nose-poke) conditioning. Sessions began with illumination of the house light and activation of the white noise generator; 10-s later, nose poke aperture 3 of 5 (center aperture) was illuminated. A behavioral response that broke the photocell in the aperture (usually, a nose poke) resulted in the extinction of the internal light; in addition, if the beam was broken for a continuous pre-set period of at least 0 (beam break with no additional hold time), 100, or 200 ms (the time requirement varied randomly from trial to trial), the action was reinforced by the delivery of 20–21μl of Boost solution; after each reinforcer was retrieved, a new trial was initiated 1.5-s later (signaled by illumination of the center nose poke aperture). If a response was initiated but was not sustained for the preset period, a time out period of 2-s occurred, during which time the central nose poke light and house light were extinguished. If a mouse did not voluntarily respond in the center hole for at least 15 minutes, the center hole was baited with a Boost-saturated cotton swab. Daily sessions lasted up to 1-h but were also terminated if an individual mouse earned 50 reinforcers. Each mouse was tested daily on this stage until it received at least 50 reinforcers in a single session, at which time it progressed to the next stage.

  • Stage 4: Mice were tested under the same basic conditions outlined in Stage 3, except that a minimum duration nose poke of 100- or 200-ms was required to produce reinforcement. If a mouse had not responded in the central illuminated hole for 15 minutes, the center hole was again baited with a Boost-saturated cotton swab. When the mouse earned 50 reinforcers in a single session, it progressed to Stage 5. If the mouse had not met criteria after 10 days, it was regressed to Stage 3. If the subject returned to Stage 4 but still did not meet criteria after another 10 test days, they were removed from the study due to failure to progress. Across all Stages, a mouse could only regress once. For example, if a mouse did not pass Stage 4 in 10 days and regressed to Stage 3, then later did not pass Stage 5 within 10 days, the mouse was removed from the study.

  • Stage 5: In this phase, mice were tested under the same basic conditions as outlined in Stages 3 and 4, except that a minimum duration nose poke of 100-, 200-, or 300-ms was required to trigger reinforcement delivery. If a mouse did not respond in the center illuminated hole for 15 minutes, the center hole was baited with a Boost-saturated cotton swab. When the mouse earned 50 reinforcers in a single session, it progressed to the Discrimination Learning stage. If the mouse did not meet passing criteria after 10 days, they regressed to Stage 4. If the subject returned to Stage 5 but still did not meet criteria after another second of 10 test days, they were removed from the study due to regression failure.

  • Stage 6: Discrimination learning. As above, session onset was signaled by illumination of the house light and activation of the white noise generator; trial onset was signaled by illumination of the center nose poke aperture. As in Stage 5, mice were required to first complete an observing response into the central aperture of 100-, or 200-ms duration; any nosepokes into the target (flanking) holes before completing the observing response and successfully initiating a trial were counted as premature/anticipatory responses. Once a trial was successfully initiated with an observing response, the two apertures flanking the central hole (hole 2 and 4) were immediately illuminated. A response into one of the two apertures (pseudorandomly assigned across strains) resulted in the delivery of a Boost reinforcer (this was counted as a correct choice). Poking into the other hole - or not making any response within 30-s, triggered a time out, during which time the house light was extinguished; these outcomes were counted as an incorrect choice or an omission, respectively. Daily sessions of 1-hr were conducted until learning criteria were met; these criteria included a mouse completing at least 20 trials in a single session and achieving at least 80% accuracy over a running window including the last 20 trials. A mouse is regressed to Stage 5 if it does not complete at least 10 trials for three consecutive days. If 300 trials are conducted without meeting passing criteria, the mouse is removed from the study due to Stage 6 failure.

  • Stage 7: Reversal learning stage. Testing was nearly identical to that described above in Stage 6, with the exception that the reinforcement contingencies associated with the two holes were switched. Testing progressed in daily sessions until animals once again met the same learning criteria rule described above, and the same dependent variables were collected (see below). After reversal was completed, mice were gradually adjusted back onto an ad libitum feeding schedule. Subjects were removed from the study due to Stage 7 failure if 400 trials were conducted without meeting criteria or if 8 weeks of testing passed.

Key dependent variables for the discrimination learning and reversal learning stages were: total trials required to reach criteria (TTC) in each stage and premature responding. TTC was calculated as the total number of completed trials (all trials ending in an incorrect or correct response) until it met the performance criteria. The difference in TTC in the reversal stage to TTC in the discrimination learning stage demonstrates each animal’s ability to alter responding under a changing reward contingency, with a non-zero, positive difference score indicating difficulty with altering its behavior and/or inhibiting the initially trained response.

Premature responses are nose pokes into one of the flanking target holes before a trial is successfully initiated, a measure roughly analogous to that collected in the 5-choice serial reaction time (Bari et al. 2008). Premature responses were separately counted for the correct and incorrect target aperture. Premature responding thus has four values: premature responding in the correct hole at acquisition, premature responding in the incorrect hole at acquisition, premature responding in the correct hole at reversal, and premature responding in the incorrect hole at reversal. All premature responding values were further divided by the animal’s TTC in that stage to estimate the average number of premature responses made per trial. Of particular interest is premature responding in the correct hole during acquisition and in the incorrect hole at reversal, as these are the dominant types of responses made.

Other variables measured were the frequency of omissions (total omissions/TTC for each stage); average correct trials (total correct trials/TTC; average trial initiation latency (total trial initiation latency/TTC), which is the average amount of time that passes between the end of one trial and the successful initiation of the next one; and average reward retrieval time (total reward retrieval time/total correct trials), which is the average amount of time that passes between a reward being administered and the animal’s head entering the magazine.

Operant Conditioning – Delay Discounting

As described above, mice were transitioned to a limited food access schedule, once lickometer testing was completed. Once targeted reductions in body weights were achieved, half of the mice from each sex and strain were randomly designated for evaluation using a delay discounting procedure.

Each mouse was sequentially tested in a series of training stages through which they transitioned individually, as they met criterion performance (see below). Mice underwent the following programs:

  • Stage 1: Box habituation. House light and white noise were active. No reinforcements were provided. Box habituation comprised of one session that lasted 1-h.

  • Stage 2: Magazine training. Again, the house light and white noise were active for the duration of the test, and 20–21μl Boost is dispensed every 30 s. The duration of testing was 1 h. A mouse progressed to Stage 3 when it earned 30 or more rewards within a session.

  • Stage 3: Initial operant (lever press) conditioning. Session onset was signaled by illumination of the house light and activation of the white noise generator. On each trial, one lever (left or right) was inserted to the chamber and actuation of the lever by the mouse triggered delivery of 20–21 μl of Boost on a fixed-ratio 1 schedule of reinforcement. Across trials, the lever that was inserted (left or right) was pseudorandomly varied, such that each mouse actuated each lever a roughly equal number of times. Each daily session ended after 1-h or after 60 reinforcers were obtained, whichever came first. A mouse progressed to Stage 4 when it earned 60 reinforcers within a session.

  • Stage 4: In this stage of lever press training, a procedure nearly identical to that used in Stage 3, described above, was employed. The only difference was that responses on the inserted lever were reinforced on a fixed ratio 3 schedule of reinforcement. The program ended after 1-h or after 60 rewards were obtained. A mouse progressed to Stage 5 when it earned 60 reinforcers within a session.

  • Stage 5: Observing response training. The only difference from Stage 4 was that the mouse was required to complete an observing response (nose poke response into the central nose-poke hole [aperture 3 of 5] on the side of the chamber opposite to the levers in order to trigger insertion of a lever). Responses on that lever were still reinforced on a fixed-ratio 3 schedule. The program ended after 1-h or after 60 rewards were obtained. A mouse progressed to Stage 6 when it earned 60 reinforcers within a session.

  • Stage 6: In this stage of training, a procedure nearly identical to that described in Stage 5 was used. The only difference was that the program ended after 1.5 hours, or after 80 rewards were obtained, whichever came first. A mouse progressed to Side bias determination when it earned 80 reinforcers within a session.

  • Stage 7: Side bias determination. Trials began with presentation of both levers; responding on either lever was reinforced by 20–21 μl of Boost on a fixed-ratio 3 schedule. After a 10-s inter-trial interval, both levers are again presented, but only responses on the alternate lever were rewarded. A trial was only counted if the mouse successfully pressed the alternate lever. The program ended after 40 trials, or after 1.5 hours. The lever (right or left) on which each trial was initiated was recorded. The fraction of trials initiated with left versus right responses was determined, and the lever with the highest fraction was selected to be the one associated with the large-delayed outcome during the subsequent delay discounting testing. After a single session, a mouse progressed to Delay discounting assessment.

