Abstract
Background:
The intellectually demanding modern workplace is often dependent on good cognitive health, yet there is little understanding of how neurocognitive dysfunction related to HIV presents in employed individuals working in high risk vocations like driving. HIV-associated neurocognitive impairment is also associated with poorer long term cognitive, health and employment outcomes.
Setting:
This study, set in Cape Town, South Africa, assessed the effects of HIV on neuropsychological test performance in employed male professional drivers.
Method:
We administered a neuropsychological test battery spanning seven cognitive domains and obtained behavioral data, anthropometry, and medical biomarkers from three groups of professional drivers (68 men with HIV, 55 men with cardiovascular risk, and 81 controls). We compared the drivers’ cognitive profiles, and used multiple regression modelling to investigate whether between-group differences persisted after considering potentially confounding sociodemographic and clinical variables (i.e., income, home language, depression, and Framingham Risk Score).
Results:
Relative to other study participants, professional drivers with HIV performed significantly more poorly on tests assessing processing speed (p<.003) and attention and working memory (p=.018). Group membership remained a predictor of cognitive performance after controlling for potential confounders. The cognitive deficits observed in men with HIV were, however, largely characterized as being mild or asymptomatic. Consistent with this characterization, their relatively poor performance on neuropsychological testing did not generalize to self-reported impairment on activities of daily living.
Conclusion:
Drivers with HIV may be at risk for poorer long-term health and employment outcomes. Programs that monitor and support their long-term cognitive health are needed.
Keywords: Cognition, Automobile Driving, HIV-associated Neurocognitive Disorders, Occupational Health, Activities of Daily Living, Cardiovascular Disease
INTRODUCTION
Chronic diseases such as HIV, diabetes mellitus type 2, and hypertension are often associated with cognitive dysfunction1–4. Such dysfunction may lead to premature socioeconomic inactivity, particularly in professions like vocational driving, in which these medical conditions appear commonly (probably due to vocation-related lifestyle factors such as being away from home for extended periods, poor diet, and sitting for long stretches of time)5–7. For professional drivers, cognitive dysfunction also confers an increased safety risk with potentially harsh consequences.
During the Fourth Industrial Revolution8, rapidly evolving workplaces frequently present complex and intellectually demanding challenges9. Task outcomes are often dependent on good mental health, which therefore plays a critical role in the ability to perform work-related duties10,11. Nonetheless, there is little understanding of how, for instance, HIV-related cognitive dysfunction might present in actively-employed people and how it might affect current and future work products. Moreover, few guidelines describe how to identify (e.g., via screening instruments) and manage HIV+ employees with symptoms of cognitive impairment12. Such guidelines are strikingly absent despite the well-documented neurocognitive effects associated with HIV13.
HIV-associated neurocognitive impairment (HNCI) or HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder is observed in 15%- 55% of people with HIV (PWH)14,15 and prevalence rates as high as 70% have been reported in sub-Saharan Africa16,17. Dysfunction can range from mild (asymptomatic neurocognitive impairment and mild neurocognitive disorder) to severe (HIV-associated dementia), with effects across the domains of motor functioning, processing speed, attention, language, memory, and executive functioning2,15,18,19. Most people with HNCI who are virally suppressed and on antiretroviral treatment (ART) remain cognitively stable20. They also remain in the workplace longer21–23. In fact, many people with mild HNCI maintain steady employment24. However, the health, medical, and functional consequences of even mild HNCI can be significant, and therefore people who experience the condition are more likely to have difficulty completing work-related activities than those who do not25–28. Of pertinence to this paper are studies showing that HNCI can impact driving ability adversely29–38.
In people with diabetes and hypertension (both risk factors for cardiovascular disease), relatively subtle and slowly progressive cognitive decrements occur at all ages1,39. Diabetes increases the risk of mild cognitive impairment, and in older adults is associated with increased risk of Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias40–43. In diabetics, mild impairment is observed in motor function, processing speed, memory, and executive function39,44–49. Progression of cognitive decline can mirror normal ageing or can occur up to 50% faster than that43,50,51. Diabetes can also impact driving performance52,53. Hypertension, the leading risk factor for stroke and a well-established risk factor for vascular cognitive impairment, is associated with greater incidence of mild cognitive impairment (mostly in the domains of processing speed and executive function), a relatively steep gradient of age-related cognitive decline, and dementia1,54–58. Few studies have investigated the effects of hypertension on driving performance despite relatively high rates of the condition in professional drivers59. However, a study of non-professional older drivers suggested that people with hypertension may not experience more driving difficulty, but that they do reduce their frequency of driving compared to healthy peers60.
In many jurisdictions, fitness to drive is a regulatory requirement for professional drivers. Such fitness is a public health concern: Because those drivers are on the road for extended periods and because of the characteristics of their vehicles (e.g., petrochemical trucks, buses), there is a high risk of third-party harm if they drive unsafely. No previous studies of PWH have used a cohort that was uniformly employed in a reasonably cognitive demanding profession such as driving, or examined the relative risk of impairment in PWH compared to other conditions, such as diabetes and hypertension, with high prevalence in the same profession.
The current study examined, using a sample of actively employed professional drivers, the relative risk of cognitive impairment (and hence potentially reduced driving performance) in men with HIV (MWH) compared to that in men with cardiovascular risk factors (MCVR; diabetes mellitus type 2 and/or hypertension) and in controls. We hypothesised that neuropsychological test performance would be worst among MWH, and that MCVR would also perform more poorly than controls. We also measured, within each clinical group, associations between sociodemographic/clinical risk factors and neuropsychological test performance.
METHOD
Participants
This study is nested within a research program assessing effects of HNCI on driving performance in professional drivers from South Africa. Data were collected between August 2017 and March 2020. Convenience and snowball sampling recruited male professional drivers from occupational and primary healthcare clinics, a mobile-wellness clinic for truckers, an HIV-patient health management company, and social media platforms. The final sample comprised 204 participants (68 MWH, 55 MCVR [32 with hypertension, 23 with diabetes], 81 controls).
Inclusion criteria were: ≥1 year employment as a professional driver; ≥12 hours of professional driving per week; age ≥18 years; English fluency; and a valid South African professional driver’s permit. For the two clinical groups, (1) MWH had to have a confirmed prior diagnosis of HIV (we did not exclude MWH who also had cardiovascular risk factors); and (2) MCVR had to have a confirmed prior diagnosis of diabetes and/or hypertension (drivers with both diabetes and hypertension were classified as diabetic), and a HIV-negative status confirmed via ELISA finger prick test. Participants with HIV or hypertension were required to have initiated treatment ≥3 months prior to study enrolment.
Exclusion criteria were: history of non-HIV-related neurological disorder or medical disorder affecting the nervous system (e.g., stroke, epilepsy, or head injury with consequent hospitalization and/or loss of consciousness for ≥30 minutes); presence of an Axis I DSM disorder, excluding major depressive disorder (due to the high prevalence of depressive symptoms in professional drivers and in PWH61,62); self-reported history of learning disability; self-reported diagnosis of diabetes mellitus type 1; and current substance abuse or dependence, assessed using the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT; cut-off score ≥8)63, and a five-panel urine toxicology screen for tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA), cocaine, opioids, and amphetamines. Participants who tested positive for THC were only excluded if they had used marijuana within the previous 24 hours.
Additional exclusion criteria for the control group were: self-reported diagnosis of diabetes; self-reported prior prescription for hypertension medication or a blood pressure measure by research staff ≥140/90 mmHg64; and HIV-negative status confirmed via ELISA finger prick test on the research day.
