Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2022 Apr 25;11:e75018. doi: 10.7554/eLife.75018

Distinct representations of body and head motion are dynamically encoded by Purkinje cell populations in the macaque cerebellum

Omid A Zobeiri 1, Kathleen E Cullen 2,3,4,5,
Editors: Megan R Carey6, Andrew J King7
PMCID: PMC9075952  PMID: 35467528

Abstract

The ability to accurately control our posture and perceive our spatial orientation during self-motion requires knowledge of the motion of both the head and body. However, while the vestibular sensors and nuclei directly encode head motion, no sensors directly encode body motion. Instead, the integration of vestibular and neck proprioceptive inputs is necessary to transform vestibular information into the body-centric reference frame required for postural control. The anterior vermis of the cerebellum is thought to play a key role in this transformation, yet how its Purkinje cells transform multiple streams of sensory information into an estimate of body motion remains unknown. Here, we recorded the activity of individual anterior vermis Purkinje cells in alert monkeys during passively applied whole-body, body-under-head, and head-on-body rotations. Most Purkinje cells dynamically encoded an intermediate representation of self-motion between head and body motion. Notably, Purkinje cells responded to both vestibular and neck proprioceptive stimulation with considerable heterogeneity in their response dynamics. Furthermore, their vestibular responses were tuned to head-on-body position. In contrast, targeted neurons in the deep cerebellar nuclei are known to unambiguously encode either head or body motion across conditions. Using a simple population model, we established that combining responses of~40-50 Purkinje cells could explain the responses of these deep cerebellar nuclei neurons across all self-motion conditions. We propose that the observed heterogeneity in Purkinje cell response dynamics underlies the cerebellum’s capacity to compute the dynamic representation of body motion required to ensure accurate postural control and perceptual stability in our daily lives.

Research organism: Rhesus macaque

Introduction

The cerebellum guides motor performance by computing differences between the expected versus actual consequences of movements and then adjusting the commands sent to the motor system (reviewed in Wolpert et al., 1998; Raymond and Medina, 2018). Patients with damage to the anterior vermis of the cerebellum show impaired posture and balance, as well as deficits in motor coordination (Diener et al., 1984; Bastian et al., 1998; Ilg et al., 2008; Sullivan et al., 2006; Mitoma et al., 2020). In this context, the anterior vermis has a vital role in the vestibulospinal pathways that generate the postural adjustments required to ensure the maintenance of balance during our everyday activities. Additionally, there is an emerging consensus that the cerebellum contributes to our self-motion perception. Indeed, patients with degeneration of the cerebellar vermis demonstrate reduced perceptual time constants and detection thresholds to externally applied rotations (Bronstein et al., 2008; Dahlem et al., 2016).

The prevailing view is that the vestibular pathways mediating vestibulospinal reflexes and the stable perception of self-motion explicitly transform vestibular information from a head-centered to a body-centered reference frame. The vestibular sensory organs are located within the head, making the vestibular system’s native reference frame head-centered (reviewed in Cullen, 2019). In turn, vestibular nerve afferents and their targets in the vestibular nuclei also encode information in a head-centered reference frame (Roy and Cullen, 1998; Roy and Cullen, 2001; Roy and Cullen, 2004; Carriot et al., 2013; Brooks and Cullen, 2014; Sadeghi et al., 2007; Jamali et al., 2009; Cullen and Minor, 2002). However, the brain accounts for the position of the head relative to the body for vestibulospinal reflexes to accurately control the musculature required to maintain upright posture and balance (Tokita et al., 1989; Tokita et al., 2009 Kennedy and Inglis, 2002). Indeed, distinct representations of body versus head motion are encoded by individual neurons in the fastigial nucleus (Brooks and Cullen, 2009; Brooks and Cullen, 2014) – the most medial of deep cerebellar nuclei – which lesion studies have shown serves an important role in the control of posture and balance (Thach et al., 1992; Kurzan et al., 1993; Pélisson et al., 1998). Neck proprioceptors provide the head position information required for this transformation (reviewed in Cullen and Zobeiri, 2021). Thus, the integration of neck proprioceptive and vestibular signals is thought to underlie the transformation from a head-centered to a body-centered reference frame in vestibulospinal reflexes pathways as well as our ability to perceive body motion independently of head motion (Mergner et al., 1997; Peterka, 2002).

There are many reasons to believe that the anterior region of the cerebellar vermis is vital in the transformation of vestibular information from a head-centered to a body-centered reference frame. First, Purkinje cells in this region project to the rostral portion of the fastigial nucleus (Batton et al., 1977; Yamada and Noda, 1987), which lesion studies have shown serves an important role in the control of posture and balance (Thach et al., 1992; Kurzan et al., 1993; Pélisson et al., 1998). Second, inhibition of the cerebellar vermis via continuous theta-burst stimulation impairs the modulation of vestibulospinal pathways that normally accounts for changes in the position of the head relative to the body (Lam et al., 2016). Third, neuronal recordings from anterior vermis Purkinje cells in decerebrate cats have demonstrated that individual neurons can encode both vestibular and neck proprioceptive-related information (Denoth et al., 1979; Manzoni et al., 1998; Manzoni et al., 1999; Manzoni et al., 2004), thereby providing a neural substrate for the coordinate transformation using neck proprioceptive signals to convert head-centered vestibular signals to a body-centered reference frame. However, these studies stopped short of establishing whether Purkinje cells integrate vestibular and neck proprioceptive signals to dynamically encode head or body movement.

Thus, a key question yet to be answered is: Does the cerebellum integrate vestibular and neck proprioceptive signals to provide a dynamic representation of body motion relative to space? Here, we recorded the activity of single Purkinje cells in the anterior vermis during head motion, body motion, and combined head and body motion. We found considerable heterogeneity across individual Purkinje cells in their encoding of head versus body motion, with most (~75%) neurons dynamically encoding an intermediate representation of self-motion between head and body motion. These neurons, termed bimodal neurons, responded to both vestibular and neck proprioceptive stimulation and displayed head-position-dependent tuning in their sensitivity to vestibular stimulation. In contrast, a minority of cells, termed unimodal neurons, only responded to vestibular stimulation and unambiguously encoded the motion of the head in space. Across all cells, the linear combination of a given neuron’s response sensitivity to dynamic neck and vestibular stimulation alone well estimated its response during combined stimulation. Finally, we found that a simple linear combination model combining the responses of ~40 Purkinje cells could account for the more homogeneous responses of target neurons in the deep cerebellar nuclei (i.e., the rostral fastigial nucleus [rFN]) during applied self-motion. Our results provide the first evidence, at the level of single Purkinje cells, that a sequential transformation from a head-centered to body-centered reference frame occurs between the cerebellum and deep cerebellar nucleus to ensure postural and perceptual stability in everyday life.

Results

Most vestibular-sensitive Purkinje cells in the anterior vermis are also sensitive to stimulation of neck proprioceptors

Each Purkinje cell in our population (n = 73) was responsive to vestibular stimulation and was insensitive to eye movements. To assess each neuron’s vestibular sensitivity, we applied ipsilaterally and contralaterally directed whole-body rotations in the dark (i.e., whole-body-rotations; see Materials and methods). As illustrated in Figure 1A, we found considerable heterogeneity in vestibular sensitivities across our Purkinje cell population. Some neurons generated excitatory versus inhibitory responses for oppositely directed head movements (Figure 1A; left, linear). Alternately, some neurons generated bidirectional excitatory responses (center, v-shaped), while others largely only generated excitatory responses for one movement direction (Figure 1A; right, rectifying). This contrasts with the vestibular responses recorded in areas targeted by Purkinje cells in the anterior vermis. Notably, vestibular-only neurons in the rFN and vestibular nucleus consistently show excitatory versus inhibitory responses for oppositely directed head movements (rFN: Gardner and Fuchs, 1975; Shaikh et al., 2005; vestibular nucleus: Scudder and Fuchs, 1992; Cullen and McCrea, 1993; McCrea et al., 1999; Roy and Cullen, 2004).

Figure 1. Purkinje cell simple spike’s responses to vestibular stimulation.