  • Stage 8: Delay discounting assessment. These daily tests began with activation of the house light and white noise generator. Individual trials commenced with activation of the light in aperture 3 of 5. A nose poke into the aperture triggered presentation of both levers. Completion of the FR3 schedule on one lever led to a small (10μl) reward delivered immediately (termed the SI option), while completion of the schedule on the other led to a larger (20μl) reward delayed by 0–9 s (termed the LD option). Delays were varied between session; all mice encountered delays of 0, 3, 6 or 9 s. Each delay was encountered for three consecutive days, and the order of delays experienced was controlled by a cycle Latin square design, balanced across strains.

Reward magnitude was adjusted within session. Specifically, the reward volume delivered increased or decreased by 10% depending on the subject’s prior lever choice, such that choosing the SI lever would cause the SI reward to decrease by 10% and the LD to increase by 10% on the next trial, and vice versa if the LD lever is selected. Selecting the same reward lever five times in a row resulted in a forced choice trial, in which only the unchosen lever was ejected, forcing the subject to select that option. Reward amounts were not titrated for the forced trial and forced trials were not included in data analyses.

The final volume delivered on the SI lever was measured and subtracted from the final volume delivered on the LD lever; positive values indicate greater discounting, as the volume of the LD outcome needed to be substantially greater than that of the SI option, due to the delay. The reward magnitude for each option was averaged over the last 30 trials, and those values were averaged across the three days that delay was tested.

Once the first Latin square is completed, mice receive a two-day break, and they then experienced a second Latin square design varying delay across sessions.

Statistical Analyses

Data for each variable was analyzed using SPSS Statistics (Version 23). Data was first examined using a body plot, and outliers two standard deviations from the mean within strain were removed. Normality of residuals was examined using the Shapiro-Wilk test, and data that violated normality was percentile rank transformed prior to statistical analyses (Templeton, 2011). Statistical significance was established as a probability level of p<.05. Independent variables were strain and sex. A total of eighteen strains (eight founders and ten CC) was utilized for all analyses except for reversal learning, which included the eight founders and thirty-six CC strains.

Heritability estimates were derived for each significant main effect of strain using effect size, which is an estimate of the variance accounted for by the independent variable divided by the total amount of variance. The proportion of variance explained by strain reveals the portion of all phenotypic variation that is heritable (Philip et al., 2009).

h2=σ2BetweenStrain/(σ2WithinStrain+σ2BetweenStrain)

Strain-level correlations were conducted only on subjects (8 founder strains and 10 CC strains) at the Binghamton site. A Pearson’s correlation and Spearman’s correlation were used to analyze the relationship between key variables of interest from each behavioral test within this study, as well as key variables from within the study to other phenotypes characterized in these strains. Alpha level was adjusted with a Bonferroni correction to account for multiple comparisons.

Data Availability Statement

All data reported in this manuscript have been deposited into a password-protected area of the Mouse Phenome Database (MPD); requests for access to the data can be sent to the Corresponding Author. Upon acceptance of this manuscript for publication, all the associated data will be made publicly available via MPD.

Results

Strains exhibited heritable variation in free-feeding body weights (Tables 2 and 3); ANOVA revealed an interaction of strain by sex (F[17,310]=1.720, p<.05), along with main effects of strain (F[17,310]=93.421, p<.001) and sex (F[1,310]=152.463, p<.001). Cohort effects were examined for Binghamton site mice and no effect was detected for TTC, premature responding in acquisition or reversal, or the 0s, 3s, and 6s delay discounting measures (p>.05 for all). A cohort effect was found for the 9s delay (F[1,5]=2.628, p<.05), driven by Cohort 2 exhibiting significantly lower 9s difference scores than the other cohorts.

Locomotor Response to Novelty and Habituation

Total distance traveled in the open field chambers was assessed in 2 consecutive daily test sessions (Figure 1). Repeated measures ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of strain (F[17,247]=9.486, p<.001), yielding a heritability estimate of 0.392; this analysis also reported an interaction between day and strain (F[17,24]= 5.124, p<.001, ηp2=.258) and an interaction of strain and sex (F[17,247]=1.956, p<.05, ηp2=.117); there were no other significant interactions involving strain. Main effects of day (F[1,247]=53.667, p<.001, ηp2=.177) and sex (F[1,247]=8.177, p<.01, ηp2=.032) were also observed. Notably in the founder strains, two patterns of response were observed: strains with high ambulatory distance traveled (C57BL/6J, NOD/ShiLtJ, CAST/EiJ, PWK/PhJ, WSB/EiJ) and strains with low ambulatory distance traveled (NZO/HlLtJ, 129S1/SvImJ, and A/J). CC strains, on the other hand, demonstrated more continuous variation in ambulatory distance, a trait previously observed for behavioral wildness in this panel (Philip et al., 2011). Further analysis of the founder strains utilizing pairwise comparisons revealed that the CAST/EiJ, C57BL/6J, NZO/HlLtJ, and WSB/EiJ strains exhibited clear evidence of habituation with significantly less distance traveled on day 2 than on day 1 (all p<0.05), while the 129S1/SvImJ, A/J, PWK/PhJ, and NOD/ShiLtJ strains did not habituate.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Ambulatory distance difference (Day 2-Day 1) calculated by total centimeters in the open field chamber.

Duration of time (s) spent ambulating, resting, performing stereotypy, or rearing was also examined. Results of these analyses (see Table 5) demonstrated consistent main effects of strain (with heritability estimates ranging from 0.566 to 0.832) for all measures. Strain significantly moderated observed habituation in ambulation, stereotypy, and rearing time, as evidenced by day by strain interactions (Table 5). Habituation of ambulation time was observed only in the CAST/EiJ, C57BL/6J, NOD/ShiLtJ, and WSB/EiJ strains; habituation of stereotypy was found in the A/J, CAST/EiJ, C57BL/6J, NOD/ShiLtJ, PWK/PhJ, and WSB/EiJ strains; and habituation of rearing was observed in the A/J, NOD/ShiLtJ, and NZO/HlLtJ strains.

Table 5.

ANOVA statistics for duration of ambulation, resting, stereotypy and rearing (s), as well as ambulation speed (cm/s) during the open field test (40 min). Significant findings are bolded and italicized.

Strain Sex Strain × Sex Strain × Day Strain × Sex × Day
Ambulation time F[17,250]=43.597, p<.001 ηp2=.748 F[1,250]=8.595, p<.01, ηp 2 =.033 F[17,250]=1.877, p<.05, ηp2=.113 F[17,250]=2.555, p<.001 ηp2=.148 F[17,250]=1.321, p>.05
Resting time F[17,250]=73.019, p<.001, ηp 2 =.832 F[1,250]=11.533, p<.001, ηp 2 =.044 F[17,250]=3.659, p<.001, ηp 2 =.199 F[17,250]=.799. p>.05 F[17,250]=.596, p>.05
Stereotypy time F[17,250]=26.7183, p<.001, ηp 2 =.645 F[1,250]=3.629, p=.058 F[17,250]=1.651 4, p=.053 F[17,250]=2.650, p<.001, ηp 2 =.153 F[17,250]=2.263, p<.01, ηp 2 =.133
Rearing time F[17,2]=34.785, p<.001, ηp 2 =.715 F[1,236]=.667, p>.05 F[17,236]=1.307, p>.05 F[17,236]=3.228, p<.001, ηp 2 =.189 F[17,236]=4.372, p<.001, ηp 2 =239
Speed F[17,250]=19.187, p<.001, ηp 2 =.566 F[1,250]=.180, p>.05 F[1,250]=1.111, p>.05 F[17,250]=1.730, p<.05, ηp 2 =.105 F[17,250]=.568, p>.05

Ambulation and resting time phenotypes also revealed interactions between strain and sex (Table 5). Overall, the data reveals a trend for males and females to exhibit similar ambulatory phenotypes except in select strains. This is demonstrated by the PWK/PhJ and CC004/TauUncJ females that spent more time ambulating than did males from these strains, and CAST/EiJ, WSB/EiJ, CC004/TauUncJ, and CC006/UncJ females spent less time resting than males. Notably, the strains that show significant sex differences in exploration include all three of the wild-derived founder strains (CAST/EiJ, WSB/EiJ, and PWK/PhJ).