Our institution’s Human Research Ethics Committee approved this study. Participants provided written informed consent and were compensated the equivalent of US$40.
Measures and Procedure
A psychometric technician administered the study measures. We used standard versions of all tests except the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R); in that case, we used a culturally adapted version that combines items from Forms 1 and 4 of the original test65,66.
Test administrators and scorers were trained, supervised, and monitored by two clinical neuropsychologists (HG, CJM).
Demographic measures
Study-specific questionnaires gathered demographic information (e.g., regarding socioeconomic status and occupational history) and enquired about neuromedical history (e.g., cognitive changes and neurological symptoms).
Anthropometry and medical biomarkers
On the day of the research visit, we collected blood for lipid testing (including total cholesterol, HDL, LDL, and triglycerides), plasma viral load and CD4 count (for MWH), and glycated hemoglobin A1c (for diabetics); measured blood pressure, waist circumference, hip circumference, and weight; and administered a cotinine test (to confirm smoking status) and a five-panel urine toxicology screen. We calculated a Framingham Risk Score (FRS; 10-year cardiovascular risk prediction score informed by the D’Agostino et al, [2008]67 equation) using the calculator available at https://framinghamheartstudy.org/fhs-risk-functions/cardiovascular-disease-10-year-risk/ 68.
Behavioral scales
The Beck Depression Inventory-II assessed self-reported severity of depressive symptomatology69. The Patient’s Assessment of Own Functioning (PAOFI) assessed self-reported functioning in the domains of memory, language and communication, use of hands, sensory-perception, and higher-level cognitive and intellectual functions70. We used the Woods et al. (2004)71 guidelines to calculate everyday functional ability. An endorsement of “fairly often” through “almost always” on ≥3 questions within any domain was taken as an indication of self-reported cognitive difficulties.
Neuropsychological assessment
A neuropsychological test battery assessed performance on 16 measures, across seven cognitive domains: motor function (indexed by completion time on the Grooved Pegboard Test [GPT] dominant and non-dominant hands); processing speed (Trail Making Test Part A [TMT-A], completion time; Color Trails Test [CTT1], completion time; Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition [WAIS-III] Digit Symbol Coding, total score; WAIS-III Symbol Search, total score); attention/working memory (Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition [WMS-III] Spatial Span, total score; WAIS-III Digit Span, total score;); language (category fluency, total number of animals/total number of fruits and vegetables named in 1 minute); learning (HVLT-R, total learning; Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised [BVMT-R], total learning); memory (HVLT-R, delayed recall total; BVMT-R, delayed recall total); and executive functioning (CTT2, completion time; Wisconsin Card Sorting Test [WCST], total correct).
This battery has demonstrated evidence of psychometric validity in South Africa72.
Statistical Analyses
We used RStudio (version 1.2.5019), R (version R-4.0.3), and SPSS (version 27.0). The threshold for statistical significance was set at α=.05. Effect size estimates (ESE) were calculated for each analysis. Specifically, we used Cramer’s V for chi-square tests and partial eta squared [ηp2] for ANOVAs. Interpretation of effect sizes followed convention: For Cramer’s V, small effect size ≤ 0.2; medium 0.2 to ≤ 0.6; and large > 0.6; for ηp2, small <.06; medium .06 to .14; and large ≥.1473.
First, one-way ANOVAs (for continuous variables) and chi-square tests (for categorical variables) investigated between-group (controls, MWH and MCVR) differences regarding participant sociodemographic and clinical characteristics. Where appropriate, we followed up with post-hoc pairwise comparisons using Tukey’s Honestly Significant Difference74. The purpose here was to identify potential confounders that would need to be controlled for in subsequent analyses.
Second, we processed the neuropsychological test data. For each outcome variable, the raw score was transformed into a standardized z-score (mean [M] = 0, standard deviation [SD] = 1) using existing regression-based norms75. Scores were modified so that lower totals indicated poorer performance on all tests. Z-scores were then converted to T-scores (M = 50, SD = 10). An average domain T-score was calculated by taking the mean of all T-scores within each domain. A global T-score was calculated by taking the mean across domain T-scores. A global deficit score (GDS) was calculated by, first, converting each T-score to a deficit score following these guidelines: T > 39 = 0 (normal); T ≥ 35 – 39 = 1 (mild impairment); T ≥ 30 – 34 = 2 (mild-to-moderate impairment); T ≥ 25 – 29 = 3 (moderate impairment); T ≥ 20 – 24 = 4 (moderate-to-severe impairment); T < 20 = 5 (severe impairment). Then, the sum of the deficit scores was divided by the number of tests to compute the overall GDS76. Thus, lower T-scores and higher GDS scores indicate more severe impairment.
Third, multiple linear regression models assessed the influence group status had on cognitive outcomes after controlling for the potential confounders identified earlier. For each model, the outcome variable was a domain T-score, the global T-score, or the GDS.
Finally, chi-square tests (initially comparing all three groups, and following up with pairwise comparisons where appropriate) determined between-group differences in proportion of participants classed as showing cognitive impairment on each outcome measure, with the threshold for such impairment set at z <−1.00 and at GDS ≥0.5.
RESULTS
Sample Sociodemographic and Clinical Characteristics
Analyses detected significant between-group differences with regard to age and monthly income, but not education (see Table 1). On average, MWH and controls were significantly younger than MCVR (p=.001 and .008, respectively), but controls and MWH were similarly aged (p=.417). MWH had a significantly lower monthly income than MCVR and controls (p=.001 and <.001, respectively), with no significant difference between the latter two groups (p=.566).
Table 1.
Controls (n = 81) |
MWH (n = 68) |
MCVR (n = 55) |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Continuous Variables | M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | F | df | p | ESE d |
| |||||||
Age (yrs) | 40.85 (10.50) | 39.58 (9.05) | 45.27 (8.14) | 5.96 | 2,201 | .003** | .056 |
Education (yrs completed) | 11.14 (1.27) | 11.07 (1.42) | 10.85 (1.76) | 0.63 | 2,201 | .532 | .006 |
Monthly income (ZAR) | 12 959.63 (6 133.22) | 7 957.58 (4 736.67) b | 13 456.60 (6 164.77) c | 17.28 | 2,198 | < .001*** | .149 |
| |||||||
Categorical Variables | f (%) | f (%) | f (%) | χ2 | df | p | ESE e |
| |||||||
Home language | 70.27 | 8 | < .001*** | .415 | |||
English | 15 (18.5%) | 2 (2.9%) | 15 (27.2%) | - | |||
Afrikaans | 28 (34.6%) | 1 (1.5%) | 19 (34.5%) | - | |||
Xhosa | 23 (28.4%) | 57 (83.8%) | 11 (20%) | - | |||
Shona | 10(12.3%) | 5 (7.4%) | 7 (12.7%) | - | |||
Other | 5 (6.2%) | 3 (4.4%) | 3 (5.5%) | - | |||
Schooling language a | 44.89 | 8 | < .001*** | .352 | |||
English | 30 (48.4%) | 37 (54.4%) | 25 (49%) | - | |||
Afrikaans | 24 (38.7%) | 1 (1.5%) | 19 (37.3%) | - | |||
Xhosa | 7 (17.9%) | 28 (41.2%) | 4 (7.8%) | - | |||
Other | 1 (1.6%) | 2 (2.9%) | 3 (5.9%) | - | |||
Employment status | 12.03 | 2 | .002** | .243 | |||
Full-time | 72 (88.9%) | 48 (70.6%) | 50 (90.9%) | - | |||
Part-time | 9 (11.1%) | 20 (29.4%) | 5 (9.1%) | - | |||
Employment type | 16.04 | 2 | < .001*** | .282 | |||
Truck driver | 64 (79%) | 33 (50%) | 29 (52.7%) | - | |||
Not truck driver | 17 (21%) | 33 (50%) | 26 (47.3%) | - |
Note. MWH = men with HIV; MCVR = men with cardiovascular risk; M = mean; SD = standard deviation; ESE = effect size estimate.
