Abstract
Current theories of human neural development emphasize the posterior-to-anterior pattern of brain maturation. However, this scenario leaves out significant brain areas not directly involved with sensory input and behavioral control. Suggesting the relevance of cortical activity unrelated to sensory stimulation, such as sleep, we investigated adolescent transformations in the topography of sleep spindles. Sleep spindles are known to be involved in neural plasticity and in adults have a bimodal topography: slow spindles are frontally dominant, while fast spindles have a parietal/precuneal origin. The late functional segregation of the precuneus from the frontoparietal network during adolescence suggests that spindle topography might approach the adult state relatively late in development, and it may not be a result of the posterior-to-anterior maturational pattern. We analyzed the topographical distribution of spindle parameters in HD-EEG polysomnographic sleep recordings of adolescents and found that slow spindle duration maxima traveled from central to anterior brain regions, while fast spindle density, amplitude and frequency peaks traveled from central to more posterior brain regions. These results provide evidence for the gradual posteriorization of the anatomical localization of fast sleep spindles during adolescence and indicate the existence of an anterior-to-posterior pattern of human brain maturation.
Subject terms: Sleep, Neuroscience, Non-REM sleep, Electroencephalography - EEG
Introduction
Details on the protracted maturational course of the human brain have been accumulating due to advanced brain imaging technologies1–3, and the characteristic back-to-front pattern has become a dominant presumption with respect to large-scale structural maturation4. According to the latest accounts of adolescent functional connectivity, the association areas or the so-called default mode network (DMN) of the brain are remodeled during development at an even later age than those areas that are more related to immediate sensory input and behavioral control5,6. While the latest maturing hot-spots in the network subserving “internal” cognition are believed to be fundamental to human evolution7–9, their exact connectivity and contribution to adolescent development is not known. We suggest that the investigation of spontaneous cortical activity in the absence of sensory stimulation, such as sleep, might help to move this field forward, and a more complete account of the developing large-scale functional networks will emerge.
The dynamics of large-scale brain networks are tightly linked with neural oscillatory activities. Sleep spindle oscillations are groups of 11–16 Hz waves emerging in nonrapid eye movement (NREM) sleep resulting from the rhythmic hyperpolarization rebound sequences of thalamocortical neurons10,11, the inhibition of which is caused by the NREM-dependent activation of GABAergic neurons in the reticular thalamic nucleus. Sleep spindles emerging from spontaneous neural activity during sleep reflect the underlying large-scale cortical functional networks12. A bimodal frequency and topography of sleep spindles has been described in adults. Slow (~ 12 Hz) sleep spindles are frontally dominant, whereas fast (~ 14 Hz) sleep spindles are of parietal/precuneal origin in adults13,14.
The precuneus is a major hub of the DMN, part of the complex neurocognitive network involved in the maintenance of conscious awareness, self-reflection, visuospatial integration and episodic memory9,15. Similar to the frontal cortex, precuneus expansion is a neurological specialization of Homo sapiens and is assumed to be associated with recent cognitive specializations16. The precuneus has been characterized by increasing functional segregation from the frontoparietal network between 8 and 26 years of age17. In addition to the late maturation of frontal lobe functions characterized by the well-known back-to-front maturational pattern of the brain2–4,18, the precuneus is also a subject of significant developmental changes in adolescence.
Based on the relatively late functional segregation of the precuneus from the frontoparietal network, as well as on the bimodal prefrontal versus precuneal sources of slow and fast sleep spindles in the adult brain, we postulate that sleep spindles undergo a topographical transformation characterized by increasing anatomical segregation during adolescent development. We assume that slow and fast sleep spindles are gradually relocated from the more central toward the prefrontal and parietal regions, respectively. Age-related topographical transfer is assumed to reflect the increasing differentiation and segregation of the frontal and precuneal subsystems in the DMN and the associated late development of phylogenetically new neuroanatomical structures in humans.
Here, we focus on the late maturation of slow frontal and fast parietal sleep spindle features in adolescence by using high-density electroencephalography (HD-EEG) recordings in three subgroups of different ages (12, 16 and 20 years). We hypothesize that the two types of sleep spindles index distinct neurodevelopmental processes during the course of adolescent brain maturation and functional specialization. We believe that focusing on the bimodal antero-posterior source and frequency distribution of sleep spindle oscillations13,14, contrasting the unimodal, frontal maxima of slow waves19,20 provides us with a better understanding of adolescent remodeling of the human brain.