Palatable Food Consumption

Spout contacts were analyzed within daily, 2-h sessions across 7 consecutive days of testing as well as averaged across the last three days of testing (days 5–7). Unadjusted lick data were analyzed as well as lick data adjusted for the number of licks by each animal’s body weight (Licks/Body weight) to control for the possibility that body size could influence strain difference in consummatory behavior. These adjusted and unadjusted values were compared with a Spearman’s correlation, and a significant positive correlation was detected (rs[16]=.872, p<.001), showing that the rank order of strain differences was not significantly altered by adjusting for body weight; consequently, values adjusted for body weight were used as the primary variable for analyses.

A repeated measures ANOVA on licks across the seven days identified a main effect of strain (F[17,228]=8.709, p<.001; Figure 2) yielding a heritability estimate of 0.434; the analysis also reported an interaction of day by strain (F[6,228]=1.426, p<.01, ηp2=.112). No effect of sex or interactions of sex with day or strain were found. Water consumption was unchanged across the 7 days and showed no effects of strain or sex (p>.05 for all).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Average total number of Boost licks adjusted for body weight (Licks/g body weight) for each strain daily across seven consecutive days of testing.

A between subjects ANOVA conducted on licks across the final three day average showed a main effect of strain (F[17,278]=13.587, p<.001, ηp2=.484), though no main effect of sex or interaction of strain by sex were found (Figure 3). Whether the raw unadjusted or body-weight adjusted licks are examined, CAST/EiJ and A/J mice are the highest licking founder strains, while 129S1/SvImJ and WSB/EiJ are the lowest.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Average of the Boost licks on the last three days of testing (D5-D7) across strains, adjusted for body weight (licks/g body weight).

Finally, a preference score was calculated for each subject by dividing the number of licks on the Boost spout by the number of total licks (Boost + water spouts). This preference score was again averaged across the final three days of testing and analyzed using a one-way ANOVA. No strain, sex, or strain by sex effects emerged (p>.05 for all), though this is likely due to a ceiling effect, as all strains exhibited a very high preference for Boost over water.

Reversal learning

Reversal learning data was collected from partially overlapping strain panels at two sites (Binghamton University and JAX), and a repeated measures ANOVA was conducted on each dependent variable with site as a main effect. We first examined whether site of assessment affected performance. A test stage (acquisition vs. reversal) by site effect was found for the percentage of correct trials (F[1, 408]=6.890, p<.05, ηp2=.024) and reward retrieval time (F[1, 408]=9.964, p<.05, ηp2=.033), and a main effect of site was observed for premature responding in the correct hole in acquisition (F[1, 408]=4.240, p<.05, ηp2=.015) and premature responding in the incorrect hole in reversal (F[1, 408]=7.704, p<.05, ηp2=.026), driven by JAX subjects exhibiting higher premature responding overall. As a consequence, site was included as a covariate in analyses of the premature responding variable. No site effect was observed for any other variables (p>.05 for all), and site was subsequently dropped from the model for those variables.

A between subjects ANOVA was conducted on the TTC difference score (Figure 4). A main effect of strain was observed (F[43,408]=1.514, p<.05) reporting a heritability estimate of 0.169, with no effect of sex or interaction of strain by sex. Further analysis on the founders using a t-test revealed that the 129S1/SvImJ, NZO/HlLtJ, PWK/PhJ, and WSB/EiJ had difference scores significantly greater than 0, indicating that those four strains took significantly more trials to reach criteria in reversal than in acquisition. None of the founder strains showed difference scores significantly less than 0.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Average total trials in acquisition and reversal stages across strain displayed as a difference score (Reversal-Acquisition). A positive value shows an increase from acquisition to reversal, which demonstrates cognitive inflexibility and impulsive action.

Premature responding was analyzed through two separate between-subjects ANOVAs conducted on premature responding in the correct (reinforced) hole at acquisition and incorrect (previously reinforced) hole at reversal. Premature responses in acquisition demonstrated a main effect of strain (F[43, 408]=13.402, p<.001, ηp2=.339; Figure 5). A/J, CC015/UncJ, CC036/UncJ, and CC059/TauUncJ strains were determined to be extreme high premature responders by a Tukey post-hoc comparison, while no extreme lows were detected. Premature responses in reversal showed a main effect of strain (F[43, 408]=3.486, p<.001, ηp2=.343); no sex effects were observed (Figure 6). A/J and CC036/UncJ strains again emerged as an extreme high responder, as did the NZO/HlLtJ and CC061/GeniUncJ. Two wild-derived strains, PWK/PhJ and CAST/EiJ, were found to be extreme lower responders.

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Average premature responses in the correct (rewarded) hole per trial (total premature responses/total trials) in acquisition across strain. A high value represents an inability to withhold responding and high waiting impulsivity.

Figure 6.

Figure 6.

Average premature responses in the incorrect (previously rewarded but currently unrewarded) hole per trial (total premature responses/total trials) in reversal across strain. A high value represents an inability to withhold responding in a previously rewarded hole and high waiting impulsivity.

Ancillary variables were also examined (Table 6) for heritability. Strain impacted the tendency to omit response on trials, as well as the time required to initiate trials and to retrieve earned rewards. None of these strain effects were moderated by sex.

Table 6.

ANOVA statistics for ancillary variables examined in reversal learning. Significant findings are bolded and italicized.

Strain Sex Stage Strain × Sex Strain × Stage Strain × Sex × Stage
Average correct trials F[44,320]=1.256, p>.05 F[1,320]=2.097, p>.05 F[1,320]=76.119, p>.05 F[43,320]=1.139, p>.05 F[44,320]=1.021, p>.05 F[43,320]=.708, p>.05
Average omissions F[44,320]=33.524, p<.001, ηp 2 =.695 F[1,320]=.745, p>.05 F[1,320]=31.542, p<.001, ηp 2 =.090 F[43,320]=.831, p>.05 F[44,320]=1.725. p<.01, ηp 2 =.192 F[43,320]=1.400, p=.057, ηp2=.158
Trial initiation latency F[44,320]=4.468, p<.001, ηp 2 =.378 F[1,320]=482, p>.05 F[1,320]=13.892, p<.001, ηp 2 =.041 F[43,320]=.722, p>.05 F[44,320]=1.670. p<.01, ηp 2 =.185 F[43,320]=1.369. p>.05
Reward retrieval time (JAX only) F[44,174]=6.507, p<.001, ηp 2 =.622 F[1,174]=1.155, p>.05 F[1,174]=15.048, p<.001, ηp 2 =.080 F[42,174]=.952, p>.05 F[44,174]=1.11, p>.05 F[42,174]=.961, p>.05

Delay discounting

A repeated measures ANOVA was conducted on difference score variables as a function of four delay times, revealing a three-way interaction between strain, sex, and delay (F[51, 117]=1.463, p<.05, ηp2=.175), a trending interaction of strain and delay (F[51, 117]=1.376, p=.056, ηp2=.167) and expected main effects of delay (F[3, 117]=28.084, p<.001, ηp2=.194) and strain (F[17, 117]=2.296, p<.01, ηp2=.250). Further analysis on each founder strain showed significant discounting in the NOD/ShiLtJ, A/J, C57BL/6J, CAST/EiJ, and PWK/PhJ strains, while the 129S1/SvImJ, NZO/HlLtJ, and WSB/EiJ did not exhibit significant discounting under these test conditions (Figure 7).

Figure 7.

Figure 7.

The large delayed (LD) – small immediate (SI) reward amounts at the different delays in delay discounting. A more positive value indicates the LD value was discounted more, demonstrating the SI picked more often and the LD reward value increasing in response. Therefore strain with a steeper slope across the delays more readily discount and show increased impulsive choice, whereas animals with a flatter slope do not demonstrate high discounting behavior.

Phenotypic correlations

As several studies and groups have used the founder strains in tests of ambulatory response in an open field, we first asked whether our locomotor activity data reproduced existing data. Total ambulatory distance traveled was positively correlated with measures of activity in an open field gathered in other published, publicly available datasets, including Geuther et al., 2019 (MPD(Mouse Phenome Database #):650; r[4]=.956, p<.001), Kollmus et al., 2020 (MPD:550; r[6]=.762, p<.01), Kliethermes & Crabbe, 2006 (MPD:599; r[6]=.986, p<.001), and Wiltshire et al., 2015 (MPD:214; r[6]=.951, p<.01). Rearing behavior in our dataset was also correlated with that reported by Kliethermes & Crabbe (r[6]=.785, p<.05) and Wiltshire et al. (rs[6]=.886, p<.05).