Data based on controls n = 62 and MCVR n = 51 (missing data).
Data based on MWH n = 66 (missing data).
Data based on MCVR n = 53 (one participant did not disclose their income and the other is missing data).
The effect size here is estimated by the partial eta squared (ηp2) statistic.
The effect size here is estimated by the Cramer’s V statistic.
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
Analyses detected significant between-group differences in terms of both home language and medium of schooling instruction. The MWH and control groups both consisted predominantly of Xhosa speakers, whereas the MCVR group consisted predominantly of Afrikaans speakers. Within each group, approximately 50% of all participants had been schooled in English; however, more than 40% of MWH had been schooled in Xhosa whereas almost 40% of both controls and MCVR had been schooled in Afrikaans.
Although most participants in all groups were employed full-time, significantly more MWH than controls and MCVR were employed part-time.
Regarding the sample’s clinical characteristics, most MWH (71%) were virally suppressed (viral load <20 copies/mL) at study enrolment. The median and interquartile range values for relevant variables were: plasma viral load = 0 (0–33) copies mL, CD4 count = 501 (328–674) cells/μl, nadir CD4 count = 270 (103–408) cells/μl. For participants with diabetes mellitus type 2, glycated haemoglobin (NGSP and IFCC) values were 8.3 (6.5–10.7) and 62.5 (46.5–91.2) respectively, with an average glucose (Eag) value of 62.5 (7.6–91.2).
Table 2 illustrates the numerous clinical variables on which analyses detected significant between-group differences. Follow-up pairwise comparisons indicated that, relative to controls and MWH, MCVR had significantly higher (a) risk for cardiovascular disease, as measured by the FRS (all p-values [ps] <.001); (b) systolic and diastolic blood pressure (ps<.05); (c) body mass index (BMI; p=.005 and <.001, respectively); (d) triglyceride levels (ps=.003); and (e) total cholesterol (p=.006 and .009, respectively). MCVR also had a higher hip-waist ratio than controls (p=.031), and higher LDL cholesterol than MWH (p=.026). All effect sizes were in the low range. Controls and MWH did not differ significantly on any of the measured clinical variables.
Table 2.
Group | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Controls (n = 81) |
MWH (n = 68) |
MCVR (n = 55) |
|
|||
Continuous Variables | M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | F (df) | p | ESE |
| ||||||
PAOFI total score | 168.99 (27.60) | 169.62 (27.81) | 165.25 (39.90) | 0.34 (2,201) | .715 | .003 |
BDI-II total score | 5.85 (6.74) | 7.18 (8.41) | 7.91 (8.40) | 1.21 (2,198) | .301 | .01 |
Framingham Risk Score (%) a | 7.78 (8.85) | 6.15 (6.09) | 15.99 (8.77) | 21.57 (2,152) | < .001*** | .22 |
Systolic blood pressure | 129.15 (14.96) | 129.48 (19.34) | 137.45 (16.73) | 3.85 (2,172) | .023* | .04 |
Diastolic blood pressure | 80.65 (9.70) | 83.86 (12.58) | 86.66 (13.58) | 3.45 (2,172) | .034* | .04 |
Pulse rate (beats/min) | 69.37 (8.59) | 71.59 (12.08) | 73.70 (11.12) | 2.21 (2,172) | .112 | .03 |
Body mass index | 28.61 (6.24) | 26.50 (5.22) | 32.33 (8.24) | 11.01 (2,169) | < .001*** | .11 |
Hip-to-Waist Ratio | 0.96 (0.12) | 0.99 (0.08) | 1.00 (0.09) | 3.55 (2,192) | .031* | .04 |
Triglyceride | 1.45 (0.78) | 1.53 (1.00) | 3.11 (4.36) | 7.21 (2,157) | .001** | .08 |
Cholesterol | ||||||
High-density lipoprotein (HDL) | 1.25 (0.38) | 1.37 (0.41) | 1.19 (0.43) | 2.90 (2,157) | .058 | .04 |
Low-density lipoprotein (LDL) | 2.57 (0.89) | 2.42 (1.08) | 2.92 (0.78) | 3.47 (2,152) | .034* | .04 |
Total | 4.51 (0.90) | 4.51 (1.16) | 5.19 (1.23) | 5.97 (2,157) | .003** | .07 |
| ||||||
Categorical Variables | f (%) | f (%) | f (%) | χ2 (df) | p | ESE |
| ||||||
PAOFI b | 10 (12.35) | 14 (20.59) | 9 (16.36) | 1.85 (2) | .395 | 0.10 |
BDI-II c | 4 (4.94) | 8 (11.76) | 10 (18.18) | 6.07 (2) | .048* | 0.17 |
Note. MWH = men with HIV; MCVR = men with cardiovascular risk; ESE = effect size estimate (in this case, partial eta squared [ηp2] for continuous variables and Cramer’s V for Categorical Variables); PAOFI = Patient Assessment of Own Functioning Inventory; BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory-II.
Percentage of men at risk of cardiovascular disease, as estimated by Framingham Risk Score.
Data are percentage of men who endorsed “almost always” on three or more questions within any domain.
Data are percentage of participants who scored above the cut-off score of ≥19.
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
Analyses detected no significant between-group differences in the number of participants who met the clinical cut-off for functioning on PAOFI scores. However, there was a significant between-group difference in number of participants who scored above the BDI-II threshold of ≥19 indicating depressive symptomatology (MCVR=18.18%; MWH=11.76%; controls=4.96%).
In summary, there were significant differences between the groups on four sociodemographic variables (age, monthly income, home language, schooling language) and on six clinical variables (BDI-II score, FRS, blood pressure, BMI, hip-waist ratio, cholesterol). Subsequent analyses controlled for these potential confounders. To avoid multicollinearity we used only FRS as an indicator of vascular risk (i.e., we did not include blood pressure, BMI, hip-waist ratio, and cholesterol in subsequent modelling), and we used home language rather than schooling language because home language is positively associated with academic ablility77.
Cognitive Performance
Table 3 presents within-group descriptive statistics for domain T-scores, global T-scores, and GDS data. Without controlling for any potential confounders, analyses suggested that (a) MWH performed significantly more poorly than MCVR on all single-domain outcome variables, as well as on the global T-score and GDS; (b) MWH performed more poorly than controls on tests assessing processing speed, attention and working memory, language, memory and executive function, as well as on the global T-score and GDS; and (c) MCVR performed significantly better than controls on memory.
Table 3.