Results
We recorded 128 channel full-night sleep HD-EEG data and detected individual-specific slow and fast spindles by the Individual Adjustment Method (IAM, see Methods/Procedure and ref.21). We analyzed the derivation-specific topographical distribution of the spindle parameters (density, duration, maximum amplitude and peak frequency) for NREM sleep. The distribution of the spindle parameters along the midline derivations (from Fpz to Iz) was calculated, and the maximum position of the distribution for every spindle parameter was determined. We scrutinized anterior–posterior shifts of the maximum locations as a function of age.
Here, we first describe the topographical distribution and age-related topographical changes of slow and fast spindle parameters (Figs. 1 and 2). Second, we present evidence for age-related antero-posterior shifts in sleep spindle parameters (Fig. 3).
Topographical distribution and age-related topographical changes of slow and fast spindle parameters
The age-dependent topographical distributions of slow and fast spindle parameters are shown in Figs. 1 and 2, respectively.
In terms of duration, the decrease in slow spindles from age 12 to 16 was Rüger area significant for all recording locations (T = (− 2.05–− 3.29), p = (0.002–0.04)). Maximum amplitude values of slow spindles decreased with frontal and frontopolar foci between all age groups from 12 to 20 years (T = (− 2.03–− 2.99), (p < 0.001–0.048)). Frequency increased Rüger area significantly at all derivations with a frontal focus throughout the entire adolescent period (T = (3.2–3.28), p = (0.002–0.003)), see Fig. 1.
Fast spindle density decreased Rüger area significantly at the perimeter of the scalp (T = (− 2.07–− 3.06), p = (0.04 – 0.004)). Fast spindles revealed a significant decrease in duration for all recording derivations over the entire adolescent period from age 12 to 20 (T = (− 2.06–− 4.54), (p < 0.001–0.047)). Fast spindle maximum amplitude values decreased significantly in all areas with a frontal and central focus (T = (− 2.05–− 4.54), (p < 0.001–0.048)). Fast spindle frequency values increased with age. The differences are Rüger area significant for the entire cortex in all age groups (T = (5.37–5.46), p < 0.001), see Fig. 2.
Age-related shifts in sleep spindle parameters
Topographical shifts of slow and fast spindle parameters can be visually assessed in the topographical distribution maps of Figs. 1 and 2, respectively. While slow spindle parameters tended to shift toward anterior electrode positions, fast sleep spindle measures seemed to shift toward posterior electrode positions with age. To more precisely quantify these observable tendencies, we calculated the distribution of spindle parameters along the midline derivations (from Fpz to Iz) and determined the maximum position of the distribution for every spindle parameter.
Maximum values of slow spindle duration shifted toward frontal electrode positions. The difference in the positions between the age groups of 12 and 16 years of age was statistically significant (F(1,38) = 4.3; p = 0.045; ANOVA). In terms of fast spindle density, the difference in positions of maximum values was significant in the age groups of 12 to 16 (F(1,38) = 11.27; p = 0.002; ANOVA) and 12 to 20 (F(1,38) = 23.84; p < 0.001; ANOVA), revealing a central to centroparietal shift with age. Maximum values of the positions of fast spindle maximum amplitudes shifted significantly from central to centroparietal electrode positions in the age groups of 16 to 20 (F(1,38) = 4.5; p = 0.04; ANOVA) and 12 to 20 (F(1,38) = 13.2; p < 0.001; ANOVA) but not between ages 12 to 16. Maximum values of the positions of fast spindle frequency also shifted significantly from central to centroparietal electrode positions in the age groups of 16 to 20 (F(1,38) = 11.76; p = 0.001; ANOVA) and 12 to 20 (F(1,38) = 9.77; p = 0.003; ANOVA) (Fig. 3). SI Fig. 1. shows age-related mean positions of the maxima of sleep spindle parameters along the antero-posterior axis in the midline EEG recording locations. Positions of the maxima of fast spindle density, maximum amplitude and frequency age-dependently relocate from central (12 years) to parietal (20 years) regions, whereas slow spindle duration express their maxima in more anterior regions in the 16 and 20 years old subjects as compared to the 12 years group (shifts forward from Fz to AFFz).