(A) Vestibular stimulation was generated by applying passive whole-body rotations about the vertical axis. The resulting neural responses are shown for three example Purkinje cells. The top two rows illustrate rotational head and body velocities. The bottom row shows the simple spike firing rate (gray shaded regions) with the linear estimation of the firing rate based on head motion superimposed (blue traces). The heat maps show the simple spike firing rate for individual trials. Insets: the relationship between simple spike firing rate (phase-corrected) and angular head-in-space velocity. (B,C) Distribution of vestibular sensitivities for motion in the preferred (B) direction (i.e., the direction resulting in the larger increase in simple spike firing rate) and non-preferred (C) direction. The dashed lines are fits on the distributions. Note, by convention positive and negative values in (B) represent cells with Type I versus II vestibular responses (i.e., preferred direction was ipsilateral versus contralateral, respectively). Insets: polar plots where the vector length and angle represent each neuron’s vestibular response sensitivity and phase, respectively. Filled and open arrows represent the population-averaged vectors for Type I and II cells, respectively.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Purkinje cells show heterogeneity in their simple spike responses to vestibular stimulation.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) The contribution of each kinematic term (i.e., position, velocity, acceleration) in estimating the firing rate for the preferred (left) and non-preferred (right) direction of whole-body movement, computed as the % drop in total variance-accounted-for (VAF) when removed from the full model. Neurons that did not significantly respond to the non-preferred direction are shown as gray bars. Note, we sorted Purkinje cells based on the importance of the velocity term, since this is what is predominately encoded by their target neurons in the rostral fastigial nucleus (rFN) and vestibular nuclei. Interestingly, in the non-preferred direction, the acceleration term (yellow) contributes more to estimated firing rate (i.e., greater reduction VAF when removed) indicating that Purkinje cell’s responses generally led stimulation velocity. (B) The computed VAF (mean ± 95% CI) in estimating the firing rate for the preferred (left) and non-preferred (right) direction of whole-body movement. Black and gray bars represent bimodal and unimodal Purkinje cells, respectively. The red area corresponds to non-significant VAF values computed from the same stimulation and shuffled inter-spike intervals (ISI). (C) The response of the Purkinje cells grouped as (i) linear, which demonstrated increased and decreased firing rate in the preferred and non-preferred directions, respectively, (ii) v-shape, which demonstrated increased firing rate in both directions, (iii) rectifying, which demonstrated increased firing rate in the preferred direction and minimal modulation in the non-preferred direction, and (iv) others, which did not meet any of the mentioned criteria. (D) The pie chart illustrates the percentage of each category within the Purkinje cells.

To quantify the vestibular sensitivity of each Purkinje cell in our population, we fit a least-squares dynamic regression model with three kinematic terms (i.e., head-in-space position, velocity, and acceleration) to responses for movements in each direction (Figure 1—figure supplement 1, see Materials and methods). We found that the preferred movement direction (i.e., the direction that resulted in an excitatory response, or in the greater excitatory response in the case of v-shaped neurons) could be either ipsilateral or contralateral for a given Purkinje cell. Neurons with preferred responses for ipsilaterally (n = 32, e.g., Figure 1A, right panel) or contralaterally (n = 41, Figure 1A, left and middle panels) directed rotations were accordingly classified as Type I or Type II, respectively. Our analysis further revealed that the response dynamics varied considerably across neurons, with 43% neurons demonstrating responses that were relatively in-phase (±15°) with head velocity (1.8 ± 7.7°), while others demonstrated marked response leads (32%, 57 ± 31) or lags (25%, –47.5 ± 22°). Figure 1B, C illustrates the vestibular response vectors for our populations of neurons computed in their preferred and non-preferred directions, respectively. The vector represents the gain (length) and phase (angle) of the neural responses to each stimulus computed at 1 Hz (see Materials and methods). The large arrows represent average neuronal responses to vestibular stimuli for Type I (Svest. = 0.42 ± 0.37 (sp/s)/(°/s), Phasevest. = 6 ± 31) and Type II neurons (Svest. = 0.31 ± 0.34 (sp/s)/(°/s), Phasevest. = 172 ± 42°), respectively.

We next addressed whether the Purkinje cells that responded to vestibular stimulation also responded to the activation of neck proprioceptors. To assess each neuron’s proprioceptive sensitivity, we applied ipsilaterally and contralaterally directed rotations to the monkey’s body while its head was held stationary relative to space (i.e., body-under-head rotations; see Materials and methods), with the same motion profiles as those used for the assessment of vestibular sensitivities. Thus, since the head did not move relative to space, neck proprioceptors but not the vestibular system were stimulated in this condition. Figure 2A illustrates the responses recorded from the same three example neurons shown in Figure 1A. We quantified the neck proprioceptive sensitivity of each Purkinje cell using least-squares dynamic regression (see Materials and methods) and found that most neurons (~75%, n = 54) were sensitive to passive proprioceptive stimulation (Figure 2B, C, filled bars; bimodal neurons), whereas the remaining ~25% (n = 19) were insensitive (Figure 2, Figure 2B, C, open bars; unimodal neurons). Overall, similar to our findings above regarding vestibular stimulation, the dynamics of responses to proprioceptive stimulation varied considerably across Purkinje cells (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). The insets in Figure 2B, C illustrate the response vectors for proprioceptive stimulation across our populations of neurons in the preferred and non-preferred directions, respectively. As in Figure 1B, C above, the vector was computed based on the gain (length) and phase (angle) of the neural responses to each stimulus at 1 Hz (see Materials and methods) and large arrows represent average neuronal responses measured in response to proprioceptive stimuli for neurons with Type I (Sprop. = 0.12 ± 0.44 (sp/s)/(°/s), Phaseprop. = 159 ± 29°) versus Type II (Sprop. = 0.13 ± 0.46 (sp/s)/(°/s), Phaseprop. = –27 ± 20°) responses. Furthermore, Purkinje cells showed considerable heterogeneity in their simple spike response dynamics to vestibular versus proprioceptive stimulation (Figure 2—figure supplement 2). Indeed, neurons typically did not show the same patterns (linear, v-shaped, rectified) to vestibular versus proprioceptive stimulation (Figure 2—figure supplement 3).

Figure 2. Purkinje cell simple spike’s responses to neck proprioceptive stimulation.

(A) Proprioceptive stimulation was generated by applying body-under-head rotation about the vertical axis while holding the head earth. The resulting neural responses are illustrated for the same three example Purkinje cells shown above in Figure 1. The top two rows illustrate rotational head and body velocities. The bottom row shows the resultant simple spike firing rate (gray shaded regions) with the linear estimation of the firing rate based on body motion superimposed (green traces). The heat maps show the simple spike firing rate for individual trials. Insets: the relationship between simple spike firing rate (phase-corrected) and angular body-in-space velocity. (B, C) Distribution of proprioceptive sensitivities for the preferred (B) and non-preferred (C) directions of body movement. Filled versus open bars represent neurons that were sensitive versus insensitive to neck proprioceptive stimulation (i.e., bimodal versus unimodal cells, respectively). The dashed lines are fits on the distributions. Insets: polar plots where the vector length and angle represent each neuron’s proprioceptive response sensitivity and phase, respectively. Filled versus open arrows represent the population-averaged vectors for neurons with Type I versus II vestibular responses (i.e., Figure 1), respectively.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Purkinje cells show heterogeneity in their simple spike responses to proprioceptive stimulation.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) The contribution of each kinematic term (i.e., position, velocity, acceleration) in estimating the firing rate for the preferred (left) and non-preferred (right) direction of body movement (i.e., body-under-head) for bimodal Purkinje cells, computed as the % drop in total variance-accounted-for (VAF) when removed from the full model. Neurons that did not significantly respond to the non-preferred direction are shown as gray bars. Note, we sorted the Purkinje cells based on the importance of the velocity term, since this is what is predominately encoded by the target neurons in the rostral fastigial nucleus (rFN) and vestibular nuclei. (B) The computed VAF (mean ± 95% CI) in estimating the firing rate for preferred (left) and non-preferred (right) direction of whole-body movement. Black and gray bars represent bimodal and unimodal Purkinje cells, respectively. The red area corresponds to non-significant VAF values computed from the same stimulation and shuffled inter-spike intervals (ISI). (C) The response of the bimodal Purkinje cells, which are grouped as (i) linear, which demonstrated increased and decreased firing rate in the preferred and non-preferred directions, respectively, (ii) V-shape, which demonstrated increased firing rate in both directions, (iii) rectifying, which demonstrated increased firing rate in the preferred direction and minimal modulation in the non-preferred direction, and (iv) others, which did not meet any of the mentioned criteria. (D) The pie chart illustrates the percentage of each category within the Purkinje cells.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Purkinje cells show heterogeneity in their simple spike responses to vestibular versus proprioceptive stimulation.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(A, B) Scatter plots comparing the sensitivity to vestibular and neck proprioceptive stimulation for unimodal (white circles) and bimodal (black circles) Purkinje cells for preferred (A) and non-preferred (B) direction of movement. The gray shaded area corresponds to the Purkinje cells with larger sensitivity to vestibular compared to neck proprioceptive sensitivity. (C, D) Scatter plots comparing the phase of the responses to vestibular and neck proprioceptive stimulation for bimodal Purkinje cells for the preferred (C) and non-preferred (D) direction of movement. (E, F) The distribution of the neuronal sensitivity of the Purkinje cells simple spikes’ response to the vestibular stimulation for the preferred (E) and non-preferred (F) direction of movement. Open bars correspond to the cells that did not show a significant response to the neck proprioceptive stimulation (i.e., unimodal cells).
Figure 2—figure supplement 3. For most of the Purkinje cells, the responses to vestibular and neck proprioceptive stimulation were classified in different groups.