Group |
|
|
|||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Controls | MWH | MCVR | |||
Variable | (n = 81) | (n = 68) | (n = 55) | p | Partial eta2 |
| |||||
Domain T−score | |||||
Motor skills | 49.24 (8.27) | 46.97 (9.56) | 50.25 (9.83) | .120 | .021 |
Processing speed | 54.41 (12.21) | 45.12 (8.40) | 54.95 (9.03) | < .001*** a | .163 |
Attention and working memory | 51.76 (8.66) | 46.90 (6.13) | 53.98 (6.54) | < .001*** b | .133 |
Language | 54.92 (10.19) | 50.59 (10.60) | 56.03 (10.56) | .008** c | .047 |
Learning | 51.74 (9.95) | 48.90 (8.01) | 54.63 (8.42) | .002** d | .059 |
Memory | 54.46 (10.29) | 50.89 (7.68) | 58.26 (9.01) | < .001*** e | .090 |
Executive function | 52.51 (10.73) | 47.35 (8.29) | 53.71 (9.44) | < .001***f | .074 |
Global T−score | 52.72 (7.54) | 48.10 (5.09) | 54.88 (5.74) | < .001***g | .158 |
GDS | 0.29 (0.40) | 0.47 (0.36) | 0.19 (0.22) | < .001***h | .096 |
Note. Data presented are means, with standard deviations in parentheses. MWH = men with HIV; MCVR = men with cardiovascular risk; GDS = global deficit score.
Post-hoc pairwise comparisons: MWH vs MCVR, p < .001; MWH vs controls, p < .001; MCVR vs controls, p = .950.
Post-hoc pairwise comparisons: MWH vs MCVR, p < .001; MWH vs controls, p < .001; MCVR vs controls, p = .196.
Post-hoc pairwise comparisons: MWH vs MCVR, p = .004; MWH vs controls, p = .012; MCVR vs controls, p = .544.
Post-hoc pairwise comparisons: MWH vs MCVR, p = .001; MWH vs controls, p = .132; MCVR vs controls, p = .157.
Post-hoc pairwise comparisons: MWH vs MCVR, p < .001; MWH vs controls, p = .048; MCVR vs controls, p = .048.
Post-hoc pairwise comparisons: MWH vs MCVR, p = .001; MWH vs controls, p = .004; MCVR vs controls, p = .756.
Post-hoc pairwise comparisons: MWH vs MCVR, p < .001; MWH vs controls, p < .001; MCVR vs controls, p = .127.
Post-hoc pairwise comparisons: MWH vs MCVR, p < .001; MWH vs controls, p = .006; MCVR vs controls, p = .191.
Multiple Regression Modelling
Each regression model set out to investigate whether between-group differences in cognitive performance persisted even after considering the total variance accounted for by the potential confounders of income, home language, BDI-II score, and FRS.
After controlling for those potential confounders, group status was significantly associated with test performance in the domains of attention and working memory, learning and memory, and with both measures of overall cognitive performance (see Table 4). More specifically, MWH performed significantly more poorly than MCVR.
Table 4.
Motor Skills |
Processing Speed |
Attention and Working Memory |
||||||||||
Predictor | β | SE | t | p | β | SE | t | p | β | SE | t | p |
| ||||||||||||
Group (vs MWH) | ||||||||||||
MCVR | 1.39 | 2.24 | 0.62 | .537 | 2.67 | 2.12 | 1.26 | .210 | 3.82 | 1.67 | 2.29 | .024* |
Controls | −0.29 | 1.89 | −0.15 | .880 | 3.27 | 1.80 | 1.82 | .071 | 0.93 | 1.42 | 0.66 | .513 |
Home language (vs English) | ||||||||||||
Afrikaans | 6.76 | 2.74 | 2.47 | .015* | 0.88 | 2.60 | 0.34 | .736 | −0.16 | 2.04 | −0.08 | .936 |
Xhosa | 1.19 | 2.81 | 0.42 | .673 | −6.93 | 2.66 | −2.61 | .010* | −4.09 | 2.09 | −1.96 | .052 |
Shona | −2.74 | 3.21 | −0.85 | .394 | −9.86 | 3.04 | −3.24 | .002** | −7.37 | 2.39 | −3.08 | .002** |
Other | −0.92 | 3.93 | −0.23 | .816 | −6.37 | 3.73 | −1.71 | .090 | −2.23 | 2.94 | −0.76 | .448 |
Monthly income (ZAR) | 0.00 | 0.00 | −0.02 | .981 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.51 | .612 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 1.93 | .056 |
BDI-II (cut-off ≥19) | −0.90 | 2.50 | −0.36 | .716 | −4.67 | 2.35 | −2.29 | .024* | −0.21 | 1.85 | −0.11 | .909 |
FRS (%) | −0.04 | 0.10 | −0.40 | .690 | 0.27 | 0.10 | 2.81 | .006** | −0.01 | 0.08 | −0.09 | .928 |
| ||||||||||||
Language |
Learning |
Memory |
||||||||||
Predictor | β | SE | t | p | β | SE | t | p | β | SE | t | p |
| ||||||||||||
Group (vs MWH) | ||||||||||||
MCVR | 4.44 | 2.68 | 1.66 | .100 | 5.13 | 2.21 | 2.32 | .022* | 4.81 | 2.23 | 2.16 | .032* |
Controls | 2.83 | 2.27 | 1.25 | .214 | 1.92 | 1.87 | 1.03 | .307 | 2.30 | 1.88 | 1.22 | .224 |
Home language (vs English) | ||||||||||||
Afrikaans | −0.75 | 3.28 | −0.23 | .819 | −2.57 | 2.71 | −0.95 | .344 | −0.65 | 2.72 | −0.24 | .812 |
Xhosa | −2.49 | 3.35 | −0.74 | .459 | −7.17 | 2.77 | −2.59 | .011* | −6.67 | 2.78 | −2.39 | .018* |
Shona | −6.03 | 3.84 | −1.57 | .118 | −5.86 | 3.17 | −1.85 | .067 | −7.02 | 3.19 | −2.20 | .029* |
Other | −9.41 | 4.71 | −2.00 | .048* | −7.01 | 3.89 | −1.80 | .074 | −7.69 | 3.91 | −1.97 | .051 |
Monthly income (ZAR) | 0.00 | 0.00 | 1.00 | .318 | 0.00 | 0.00 | −0.43 | .670 | −5.07 | 0.00 | −0.36 | .718 |
BDI-II (cut-off ≥19) | 0.70 | 2.97 | 0.24 | .813 | −1.08 | 2.45 | −0.44 | .660 | 2.60 | 2.46 | 0.11 | .916 |
FRS (%) | −0.08 | 0.12 | −0.63 | .531 | −0.20 | 0.10 | −1.98 | .050* | −0.18 | 0.10 | −1.81 | .072 |
| ||||||||||||
Executive Function |
Global Deficit Score |
Global T−Score |
||||||||||
Predictor | β | SE | t | p | β | SE | t | p | β | SE | t | p |
| ||||||||||||
Group (vs MWH) | ||||||||||||
MCVR | 1.10 | 2.25 | 0.49 | .624 | −1.45 | 0.40 | −2.26 | .024* | 3.62 | 1.50 | 2.42 | .017* |
Controls | 1.64 | 1.90 | 0.86 | .392 | −0.84 | 0.50 | −1.68 | .093 | 1.76 | 1.27 | 1.39 | .166 |
Home language (vs English) | ||||||||||||
Afrikaans | −0.58 | 2.75 | −0.21 | .834 | −0.42 | 0.91 | −0.46 | .648 | 0.43 | 1.83 | 0.23 | .816 |
Xhosa | −5.70 | 2.81 | −2.03 | .045* | −0.11 | 0.81 | −0.14 | .891 | −4.08 | 1.87 | −2.18 | .031* |
Shona | −8.14 | 3.22 | −2.53 | .013* | 1.37 | 0.87 | 1.57 | .116 | −5.28 | 2.14 | −2.47 | .015* |
Other | −2.32 | 3.95 | −0.59 | .558 | 0.64 | 1.04 | 0.62 | .538 | −4.77 | 2.63 | −1.82 | .078 |
Monthly income (ZAR) | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.43 | .665 | 0.00 | 0.00 | −0.87 | .386 | 7.53 | 0.00 | 0.80 | .424 |
BDI-II (cut-off ≥19) | 0.69 | 2.49 | 0.28 | .783 | 0.57 | 0.62 | 0.91 | .361 | −1.07 | 1.66 | −0.65 | .520 |
FRS (%) | 0.11 | 0.10 | 1.09 | .278 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.04 | .969 | 0.00 | 0.07 | 0.12 | .902 |
Note. MWH = men with HIV; MCVR = men with cardiovascular risk; BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory-II; FRS (%) = Framingham Risk Score 10-year cardiovascular disease risk percentage.