Correlations were run on key variables selected from each behavioral test. A priori correlations are bolded to show a predicted relationship (Table 7). Locomotor response (distance traveled) to novelty and impulsivity have a conflicting relationship in the literature, with an early study reporting hyperactivity was correlated with impulsive choice (Isles et al., 2004), while later studies found no relationship between activity and impulsive premature responding (Belin et al., 2008; Loos et al., 2009) or delay discounting (Perry et al., 2005). Additionally, the facets of impulsive behavior are dictated by different neural circuits and are thought to reflect uncorrelated phenotypes (Barrus et al., 2015; Dougherty et al., 2009; Nautiyal et al., 2017; Reynolds et al., 2008). However, multiple facets of impulsivity have been identified as having a relationship with over-eating in humans, including non-planning impulsivity, attentional impulsivity, and motor impulsivity (Garza et al., 2016; Georgii et al., 2017; Maxwell, Gardiner, & Loxton, 2020). This relationship has also been observed within rodents as well, with high impulsive rats engaging in more “binge-like” and excessive consumption of highly palatable foods (Anastasio et al., 2019; Velázquez-Sánchez et al., 2014), potentially due to convergent neurocircuitry of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (Anastasio et al., 2019). A positive correlation is therefore hypothesized between the dimensions of impulsivity and Boost intake.

Table 7.

Correlation table for strains collapsed by sex. A priori correlations are highlighted in green and significant a priori correlations are highlighted in yellow. Spearman correlations are reported on the upper right and Pearson correlations are reported on the lower left.

A B C D E F G H I J
1. rs=−.554
p=.017
rs=−.204
p=.926
rs=−.340
p=.168
rs=−.527
p=.025
rs=−.490
p=.039
rs=.030
p=.906
rs=−.007
p=.977
rs=.503
p=.034
rs=.079
p=.754
2. r=−.535
p=.022
rs=.226
p=.367
rs=.139
p=.581
rs=.104
p=.681
rs=.086
p=.735
rs=.195
p=.438
rs=−.595
p=.009
rs=−.610
p=.007
rs=.174
p=.489
3. r=−.026
p=.917
r=.235
p=.348
rs=−.201
p=.423
rs=−.273
p=.272
rs=.160
p=.536
rs=−.013
p=.958
rs=−.088
p=.729
rs=.001
p=.997
rs=.269
p=.280
4. r=−.332
p=.179
r=.056
p=.826
r=−.249
p=.319
rs=.711
p=.001
rs=−.158
p=.531
rs=−.030
p=.906
rs=−.007
p=.977
rs=.166
p=.510
rs=−.203
p=.418
5. r=−.450
p=.061
r=.023
p=.927
r=−.385
p=.114
r=−.776
p=.001
rs=.051
p=.842
rs=−.360
p=.142
rs=.005
p=.984
rs=−.028
p=.913
rs=−.071
p=.779
6. r=−.421
p=.082
r=−.092
p=.715
r=.202
p=.421
r=−.285
p=.251
r=−.111
p=.660
rs=.005
p=.984
rs=.337
p=.171
rs=−.102
p=.687
rs=.075
p=.776
7. r=−.012
p=.964
r=.173
p=.492
r=.021
p=.933
r=−.269
p=.281
r=−.414
p=.088
r=.270
p=.278
rs=−.164
p=.515
rs=−.296
p=.233
rs=−.131
p=.604
8. r=−.005
p=.985
r=−.629
p=.005
r=−.104
p=.681
r=−.023
p=.929
r=−.062
p=.806
r=.309
p=.213
r=−.089
p=.726
rs=.379
p=.121
rs=.218
p=.385
9. r=.513
p=.030
r=−.480
p=.044
r=.047
p=.854
r=.090
p=.723
r=−.177
p=.482
r=−.026
p=.920
r=−.279
p=.262
r=.399
p=.101
rs=.263
p=.291
10. r=.130
p=.608
r=−.180
p=.476
r=.173
p=.492
r=−.016
p=.949
r=−.026
p=.919
r=.036
p=.889
r=−.176
p=.486
r=−.277
p=.267
r=.314
p=.204

The variables are as follows:

1.

Ambulatory distance on D1 of locomotor

2.

Ambulatory distance difference score (D2-D1)

3.

Boost licks averaged over the final 3 days adjusted for body weight

4.

Average premature responses in the correct hole in acquisition

5.

Average premature responses in the incorrect hole in reversal

6.

Total trial to criteria difference score (Reversal-Acquisition)

7.

LD-SI amount at 0s

8.

LD-SI amount at 3s

9.

LD-SI amount at 6s

10.

LD-SI amount at 9s

No strain-level relationship was found between Boost intake and TTC difference score (r[16]=.202, p=.421), premature responses in the correct hole at acquisition (r[16]=−249, p=.319) premature responses in the correct hole at reversal (r[16]=−.385, p=.114), or any of the delay time difference scores (r[16]=.021–.173, p>.05 for all).

Rate of premature responding during the acquisition and reversal stages was highly correlated (rs[16]=.711, p<.001). While strain-level heritability was identified for each impulsivity measure, no genetic correlation was found between TTC and premature responding (reversal learning) or LD-SI reward difference at any of the delays (delay discounting), suggesting that they are influenced by from distinct genetic architectures (r[16]=−.203–.166, p>.05 for all).

We previously reported on heritable variation in ethanol consumption and preference in a sample of CC and founder strains (Bagley et al., 2021). Premature responding during the acquisition stage was positively correlated with ethanol intake (adjusted for body weight) assessed across sequential testing days (day 1 (r[18]=0.5, p<.05), 2 (r[18]=.63, p<.001), and 3 (r[18]=.62, p<.001)) in a two bottle choice modified drinking in the dark paradigm. This correlation was driven by the high drinking and high premature responding phenotype of the PWK/PhJ male mice, and the correlation became non-significant when these mice were excluded. No relationship was found between ambulation, palatable food reward intake, or TTC scores with measures of ethanol intake (r[16]=−0.359—0.209, p>.05 for all) or preference (r[16]=0–302—0.130, p>.05 for all).

Finally, we correlated locomotor and impulsivity measures to acoustic startle response (ASR) and pre-pulse inhibition (PPI) data reported in a separate study (Kollmus et al., 2020) in the founder strains. Impulsive phenotypes and ASR/PPI are dependent on ventrostriatal dopamine transmission, and a correlation between impulsivity and impaired PPI has been identified within human literature (Gee et al., 2015; Swann et al., 2013). No relationship emerged between impulsivity and ASR or PPI, though a strong negative correlation was found between ASR (120DB) and degree of habituation for rearing (r[6]=−817, p<.05) and stereotypy (r[6]=−.957, p<.001). This correlation demonstrates that a heightened acoustic startle response appears to be correlated in strains that do not habituate rearing and stereotypy behaviors upon repeated exposure to a novel environment.

Discussion

The goal of this study was to examine a panel of CC strains and CC founders for heritable addiction-related traits and further understand the relationship between different components of impulsivity. We have identified multiple heritable phenotypes in the founder strains and the ten CC strains, specifically locomotor activity in response to novelty, degree of habituation within the open field apparatus, reward sensitivity as measured by total Boost consumption, aspects of behavioral flexibility and impulsivity measured in reversal learning, and impulsive choice in delay discounting. Overall our results demonstrate a genetic component of these phenotypes, with varying degrees of heritability.

Heritability estimates are the proportion of phenotypic variation that is attributed to strain membership and therefore to genetic variation. . As the ratio of between strain variance to total variance increases, heritability estimates increase. All the behavioral phenotypes assessed in our study exhibited significant main effects of strain, with significant heritability estimates ranging from .169 to .832. This demonstrates that genetic variation affects all these behavioral endpoint measures, albeit to different degrees.

Other factors affect phenotypic variance, including shared or unique environmental variation and measurement error. Even when the contribution of these factors are greater than that of genetic variation, genetic mapping studies can still be successful.