Discussion
Here, we report evidence for distinct shifts in the predominant location of slow and fast sleep spindle oscillations during adolescent development. We found evidence for slow sleep spindle duration maxima traveling from central to anterior brain regions and fast spindle density, amplitude and frequency peaks traveling from central to more posterior brain regions. These findings coincide with reports of an age-related increase in the functional segregation of the precuneus (a major source of fast sleep spindle oscillations in humans13,14) from the frontoparietal region from childhood to adulthood17.
In the late maturing frontal and parietal cortical areas, the short-range local connections are dominated by long-range functional networks during development22. In the course of adolescence, the anterior–posterior connectivity of the frontal and parietal lobes increases in the DMN23,24. Although functional connectivity may be reduced in the DMN in deep sleep stages25,26, it is preserved to such an extent that oscillatory activity-related functional connectivity between anterior and posterior areas corresponds to DMN characteristics during sleep spindling27.
Previous HD-EEG reports, based on sleep slow waves, have shown the anteriorization of sleep state-dependent neural oscillations during human ontogenesis (including the period of adolescence)18; however, age-related posteriorization of any frequency band has not been shown before. Thus, the late maturation of the frontal lobes is clearly supported by findings of former sleep neurophysiological studies, as well as our present report on slow sleep spindle oscillation; however, the phylogenetically new precuneal region has not yet been targeted by such studies. Although it could have been assumed that the late maturing precuneus is characterized by age-related posteriorization of sleep-state-specific oscillations, indexing the timing of developmental plasticity, such findings have been obscured by the insufficient frequency resolution of former reports.
Studies investigating the development of sleep spindles in children or adolescents do not cover the age-dependent topographical maturation of sleep spindles due to the low number of EEG electrodes28–30. Here, we use an individually adjusted frequency analysis of sleep spindle oscillations, with high frequency resolution and 128 channel HD-EEG recording, which is sufficiently sensitive to capture this process. Recent findings indicate the need of increased sensitivity and decreased specificity of sleep spindle detection procedures, as sleep spindles comprise a subset of a broader class of electroencephalogram events31. IAM was shown to be characterized by increased sensitivity and decreased specificity compared with other sleep spindle detection methods32. With these more sensitive methods, we have been able to show that individual-specific fast sleep spindle features unequivocally travel to more posterior areas during adolescence. Considering the precuneal origin of fast sleep spindles in adult human subjects13,14, it seems that individualized sleep spindle analysis is a forceful tool to detect the dominant anatomical directions of brain maturation in humans.
Prevailing concepts of complex neural development and brain maturation emphasize the posterior-to-anterior or back-to-front pattern2–4 as an unequivocal ontogenetic feature, finding its roots in the late maturing frontal lobes. The back-to-front pattern has received substantial support from HD-EEG studies examining the age-related changes in the topography of sleep slow waves18. However, the above scenario of unidirectional developmental and anatomical route disregards the major hub of the DMN, which is a human-specific and late maturing neural structure of the brain, undergoing significant differentiation during adolescence. In addition to slow waves, sleep spindles are the major hallmarks of NREM sleep, known to be involved in neural plasticity with major sources in the precuneus, at least in healthy adult human subjects. Here, we provide evidence for a backward move in the anatomical localization of fast-type sleep spindles; that is, the predominantly central maxima in density and amplitude at the age of 12 years gradually become replaced with parietal electrode locations by the age of 20 years. Similar changes, albeit at a smaller anatomical distance, take place in terms of fast sleep spindle frequency. These findings complete the back-to-front pattern of brain maturation and cohere with reports of increased differentiation of the precuneus during adolescence. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first report of an age-related front-to-back transfer of the maxima of a neural plasticity-related sleep EEG index.
Direct cortical records of sleep spindles in children of prepubertal/early pubertal ages revealed prevailing perirolandic sources33 potentially indicating the initial maturational stage before the accomplishment of the topographical relocation described in our present study. Furthermore, the findings of Zerouali et al. (2014)34 indicate that precuneal, posterior sources are potential initiators of overlapping fast and slow sleep spindle complexes, providing a leading role of this area in off-line, adult-type distributed neural network processing reflected by such overlapping wave sequences. Although we did not specifically analyze spindle co-occurrences in our study, such analyses could reveal the dynamic properties of topographical distinction and perhaps cortical maturation and specialization during adolescent development. We have to note however, that according to earlier studies the subpopulation of overlapping slow and fast sleep spindles represent only 5% of all spindles35. We aim to elucidate these details in our future studies.