Figure 2—figure supplement 3.

(A) Venn diagram showing the number of Purkinje cells that were grouped as linear, v-shape, rectifying, and other for vestibular and neck proprioceptive responses and their overlap (filled area). (B) The number of Purkinje cells that were classified in pairs of groups based on their response to vestibular (y-axis) and neck proprioceptive (x-axis) inputs.

Purkinje cell’s responses to simultaneous proprioceptive and vestibular stimulation

So far, we have shown that most Purkinje cells in our population were sensitive to neck proprioceptive as well as vestibular stimulation, and that we categorized these neurons as ‘bimodal’ versus neurons that were only responsive to vestibular stimulation as ‘unimodal’. The vestibular sensitivities of bimodal Purkinje cells were comparable to those of their unimodal counterparts (p = 0.17). We further found that the neck sensitivities of bimodal neurons were most often (67%) antagonistic relative to their vestibular sensitivities in the preferred direction. This can be seen in Figure 3A and B, where the average vectors representing the neuronal response to the vestibular and proprioceptive stimulation point in opposite directions (Figure 3A and B, thick blue versus green arrows, respectively). In contrast, relative to vestibular sensitivities in non-preferred direction, bimodal cells were as likely to have antagonistic as agonistic responses to neck proprioceptive stimulation (Figure 3—figure supplement 1).

Figure 3. Purkinje cells simple spike’s responses to combined vestibular-proprioceptive stimulation.

(A, B) Polar plots illustrating the vestibular (blue) and neck proprioceptive (green) neuronal response sensitivities of Type I (A) and Type II (B) Purkinje cells for preferred direction of vestibular stimulation and complementary direction proprioceptive stimulation (i.e., body-under-head motion). Bold blue and green arrows represent the mean population vectors, respectively. Inset: scatter plots comparing the sensitivity of Type I (A) and Type II (B) Purkinje cells to vestibular and neck proprioceptive inputs. (C) Combined vestibular-proprioceptive stimulation was generated by applying passive head-on-body rotations about the vertical axis. The resulting neural responses are shown for the same three example Purkinje cells shown above in Figures 1 and 2. The top two rows illustrate rotational head and body velocity. The bottom row shows the resultant simple spike firing rate (gray shaded regions). The linear estimation of firing rate based on head motion (solid black traces) and the firing rate prediction based on the linear summation of neck proprioceptive and vestibular sensitivities (dashed red traces) are both superimposed. Each neuron’s preferred motion direction for vestibular stimulation is indicated by the gray column. Polar plots (top) represent the sensitivity and phase of each neuron’s response to vestibular, proprioceptive, and combined stimulation as well as the response predicted by the summation model. (D, E) Bar plots comparing the sensitivities of bimodal and unimodal Purkinje cells to vestibular, proprioceptive, and combined stimulation in the preferred (D) and non-preferred (E) motion directions, as defined by each neuron’s responses to vestibular stimulation. The response sensitivities of Type I and II neurons are reported as positive values relative to ipsilaterally and contralaterally directed head movements, respectively, to facilitate comparison across all Purkinje cells.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Purkinje cell’s responses to combined vestibular-neck proprioceptive stimulation in the non-preferred direction of vestibular stimulation.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A, B) Polar plots illustrating the vestibular (blue) and neck proprioceptive (green) neuronal response sensitivities of Type I (A) and Type II (B) Purkinje cells for non-preferred direction of vestibular stimulation and complementary direction proprioceptive stimulation (i.e., body-under-head motion). Superimposed blue and green arrows represent the mean population vectors, respectively.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Polar representations of Purkinje cells simple spike’s response.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

Polar plots representating each individual Purkinje cells’ response ordered by increasing discrepancy between the summed and combined responses in the preferred direction. Response vectors predicted by the summation model (red) are superimposed on those for vestibular (blue), proprioceptive (green), and combined stimulation (black).

During everyday activities, we move our head relative to our body, and thus simultaneously activate both vestibular sensors and neck proprioceptors. To directly establish how vestibular and neck proprioceptive information is integrated in the anterior vermis, we next recorded the responses of the same Purkinje cell populations during combined stimulation of neck proprioceptors and the vestibular system. Specifically, we applied ipsilaterally and contralaterally directed rotations of the monkey’s head relative to its earth-stationary body (i.e., head-on-body rotations; see Materials and methods), again with the same motion profiles as those used above in the assessment of neuronal vestibular and neck proprioceptive sensitivities. The responses of the same three example neurons above in Figures 1 and 2 are shown in Figure 3C. The head motion-based linear estimation of firing rate (solid black traces, see Materials and methods) is plotted on the firing rate for each cell. A firing rate prediction (dashed red traces) based on the linear summation of each neuron’s sensitivity to vestibular and neck proprioceptive stimulation when each was applied in isolation (i.e., Figures 1 and 2, respectively) is superimposed for comparison. The example neurons were typical in that each neuron’s modulation for combined stimulation in the preferred direction (i.e., gray columns) was well predicted by the linear summation of the neuron’s vestibular and proprioception sensitivities. The polar plots show the vector summation (dashed red arrow) of the example neuron’s response to vestibular and proprioceptive stimulation (blue and green arrows) when applied in isolation versus the response vector for combined stimulation (black arrow; Figure 3—figure supplement 2). Correspondingly, there was good alignment between the vector length and direction computed for the firing rate estimate and prediction for these three example neurons for the head-on-body motion in the preferred direction.

Figure 3D and E summarizes the population data for unimodal and bimodal Purkinje cells in each of the three stimulation conditions. Average response sensitivities are shown for preferred (Figure 3D) and non-preferred (Figure 3E) direction motion. Note that since there were no significant differences between the response of Type I and II cells (other than their preferred direction), we reported the responses of both groups together, by accounting for the difference in the direction of the modulation of Type II cells. We first hypothesized that the vestibular sensitivity of unimodal neurons should remain constant across conditions regardless of whether the neck proprioceptors were stimulated. Consistent with this proposal, we found that unimodal cell response sensitivities (open bars) were comparable during the vestibular-only and combined stimulation conditions (Figure 3D and E, p > 0.22). Likewise, response phases of unimodal neurons were comparable for both conditions (preferred: 11 ± 5 versus 11 ± 14; p = 0.21; versus non-preferred: 27 ± 8 versus 14 ± 18; p = 0.77). In contrast, we hypothesized that since the vestibular and proprioceptive sensitivities of bimodal Purkinje cells were generally antagonist (i.e., Figure 3A, Figure 3—figure supplement 1), the oppositely modulated inputs from neck proprioceptors should effectively suppress the vestibular-driven responses during the combined conditions. Indeed, consistent with this prediction, the sensitivities of bimodal Purkinje cells were reduced during the combined stimulation condition relative to the vestibular-only condition (Figure 3D and E, filled bars, p < 0.027).

Above, we showed that the responses of our three example neurons in the combined stimulation were well predicted by the linear summation of the neuron’s vestibular and proprioception sensitivities applied in isolation, particularly for stimulation in the preferred direction. We next explicitly addressed whether this simple linear model of vestibular-proprioceptive integration could reliably predict neuronal responses in the combined condition across our population of Purkinje cells. To do this, we compared the estimated and predicted head-on-body rotation sensitivities (Figure 4A) and phases (Figure 4B) for all the Purkinje cells in our sample on a neuron by neuron basis. Overall, estimated and predicted sensitivities and phases were comparable (R2 = 0.77 and 0.75 for sensitivity and phase, respectively, p < 0.001). The similarity between values is shown by the slope of the line fitted to the data which were not different from 1 (p = 0.42 and 0.83 for gain and phase, respectively). Thus, the summation model provided a good estimate of the gain of the preferred direction responses during combined stimulation. Similar results were obtained in our analysis of the non-preferred direction responses (Figure 4—figure supplement 1).

Figure 4. Purkinje cell simple spike’s responses to combined stimulation are well predicted by the linear summation of a given neuron’s responses to vestibular and proprioceptive stimulation when applied alone.

(A, B) Comparison of estimated and predicted sensitivities (A) and phases (B) of Purkinje cell’s responses to head-on-body rotations in the preferred movement direction. The linear summation of a given neuron’s vestibular and neck proprioceptive sensitivities well predicts both sensitivity and phase measures in the combined condition. Blue lines and shading denote the mean ± 95% CI of linear fit.

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Purkinje cell simple spike’s responses to combined stimulation are well predicted by the linear summation of a given neuron’s responses to vestibular and proprioceptive stimulation when applied alone.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A, B) Comparison of estimated and predicted sensitivities (A) and phases (B) of Purkinje cell’s responses to head-on-body rotations in the non-preferred movement direction. The linear summation of a given neuron’s vestibular and neck proprioceptive sensitivities well predicted both measures in the combined condition. Blue lines and shading denote the mean ± 95% CI of linear fit.