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
Home language was a significant predictor of performance on several different outcome variables. Compared to participants who spoke English as a home language, those who (a) spoke Afrikaans as a home language performed better on tests assessing motor function; (b) spoke Xhosa as a home language performed more poorly on tests assessing processing speed, attention and working memory, learning, and memory, and on the global T-score; (c) spoke Shona as a home language performed more poorly on tests assessing processing speed, attention and working memory, memory, and executive functioning, and on the global T-score; (d) indicated ‘other’ as home language performed more poorly on tests assessing language. FRS was a significant predictor for better performance on tests of processing speed and learning. A higher BDI-II score predicted poorer performance on tests of processing speed (see Table 4).
Rates of Cognitive Impairment
Table 5 presents within-group data on the number of participants presenting with cognitive impairment (globally and within each domain), as well as results of between-group comparisons for rates of cognitive impairment. As the Table shows, initial analyses detected significant small to medium sized between-group differences on four different outcome variables with MWH presenting with the highest frequency of impairment. Follow-up pairwise comparisons indicated that (a) significantly more PWH than HC and PCVR presented with impaired performance on tests of processing speed (p=.003 and <.001, respectively) (b) significantly more PWH than PCVR presented with impaired performance on tests of attention and working memory (p=.018), (c) significantly more PWH than HC and PCVR presented with GDS in the impaired range (ps<.001), and (d) with regard to EF, there were no significant between-group differences although there was a trend toward significantly more PWH than HC and PCVR being impaired (ps<.065).
Table 5.
Group |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Controls | MWH | MCVR | ||||
Variable | (n = 81) | (n = 68) | (n = 55) | χ2 | p | ESE |
| ||||||
Cognitive domain T−score | ||||||
Motor skills | 11 (13.6%) | 14 (20.6%) | 9 (16.4%) | 1.31 | .519 | .08 |
Processing speed | 9 (11.1%) | 22 (32.4%) | 2 (3.6%) | 21.03 | < .001*** | .32 |
Attention and working memory | 7 (8.6%) | 11 (16.2%) | 1 (1.8%) | 7.49 | .024* | .19 |
Verbal fluency | 5 (6.2%) | 7 (10.3%) | 5 (9.1%) | 0.88 | .645 | .07 |
Learning | 8 (9.9%) | 9 (13.2%) | 3 (5.5%) | 2.08 | .353 | .10 |
Memory | 8 (9.9%) | 7 (10.3%) | 3 (5.5%) | 1.07 | .586 | .07 |
Executive function | 7 (8.6%) | 14 (20.6%) | 4 (7.3%) | 6.64 | .036* | .18 |
Global T−score | 2 (2.5%) | 2 (2.9%) | 0 (0) | a | .565 | .09 |
GDS | 15 (18.5%) | 28 (41.2%) | 8 (14.5%) | 14.51 | < .001*** | .27 |
Note. Data presented are raw frequencies (percentages) of participants who presented with cognitive impairment (for individual cognitive domains, z < −1.00; for GDS, score ≥ 0.05). For each between-group comparison, degrees of freedom were 2. MWH = men with HIV; MCVR = men with cardiovascular risk; ESE = effect size estimate (in this case, Cramer’s V); GDS = global deficit score.
Fisher’s Exact Test performed.
p < .05.
p < .01
p < .001.
DISCUSSION
We assessed cognitive performance in professional drivers with and without chronic medical conditions, hypothesizing that men with HIV (MWH) and men with cardiovascular risk factors (MCVR; either hypertension or diabetes) would perform more poorly than matched controls, with PWH performing worst. This hypothesis was partially confirmed: MWH presented with the poorest cognitive outcomes and highest rate of cognitive impairment, but MCVR did not perform more poorly than controls. Group membership remained a predictor of cognitive performance after controlling for potential confounders (age, monthly income, home language, depressive symptomatology, and cardiovascular risk).
On average, MCVR were older than MWH and controls, and MWH had a significantly lower monthly income than MCVR and controls. This difference in socioeconomic status is likely explained by the fact that a larger proportion of MWH worked part-time rather than full-time. The relevance of this difference for the current study, and one reason why we included monthly income as a predictor in our ultimate regression models, is that higher income may translate into better health care and cognitive health78,79.
Further regarding sociodemographic characteristics, our sample’s language profile reflects cultural aspects of South African society. English was the predominant language of academic instruction, regardless of participants’ group assignment or home language. Notably, 84% of MWH had Xhosa as their home language but 54% were schooled in English. Although this suggests that these participants were bilingual to at least some degree, their relative fluency in each language was not measured and we can thus not speculate about effects of language profile on cognitive test performance. Nonetheless, we included home language as a predictor in our ultimate regression models.
Regarding the sample’s clinical characteristics, MCVR presented with the highest number of depressive symptoms, but also performed best on cognitive testing. Although major depressive disorder is frequently associated with poorer cognitive performance, milder depressive symptoms in adults with cardiovascular risk factors may not have the same association with cognition80. Overall, depression was associated with slower processing speed81.
Our major analyses indicated that group status was a significant predictor of performance on tests of attention and working memory, learning, and memory, as well as on both measures of overall cognitive functioning (global T-score and GDS). As expected, given previous reports of more severe cognitive impairment associated with HIV than with diabetes or hypertension44,82,83, these between-group differences were driven by MWH performing significantly more poorly than MCVR. Of note is that these between-group differences persisted even after the analyses controlled for potential confounders (i.e., sociodemographic and clinical variables on which previous analyses had detected significant between-group differences). We therefore conclude that in this sample of professional drivers HIV-related factors are sufficient to account for poor cognitive performance. We consider MCVR’s marginal and non-significant superior performance over controls, as well as the association of processing speed with higher FRS risk, as potentially spurious results
Although the cognitive performance of the MWH group was poor relative to that of the MCVR group, for the most part the individual test scores of MWH participants were only marginally greater than or within 1 SD of the normative mean. Hence, their performance would, broadly speaking, be considered as falling within the range conventionally described as asymptomatic or mild. This categorization is consistent with the fact that MWH PAOFI reports indicated no significant everyday functional impairment.
Although asymptomatic/mildly impaired cognitive performance in professional drivers with HIV may not appear to be immediately concerning, it is important to identify and monitor them because research suggests that (a) MWH with that degree of impairment have a 2- to 6-fold increase in risk for earlier development of symptomatic HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders and for early mortality28,84, and (b) poorer cognitive performance is associated with lower employment status in older PWH85. Drivers could thus benefit, both in terms of health and risk management, from being monitored for HNCI.
A significantly greater proportion of MWH than MCVR presented with cognitive impairment (as defined by a domain z-score <−1.00) on tests of processing speed and of attention and working memory. There was also a strong trend toward poorer executive functioning in MWH than MCVR. Furthermore, significantly more MWH (41% of the group) than MCVR and controls met the GDS cut-off for cognitive impairment. These data are consistent with the profile of HIV-associated neurocognitive impairment in the cART era and previously published reports regarding the prevalence of cognitive impairment in MWH2,16,86.