Apparently, adolescence is a timeframe of significant changes in the EEG spectra, including shifts in sleep spindle characteristics28–30,36. Sleep spindles have outstanding relevance from a clinical point of view since adolescence-related psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia37, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)38 and depression39 have all been associated with anomalies in sleep spindles. It has also been suggested that sleep spindle abnormalities are an endophenotype of schizophrenia37,40. Moreover, these disorders can also be characterized by cognitive impairments41–43. It has been demonstrated that sleep spindles and cognitive function are strongly associated44,45, and as such, sleep spindle abnormalities might provide a key to interpreting cognitive malfunction10. Further investigating the mechanisms and developmental trajectory of sleep spindles in adolescence could lead to a better understanding of the aforementioned disorders.
Although genetic factors play a pivotal role in shaping the individual-specific parameters of slow and fast sleep spindle frequency activities46 and sleep spindles themselves47, we were able to detect meaningful age group effects, suggesting that the individualization of the parameters (IAM) combined with HD-EEG provides a powerful strategy in capturing developmental processes in the sleep neurophysiology domain. The reported differences in the contributions of genetic factors to slow and fast sleep spindle parameters are controversial; however, the almost perfect night-to-night stability of these measures is unequivocal47,48. That is, cross sectional studies are plausible approaches in unravelling the maturational routes in sleep spindles. Complementing the inferences of a cross-sectional study with inferences from a longitudinal design could map out a more precise developmental trajectory and further reduce the bias from individual differences.
A limitation of the current study is that analysis of EEG data can only provide indirect evidence for the precuneal origin of fast spindles. Source localization analysis has been beyond the scope of the current study, but applying such techniques would provide more convincing evidence for the link of the topographical shift of fast spindle frequency peaks and the ontogenetic development of the precuneus.
In conclusion, we focused on the late maturation of individually adjusted slow-frontal and fast-parietal sleep spindle features in adolescence by using HD-EEG recordings. Our hypothesis assuming that the two sleep spindle types might index distinct neurodevelopmental processes during the course of adolescent brain maturation and functional specialization has been confirmed by the data. We are convinced that focusing on the bimodal antero-posterior source and frequency distribution of sleep spindle oscillations has taken us closer to a better understanding of adolescent remodeling of the human brain.
Methods
Participants
We recruited 60 adolescent and young adult participants (30 females and 30 males, mean age 16.55 ± SD 3.70) via social media in three age groups, with an equal number of females and males in each age group: 12-year-olds (n = 20, mean age = 12.45 ± SD 0.57 years), 16-year-olds (n = 20, mean age = 15.91 ± SD 0.48 years), and young adults referred to as 20-year-olds (n = 20, mean age = 21.29 ± SD 0.51 years). Subjects with an existing neurological condition or sleep disorder were not included in the study. All subjects received a voucher in the value of HUF 20,000 (cca. USD 70.00) for their participation. The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Pázmány Péter Catholic University for Psychological Experiments. Adult participants and parents or legal guardians of the underage participants signed informed consent for the participation in the study according to the Declaration of Helsinki.
Procedure
We performed 128 channel HD-EEG polysomnographic (PSG) sleep recordings of two consecutive nights in the Sleep Laboratory of Pázmány Péter Catholic University Budapest. We requested participants to maintain a regular sleep–wake schedule for 5 nights before recordings, but we did not monitor compliance. We asked participants not to consume any drugs other than contraceptives and coffee and to refrain from napping on the afternoons of the days of the experiment. Participants with any history of sleep problems or mental disorder or any neurological or medical condition were excluded. We verified such criteria by personal interviews and questionnaires completed by the parents or young adult participants themselves. We applied electrodes for electroencephalography (EEG), electromyography (EMG) and electrooculography (EOG). After checking the electrodes for skin contact and impedance levels, all participants went to sleep in the laboratory between 10.00 p.m. and 11.30 p.m. according to their preference. They slept until they awoke spontaneously.