As stated above, the generation of vestibulospinal reflexes requires central pathways to explicitly transform vestibular information from a head-centered to a body-centered reference frame during self-motion. To better understand the coding by our population of neurons, we first computed a ‘head sensitivity’ and a ‘body sensitivity’ ratio for each neuron (Figure 5A, see Materials and methods). Two theoretical neurons, one that selectively encodes head-in-space and the other that selectively encodes body movement, are indicated by red and orange stars, respectively. A neuron selectively encoding head-in-space motion (red star) would display comparable responses to whole-body and head-on-body rotations (head sensitivity ratio = 1), while not responding to body-under-head rotations (body sensitivity ratio = 0). On the other hand, a neuron selectively encoding body motion would display a comparable response to whole-body and body-under-head rotations (orange star, body sensitivity ratio = 1), while not responding to head-on-body rotations (head sensitivity ratio = 0). In contrast, comparison of these ratios across each of the Purkinje cells in our neuronal population revealed considerable heterogeneity in the relationship between these two measures relative to these theoretical neurons. As reviewed above, anterior vermis Purkinje cells target neurons in the deep cerebellar nuclei (i.e., rFN) (Batton et al., 1977; Yamada and Noda, 1987). Thus, for comparison, we computed these ratios for rFN neurons that had been studied during the same conditions (Brooks and Cullen, 2009). The red and orange shaded areas represent the distribution of our sensitivity ratios for the unimodal and bimodal rFN neuron populations reported in this prior study. Notably, in contrast to the Purkinje cells of our present study, the relationship between the rFN unimodal and bimodal neuron sensitivity ratios are well aligned with that of our theoretical neurons that selectively encoded head and body movement, respectively.

Figure 5. Heterogeneity in Purkinje cell simple spike encoding of head and body movement.

(A) Scatter plot of the relationship between the head sensitivity ratio (Svest.+prop./Svest.) and body sensitivity ratio (Sprop./Svest.) for the preferred direction. Histograms (top and right) illustrate the distributions of body and head sensitivity ratios, respectively. Orange versus red stars indicate ideal encoding of body versus head movement in space, respectively. For comparison, the red and orange shaded areas representing the distribution of values estimated for unimodal and bimodal rostral fastigial nucleus (rFN) neurons (Brooks and Cullen, 2009) are superimposed. Inset: examples of the responses of a bimodal (orange) and unimodal (red) rFN neurons during whole-body, body-under-head, and head-on-body movement are shown for comparison (Figure 5A has been adapted from Figure 1 from Brooks and Cullen, 2013). (B) Distribution of coding indexes (see Materials and methods). Positive and negative values correspond to agonistic and antagonistic responses to head versus body encoding, respectively. Inset: the distribution of coding indices estimated for rFN neurons (Brooks and Cullen, 2009) is shown for comparison.

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Heterogeneity in Purkinje cell simple spike encoding of head and body movement for the non-preferred direction.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(A) Scatter plot of the relationship between the head sensitivity ratio (Svest.+prop./Svest.) and body sensitivity ratio (Sprop./Svest.) for the non-preferred direction. Histograms (top and right) illustrate the distributions of body and head sensitivity ratios, respectively. Orange versus red stars indicate ideal encoding of body versus head movement in space, respectively. For comparison, the red and orange shaded areas representing the distribution of values estimated for unimodal and bimodal rostral fastigial nucleus (rFN) neurons (Brooks and Cullen, 2009) are superimposed. (B) Distribution of coding indexes (see Materials and methods). Positive and negative values correspond to agonistic and antagonistic responses to head versus body encoding, respectively.

To next evaluate the transformation of vestibular information from a head-centered to body-centered reference frame during self-motion for each Purkinje cell, we computed a coding index (see Materials and methods). Specifically, this index compared each neuron’s sensitivity when only the head moved relative to space (i.e., head-on-body rotation, Figure 3) versus when only the body moved relative to space (i.e., body-under-head rotation, Figure 2), to the combined stimulation condition. The results from the analysis of our Purkinje cell population are shown in Figure 5B. Indeed, only a minority of neurons were designated as primarily head (26%) or body (2%) encoding (light and dark orange bars, respectively). A complementary analysis of non-preferred direction responses (relative to vestibular stimulation) revealed similar results (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). Again, the corresponding distribution of coding indices estimated for rFN neurons from Brooks and Cullen, 2009, is shown for comparison (Figure 5B, top right inset). In contrast to Purkinje cells, the majority of neurons were designated as primarily head (34%) or body (26%) encoding.

Influence of head position on Purkinje cell’s vestibular responses

In theoretical models of reference frame transformations, responses to the sensory inputs are generally modulated by a postural signal (e.g., the position of the head relative to the body) (Pouget and Snyder, 2000, Salinas and Abbott, 2001). Indeed, neurons in the deep cerebellar nuclei of primates show such tuning. Specifically, the vestibular responses of bimodal neurons in the rFN modulate as a function of head position (Brooks and Cullen, 2009; Kleine et al., 2004; Shaikh et al., 2004). This finding has been taken as support for the view that a reference frame transformation of vestibular signals from head- to body-centered occurs in the cerebellar vermis (reviewed in Cullen, 2019). Thus, we next asked: How is this tuning generated? And more specifically, is it computed within the deep cerebellar nuclei or instead inherited from the Purkinje cells that target neurons in the deep cerebellar nuclei? To address these questions, we first determined whether the vestibular responses of Purkinje cells were affected by static changes in head-on-body position (i.e., ‘gain-field’ condition, see Materials and methods). We measured neuronal responses to vestibular stimulation (i.e., whole-body rotation) applied with the head positioned at five different orientations ranging from −30 (left) to +30° (right) relative to the body (−30°, –15°, 0°, 15°, and 30°). The example bimodal neuron was typical in that it displayed marked changes in vestibular sensitivity with changes in head-on-body position (Figure 6A). In contrast, we did not find evidence for such tuning in unimodal neurons (Figure 6B).

Figure 6. The vestibular responses of bimodal Purkinje cells show head-on-body position-dependent tuning.

Figure 6.

(A, B) Tuning curves for the vestibular sensitivities of an example bimodal (A) and unimodal (B) Purkinje cell measured by applying whole-body rotation with the head oriented at different positions relative to the body. Note, bimodal neurons, but not unimodal neurons, show tuning as a function of head-on-body position. (C) Top panel: Distributions of tuning widths (left), amplitudes (middle), and means (right) for bimodal (filled bars, N = 13) and unimodal (open bars, N = 4) Purkinje cells. Bottom panel: For comparison, the same distributions are plotted for a population of rostral fastigial nucleus (rFN) neurons previously characterized using a comparable approach (Figure 6C has been adapted from Figure 5 from Brooks and Cullen, 2009). (D) Average tuning curves computed by aligning the peak of each individual neuron’s tuning curve. Average tuning curves are shown for bimodal and unimodal Purkinje cells (blue) for vestibular stimulation with the head oriented at different positions relative to the body. Again, for comparison, the average tuning curves of rFN neurons are superimposed (Figure 6D has been adapted from Figure 6 from Brooks and Cullen, 2009).

To quantify each Purkinje cell’s tuning, we fit a Gaussian function to vestibular sensitivity as a function of head position (see Materials and methods), and computed the tuning width, amplitude, and mean direction provided by the best fit to each neuron (Figure 6C, top row; filled and open blue bars denote bimodal and unimodal neurons N = 13 versus 4, respectively). Note that the small number of unimodal neurons tested in this condition reflects that they constituted a relatively small percentage of our overall Purkinje cell population. First, bimodal neurons were more narrowly tuned than were unimodal neurons (mean tuning widths; 7.2 versus 15°, respectively). Additionally, bimodal neurons showed stronger tuning relative to unimodal neurons (mean tuning amplitude; 0.52 versus 0.05 (sp/s)/(°/s)), respectively. Finally, there was no difference in the mean of the tuning curve between unimodal neurons and bimodal neurons (p > 0.37). We next compared the tuning of our bimodal and unimodal Purkinje cells with that previously described for their target neurons in the rFN (Brooks and Cullen, 2009). The corresponding distributions of rFN neuron tuning width, amplitude, and mean direction are plotted in the bottom row of Figure 6C. To facilitate comparison between the tuning of Purkinje and rFN cells, we aligned the peak of each individual neuron’s tuning curve with zero and averaged the resultant curves across bimodal and unimodal groups for each (Figure 6D). Overall, the strength of tuning was significantly higher for bimodal rFN than Purkinje cells (Figure 6D, compare solid gray and black lines, ~30% reduction for Purkinje cells, p < 0.001). Tuning width was also reduced for bimodal Purkinje cells (~40% reduction), while mean tuning direction was comparable for both cell groups (p > 0.05). Moreover, tuning was consistently stronger for bimodal than unimodal neurons in Purkinje cells as has previously been shown for rFN neurons (Figure 6D, compare solid and dashed lines). We note that because the interaction between vestibular responses and head-on-body position that underlies the tuning shown in Figure 6 is inherently nonlinear, this tuning cannot be predicted by the component of the Purkinje cells’ dynamic modulation that is in phase with head position during body-under-head rotation (i.e., Figure 2A).