The implications of these findings are that heritable traits can be subject to forwards genetic analyses, resulting in candidate genes that may functionally regulate these behaviors. In one example, our previous study of reversal learning in BXD mice revealed a heritability estimate of 0.31; though this means that genetic variation only explained 31% of the total phenotypic variation, that was sufficient to result in the discovery of a genome-wide significant quantitative trait locus on mouse chromosome 10 and the nomination of the candidate gene Syn3, encoding the synapsin III protein (Laughlin et al., 2011). Notably, a recent study from our group validated the role for Syn3 in reversal learning; Syn3 null mice were found to exhibit reversal learning impairments, as was predicted by our systems genetic analysis of the phenotype in BXD mice (Moore, Linden, & Jentsch, 2021).

Though the number of CC strains is insufficient to carry out an adequately powered linkage study, the Diversity Outbred mice that were generated from them (Chesler, 2014), can be phenotyped using these procedures in an adequately powered study using a genome-wide association design. These studies are in progress in our laboratories.

Locomotor response

Multiple facets of behavior measured in the open field were found to be heritable across the founders and CC strains. Distance traveled in a novel environment, as well as the degree of habituation of this response during a second test, were both observed to be heritable (heritability of 0.392 and 0.258, respectively), as were measures of the duration of ambulation, resting, stereotypy, and rearing (heritability between 0.566 to 0.832 for all measures). These results are consistent with prior reports, as heritability of related phenotypes is well-established within the literature on inbred mouse models (Jeste et al., 1984; Mhyre et al., 2005; Swallow et al., 1998).

As previously discussed, ambulatory distance traveled in response to novelty has a conflicting relationship with impulsivity in the literature. Isles et al. (2004) reported exploratory activity to covary with impulsive choice in four inbred mouse strains. This relationship has been described as impulsive activity, a trait with relevance to attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Itohara, Kobayashi, & Nakashiba, 2015). However, subsequent studies have supplied evidence that these two phenotypes are not co-heritable in various strains of laboratory rats (Belin et al., 2008; Loos et al., 2009; Perry et al., 2005), with Perry et al. specifically examining delay discounting, the measure of impulsivity utilized by Isles et al. Ultimately, the lack of a correlation between any traits of ambulation and impulsive behavior in this study support the previous findings that ambulation and impulsivity are disassociated traits (Loos et al., 2009), though this relationship may also be a distinction between rats and mice, or otherwise dependent on the subjects’ genetic background.

Palatable food intake

The strains were phenotyped for intake of a highly palatable food, providing essential information about hedonic responses to that food and/or reward sensitivity. Past research has suggested sensitivity to reward is a trait that predicts motivation to seek out reinforcing stimuli (Davis & Fox, 2008), and presentation of food and drug cues result in activation of similar regions, as well as activation of similar gene expression patterns (Kelley, Schiltz, & Landry, 2005). Moderate heritability estimates were found for palatable food intake (estimates of .434 and .484 for overall average intake or last three days, respectively). No genetic relationship was found between palatable food intake and impulsivity in the strains of the current study. These findings support the idea that heritable differences in reward sensitivity (which includes, but is not limited to, taste sensitivity) are not driving the heritable differences impulsive responding measured in the operant tests. Therefore it can be concluded that the impulsive behaviors measured here cannot be wholly or even mostly attributed to differences in reward and/or taste sensitivity.

Human studies have demonstrated that food cravings are a component of trait impulsivity (Garza et al., 2016; Georgii et al., 2017; Nederkoorn et al., 2009), perhaps due to convergent circuitry in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Georgii et al., 2017). This is further supported by a study that segregated Sprague-Dawley rats based on motor impulsivity (determined by the 1-CSRTT), finding that highly impulsive rats engaged in more vigorous binge-like consumption of a high-fat food and that activation of the ventromedial frontal cortex suppressed both impulsive and binge-like feeding behaviors (Anastasio et al., 2019). However, at least one mouse study has found the opposite relationship. When Warthen et al. (2016) stimulated pyramidal neurons in the medial frontal cortex of mice, operant responding for food rewards was increased, while impulsive responding was suppressed. Importantly, this study also found no effect on consumption of freely available food. It is likely that the interaction between trait impulsivity and consumption behaviors, including reward sensitivity, is dependent on a myriad of factors, including internal states such as mood (Herman et al, 2018; Tice et al., 2001) and interoception (Herman et al., 2018; Herman et al., 2019). Nevertheless, results from the current study support the idea that impulsivity phenotypes and consumption of a palatable food reward do not covary in the CC population.

Impulsivity

Impulsive action and choice were characterized in all strains and found to be significantly heritable for all outcomes which included: reversal learning (0.169), premature responding (0.339 and 0.343 for acquisition and reversal, respectively), and discounting of delayed rewards (0.250). This supports previous findings in both human and animal literature that impulsivity is a heritable trait. Heritability estimates for premature responding reported here are greater than past estimates derived from measures of response to selection in outbred rats (Jupp et al., 2020). Delay discounting heritability estimates are on par with estimates that have been reported previously for inbred mice populations (Isles et al., 2004; Wilhelm & Mitchell, 2009), while estimates for behavioral flexibility in reversal learning are lower than what has been reported in inbred BXD mice (Laughlin et al., 2011). Heritability estimates are no doubt influenced by the specific species and population being evaluated.

The results of this study support the theory proposed by Evenden (1998) that dimensions of impulsivity are dissociable. We found no correlations between impulsivity-relevant phenotypes measured during reversal learning or delay discounting. While each of the types of impulsivity tested do share some mechanistic basis within the frontal cortex and striatal circuitry, each trait demonstrates unique patterns of regional activation. Reversal learning requires function of the orbitofrontal cortex and dorsomedial striatum for optimal inhibitory control (Boulougouris, Dalley, & Robbins, 2007; Chudasama & Robbins, 2003; Clarke, Robbins, & Roberts, 2008; Jentsch et al, 2014), also demonstrated by a functional neuroimaging study in human adults (Ghahremani et al., 2010). Conversely, tests of impulsive choice rely less robustly on the orbitofrontal cortex and are more strongly associated with activation of the lateral frontal cortex (Cho et al., 2010; Hinvest et al., 2011) and the hippocampus (Abela & Chudasama, 2012; Cheung & Cardinal, 2005; Jentsch et al., 2014; McHugh et al., 2008). Waiting impulsivity, by contrast, is most dependent upon activation of the infralimbic cortex, nucleus accumbens, and the subthalamic nucleus (Baunez et al., 1995; Chuadasama et al., 2003; Jentsch et al., 2014; Morris et al., 2016; Voon, 2014). Altogether, these three types of impulsivity seem to be distinct phenotypes that arise from cortico-striato-thalamo dysfunction, which are in part influenced by genetics.

Ultimately only one dimension of impulsivity was correlated with overnight ethanol consumption and preference, though it was driven by the extreme high premature responding and relatively high drinking phenotypes of the PWK/PhJ male mice. Waiting impulsivity, a form of impulsive action, exhibited a positive correlation with ethanol consumption when PWK/PhJ mice were included in the design, though not when they were excluded. Considering female PWK/PhJ mice were also high ethanol consumers without displaying the high premature responding phenotype, this suggests extremely aberrant waiting impulsivity behavior in this particular strain that is sex-dependent.

Inter-lab replicability

Separate reversal learning testing at two sites (Binghamton and JAX) showed that majority of variables analyzed did not have a site effect and therefore demonstrated strong replicability. The site-dependent effects were observed for correct trials and premature responding, the latter of which was driven by JAX site mice demonstrating higher premature responding overall. While each site followed identical testing procedures, Binghamton mice were tested on reward sensitivity and open field prior to reversal learning, while JAX mice were first tested on open field, light/dark, hole board, and novelty place preference. The site effect only on select variables also indicates that certain phenotypes may be uniquely affected by adolescent shipping and separate site variability. Suppressed premature responding at the Binghamton site is a unique finding that cannot definitely be attributed to any factor between the sites. Further research is needed to understand the potential relationship between motor impulsivity and factors such as adolescent shipping stress despite a lengthy habituation period.