We used a built-in EEG channel-Quick Cap (Compumedics, Australia) with 128 electrodes in three different head sizes to record EEG signals. We roughly evenly spaced the passive Ag/AgCl electrodes down to line T9-Iz-T10. The monopolar channels were referenced to a frontocentrally placed ground electrode. We applied bipolar EOG and EMG channels to measure eye movements and muscle tone, respectively, and placed the EOG channels below and to the left of the left eye (1 cm below the left outer canthus) and above and to the right of the right eye (1 cm above the right outer canthus). We determined the locations of the EMG electrodes in accordance with the recommendations of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM)49. We recorded the data using a BQ KIT SD LTM 128 EXPRESS (2 X 64 channels in master and slave mode, Micromed, Mogliano Veneto, Italy) recording device and subsequently visualized, saved and exported them using System Plus Evolution software (Micromed, Mogliano Veneto, Italy). We recorded all HD-EEG, EOG and EMG data with an effective sampling rate of synchronous 4096 Hz/channel with 22 bit equivalent resolution and prefiltered and amplified them with 40 dB/decade high- and low-pass input filters of 0.15–250 Hz. We applied a digital low pass filter at 231.7 Hz and downsampled the data to 512 Hz/channel by firmware.
From the recorded nighttime EEG recordings, the current analysis deals with the second night data as that was less impacted by adaptation effects. We manually scored sleep states of 20 s epochs of whole night NREM EEG recordings by visual inspection according to standardized criteria50 and revised hypnograms. Then, we removed artifacts on four-second long epochs and scanned the whole-night recordings four times to overcome the limitations of the capacity of the visual inspector (only 32 channels of the 128 were visually inspected in a run), and finally, we excluded any epoch marked as artifact in any run from further analysis.
We detected individual-specific slow and fast spindles by using the Individual Adjustment Method (IAM)21. The theoretical background of the IAM relies on the thesis that sleep spindles are the wave segments which make up the individual-specific spectral peaks in the NREM sleep EEG spectra. After removing sleep spindles detected by the IAM, the spectra of the non-spindle parts of NREM sleep EEG is of aperiodic, fractal-type nature, with no spectral peaks in the 9–16 Hz range. The other theoretical point of IAM finds its roots in the reports indicating spatially non-continuous antero-posterior changes in sleep spindle frequencies. As regarding the details of the IAM, the latter consists of the following main steps:
Calculating the amplitude spectra of artifact-free NREM sleep EEG by FFT for all available EEG locations.
Calculating the second derivatives of the 9–16 Hz spectra at all EEG recording locations and averaging them over locations (this results in one averaged second derivative).
Selecting the individual slow and fast sleep spindle boundaries by looking for zero crossings of the averaged second derivative surrounding negative peaks (positive peaks are turned to negative in the second derivatives).
Defining individual- and EEG location-specific amplitude criteria of slow and fast sleep spindles by linearly interpolating the amplitude spectra between the specific frequencies determined above.
Band-pass filtering the artifact-free MREM sleep EEG time series at individual-specific slow and fast sleep spindles separately, determining the envelopes of the slow- and fast sleep spindle filtered signals and applying the above determined amplitude criteria for slow and fast sleep spindles at each EEG location.
The individual fast and slow spindle frequency ranges and the midpoints of the frequency ranges for the 60 subjects included in the analyses are illustrated in SI Fig. 2. A scheme of spindle detection is provided in Supplementary Fig. 3. In fact, these segments contribute to the individual- and derivation-specific lower- and higher-frequency spectral peaks between 9 and 16 Hz. Based on the IAM approach, we determined individual and derivation-specific densities (spindles × min − 1), durations (s), and amplitudes (µV) of slow, frontally dominant and fast, centro-parietally dominant sleep spindles.
We determined the peak frequency using the center of gravity (CoG) of the individualized spectral peak frequencies in the spindle range (11–16 Hz). We obtained values of CoG by using the method described in the study conducted by Dustman51 In general the CoG is the average location or the balance point of all the weight of an object. In our case it is the average frequency location of the sleep spindle activity. It is calculated based on the spectrum of the EEG. CoG frequency for a given sleep spindle type (slow or fast) of a given channel of a given subject is calculated by:
where MinFreq equals to the lower threshold (rounded up to the nearest 1/16 Hz) and MaxFreq equals to the upper threshold (rounded down to nearest 1/16 Hz) of the individual frequency range of the given spindle type. The step size in the sum is 0.0625 Hz (1/16 Hz) due to the resolution of the spectrum. Ampn equals to the amplitude of the spectrum at frequency n of the given channel of the given subject and it represents the weights in the equation. We excluded outliers from the original data set by using Tukey's fences method52. This method is based on the quartiles of the data, the outliers are determined by the interquartile ranges. Let Q1 be the lower quartile and Q3 the upper quartile. The values outside the range [Q1-k(Q3-Q1), Q3 + k(Q3-Q1)] are considered outliers. The value of k is traditionally chosen to be 1.5 (outlier) or 3 (far out). We used the more stringent value of 1.5 in our analysis.