Linear combination of the Purkinje cells’ response can encode head and body motion

To summarize, our results have shown that while most vestibular-sensitive Purkinje cells in the anterior vermis integrate vestibular and neck proprioceptive signals, the transformation from head- to body-centered reference frame is not complete. Instead, single bimodal Purkinje cells generally dynamically encoded intermediate representations of self-motion that were between head and body motion. In contrast, bimodal neurons in the deep cerebellar nuclei – the primary target of these Purkinje cells (Figure 7A; rFN) – dynamically encode body motion (i.e., orange shaded region, Figure 5C) and also show stronger vestibular tuning as a function of head-on-body-position. Thus, taken together, our present results suggest that the transformation from a head- to body-centered representation of self-motion is achieved by integrating the activities of multiple Purkinje cells.

Figure 7. A simple linear population model of Purkinje cell integration can explain the responses of target bimodal neurons in deep cerebellar nuclei across all self-movement conditions.

(A) Illustration of the convergence of multiple Purkinje cells onto a single neuron in the rostral fastigial nucleus (rFN), with different shades of red representing theoretical differences in the weighing of each Purkinje cell’s synapse with the target rFN neuron. (B) Schematic of the linear summation population model used to estimate the firing rate of a target neuron in the rFN. Each Purkinje cell’s weight was optimized to generate the best estimate of the average bimodal rFN neuron across conditions (Brooks and Cullen, 2009). (C) Model performance as a function of the number of Purkinje cells. Black curve corresponds to model fit to the simple spike firing rates of all 73 Purkinje cells recorded during our three dynamic conditions (i.e., whole-body, body-under-head, and head-on-body movements). Blue curve corresponds to the model fit to simple spike firing rates of all 73 Purkinje cells during these same three dynamic conditions as well as simulated responses of these cells recorded in the gain-field condition (Figure 6). The variability estimated from a population of rFN bimodal neurons previously described by Brooks and Cullen, 2009, is represented by the green shaded band. Inset: the distribution of computed weights for each Purkinje cell modeled during our three dynamic conditions with 40 Purkinje cells, sorted based on average weight. (D) Estimated model firing rates based on a population of 40 Purkinje cells superimposed on the actual average firing rate of a bimodal rFN neuron (gray shaded region). Solid black lines versus dashed blue lines illustrate firing rate estimations from models that included (i) the three dynamic head/body rotation conditions (left) versus (ii) the three dynamic conditions as well as the gain-field condition (right).

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. The distribution of the weights of the inputs to the model with 40 Purkinje cells projecting to a bimodal rostral fastigial nucleus (rFN) neuron.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

The distribution of the Purkinje cell weights in a model with 40 Purkinje cells projecting to (A) Type I and (B) Type II bimodal rFN neuron that were classified as (A1, B1) linear versus v-shaped versus rectifying Purkinje cells, (A2, B2) bimodal versus unimodal Purkinje cells, (A3, B3) Type I versus Type II Purkinje cells, and (A4, B4) agonistic (i.e., same vestibular and proprioceptive sensitivity sign) versus antagonistic (i.e., opposite vestibular and proprioceptive sensitivity sign).
Figure 7—figure supplement 2. The distribution of the weights of the inputs to a model with 40 Purkinje cells projecting to a unimodal rostral fastigial nucleus (rFN) neuron.

Figure 7—figure supplement 2.

The distribution of the Purkinje cell weights in a model with 40 Purkinje cells projecting to (A) Type I and (B) Type II unimodal rFN neuron that were classified as (A1, B1) linear versus v-shaped versus rectifying Purkinje cells, (A2, B2) bimodal versus unimodal Purkinje cells, (A3, B3) Type I versus Type II Purkinje cells, and (A4, B4) agonistic (i.e., same vestibular and proprioceptive sensitivity sign) versus antagonistic (i.e., opposite vestibular and proprioceptive sensitivity sign).
Figure 7—figure supplement 3. Modeling the mossy fiber inputs to unimodal fastigial neurons.

Figure 7—figure supplement 3.

(A) Schematic of a model with a mossy fiber input that is simulated as the summation of random patterns of responses to vestibular and neck proprioceptive input. The gains and phases were randomly drawn from a distribution comparable to that previously reported in the vestibular nuclei (0.6 ± 0.1 (sp/s)/(°/s) and 20 ± 5°, respectively; Mitchell et al., 2017). (B) The performance of the Purkinje cell population in predicting the response of the bimodal (top) and unimodal (bottom) rostral fastigial nucleus (rFN) neurons in the presence (gray) and absence (red) of mossy fibers in (A).
Figure 7—figure supplement 4. Exploring the effect of systematically altering the distribution of gain and phase values in this simulated mossy fiber input.

Figure 7—figure supplement 4.

(A) Five different distributions of mossy fiber inputs. The reference distribution is similar to the neurons in the vestibular nuclei (red; gain: 0.6 ± 0.1 (sp/s)/(°/s), phase: 20 ± 5°). Four other distributions were generated by systematically altering the distribution of gain and phase values for the reference distribution by (i) doubling the gain (orange), (ii) reducing the gain by half (dark blue), (iii) doubling the phase (blue), and (iv) reducing the phase by half (green). (B) The performance of a population model of Purkinje cells projecting to a bimodal Type I rostral fastigial nucleus (rFN) neuron considering mossy fiber inputs with the five distributions shown in (A). The performance was similar for all distributions of mossy fibers. (C) The distribution of the weights of the Purkinje cells in the model with 40 Purkinje cells considering mossy fiber inputs with the distributions shown in (A). Changing the gain but not the phase of mossy fiber inputs affects the weighting of Purkinje cell inputs.
Figure 7—figure supplement 5. A simple linear population model of Purkinje cell integration can explain the responses of target unimodal neurons in deep cerebellar nuclei across all self-movement conditions.

Figure 7—figure supplement 5.

(A) Model performance as a function of the number of Purkinje cells. Black curves correspond to the population modeling that was performed on the simple spike firing rate of all 73 Purkinje cells recorded during three dynamic conditions (i.e., whole-body, body-under-head, and head-on-body movements). Blue curves correspond to the modeling of the simple spike firing rate of all 73 Purkinje cells during these same three dynamic conditions as well as the gain-field condition (Figure 5). The variability estimated from population of rostral fastigial nucleus (rFN) unimodal neurons previously described by Brooks and Cullen, 2009, is represented by the green shaded band. Inset: the distribution of computed weights for each Purkinje cell in a model with 40 Purkinje cells during our three dynamic conditions, sorted based on average weight. (B) Typical firing rate of unimodal neurons in the rFN (gray shaded region) and the estimated firing rate based on a population of 50 Purkinje cells (mean ± SD) during three dynamic head/body rotation conditions (left) and the static changes in head-on-body position condition (right). We also performed population modeling on rFN Type II cells and found similar results.

Accordingly, we next tested this hypothesis. Specifically, to quantify the actual number of Purkinje cells necessary to explain the responses of bimodal rFN neurons, we determined whether a simple linear model optimizing the weights of the activities of multiple Purkinje cells (see Materials and methods) could generate bimodal rFN neural responses across conditions (Figure 7B). As expected, combining the activities of more Purkinje cells (i.e., increasing population size) led to an increase in the goodness of fit (Figure 7C). Datasets used for this modeling first included Purkinje cell responses recorded during (i) our three dynamic conditions (i.e., whole-body, body-under-head, head-on-body rotations) alone (black curve) and (ii) these same three dynamic conditions as well as the the gain-field condition (i.e., Figure 6; dashed blue curve). In the latter case, the tuning of Purkinje cells that were not held long enough to test during gain-field paradigm were generated from the tuning curves distribution of the tested neurons (see Materials and methods). Importantly, in both cases we found that the weighted activities of ~40 neurons generated responses that well approximated those previously reported for bimodal rFN neurons (Figure 7C, red arrow); the confidence intervals of our model estimations and that of the rFN neural responses completely overlapped for a population of ~40 neurons. Thus, a population of 40 Purkinje cells could explain the dynamic representation of body motion across conditions (Figure 7D), as well as robust encoding of vestibular stimuli as a function of static head position observed in bimodal rFN neurons (Figure 7D, right panel).