Limitations

Single housing was utilized during testing due to high levels of aggression observed in some strains. Group housing was utilized upon initial acclimation to minimize shipping and acclimation stress and because aggression was observed more often once food restriction began. Evidence for an effect of single housing on impulsivity is mixed. One study isolated rats at PND 28 and found no difference on impulsivity in the 5-CSRTT at adulthood, though did note that these isolated animals were slower to collect food rewards (Dalley et al., 2002). Interestingly, a separate study examined the effects of isolation rearing on delay discounting and noted that isolation reared rats showed reduced impulsive action and impulsive choice (Liu, Wilkinson, & Robbins, 2017). It is possible that certain strains may be more sensitive to the effects of isolation housing, and the degree to which operant performance is altered may vary. Notably, however, the studies cited above all examined the effects of social isolation in adolescent animals, whereas subjects in the present study were housed individually only in adulthood. Adult isolation housing has many fewer effects on behavior (Hall, 1998; Hall & Perona, 2012), and it does not appear to have any effect on reversal learning, in particular (Lander, Linder-Shacham, & Gaisler-Salomon, 2017). Thus, while social isolation should be considered as a factor, it is unlikely to have confounded the results.

A small amount of caffeine is present in the Boost reinforcer and may exert an effect on animals tested at the Binghamton site, particularly if one or more strains are exceptionally sensitive to it. Chocolate Boost contains .62 mg of caffeine per fluid ounce (Caffeine Informer). Each reward delivery is approximately 20 μl, or .00068 fluid ounces, resulting in mice receiving .00418 mg caffeine per reinforcer. The maximum reinforcers a mouse received on average was 80 in delay discounting, with less rewards being received in reversal learning. Thus, a mouse could possibly receive at maximum of .3344 mg of caffeine per day. This was converted into a mg/kg dose for a low weight animal (12g) and a high weight animal (40g). Respectively the daily dose was calculated to be 10 mg/kg and 3 mg/kg (PO). A bolus dose of 15 mg/kg i.p. caffeine is described as being a moderate dose (Hnasko, Sotak, & Palmiter, 2005), and 1.5 mg/kg i.p. doses were found to be enough to produce conditioned place preference in mice (Patkina & Zvartau, 1998). Past studies have shown that .5–16 mg/kg i.p. dose of caffeine increases locomotor activity in mice (Kayir & Uzbay, 2004), and i.p. doses of 5mg/kg-15mg/kg increase wakefulness (Huang et al., 2005). Route of administration additionally plays a factor: oral consumption of caffeine decreased the amount of cocaine later self-administered, while 3 mg/kg i.p. injections increased it despite similar metabolite levels (Kuzmin et al., 2000). This information indicates that mice in the present study were receiving variable levels of caffeine that depended on number of reinforcers received, though this was at maximum a low-moderate dose, and consumption was not impacted by body weight.

Conclusion

Altogether, the present study marks one of the first systematic attempts to phenotype the CC founder strains for ambulatory activity, reward sensitivity, and three types of impulsivity. Heritability for these traits has been identified, and the lack of genetic correlation between each of these types of impulsivity implicate unique neurogenetic factors between traits. These results suggest that genetically diverse populations like the CC used in this study and Diversity Outbred (DO) populations are suited for forward genetic approaches to reveal unique genetic factors that influence dimensions of impulsivity and how these unique factors influence risk for SUDs.

Acknowledgements:

This work was supported, in part, by Public Health Service Grants P50-DA 039841 (to JDJ, VP, LR, SJS-R, LG and EJC) and T32-AA205606 (to LB and JDJ). We gratefully acknowledge the technical assistance of Ms. Barbara Force and the contribution of the Computational Sciences Service at The Jackson Laboratory for expert assistance with the work described in this publication.