We analyzed the derivation-specific topographical distribution of spindle parameters and their age-dependent differences. We used a t-test to compare the activity of the corresponding electrodes between each group. In order to provide a control of multiple testing and the consequent inflation of type I error, the procedure of descriptive data analysis53 adapted to quantified neurophysiology with mapping54 was applied. This procedure tests the global null hypothesis (“all individual null hypotheses in the respective region are true”) at level α = 0.05, against the alternative that at least one of the null hypotheses is wrong. According to Abt53 and Duffy et al.54 local, uncorrected and spatially contiguous significances at the level of α = 0.05 (descriptive significances) define the Rüger’s areas55. Areas with fewer than three adjacent derivations are not included in the Rüger significant regions. If N is the number of electrodes in the Rüger’s area, the investigator is required to choose a minimal number of unspecified null hypotheses (M), less than N, to be nominally rejected at a new, more conservative α level. Typically the value M/N is 1/2 or 1/3. The corresponding new α levels for these values are α/2 = 0.025 and α/3 = 0.017, respectively. We will use M/N values of 1/2 and 1/3 with corresponding new α levels of 0.025 and 0.017, respectively. If any M values (half of the t-tests if M/N = 1/2 and third of the t-tests if M/N = 1/3) within the Rüger’s area individually reach the new α level of significance (0.025 and 0.017, respectively) the overall null hypothesis is rejected for the Rüger’s area at the 0.05 level. This means that for at least one EEG derivation in the Rüger’s area the relationship is significant, allowing the investigator to make global confirmatory statement with controlled uncertainty.
Finally, we calculated the distribution of the spindle parameters along the midline derivations (from Fpz to Iz) and determined the maximum position of the distribution for every spindle parameter. We examined the anterior–posterior shifts of the maximum locations as a function of age. Statistical comparisons were conducted by one-way ANOVA models (one for each spindle parameter) with the anteroposterior location of the spindle parameter as the dependent variable and age group as the categorical independent variable.
For the processing of sleep data (sleep state classification, artifact rejection and spindle detection with IAM), we used the FerciosEEGPlus 1.3 program (http://www.gomboshaz.hu/fercioseegplus.htm). We used Microsoft Excel for the calculation of CoG values and Rüger area corrections. We conducted all statistical tests using TIBCO STATISTICA 13.5.0.17 software (TIBCO Software Inc., 2018).
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by the Hungarian National Research, Development and Innovation Office grants NK-104481 and K-134370 to I.K. and K-128117 to B.R. and by the Higher Education Institutional Excellence Program of the Ministry of Human Capacities in Hungary within the framework of the neurology thematic program of Semmelweis University. The authors thank T. Jáger for generating the figures. We also thank the adolescents and young adults who participated in this project and Zoltán György who assisted at all sleep recordings.
Author contributions
I.K., F.G. and R.B. designed the experiments; F.G., A.P. and G.B. analyzed the data; all authors contributed to writing the paper.
Funding
Open access funding provided by Pázmány Péter Catholic University.
Data availability
The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Ferenc Gombos and Róbert Bódizs.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-022-11098-8.