Above we described how Purkinje cells demonstrate considerable heterogeneity in their responses to both vestibular and proprioceptive stimulation. Thus, we next asked whether certain Purkinje cell classes were weighted higher in our population model than others. For example, given that rFN neurons show linear tuning, one might predict that Purkinje cells with linear tuning might be weighted higher in our population model than those with v-shaped tuning. However, we found that this was not the case. The model weight distributions were similar for linear versus v-shaped versus rectifying Purkinje cells. Similarly, model weight distributions were similar for (i) bimodal versus unimodal Purkinje cells, (ii) Type I versus Type II Purkinje cells, as well as (iii) Purkinje cells with agonistic versus antagonistic vestibular and proprioceptive responses. These distributions are illustrated in Figure 7—figure supplements 1 and 2 for our modeling of bimodal versus unimodal rFN neurons, respectively.

Finally, we note that our simple population model above in Figure 7A assumed no input to the rFN neurons other than that from Purkinje cells. However, the fastigial nucleus receives mossy fiber inputs via the vestibular nuclei, reticular formation, and central cervical nucleus (reviewed in Voogd et al., 1996) that likely also encode vestibular and/or neck proprioceptive information (e.g., Roy and Cullen, 2001, Kubin et al., 1980, Kubin et al., 1981; Thomson et al., 1996). Therefore, we next tested the effect of adding simulated mossy fiber inputs to our model. Prior studies have shown that the dynamics of responses of vestibular nuclei neurons strongly resemble those of unimodal fastigial neurons in rhesus monkeys (i.e., they encode vestibular input and are insensitive to neck proprioceptive inputs, Roy and Cullen, 2001). In contrast, the response of neurons in the reticular formation and central cervical nucleus to such yaw head and/or neck rotations have not yet been described. We therefore simulated mossy fiber input first as a summation of vestibular and neck proprioceptive inputs, for which the gains and phases were randomly drawn from a distribution, comparable to that previously reported (Mitchell et al., 2017) in the vestibular nuclei (see Materials and methods). We repeated this approach for a total of 1000 simulations (Figure 7—figure supplement 3). We then further explored the effect of systematically altering this simulated mossy fiber input relative to the reference distribution of mossy fiber inputs by (i) doubling the gain, (ii) reducing the gain by half, (iii) doubling the phase, and (iv) reducing the phase by half (Figure 7—figure supplement 4). Overall, we found that the addition of such simulated mossy fiber inputs did not dramatically alter our estimate of the Purkinje cell population size required to generate rFN neurons responses (~50 versus 40; Figure 7—figure supplements 3 and 4). Furthermore, comparable results were obtained for model weight distributions as shown above in Figure 7—figure supplements 1 and 2.

Finally, for completeness, we also used the same approach to quantify the number of Purkinje cells necessary to explain the responses of unimodal rFN neurons and obtained comparable results (Figure 7—figure supplement 5). Interestingly, our finding that a population of ~40–50 Purkinje cells is again required to explain the responses of bimodal and unimodal rFN neurons matches the value established independently from anatomical studies of Purkinje cell – deep cerebellar nucleus neuron projection ratio. We further consider this point below in the Discussion.

Discussion

Summary of results

Here, we recorded the simple spike activity of Purkinje cells of the anterior vermis during passive vestibular (i.e., whole-body rotation), neck proprioceptive (i.e., body-under-head rotation), and a combination of vestibular and neck proprioceptive stimulation (i.e., head-on-body rotation). First, we found that most Purkinje cells responded to both vestibular and neck proprioceptive stimulation (i.e., bimodal neurons). Second, the linear combination of the responses to dynamic neck proprioceptive and vestibular stimulation alone provided a good estimate of each Purkinje cell’s response during combined stimulation. Third, bimodal neurons generally did not encode either the motion of the head or body in space across conditions. Instead, they dynamically encoded intermediate representations of self-motion between head and body motion. Additionally, bimodal neurons, but not unimodal neurons, showed tuning for the encoding of vestibular stimuli as a function of static head position. Finally, using a simple linear population model, we establish that combining inhibitory responses from ~40 to 50 Purkinje cells can explain the responses of target neurons in deep cerebellar nuclei across all self-movement conditions. Thus, our findings in alert monkeys provide new insight into the neural mechanisms underlying the coordinate transformation by which the cerebellum uses neck proprioceptive information to transform vestibular signals from a head- to body-centered reference frame.

Purkinje cells have diverse temporal responses to dynamic vestibular and neck proprioceptive sensory stimulation

The integration of vestibular and neck proprioceptive-related information is required to convert head-centered vestibular signals to the body-centered reference frame required for postural control (reviewed in Cullen, 2019). Our recordings in the anterior vermis of alert monkeys demonstrate that most vestibular-sensitive Purkinje cells also encode neck proprioceptive-related information. As reviewed above, anterior vermis Purkinje cells project to the rFN, the most medial of the deep cerebellar nuclei (Fujita et al., 2020; Husson et al., 2014) which plays a key role in the control of posture. Two types of rFN neurons have been previously identified in alert monkeys: unimodal and bimodal neurons (Brooks and Cullen, 2009). Unimodal neurons respond to vestibular stimulation during passive rotations and dynamically encode head movement. Bimodal rFN neurons respond to both vestibular and neck proprioceptive stimulation and dynamically encode body movement. Notably, because the vestibular and neck proprioceptive sensitivities of rFN bimodal neurons are both equal and complementary in sign, they sum linearly to effectively cancel each other during passive head-on-body rotations – a condition in which both sensory systems are activated but the body does not move in space. In contrast, here, we found that vestibular-sensitive anterior vermis Purkinje cells were on average more sensitive to vestibular than proprioceptive stimulation and that there was considerable variability in the relative signs of responses to each modality. As a result, cancellation of these two inputs during passive head-on-body rotations was the exception rather than the rule (i.e., Figure 3B) with the vast majority of bimodal Purkinje cells demonstrating significant modulation in response to passively applied head-on-body rotations. Thus, unlike bimodal rFN neurons, which dynamically encode body motion, bimodal Purkinje cells dynamically encode an intermediate representation of self-motion.

Reference frame transformations: Purkinje cell’s vestibular responses modulated by posture

Theoretical models of reference frame transformations commonly include a sensory input (e.g., vestibular or visual information) that is modulated by a postural signal (e.g., head-on-body position) (Pouget and Snyder, 2000; Salinas and Abbott, 2001). The resultant modulation of the sensory signal is commonly referred to as a gain field (Andersen and Mountcastle, 1983) and is thought to be mediated via nonlinear interactions between sensory responses and head/body referenced cues (Zipser and Andersen, 1988; Salinas and Abbott, 2001). Our present results reveal the neural substrate of such a reference frame transformation required for postural control. Notably, bimodal anterior vermis Purkinje cells displayed vestibular tuning as a function of head-on-body position during horizontal rotations. This tuning is similar but not as strong as that shown by downstream bimodal neurons in the target rFN (Brooks and Cullen, 2009), and indeed some rFN neurons do encode vestibular information in a body-centered reference frame for both two-dimensional (Kleine et al., 2004; Shaikh et al., 2004) and three-dimensional (Martin et al., 2018) self-motion. Thus, our present data establish that the modulation of vestibular information by a postural signal becomes more marked in the progression from the cerebellar cortex to the deep cerebellar nuclei. Interestingly, in the present study, such nonlinear interactions between neck position and vestibular signals were only in bimodal and not unimodal vestibular Purkinje cells. Future experiments are required to understand the implications of the dynamic coding of head rather than body movements by the unimodal neurons.

Finally, it is noteworthy that our findings regarding the transformation from a vestibular to head-centered reference frame in the anterior vermis of the alert primate contrast with those of Pompeiano, Manzoni, and colleagues in anesthetized decerebrate cats. Using dynamic tilt stimuli, they concluded that the direction of average response vector of Purkinje cells encoding both vestibular and proprioceptive information well corresponded to body tilt – consistent with a complete transformation from head- to body-centered reference frame (Denoth et al., 1979; Manzoni et al., 1998; Manzoni et al., 2004). One potential explanation for this apparent difference in neural strategy is that our studies were performed in intact alert behaving animals whereas Manzoni and colleagues completed their experiments in anesthetized decerebrate preparation where modulation/gating by cortical structures is not present. Additionally, there are significant differences across species regarding how the vestibular system integrates multimodal information even at the first stage of central processing in the vestibular nuclei (reviewed in Cullen, 2019). For instance, vestibular nuclei neurons in alert mice, cats, and cynomolgus monkeys commonly display vestibular-proprioceptive convergence (Medrea and Cullen, 2013; Cullen, 2016; McCall et al., 2017). In contrast, in rhesus monkeys, vestibular nuclei neurons are only sensitive to vestibular input, and instead, proprioceptive information is integrated only at the subsequent levels of vestibular processing, most notably in the deep nuclei of the cerebellum (Roy and Cullen, 2001; Brooks and Cullen, 2009; Carriot et al., 2013).