A noteworthy secondary finding is that home language was a significant predictor of overall cognitive performance (as measured by global T-score). Specifically, performance was better among participants with English, rather than Xhosa or Shona, as a home language. This finding can be interpreted in the light of evidence that test-taking proficiency influences neuropsychological test performance87. Given there were no significant between-group differences in educational attainment, we suggest that language may serve as a proxy for test-taking proficiency, with Xhosa and Shona speakers being less test-savvy than English speakers. This is an important consideration in clinical settings.
Limitations
We assessed daily functioning using only the PAOFI. Some evidence suggests that self-report is relatively insensitive in identifying impediments to optimal daily functioning, and that more PWH with asymptomatic cognitive impairment may have mild difficulties in daily functioning than is gauged via self-report27. Daily functioning could therefore have been assessed more completely. We did not evaluate anxiety or quality of life. Self-reported clinical variables (e.g., time since ART initiation) used in this study may be unreliable. Because so few woman work as professional drivers, we only included men in this study. Substance use other than alcohol was screened for using a urine toxicology screen only. Not all study participants were virally suppressed, but analyses comparing the cognitive test performance of MWH who were virally suppressed and those who were not detected no significant between-group differences.
SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION
After controlling for potential confounders, between group differences on cognitive performance persisted. A significant percentage of drivers with HIV presented with lower cognitive performance, largely characterised as asymptomatic/mild impairment. Although the level of cognitive impairment in these drivers might be characterised as asymptomatic/mild and did not generalise to activities of daily living, they are at risk for poorer long-term health and employment outcomes. Hence, programs that monitor and support their long-term cognitive health (e.g., cognitive remediation training) and their mental and physical health (especially as they continue to operate in occupational settings) are recommended. Future studies should directly assess, both cross-sectionally and longitudinally, the impact of cognitive impairment on vocational functioning of PWH, even if it is asymptomatic/mild.
Acknowledgements
Funding information:
This work was supported by a Fogarty International Center grant, 1K43TW010361-01.
Funding:
This study was funded by a K-43 Fogarty International Center grant, 1K43TW010361-01.
Footnotes
Competing interests:
The author(s) declare that they have no financial or personal relationship(s) that may have inappropriately influenced them in writing this article.
Data availability statement:
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, [HG], upon reasonable request.
Disclaimer:
The views expressed in the submitted article are the authors’ own and not an official position if the institution or funder.
References
- 1.Iadecola C, Gottesman RF. Neurovascular and Cognitive Dysfunction in Hypertension. Circ Res. Mar 29 2019;124(7):1025–1044. doi: 10.1161/circresaha.118.313260 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Heaton RK, Franklin DR Jr., Deutsch R, et al. Neurocognitive change in the era of HIV combination antiretroviral therapy: the longitudinal CHARTER study. Clin Infect Dis. Feb 1 2015;60(3):473–80. doi: 10.1093/cid/ciu862 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Vance DE, Nicholas N, Cody SL. HIV Neurocognitive Impairment: Perspectives on Neurocognitive Reserve and Behavioral Remediation/Compensation Strategies. Austin Journal of neuropsychiatry and Cognitive Sciences. 2015;1(1):1002. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Lee Y, Smofsky A, Nykoliation P, et al. Cognitive Impairment Mediates Workplace Impairment in Adults With Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus: Results From the Motivaction Study. Can J Diabetes. Jun 2018;42(3):289–295. doi: 10.1016/j.jcjd.2017.06.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Delany-Moretlwe S, Bello B, Kinross P, et al. HIV prevalence and risk in long-distance truck drivers in South Africa: a national cross-sectional survey. Int J STD AIDS. May 2014;25(6):428–38. doi: 10.1177/0956462413512803 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Lalla-Edward ST, Fischer AE, Venter WDF, et al. Cross-sectional study of the health of southern African truck drivers. BMJ Open. 2019;9(10):e032025. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2019-032025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Guest AJ, Chen Y-L, Pearson N, King JA, Paine NJ, Clemes SA. Cardiometabolic risk factors and mental health status among truck drivers: a systematic review. BMJ Open. 2020;10(10):e038993. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2020-038993 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Correlation of In Vivo Neuroimaging Abnormalities With Postmortem Human Immunodeficiency Virus Encephalitis and Dendritic Loss, 3 (2004). Apr 01. http://archneur.jamanetwork.com/article.aspx?articleid=785503http://archneur.jamanetwork.com/data/Journals/NEUR/13732/NOC30107.pdf [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Scully-Russ E, Torraco R. The Changing Nature and Organization of Work: An Integrative Review of the Literature. Human Resource Development Review. 2020;19(1):66–93. doi: 10.1177/1534484319886394 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Lam RW, Persad C. Cognitive Dysfunction in the Workplace: Focus on Depression. In: Riba M, Parikh, Greden J, eds. Mental Health in the Workplace Integrating Psychiatry and Primary Care. Springer; 2019. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Silvaggi F, Leonardi M, Tiraboschi P, Muscio C, Toppo C, Raggi A. Keeping People with Dementia or Mild Cognitive Impairment in Employment: A Literature Review on Its Determinants. International journal of environmental research and public health. 2020;17(3):842. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17030842 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Gouse H, Masson CJ, Henry M, et al. Assessing HIV provider knowledge, screening practices, and training needs for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. A short report. Aids Care. Mar 5 2020:1–5. doi: 10.1080/09540121.2020.1736256 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Alford K, Vera JH. Cognitive Impairment in people living with HIV in the ART era: A Review. Br Med Bull. Sep 1 2018;127(1):55–68. doi: 10.1093/bmb/ldy019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Saylor D, Dickens AM, Sacktor N, et al. HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder--pathogenesis and prospects for treatment. Review. Nat Rev Neurol. Apr 2016;12(4):234–48. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2016.27 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Antinori A, Arendt G, Becker JT, et al. Updated research nosology for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Neurology. Oct 30 2007;69(18):1789–99. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000287431.88658.8b [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Habib AG, Yakasai AM, Owolabi LF, et al. Neurocognitive impairment in HIV-1-infected adults in Sub-Saharan Africa: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Infect Dis. Oct 2013;17(10):e820–31. doi: 10.1016/j.ijid.2013.06.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kelly CM, van Oosterhout JJ, Ngwalo C, et al. HIV Associated Neurocognitive Disorders (HAND) in Malawian Adults and Effect on Adherence to Combination Anti-Retroviral Therapy: A Cross Sectional Study. Plos One. Jun 10 2014;9(6)doi:ARTN e98962 10.1371/journal.pone.0098962 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Sacktor N Changing clinical phenotypes of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Journal of Neurovirology. Apr 2018;24(2):141–145. doi: 10.1007/s13365-017-0556-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.The Mind Exchange Working Group, Antinori A, Arendt G, Grant I, Letendre S, Munoz-Moreno JA. Assessment, Diagnosis, and Treatment of HIV-Associated Neurocognitive Disorder: A Consensus Report of the Mind Exchange Program. Clinical Infectious Diseases. Apr 1 2013;56(7):1004–1017. doi: 10.1093/cid/cis975 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Sacktor N, Skolasky RL, Seaberg E, et al. Prevalence of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders in the Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study. Neurology. Jan 26 2016;86(4):334–40. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002277 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Croxford S, Kitching A, Desai S, et al. Mortality and causes of death in people diagnosed with HIV in the era of highly active antiretroviral therapy compared with the general population: an analysis of a national observational cohort. Lancet Public Health. Jan 2017;2(1):E35–E46. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(16)30020-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Lohse N, Hansen AE, Gerstoft J, Obel N. Improved survival in HIV-infected persons: consequences and perspectives. Journal of Antimicrobial Chemotherapy. 2007;60(3):461–463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.May MT, Ingle SM. Life expectancy of HIV-positive adults: a review. Sex Health. 2011;8(4):526–533. doi: 10.1071/Sh11046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Van Wijk CH, Meintjies WAJ. International HIV Dementia Scale: screening for HIV-assocaited neurocognitive disorders in occupational health settings. Occupational Health Southern Africa. 2015;21(4):10–16. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Heaton RK, Marcotte TD, White DA, et al. Nature and vocational significance of neuropsychological impairment associated with HIV infection. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. Feb 1996;10(1):1–14. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Ranka JL, Chapparo CJ. Assessment of productivity performance in men with HIV Associated Neurocognitive Disorder (HAND). Work. 2010;36(2):193–206. doi: 10.3233/WOR-2010-1020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Chiao S, Rosen HJ, Nicolas K, et al. Deficits in self-awareness impact the diagnosis of asymptomatic neurocognitive impairment in HIV. AIDS Res Hum Retroviruses. Jun 2013;29(6):949–56. doi: 10.1089/AID.2012.0229 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Grant I, Franklin DR, Deutsch R, et al. Asymptomatic HIV-associated neurocognitive impairment increases risk for symptomatic decline. Neurology. Jun 09 2014; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Marcotte TD, Heaton RK, Wolfson T, et al. The impact of HIV-related neuropsychological dysfunction on driving behavior. The HNRC Group. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. Nov 1999;5(7):579–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Rashid R, Standen P, Carpenter H, Radford K. Systematic review and meta-analysis of association between cognitive tests and on-road driving ability in people with dementia. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 2019:1–42. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2019.1603112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Jacobs M, Hart EP, Roos RAC. Driving with a neurodegenerative disorder: an overview of the current literature. J Neurol. Apr 19 2017;doi: 10.1007/s00415-017-8489-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Reger MA, Welsh RK, Watson GS, Cholerton B, Baker LD, Craft S. The relationship between neuropsychological functioning and driving ability in dementia: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychology. Jan 2004;18(1):85–93. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.1.85 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Marcotte TD, Heaton RK, Reicks C, Gonzalez R, Grant I, Group H. HIV-related neuropsychological impairment and automobile driving. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2000;6(2):233. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Marcotte TD, Lazzaretto D, Scott JC, et al. Visual attention deficits are associated with driving accidents in cognitively-impaired HIV-infected individuals. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. Jan 2006;28(1):13–28. doi: 10.1080/13803390490918048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Marcotte TD, Scott JC. Neuropsychological performance and the assessment of driving behavior. Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsychiatric disorders. 3 ed. Oxford University Press; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Marcotte TD, Wolfson T, Rosenthal TJ, et al. A multimodal assessment of driving performance in HIV infection. Neurology. Oct 26 2004;63(8):1417–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Vance DE, Fazeli PL, Ball DA, Slater LZ, Ross LA. Cognitive functioning and driving simulator performance in middle-aged and older adults with HIV. Journal of the Association of Nurses AIDS Care. Mar-Apr 2014;25(2):e11–26. doi: 10.1016/j.jana.2013.12.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Gouse H, Masson CJ, Henry M, et al. The Impact of HIV-Associated Neurocognitive Impairment on Driving Performance in Commercial Truck Drivers. AIDS Behav. Sep 10 2020;doi: 10.1007/s10461-020-03033-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Biessels GJ, Despa F. Cognitive decline and dementia in diabetes mellitus: mechanisms and clinical implications. Nat Rev Endocrinol. Oct 2018;14(10):591–604. doi: 10.1038/s41574-018-0048-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Ott A, Stolk RP, van Harskamp F, Pols HA, Hofman A, Breteler MM. Diabetes mellitus and the risk of dementia: The Rotterdam Study. Neurology. Dec 10 1999;53(9):1937–42. doi: 10.1212/wnl.53.9.1937 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Moran C, Beare R, Wang W, Callisaya M, Srikanth V. Type 2 diabetes mellitus, brain atrophy, and cognitive decline. Neurology. Feb 19 2019;92(8):e823–e830. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000006955 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Biessels GJ, Staekenborg S, Brunner E, Brayne C, Scheltens P. Risk of dementia in diabetes mellitus: a systematic review. Lancet Neurol. Jan 2006;5(1):64–74. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(05)70284-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Zilliox LA, Chadrasekaran K, Kwan JY, Russell JW. Diabetes and Cognitive Impairment. Current diabetes reports. 2016;16(9):87–87. doi: 10.1007/s11892-016-0775-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Palta P, Schneider AL, Biessels GJ, Touradji P, Hill-Briggs F. Magnitude of cognitive dysfunction in adults with type 2 diabetes: a meta-analysis of six cognitive domains and the most frequently reported neuropsychological tests within domains. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. Mar 2014;20(3):278–91. doi: 10.1017/s1355617713001483 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Sadanand S, Balachandar R, Bharath S. Memory and executive functions in persons with type 2 diabetes: a meta-analysis. Diabetes Metab Res Rev. Feb 2016;32(2):132–42. doi: 10.1002/dmrr.2664 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Monette MC, Baird A, Jackson DL. A meta-analysis of cognitive functioning in nondemented adults with type 2 diabetes mellitus. Can J Diabetes. Dec 2014;38(6):401–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jcjd.2014.01.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Mansur RB, Lee Y, Zhou AJ, et al. Determinants of cognitive function in individuals with type 2 diabetes mellitus: A meta-analysis. Ann Clin Psychiatry. Feb 2018;30(1):38–50. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Rawlings AM, Sharrett AR, Schneider AL, et al. Diabetes in midlife and cognitive change over 20 years: a cohort study. Annals of internal medicine. Dec 2 2014;161(11):785–93. doi: 10.7326/m14-0737 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Koekkoek PS, Kappelle LJ, van den Berg E, Rutten GE, Biessels GJ. Cognitive function in patients with diabetes mellitus: guidance for daily care. Lancet Neurol. Mar 2015;14(3):329–40. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(14)70249-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Biessels GJ, Strachan MW, Visseren FL, Kappelle LJ, Whitmer RA. Dementia and cognitive decline in type 2 diabetes and prediabetic stages: towards targeted interventions. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol. Mar 2014;2(3):246–55. doi: 10.1016/s2213-8587(13)70088-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Moheet A, Mangia S, Seaquist ER. Impact of diabetes on cognitive function and brain structure. Ann N Y Acad Sci. Sep 2015;1353:60–71. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12807 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Skurtveit S, Strøm H, Skrivarhaug T, Mørland J, Bramness JG, Engeland A. Road traffic accident risk in patients with diabetes mellitus receiving blood glucose-lowering drugs. Prospective follow-up study. Diabet Med. Apr 2009;26(4):404–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-5491.2009.02699.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Ma S, Zhang J, Zeng X, et al. Type 2 diabetes can undermine driving performance of middle-aged male drivers through its deterioration of perceptual and cognitive functions. Accid Anal Prev. Jan 2020;134:105334. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2019.105334 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Gąsecki D, Kwarciany M, Nyka W, Narkiewicz K. Hypertension, brain damage and cognitive decline. Curr Hypertens Rep. Dec 2013;15(6):547–58. doi: 10.1007/s11906-013-0398-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Lane CA, Barnes J, Nicholas JM, et al. Associations between blood pressure across adulthood and late-life brain structure and pathology in the neuroscience substudy of the 1946 British birth cohort (Insight 46): an epidemiological study. Lancet Neurol. Oct 2019;18(10):942–952. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(19)30228-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Iadecola C, Yaffe K, Biller J, et al. Impact of Hypertension on Cognitive Function: A Scientific Statement From the American Heart Association. Hypertension. Dec 2016;68(6):e67–e94. doi: 10.1161/hyp.0000000000000053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Muela HC, Costa-Hong VA, Yassuda MS, et al. Hypertension Severity Is Associated With Impaired Cognitive Performance. J Am Heart Assoc. Jan 11 2017;6(1)doi: 10.1161/jaha.116.004579 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Shehab A, Abdulle A. Cognitive and autonomic dysfunction measures in normal controls, white coat and borderline hypertension. BMC Cardiovasc Disord. Jan 11 2011;11:3. doi: 10.1186/1471-2261-11-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Adedokun AO, Ter Goon D, Owolabi EO, Adeniyi OV, Ajayi AI. Driving to Better Health: Screening for Hypertension and Associated Factors Among Commercial Taxi Drivers in Buffalo City Metropolitan Municipality, South Africa. The Open Public Health Journal. 2017;10(1) [Google Scholar]
- 60.Lyman JM, McGwin G, Sims RV. Factors related to driving difficulty and habits in older drivers. Accident Analysis & Prevention. 2001/05/01/ 2001;33(3):413–421. doi: 10.1016/S0001-4575(00)00055-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Wadley AL, Iacovides S, Roche J, et al. Working nights and lower leisure-time physical activity associate with chronic pain in Southern African long-distance truck drivers: A cross-sectional study. PLoS One. 2020;15(12):e0243366. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0243366 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Crizzle AM, McLean M, Malkin J. Risk Factors for Depressive Symptoms in Long-Haul Truck Drivers. Int J Environ Res Public Health. May 26 2020;17(11)doi: 10.3390/ijerph17113764 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Bohn MJ, Babor TF, Kranzler HR. The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): validation of a screening instrument for use in medical settings. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1995;56(4):423–432. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1995.56.423 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Seedat YK, Rayner BL, Veriava Y. South African hypertension practice guideline 2014. Cardiovasc J Afr. Nov-Dec 2014;25(6):288–94. doi: 10.5830/cvja-2014-062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Scott TM, Gouse H, Joska J, et al. Home-versus acquired-language test performance on the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised among multilingual South Africans. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. Sep 28 2018:1–8. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2018.1510403 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Nyamayaro P, Gouse H, Hakim J, Robbins RN, Chibanda D. Neurocognitive impairment in treatment-experienced adults living with HIV attending primary care clinics in Zimbabwe. BMC Infect Dis. May 29 2020;20(1):383. doi: 10.1186/s12879-020-05090-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.D’Agostino RB Sr., Vasan RS, Pencina MJ, et al. General cardiovascular risk profile for use in primary care: the Framingham Heart Study. Circulation. Feb 12 2008;117(6):743–53. doi: 10.1161/circulationaha.107.699579 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Framingham Heart Study. Framingham Heart Study. 2021. 2020. https://framinghamheartstudy.org/fhs-risk-functions/cardiovascular-disease-10-year-risk/).
- 69.Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown G. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. vol Database Record. Psychological Corporation; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- 70.Chelune GJ, Heaton RK, Lehman RA. Neuropsychological and personality correlates of patients’ complaints of disability. Advances in clinical neuropsychology. Springer; 1986:95–126. [Google Scholar]
- 71.Woods SP, Rippeth JD, Frol AB, et al. Interrater reliability of clinical ratings and neurocognitive diagnoses in HIV. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. Sep 2004;26(6):759–78. doi: 10.1080/13803390490509565 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Nyamayaro P, Chibanda D, Robbins RN, Hakim J, Gouse H. Assessment of neurocognitive deficits in people living with HIV in Sub Saharan Africa: A systematic review. Clin Neuropsychol. Jan-Dec 2019;33(sup1):1–26. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2019.1606284 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Lakens D Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Review. Frontiers in Psychology. 2013-November-26 2013;4(863)doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Tukey JW. Comparing individual means in the analysis of variance. Biometrics. Jun 1949;5(2):99–114. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Gouse H, Thomas KGF, Masson CJ, et al. Generating and testing neuropsychological test norms that are fair, reliable and accurate in a low- and middle-income country. Under Review. 2021;
- 76.Carey CL, Woods SP, Gonzalez R, et al. Predictive validity of global deficit scores in detecting neuropsychological impairment in HIV infection. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. May 2004;26(3):307–19. doi: 10.1080/13803390490510031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Andrea J, Visser M. Home and school environmental determinants of science achievement of South African students. South African Journal of Education. 2017;37:1–10. [Google Scholar]
- 78.Lyu J, Burr JA. Socioeconomic Status Across the Life Course and Cognitive Function Among Older Adults: An Examination of the Latency, Pathways, and Accumulation Hypotheses. J Aging Health. Feb 2016;28(1):40–67. doi: 10.1177/0898264315585504 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Wu F, Guo Y, Zheng Y, et al. Social-Economic Status and Cognitive Performance among Chinese Aged 50 Years and Older. PloS one. 2016;11(11):e0166986–e0166986. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166986 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.LaMonica HM, Biddle DJ, Naismith SL, Hickie IB, Maruff P, Glozier N. The relationship between depression and cognitive function in adults with cardiovascular risk: Evidence from a randomised attention-controlled trial. Plos One. Sep 5 2018;13(9)doi:ARTN e0203343 10.1371/journal.pone.0203343 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Nuño L, Gómez-Benito J, Carmona VR, Pino O. A Systematic Review of Executive Function and Information Processing Speed in Major Depression Disorder. Brain Sci. Jan 22 2021;11(2)doi: 10.3390/brainsci11020147 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Heaton RK, Clifford DB, Franklin DR Jr., et al. HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders persist in the era of potent antiretroviral therapy: CHARTER Study. Neurology. Dec 07 2010;75(23):2087–96. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318200d727 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Walker KA, Power MC, Gottesman RF. Defining the Relationship Between Hypertension, Cognitive Decline, and Dementia: a Review. Curr Hypertens Rep. Mar 2017;19(3):24. doi: 10.1007/s11906-017-0724-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Rourke SB, Bekele T, Rachlis A, et al. Asymptomatic neurocognitive impairment is a risk for symptomatic decline over a 3-year study period. Aids. Jan 1 2021;35(1):63–72. doi: 10.1097/qad.0000000000002709 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Kordovski VM, Woods SP, Verduzco M, Beltran J. The effects of aging and HIV disease on employment status and functioning. Rehabil Psychol. Nov 2017;62(4):591–599. doi: 10.1037/rep0000175 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Heaton RK, Franklin DR, Ellis RJ, et al. HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders before and during the era of combination antiretroviral therapy: differences in rates, nature, and predictors. Journal of Neurovirology. Feb 2011;17(1):3–16. doi: 10.1007/s13365-010-0006-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Aghvinian M, Santoro AF, Gouse H, et al. Taking the Test: A Qualitative Analysis of Cultural and Contextual Factors Impacting Neuropsychological Assessment of Xhosa-Speaking South Africans. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. Nov 25 2020;doi: 10.1093/arclin/acaa115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]