References
- 1.Paus T, et al. Maturation of white matter in the human brain: a review of magnetic resonance studies. Brain Res. Bull. 2001;54:255–266. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(00)00434-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Giedd JN. Structural Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the Adolescent Brain. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2004;1021:77–85. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Sowell ER, et al. Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat. Neurosci. 2003;6:309–315. doi: 10.1038/nn1008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Lenroot RK, Giedd JN. Brain development in children and adolescents: insights from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2006;30:718–729. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Váša F, et al. Conservative and disruptive modes of adolescent change in human brain functional connectivity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2020;117:3248–3253. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1906144117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Whitaker KJ, et al. Adolescence is associated with genomically patterned consolidation of the hubs of the human brain connectome. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2016;113:9105–9110. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1601745113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Buckner RL, Krienen FM. The evolution of distributed association networks in the human brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2013;17:648–665. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Uddin LQ, Yeo BTT, Spreng RN. Towards a Universal Taxonomy of Macro-scale Functional Human Brain Networks. Brain Topogr. 2019;32:926–942. doi: 10.1007/s10548-019-00744-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Cavanna AE, Trimble MR. The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain. 2006;129:564–583. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Fernandez LMJ, Lüthi A. Sleep spindles: Mechanisms and functions. Physiol. Rev. 2020;100:805–868. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00042.2018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kane N, Acharya J, Beniczky S, et al. A revised glossary of terms most commonly used by clinical electroencephalographers and update proposal for the report format of the EEG findings. Revision 2017. Clin. Neurophysiol. Pract. 2017;2:170–185. doi: 10.1016/j.cnp.2017.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.McVea, D. A., Murphy, T. H. & Mohajerani, M. H. Large scale cortical functional networks associated with slow-wave and spindle-burst-related spontaneous activity. Front. Neural Circuits10, 103 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 13.Anderer P, et al. Low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography revealed simultaneously active frontal and parietal sleep spindle sources in the human cortex. Neuroscience. 2001;103:581–592. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00028-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Alfonsi, V. et al. Spatiotemporal dynamics of sleep spindle sources across NREM Sleep Cycles. Front. Neurosci.13, 727 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 15.Vogt, B. A. & Laureys, S. Posterior cingulate, precuneal and retrosplenial cortices: cytology and components of the neural network correlates of consciousness, in Progress in Brain Research (ed. Laureys, S.) vol. 150, pp. 205–217 (Elsevier, 2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 16.Bruner E, Preuss TM, Chen X, Rilling JK. Evidence for expansion of the precuneus in human evolution. Brain Struct. Funct. 2017;222:1053–1060. doi: 10.1007/s00429-015-1172-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Li R, et al. Developmental maturation of the precuneus as a functional core of the default mode network. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2019;31:1506–1519. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kurth S, et al. Mapping of cortical activity in the first two decades of life: A high-density sleep electroencephalogram study. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:13211–13219. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2532-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Cajochen C, Foy R, Dijk DJ. Frontal predominance of a relative increase in sleep delta and theta EEG activity after sleep loss in humans. Sleep Res. Online SRO. 1999;2:65–69. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Finelli LA, Borbély AA, Achermann P. Functional topography of the human nonREM sleep electroencephalogram. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2001;13:2282–2290. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01597.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bódizs R, Körmendi J, Rigó P, Lázár AS. The individual adjustment method of sleep spindle analysis: Methodological improvements and roots in the fingerprint paradigm. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2009;178:205–213. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Fair DA, et al. Functional Brain Networks Develop from a “Local to Distributed” Organization. PLOS Comput. Biol. 2009;5:e1000381. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Sato JR, et al. Age effects on the default mode and control networks in typically developing children. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2014;58:89–95. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Sherman LE, et al. Development of the default mode and central executive networks across early adolescence: A longitudinal study. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2014;10:148–159. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Horovitz SG, et al. Decoupling of the brain’s default mode network during deep sleep. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2009;106:11376–11381. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0901435106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Koike T, Kan S, Misaki M, Miyauchi S. Connectivity pattern changes in default-mode network with deep non-REM and REM sleep. Neurosci. Res. 2011;69:322–330. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2010.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Fang Z, et al. Sleep spindle-dependent functional connectivity correlates with cognitive abilities. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2020;32:446–466. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Zhang ZY, Campbell IG, Dhayagude P, Espino HC, Feinberg I. Longitudinal Analysis of Sleep Spindle Maturation from Childhood through Late Adolescence. J. Neurosci. 2021;41:4253–4261. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2370-20.2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Goldstone A, et al. Sleep spindle characteristics in adolescents. Clin. Neurophysiol. Off. J. Int. Fed. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2019;130:893–902. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.02.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Purcell SM, et al. Characterizing sleep spindles in 11,630 individuals from the National Sleep Research Resource. Nat. Commun. 2017;8:15930. doi: 10.1038/ncomms15930. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Dimitrov, T., He, M., Stickgold, R. & Prerau, M.J. Sleep spindles comprise a subset of a broader class of electroencephalogram events. Sleep, 44/9:zsab099(2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 32.Ujma, P. P. et al. A comparison of two sleep spindle detection methods based on all night averages: individually adjusted vs. fixed frequencies. Front. Hum. Neurosci.9, 52 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 33.Pinto ALR, et al. Localization of Sleep Spindles, K-Complexes, and Vertex Waves With Subdural Electrodes in Children. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2014;31:367–374. doi: 10.1097/WNP.0000000000000071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Zerouali, Y. et al. A time-frequency analysis of the dynamics of cortical networks of sleep spindles from MEG-EEG recordings. Front. Neurosci.8, 310 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 35.Merica H. Fast and slow frequency spindles in sleep: Two generators? Clin. Neurophysiol. 2000;111(9):1704–1705. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00374-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Tarokh L, Van Reen E, LeBourgeois M, Seifer R, Carskadon MA. Sleep EEG provides evidence that cortical changes persist into late adolescence. Sleep. 2011;34:1385–1393. doi: 10.5665/SLEEP.1284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Manoach DS, Pan JQ, Purcell SM, Stickgold R. Reduced sleep spindles in schizophrenia: A treatable endophenotype that links risk genes to impaired cognition? Biol. Psychiatry. 2016;80:599–608. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Merikanto I, et al. ADHD symptoms are associated with decreased activity of fast sleep spindles and poorer procedural overnight learning during adolescence. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2019;157:106–113. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2018.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Plante DT, et al. Topographic and sex-related differences in sleep spindles in major depressive disorder: A high-density EEG investigation. J. Affect. Disord. 2013;146:120–125. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.06.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Merikanto, I. et al. Genetic risk factors for schizophrenia associate with sleep spindle activity in healthy adolescents. J. Sleep Res.28:e12762 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 41.Göder R, et al. Impairment of sleep-related memory consolidation in schizophrenia: relevance of sleep spindles? Sleep Med. 2015;16:564–569. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.12.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Loyer Carbonneau M, Demers M, Bigras M, Guay M-C. Meta-analysis of sex differences in ADHD symptoms and associated cognitive deficits. J. Atten. Disord. 2020 doi: 10.1177/1087054720923736. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Rock PL, Roiser JP, Riedel WJ, Blackwell AD. Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 2014;44:2029–2040. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Hahn M, et al. Developmental changes of sleep spindles and their impact on sleep-dependent memory consolidation and general cognitive abilities: A longitudinal approach. Dev. Sci. 2019;22:e12706. doi: 10.1111/desc.12706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Reynolds CM, Short MA, Gradisar M. Sleep spindles and cognitive performance across adolescence: A meta-analytic review. J. Adolesc. 2018;66:55–70. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2018.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.De Gennaro L, Ferrara M, Vecchio F, Curcio G, Bertini M. An electroencephalographic fingerprint of human sleep. Neuroimage. 2005;26:114–122. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Adamczyk, M., Genzel, L., Dresler, M., Steiger, A. & Friess, E. Automatic sleep spindle detection and genetic influence estimation using continuous wavelet transform. Front. Hum. Neurosci.9, article 624 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 48.De Gennaro L, et al. The electroencephalographic fingerprint of sleep is genetically determined: A twin study. Ann. Neurol. 2008;64:455–460. doi: 10.1002/ana.21434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Iber, C., Ancoli-Israel, S., Chesson Jr., A. L. & Quan, S. F. The AASM Manual for Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events Rules, Terminology and Technical Specifications. (American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2007).
- 50.AASM Scoring Manual - American Academy of Sleep Medicine. American Academy of Sleep Medicine—Association for Sleep Clinicians and Researchershttps://aasm.org/clinical-resources/scoring-manual/.
- 51.Dustman RE, Shearer DE, Emmerson RY. Life-span changes in EEG spectral amplitude, amplitude variability and mean frequency. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1999;110:1399–1409. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(99)00102-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Tukey JW. Exploratory Data Analysis. Addison-Wesley Publishing Company; 1977. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Abt K. Descriptive data analysis: A concept between confirmatory and exploratory data analysis. Methods Inf. Med. 1987;26:77–88. doi: 10.1055/s-0038-1635488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Duffy FH, et al. Quantified Neurophysiology with mapping: Statistical inference, Exploratory and Confirmatory data analysis. Brain Topogr. 1990;3:3–12. doi: 10.1007/BF01128856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Rüger, B. Das maximale signifikanzniveau des Tests: “LehneHo ab, wennk untern gegebenen tests zur ablehnung führen” Metrika25, 171–178 (1978).
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on request.