Population coding: the heterogeneous response of Purkinje cells and convergence in the rFN

Our results establish that there is considerable heterogeneity in the response dynamics of anterior vermis Purkinje cells to vestibular and/or neck proprioceptive sensory stimulation. Semicircular canal afferents and vestibular nuclei neurons provide the primary source of vestibular information to the cerebellum via mossy fiber input. However, while they encode head velocity with a phase lead, the responses of individual Purkinje cells actually more often lagged rather than led head velocity. Albus and Marr proposed that the divergent feedforward mossy fiber projections onto a far larger number of granule cells effectively expand the dimensionality of neural space, in turn allowing better downstream decoding to linearly classify dynamic patterns of activity (Marr, 1969; Albus, 1971). Indeed, recent studies have shown that mossy fibers from multiple sensory systems converge on each of more than 50 billion granule cells (Chabrol et al., 2015; Knogler et al., 2017; Lanore et al., 2021), with interneurons likely further contributing to the temporal diversity of granule cell responses (Rousseau et al., 2012; Kennedy et al., 2014). In turn, >100,000 granule cells project to a single Purkinje cell via parallel fibers (Fujishima et al., 2018). Thus, together these features of the cerebellar microcircuitry are well designed to generate high-dimensional dynamic coding of information by Purkinje cells relative to their mossy fiber input across regions of the cerebellum including the anterior vermis.

In this context, the heterogeneity we observed in anterior vermis Purkinje cells’ responses then contrasts strikingly with the responses of their target neurons in rFN (Brooks and Cullen, 2009). Our estimation that pooling the responses of a population of ~40–50 Purkinje cells can explain more homogeneous responses of bimodal and unimodal rFN neurons matches that established independently from anatomical studies of the Purkinje cell – deep cerebellar nucleus neuron projection ratio in rodents and cats (Person and Raman, 2012; Palkovits et al., 1977). Additionally, these Purkinje cells likely send direct projections to the vestibular nuclei. Testing with comparable stimulation protocols has established that the responses of vestibular nuclei neurons are comparable to those of unimodal rFN neurons (compare Cullen, 2019, with Brooks and Cullen, 2014). Thus, our present modeling results regarding the population convergence required to account for unimodal rFN neurons can be directly applied to vestibular nuclei neurons. Interestingly, Purkinje cells can display patterns of neuronal synchrony during active movements (Person and Raman, 2012; Sarnaik and Raman, 2018; Wu and Raman, 2017) which could, in turn, alter the timing and modulation of target neuron responses in the deep cerebellar nuclei in a nonlinear manner. Nevertheless, we found that responses could be predicted using a simple linearly weighted summation of ~40–50 neurons. In this context, we note our modeling was based on averaged Purkinje cell and rFN neuron responses. Future studies including simultaneous recordings from Purkinje cells and rFN neurons can provide additional insight into whether comparable population sizes can account for single trial responses in real time.

Finally, it is noteworthy that our study focused on the sensory responses of Purkinje cells (i.e., responses to vestibular and/or proprioceptive stimulation) during passively applied self-motion. Prior studies focused on the responses of Purkinje cells during voluntary movements have similarly concluded that they are more heterogeneous than those of their target neurons in the deep cerebellar nuclei (e.g., saccades: Thier et al., 2000; wrist control: Tomatsu et al., 2016). Interestingly, in their analysis, Tanaka et al., 2019, likewise estimated that linearly pooling the responses of ~40–50 Purkinje cells could account for the more homogeneous responses of target neurons in the deep cerebellar nucleus (i.e., the dentate nucleus) during voluntary wrist movements. We speculate that expanded dimensionality of the cerebellum provides a basis set for sensorimotor errors as well as plasticity at the level of Purkinje cells required to generate accurate movements (reviewed in Sohn et al., 2021) as well as ensure robust calibration over time. Overall, our current results reveal a striking transformation from heterogeneous response dynamics of cerebellar Purkinje cells to more stereotyped response dynamics of neurons in the targeted deep cerebellar nucleus. These findings provide new insights into the neural computations that ultimately ensure accurate postural control in our daily lives.

Materials and methods

Experimental model and subject details

Animal experimentation: All experimental protocols were approved by the Johns Hopkins University Animal Care and Use Committee and were in compliance with the guidelines of the United States National Institutes of Health (PR19M408). The cerebellar recordings were conducted in two male macaque monkeys (Macaca mulatta). The animals were housed on a 12 hr light/dark cycle. The recording sessions were about three times a week, for approximately 2 hr each session. Both animals had participated in previous studies in our laboratory, but they were in good health condition and did not require any medication.

Method details

Surgical procedures

The two animals were prepared for chronic extracellular recording using aseptic surgical techniques described previously (Massot et al., 2012). Briefly, animals were pre-anesthetized with ketamine hydrochloride (15 mg/kg i.m.) and injected with buprenorphine (0.01 mg/kg i.m.) and diazepam (1 mg/kg i.m.) to provide analgesia and muscle relaxation, respectively. Loading doses of dexamethasone (1 mg/kg i.m.) and cefazolin (50 mg/kg i.v.) were administered to minimize swelling and prevent infection, respectively. Anticholinergic glycopyrrolate (0.005 mg/kg i.m.) was also preoperatively injected to stabilize heart rate and to reduce salivation, and then again, every 2.5–3 hr during surgery. During surgery, anesthesia was maintained using isoflurane gas (0.8–1.5%), combined with a minimum 3 l/min (dose adjusted to effect) of 100% oxygen. Heart rate, blood pressure, respiration, and body temperature were monitored throughout the procedure. During the surgical procedure, a stainless-steel post for head immobilization and recording chambers were fastened to each animal’s skull with stainless-steel screws and dental acrylic. Craniotomy was performed within the recording chamber to allow electrode access to the cerebellar cortex. An 18-mm-diameter eye coil (three loops of Teflon-coated stainless-steel wire) was implanted in one eye behind the conjunctiva. Following surgery, we continued dexamethasone (0.5 mg/kg i.m.; for 4 days), anafen (2 mg/kg day 1, 1 mg/kg on subsequent days), and buprenorphine (0.01 mg/kg i.m.; every 12 hr for 2–5 days, depending on the animal’s pain level). In addition, cefazolin (25 mg/kg) was injected twice daily for 10 days. Animals recovered in 2 weeks before any experimenting began.

Data acquisition

During the experiments, the monkey sat in a primate chair secured to a turntable, and its head was centered in a coil system (CNC Engineering). Extracellular single-unit activity was recorded using enamel-insulated tungsten microelectrodes (Frederick-Haer). The location of the anterior vermis of the cerebellar cortex was determined relative to the abducens nucleus identified based on stereotypical neuronal responses during eye movements. The Purkinje cells were identified by their characteristic complex spike activity. The angular velocity of the turntable was measured using a gyroscope sensor (Watson Industries, Eau Claire, WI). Monkeys’ gaze and head angular positions were measured using the magnetic search coil technique. The neck torque produced by the monkey against its head restraint was measured using a reaction torque transducer (QWFK-8M; Honeywell, Canton, MA). All analog behavioral signals were low-pass filtered with a 125 Hz cut-off frequency and acquired at 1 kHz. The neural activity was recorded at 30 kHz using a data acquisition system (Blackrock Microsystems). Action potentials from the neural recording were sorted using a custom Matlab GUI (MathWorks), which provides threshold, clustering, and manual selection/removal methods.

Head and body motion paradigms

Two monkeys were trained to follow a target projected onto a cylindrical screen located 60 cm away from the monkey’s head. Each neuron’s insensitivity to saccades and ocular fixation was confirmed by having the head-restrained monkey attend to a target that stepped between horizontal positions over a range of ±30°. Each neuron’s lack of response to eye movements was further confirmed by absent responses to smooth pursuit eye movements during sinusoidal target motion (0.5 Hz, 40°/s peak velocity).

Next, to characterize each Purkinje cell’s vestibular and proprioceptive sensitivities, we applied rotational stimuli mimicking the monkey’s head movement generated during ±30° orienting gaze shifts in the head-unrestrained condition (i.e., ‘active-like’ condition). Use of this head movement trajectory facilitates direct comparison with rFN and vestibular nuclei neurons (e.g., Brooks and Cullen, 2014). First, vestibular sensitivities were assessed by applying whole-body rotations about an earth-vertical axis in the dark (i.e., whole-body-rotations). Second, neck proprioceptive sensitivities were assessed by rotating the monkey’s body with this same active-like trajectory while its head was held stationary relative to space (i.e., body-under-head rotations). Third, neural sensitivities to combined proprioceptive and vestibular stimulation were assessed by passively rotating the monkey’s head relative to its stationary body (i.e., head-on-body rotations) with this same trajectory. Finally, in a subset of neurons, we also applied whole-body rotations about an earth-vertical axis in the dark (1 Hz, ±40˚/s) with the head statically oriented at five different positions relative to the body (−30°, –15°, 0°, 15°, and 30°) to assess whether static neck position influenced vestibular-induced modulation during whole-body sinusoidal rotation (i.e., the ‘gain-field’ condition).