References

  1. Abela AR, Chudasama Y (2013). Dissociable contributions of the ventral hippocampus and orbitofrontal cortex to decision-making with a delayed or uncertain outcome. Eur J Neurosci, 37(4):640–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anastasio NC, Stutz SJ, Price AE, Davis-Reyes BD, Sholler DJ, Ferguson SM, Neumaier JF, Moeller FG, Hommel JD, Cunningham KA (2019). Convergent neural connectivity in motor impulsivity and high-fat food binge-like eating in male Sprague-Dawley rats. Neuropsychopharmacology, 44(10):1752–1761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aylor DL, Valdar W, Foulds-Mathes W, Buus RJ, Verdugo RA, Baric RS, Ferris MT, Frelinger JA, Heise M, Frieman MB, Galinski LE, Bell TA, Didion JD, Hua K, Nehrenberg DL, Powell CL, Steigerwalt J, Xie Y, Kelada SN, Collins FS, Yang IV, Schwartz DA, Branstetter LA, Chesler EJ, Miller DR, Spence J, Liu EY, McMillan L, Sarkar A, Wang J, Wang W, Zhang Q, Broman KW, Korstanje R, Durrant C, Mott R, Iraqi FA, Pomp D, Threadgill D, de Villena FP, Churchill GA (2011). Genetic analysis of complex traits in the emerging Collaborative Cross. Genome Res, 21(8):1213–1222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bagley JR, Chesler EJ, Philip VM, Center for the Systems Genetics of Addiction, Jentsch JD (2021). Heritability of ethanol consumption and pharmacokinetics in a genetically diverse panel of collaborative cross mouse strains and their inbred founders. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 45(4):697–708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bari A, Dalley JW, Robbins TW (2008). The application of the 5-choice serial reaction time task for the assessment of visual attentional processes and impulse control in rats. Nat Protoc, 3(5):759–767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barratt ES (1959). Anxiety and impulsiveness related to psychomotor efficiency. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 9:191–198. [Google Scholar]
  7. Barrus MM, Hosking JG, Zeeb FD, Tremblay M, Winstanley CA (2015). Disadvantageous decision-making on a rodent gambling task is associated with increased motor impulsivity in a population of male rats. J Psychiatry Neurosci, 40(2):108–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Baunez C, Robbins TW (1997). Bilateral lesions of the subthalamic nucleus induce multiple deficits in an attentional task in rats. Eur J Neurosci, 9(10);2086–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Belin D, Berson N, Balado E, Piazza PV, Deroche-Gamonet V (2011). High-novelty-preference rats are predisposed to compulsive cocaine self-administration. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(3):569–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Belin D, Mar AC, Dalley JW, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ (2008). High impulsivity predicts the switch to compulsive cocaine-taking. Science, 320(5881):1352–1355. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bevilacqua L, Goldman D (2013). Genetics of impulsive behaviour. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 368(1615): 20120380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bezdijian S, Baker LA, Tuvblad C (2011). Genetic and environmental influences on impulsivity: A meta-analysis of twin, family and adoption studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(7):1209–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Boulougouris V, Dalley JW, Robbins TW (2007). Effects of orbitofrontal, infralimbic and prelimbic cortical lesions on serial spatial reversal learning in the rat. Behav Brain Res, 179(2):219–228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Broos N, Schmaal L, Wiskerke J Kostelijk L, Lam T, Stoop N, Weierink L, Ham J, de Geus EJC, Schoffelmeer ANM, van den Brink W, Veltman DJ, de Vries TJ, Pattij T, Goudriaan AE (2012). The relationship between impulsive choice and impulsive action: A cross-species translational study. PLoS ONE, 7(5). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cervantes MC, Laughlin RE, Jentsch JD (2013). Cocaine self-administration behavior in inbred mouse lines segregating different capacities for inhibitory control. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 229(3):515–525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chesler EJ (2014). Out of the bottleneck: the Diversity Outcross and Collaborative Cross mouse populations in behavioral genetics research. Mamm Genome, 25(1–2):3–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cheung THC, Cardinal RD (2005). Hippocampal lesions facilitate instrumental learning with delayed reinforcement but induce impulsive choice in rats. BMC Neurosci, 6:36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cho SS, Ko JH, Pellecchia G, van Eimeren T, Cilia R, Strafella AP (2010). Continuous theta burst stimulation of right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex induces changes in impulsivity level. Brain Stimul, 3(3):170–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Chudasama Y, Robbins TW (2003). Dissociable contributions of the orbitofrontal and infralimbic cortex to pavlovian autoshaping and discrimination reversal learning: further evidence for the functional heterogeneity of the rodent frontal cortex. J Neurosci, 23(25):8771–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Chudasama Y, Robbins TW (2004). Dopaminergic modulation of visual attention and working memory in the rodent prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology, 29(9):1628–1636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Clarke HF, Robbins TW, Roberts AC (2008). Lesions of the Medial Striatum in Monkeys Produce Perseverative Impairments during Reversal Learning Similar to Those Produced by Lesions of the Orbitofrontal Cortex. J Neurosci, 28(43):10972–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Crist RC, Reiner BC, Berrettini WH (2019). A review of opioid addiction genetics. Current Opinion in Psychology, 27:31–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dalley JW, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW (2011). Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron, 69(4):680–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dalley JW, Theobald DE, Pereira EAC, Li PMMC, Robbins TW (2002). Specific abnormalities in serotonin release in the prefrontal cortex of isolation-reared rats measured during behavioural performance of a task assessing visuospatial attention and impulsivity. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 164(3):329–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Davis C, Fox J (2008). Sensitivity to reward and body mass index (BMI): evidence for a non-linear relationship. Appetite, 50(1):43–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Dougherty DM, Mathias CW, Marsh-Richard DM, Prevette KN, Dawes MA, Hatis ES, Palmes G, Nouvion SO (2009). Impulsivity and Clinical Symptoms among Adolescents with Non-Suicidal Self-Injury with or without Attempted Suicide. Psychiatry Res, 169(1):22–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Enoch M (2011). The Role of Early Life Stress as a Predictor for Alcohol and Drug Dependence. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 214(1):17–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Evenden JL (1998). The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats III: the effects of amphetamine, haloperidol, imipramine, chlordiazepoxide and ethanol on a paced fixed consecutive number schedule. Psychopharmacology, 138(3–4):295–304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Evenden JL (1999). Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology, 146:348–361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Evenden JL, Ryan CN (1996). The pharmacology of impulsive behavior in rats: the effects of drugs on response choice with varying delays of reinforcement. Psychopharmacology, 128:161–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Franken IH, van Strien JW, Nijs I, Muris P (2008). Impulsivity is associated with behavioral decision-making deficits. Psychiatry Res, 158(2):155–163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Garza KB, Ding M, Owensby JK, Zizza CA (2016). Impulsivity and Fast-Food Consumption: A Cross-Sectional Study among Working Adults. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics, 116(1):61–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gee L, Smith H, De La Cruz P, Campbell J, Fama C, Haller J, Ramirez-Zamora A, Durphy J, Hanspal E, Molho E, Barba A, Shin D, Pilitsis JG (2015). The Influence of Bilateral Subthalamic Nucleus Deep Brain Stimulation on Impulsivity and Prepulse Inhibition in Parkinson’s Disease Patients. Stereotact Funct Neurosurg., 93(4):265–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Georgii C, Goldhofer P, Meule A, Richard A, Blechert J (2017). Food craving, food choice and consumption: The role of impulsivity and sham-controlled tDCS stimulation of the right dlPFC. Physiol Behav, 117:20–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Geuther BQ, Deats SP, Fox KJ, Murray SA, Braun RE, White JK, Chesler EJ, Lutz CM, Kumar V (2019). Robust mouse tracking in complex environments using neural networks. Commun Biol, 2:124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Ghahremani DG, Monterosso J, Jentsch JD, Bilder RM, Poldrack RA (2010). Neural components underlying behavioral flexibility in human reversal learning. Cereb Cortex, 20(8):1843–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Gilles DM, Turk CL, Fresco DM (2006). Social anxiety, alcohol expectancies, and self-efficacy as predictors of heavy drinking in college students. Addictive Behaviors, 31(3):388–398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hall FS (1998). Social deprivation of neonatal, adolescent, and adult rats has distinct neurochemical and behavioral consequences. Crit Rev Neurobiol, 12(1–2):129–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hall FS, Perona MTG (2012). Have studies of the developmental regulation of behavioral phenotypes revealed the mechanisms of gene–environment interactions? Physiology & Behavior, 107(5):623–640. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hancock DB, Markunas CA, Bierut LJ, Johnson EO (2018). Human genetics of addiction: new insights and future directions. Current Psychiatry Reports, 20(8). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hegmann JP, Possidente B (1981). Estimating genetic correlations from inbred strains. Behav Genet,11(2):103–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Herman AM, Critchley HD, Duka T (2018). Risk-taking and impulsivity: The role of mood states and interoception. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 1625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Herman AM, Critchley HD, Duka T (2018B). The role of emotions and physiological arousal in modulating impulsive behaviour. Biol. Psychol, 133:30–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hinvest NS, Elliott R, McKie S, Anderson I (2011). Neural correlates of choice behavior related to impulsivity and venturesomeness. Neuropsychologia, 49(9):2311–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hnasko TS, Sotak BN, Palmiter RD (2005). Morphine reward in dopamine-deficient mice. Nature, 438(7069):854–857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Huang ZL, Qu WM, Eguchi N, Chen JF, Schwarzschild MA, Fredholm BB, Urade Y, Hayaishi O (2005). Adenosine A2A, but not A1, receptors mediate the arousal effect of caffeine. Nat Neurosci, 8(7):858–859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Isles AR, Humby T, Walters E, Wilkinson LS (2004). Common genetic effects on variation in impulsivity and activity in mice. J Neurosci, 24(30):6733–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Itohara S, Kobayashi Y, Nakashiba T (2015). Genetic factors underlying attention and impulsivity: Mouse models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 2:46–51. [Google Scholar]
  49. Izquierdo A, Jentsch JD (2012). Reversal learning as a measure of impulsive and compulsive behavior in addictions. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 219(2):607–620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Jentsch JD, Ashenhurst JR, Cervantes MC, James AS, Groman SM, Pennington ZT (2014). Dissecting impulsivity and its relationships to drug addictions. Ann N Y Acad Sci, 1327:1–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Jeste DV, Stoff DM, Rawlings R, Wyatt RJ (1984). Pharmacogenetics of phenylethylamine: Determination of heritability and genetic transmission of locomotor effects in recombinant inbred strains of mice. Psychopharmacology, 84:537–540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Jupp B, Pitzoi S, Petretto E et al. (2020). Impulsivity is a heritable trait in rodents and associated with a novel quantitative trait locus on chromosome 1. Sci Rep, 10:6684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kayir H, Uzbay IT (2004). Evidence for the role of nitric oxide in caffeine-induced locomotor activity in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 172(1):11–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kelley AE, Schiltz CA, Landry CF (2005). Neural systems recruited by drug- and food-related cues: studies of gene activation in corticolimbic regions. Physiol Behav, 86(1–2):11–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kendler KS, Prescott CA, Myers J, Neale MC (2003). The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 60(9):929–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kliethermes CL, Crabbe JC (2006). Genetic independence of mouse measures of some aspects of novelty seeking. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 103(13):5018–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Kollmus H, Fuchs H, Lengger C, Haselimashhadi H, Bogue MA, Östereicher MA, Horsch M, Adler T, Aguilar-Pimentel JA, Amarie OV, Becker L, Beckers J, Calzada-Wack J, Garrett L, Hans W, Hölter SM, Klein-Rodewald T, Maier H, Mayer-Kuckuk P, Miller G, Moreth K, Neff F, Rathkolb B, Rácz I, Rozman J, Spielmann N, Treise I, Busch D, Graw J, Klopstock T, Wolf E, Wurst W, Yildirim AÖ, Mason J, Torres A, Mouse Phenome Database Team., Balling R, Mehaan T, Gailus-Durner V, Schughart K, Hrabě de Angelis M (2020). A comprehensive and comparative phenotypic analysis of the collaborative founder strains identifies new and known phenotypes. Mamm Genome, 31(1–2):30–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kreek MJ, Koob GF (1998). Drug dependence: stress and dysregulation of brain reward pathways. Drug Alcohol Depend, 51(1–2):23–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kreek MJ, Nielsen DA, Butelman ER, LaForge KS (2005). Genetic influences on impulsivity, risk taking, stress responsivity and vulnerability to drug abuse and addiction. Nat Neurosci, 8(11):1450–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Kuzmin A, Johansson B, Semenova S, Fredholm BB (2000). Differences in the effect of chronic and acute caffeine on self-administration of cocaine in mice. Eur J Neurosci, 12(8):3026–3032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Lander SS, Linder-Shacham D, Gaisler-Salomon I (2017). Differential effects of social isolation in adolescent and adult mice on behavior and cortical gene expression. Behavioural Brain Research, 316:245–254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Laughlin RE, Grant TL, Williams RW, Jentsch JD (2011). Genetic dissection of behavioral flexibility: reversal learning in mice. Biol Psychiatry, 69(11):1109–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Liu Y, Wilkinson LS, Robbins TW (2017). ‘Waiting impulsivity’ in isolation-reared and socially-reared rats: effects of amphetamine. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 234(9–10):1587–1601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Logan RW, Williams WP 3rd, McClung CA (2014). Circadian rhythms and addiction: mechanistic insights and future directions. Behavioral Neuroscience, 128(3):387–412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Loos M, Van der Sluis S, Bochdanovits Z, Van Zutphen IJ, Pattij T, Stiedl O, Neuro-BSIK Mouse Phenomics consortium, Smit AB, Spijker S (2009). Activity and impulsive action are controlled by different genetic and environmental factors. Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 8(8):817–828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. MacKillop J, Weafer J, Gray JC, Oshri A, Palmer A, de Wit H (2016). The latent structure of impulsivity: impulsive choice, impulsive action, and impulsive personality traits. Psychomaracology (Berl), 233(18):3361–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Maxwell AL, Gardiner E, Loxton NJ (2020). Investigating the relationship between reward sensitivity, impulsivity, and food addiction: A systematic review. Eur Eat Discord Rev, 28(4):368–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. McHugh SB, Campbell TG, Taylor AM, Rawlins JNP, Bannerman DM (2008). A role for dorsal and ventral hippocampus in inter-temporal choice cost-benefit decision making. Behav Neurosci, 122(1):1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Mhyre TR, Chesler EJ, Thiruchelvam M, Lungu C, Cory-Slechta A, Fry JD, Richfield EK (2004). Heritability, correlations and in silico mapping of locomotor behavior and neurochemistry in inbred strains of mice. Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 4(4):209–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Bougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC (2001). Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. Am J Psychiatry, 158(11):1783–1793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Molander AC, Mar A, Norbury A, Steventon S, Moreno M, Caprioli D, Theobald DE, Belin D, Everit BJ, Robbins TW, Dalley JW (2011). High impulsivity predicting vulnerability to cocaine addiction in rats: some relationship with novelty preference but not novelty reactivity, anxiety or stress. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 215(4):721–731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Moore A, Linden J, Jentsch JD (2021). Syn3 gene knockout negatively impacts aspects of reversal learning performance. eNeuro, ENEURO.0251–21.2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Morris LS, Kundu P, Baek K, Irvine MA, Mechelmans DJ, Wood J, Harrison NA, Robbins TW, Bullmore ET, Voon V (2016). Jumping the Gun: Mapping Neural Correlates of Waiting Impulsivity and Relevance Across Alcohol Misuse. Biol Psychiatry, 79(6);499–507. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Nadal R, Armario A, Janak P (2002). Positive relationship between activity in a novel environment and operant ethanol self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacology, 162(3):333–338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Nautiyal KM, Wall MM, Wang S, Magalong VM, Ahmari SE, Balsam PD, Blanco C, Hen R (2016). Genetic and Modeling Approaches Reveal Distinct Components of Impulsive Behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology, 42:1182–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Nederkoorn C, Guerrieri R, Havermans RC, Roefs A, Jansen A (2009). The interactive effect of hunger and impulsivity on food intake and purchase in a virtual supermarket. Int J Obes (Lond)., 33(8):905–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Odet F, Pan W, Bell TA, Goodson SG, Stevans AM, Yun Z, Aylor DL, Kao C, McMillan L, Pardo-Manuel de Villena F, O’Brien DA (2015). The founder strains of the Collaborative Cross express a complex combination of advantageous and deleterious traits for male reproduction. Genes, Genomes, and Genetics, 5(12):2671–2683. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Odum AL (2013). Delay discounting: I’m a k, you’re a k. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 96(3):427:439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Patkina NA, Zvartau EE (1998). Caffeine place conditioning in rats: comparison with cocaine and ethanol. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol, 8(4):287–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Perry JL, Larson EB, German JP, Madden GJ, Carroll ME (2005). Impulsivity (delay discounting) as a predictor of acquisition of IV cocaine self-administration in female rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 178(2–3). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Philip VM, Duvvuru S, Gomero B, Ansah TA, Blaha CD, Cook MN, Hamre KM, Lariviere WR, Matthews DB, Mittleman G, Goldowitz D, Chesler EJ (2009). High‐throughput behavioral phenotyping in the expanded panel of BXD recombinant inbred strains. Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 9(2):129–159 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Philip VM, Sokoloff G, Ackert-Bicknell CL, Striz M, Branstetter L, Beckmann MA, Spence JS, Jackson BL, Galloway LD, Barker P, Wymore AM, Hunsicker PR, Durtschi DC, Shaw GS, Shinpock S, Manly KF, Miller DR, Donohue KD, Culiat CT, Churchill GA, Lariviere WR, Palmer AA, O’Hara BF, Voy BH, Chesler EJ (2011). Genetic analysis in the Collaborative Cross breeding population. Genome Res, 21(8):1223–1238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Piazza PV, Deroche-Gamonet V (2013). A multistep general theory of transition to addiction. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 229(3):387–413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Reynolds B, Penfold RB, Michele P (2008). Dimensions of impulsive behavior in adolescents: Laboratory behavioral assessments. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16(2):124–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Rosenwasser AM (2010). Circadian clock genes: Non-circadian roles in sleep, addiction, and psychiatric disorders? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(8):1249–1255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Sinha R (2008). Chronic Stress, Drug Use, and Vulnerability to Addiction. Ann N Y Acad Sci, 1141:105–130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Spanagel R, Montkowski A, Allingham K, Stöhr T, Shoaib M, Holsboer F, Landgraf R (1995). Anxiety: a potential predictor of vulnerability to the initiation of ethanol self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 122(4):369–373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Stathopoulou G, Gold AK, Hoyt DL, Milligan M, Hearon BA, Otto MW (2021). Does anxiety sensitivity predict addiction severity in opioid use disorder? Addictive Behaviors, 112:106644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Swallow JG, Carter PA, Garland T Jr (1998). Artificial selection for increased wheel-running behavior in house mice. Behav Genet, 28(3):227–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Swann AC, Lijffijt M, Lane SD, Steinberg JL, Acas MD, Cox B, Moeller FG (2013). Pre-attentive information processing and impulsivity in bipolar disorder. J Psychiatr Res, 47:1917–1924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Templeton GF (2011). A Two-Step Approach for Transforming Continuous Variables to Normal: Implications and Recommendations for IS Research. Communications of the Association for Information System, 28(4). [Google Scholar]
  92. Tice DM, Bratslavsky E, Baumeister RF (2001). Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: If you feel bad, do it! Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1):53–67. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Velázquez-Sánchez C, Ferragud A, Moore CF, Everitt BJ, Sabino V, Cottone P (2014). High Trait Impulsivity Predicts Food Addiction-Like Behavior in the Rat. Neuropsychopharmacology, 39(10):2463–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Voon V (2014). Models of Impulsivity with a Focus on Waiting Impulsivity: Translational Potential for Neuropsychiatric Disorders. Cur Addict Rep., 1(4):281–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Warthen DM, Lambeth PS, Ottolini M, Shi Y, Barker BS, Gaykema RP, Newmyer BA, Joy-Gaba J, Ohmura Y, Perez-Reyes E, Güler AD, Patel MK, Scott MM (2016). Activation of Pyramidal Neurons in Mouse Medial Prefrontal Cortex Enhances Food-Seeking Behavior While Reducing Impulsivity in the Absence of an Effect on Food Intake. Front Behav Neurosci., 10:63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Wilhelm CJ, Mitchell SH (2009). Strain differences in delay discounting using inbred rats. Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 8(4):426–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Williams RW, Bennett B, Lu L, Gu J, DeFries JC, Carosone-Link PH, Rikke BA, Belknap JK, Johnson TE (2004). Genetic structure of the LXS panel of recombinant inbred mouse strains: a powerful resource for complex trait analysis. Mammalian Genome, 15(8):637–647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Wiltshire T, Ervin RB, Duan H, Bogue MA, Zamboni WC, Cook S, Chung W, Zou F, Tarantino LM (2015). Initial locomotor sensitivity to cocaine varies widely among inbred mouse strains. Genes Brain Behav, 14(3):271–80 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Wingo T, Nesil T, Choi J, Li MD (2016). Novelty Seeking and Drug Addiction in Humans and Animals: From Behavior to Molecules. J Neuroimmune Pharmacol., 11(3):456–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Winstanley CA, Dalley JW, Theobald DE, Robbins TW (2004). Fractionating impulsivity: contrasting effects of central 5-HT depletion on different measures of impulsive behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology, 29(7):1331–1343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

All data reported in this manuscript have been deposited into a password-protected area of the Mouse Phenome Database (MPD); requests for access to the data can be sent to the Corresponding Author. Upon acceptance of this manuscript for publication, all the associated data will be made publicly available via MPD.

RESOURCES