Histological analysis confirmed that the Purkinje cells were located in lobules II–V of the anterior vermis, ~0–2 mm from the midline. We note that while we first tested the vestibular sensitivity of individual neurons, we did also test whether neurons that were insensitive to vestibular stimulation responded to neck proprioceptive stimulation. Consistent with Manzoni and colleagues’ prior studies in anesthetized cat (12%, Manzoni et al., 1998), we found that only a small portion of Purkinje cells (~10%) fell into this latter category.

Data analysis

Analysis of neuronal discharge dynamics: Data were imported into the Matlab (MathWorks) programming environment for analysis, filtering, and processing as previously described (Dale and Cullen, 2019). Neuronal firing rate was computed by filtering spike trains with a Kaiser window at twice the frequency range of the stimulus (Cherif et al., 2008). We first verified that each neuron neither paused nor burst during saccades and was unresponsive to changes in eye position during fixation. We then used a least-squares regression analysis to describe each Purkinje cell simple spike’s response to whole-body and body-under-head rotations:

fr^(t)=b+cp,iXi(t)+cv,iX˙i(t)+ca,iX¨i(t) (1)

where fr^t is the estimated firing rate, b is a bias term, cp,i , cv,i , and ca,i are coefficients representing the position, velocity, and acceleration sensitivities respectively to head (i=1) or body motion (i=2), and Xi , X˙i, and X¨i are head (i=1) or body (i=2) position, velocity, and acceleration (during whole-body and body-under-head rotations), respectively. This least-squares regression was solved for non-negative and non-positive criterion to ensure sign consistency across estimated coefficients. For each model coefficient in the analysis, we computed 95% confidence intervals using a nonparametric bootstrap approach (n = 2000; Carpenter and Bithell, 2000; Sylvestre and Cullen, 1999). All non-significant coefficients were set to zero. We then used coefficients to estimate the sensitivity and phase of the response using the following equations:

Sensitivity=sgncp,i,cp,i,cp,i×2πf2ca,i-cp.i2+2πfcv,i22πf2 (2)
Phase=tan-12πf2ca,i-cp,i2πfcv,i (3)

For which f=1Hz to match the duration of half-cycle of movements (500 ms) and the sign term (i.e., sgn(cp,i,cp,i,cp,i)) equals either 1 or –1 for positive versus negative coefficients, respectively. The sensitivity of the Purkinje cells to the neck proprioceptive stimulation (during body-under-head rotations) was used to categorize the cells into unimodal (zero sensitivity) and bimodal (non-zero sensitivity).

Neuronal tuning to vestibular and proprioceptive inputs was further categorized as linear, rectifying, or V-shaped. Linear neurons demonstrated increased and decreased firing rates in the preferred and non-preferred directions, respectively. The difference between the magnitude of sensitivities in each of the two directions was within 0.2 (sp/s)/(°/s). Rectifying neurons demonstrated increased firing rate in the preferred direction and minimal modulation (i.e., sensitivity smaller than 0.2 (sp/s)/(°/s)) in the non-preferred direction. V-shaped neurons demonstrated an increased firing rate in both directions. The difference between the magnitude of their sensitivities in each of the two directions was within 0.2 (sp/s)/(°/s). Finally, neurons that did not fit any of these criteria were characterized as ‘other’. Note that v-shaped neurons were categorized as Type I or II based on the direction for which their vestibular sensitivity was larger, since their responses in each direction were not identical.

We used a similar approach to estimate sensitivities to passive head-on-body movements. Since in this condition, it is not possible to dissociate neck proprioceptive and vestibular sensitivities, we estimated them as a single coefficient. Estimated sensitivities were compared to those predicted from the linear summation of the vestibular and proprioceptive sensitivities estimated for the same neuron during passive whole-body and body-under-head rotations (termed summation model), respectively. To quantify the ability of the linear regression analysis to model neuronal discharges, the variance-accounted-for (VAF) for each regression equation was determined as previously described (Cullen et al., 1996). Values are expressed as mean ± SD and paired-sample Student’s t-tests were used to assess differences between conditions.

Quantifying head versus body encoding: We computed a ‘head sensitivity ratio’ and ‘body sensitivity ratio’ for each Purkinje cell. These ratios were defined as the neuron’s (i) sensitivity to head-on-body rotation/sensitivity to whole-body rotation and (ii) sensitivity to body-under-head rotation/sensitivity to whole-body rotation, respectively. Further to quantify the relative encoding of head versus body motion by a given cell, we computed a ‘coding index’, which was defined as the ratio (smaller value)/(larger value) of these two ratios.

Quantification of head position on Purkinje cell vestibular sensitivity: The tuning curves for different head-on-body positions were fit with Gaussian curves with the following equation:

S=Ae-Hposition-μ22σ2 (4)

where µ represents the mean, σ is a measure of the width, and A is the amplitude from the peak to the base of the Gaussian curve (as described previously, Brooks and Cullen, 2013).

Population modeling of Purkinje cells: To determine whether integrating the activities of multiple Purkinje cells could explain the response of their target neurons in the rFN, we used the linear model below:

rFN^=i=1Nwi×Pcelli (5)

where rFN^ is a reconstructed firing rate response of an rFN neuron. The wi corresponds to weights of connection from Purkinje cells to an rFN neuron, which all considered non-positive to reflect inhibitory synapses from Purkinje cells to rFN neurons. Pcelli are observed firing rate of simple spikes N Purkinje cells, where N is a number between 1 and the total number of Purkinje cells in the dataset. For each N, we used a bootstrapping approach to find the 95% confidence intervals of the goodness of fit (R2) as well as the model predictions).

To model the population response of Purkinje cells during the ‘gain-field condition’, we first fit a Gaussian function of the tuning curve of 13 bimodal Purkinje cells that were recorded during this condition. Next, we used the parameters of these Gaussian functions to find a normal distribution representing the tuning curves of bimodal Purkinje cells. Then, for the remaining Purkinje cells that were not recorded during the ‘gain-field’ condition, we generated tuning curves by drawing from this normal distribution. Since four unimodal Purkinje cells that were recorded during the ‘gain-field’ condition did not demonstrate significant tuning, we did not consider any tuning to the remaining unimodal Purkinje cells.

Finally, we modeled the contribution of the mossy fiber input to the rFN as a summation of independent responses to vestibular and neck proprioceptive stimulation. To simulate the mossy fiber input, we randomly selected response gains and phases from normal distributions that described the responses of neurons in the vestibular nuclei (i.e., 0.6 ± 0.1 (sp/s)/(°/s) and 20 ± 5°, respectively), and repeated this for a total of 1000 simulations. We further assessed the robustness of our modeling performance by modifying these distributions. Specifically, we tested the effect of either doubling or halving the gain (i.e., 1.2 ± 0.2 and 0.3 ± 0.05 (sp/s)/(°/s), respectively) or phase (i.e., 40 ± 10° and 10 ± 2.5°, respectively), resulting simulations based on four modifications of the original distribution.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by grants R01-DC002390 and R01-DC018061 from the National Institutes of Health (KEC). We would like to thank Dale Roberts for his technical support and Jessica Brooks, Robyn Mildren, Pum Wiboonsaksakul, and members of the Cullen lab for helpful discussions.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Kathleen E Cullen, Email: kathleen.cullen@jhu.edu.

Megan R Carey, Champalimaud Foundation, Portugal.

Andrew J King, University of Oxford, United Kingdom.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders R01-DC002390 to Kathleen E Cullen.

  • National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders R01-DC018061 to Kathleen E Cullen.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Ethics

All experimental protocols were approved by the Johns Hopkins University Animal Care and Use Committee and were in compliance with the guidelines of the United States National Institutes of Health.(PR19M408).

Additional files

MDAR checklist

Data availability

All data and codes to generate figures are available on Figshare under the URL: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.19362239.

References

  1. Albus JS. A theory of cerebellar function. Mathematical Biosciences. 1971;10:25–61. doi: 10.1016/0025-5564(71)90051-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Andersen RA, Mountcastle VB. The influence of the angle of gaze upon the excitability of the light-sensitive neurons of the posterior parietal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1983;3:532–548. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.03-03-00532.1983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bastian AJ, Mink JW, Kaufman BA, Thach WT. Posterior vermal split syndrome. Annals of Neurology. 1998;44:601–610. doi: 10.1002/ana.410440405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Batton RR, Jayaraman A, Ruggiero D, Carpenter MB. Fastigial efferent projections in the monkey: an autoradiographic study. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1977;174:281–305. doi: 10.1002/cne.901740206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bronstein AM, Grunfeld EA, Faldon M, Okada T. Reduced self-motion perception in patients with midline cerebellar lesions. Neuroreport. 2008;19:691–693. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282fbf9f6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brooks JX, Cullen KE. Multimodal integration in rostral fastigial nucleus provides an estimate of body movement. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:10499–10511. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1937-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brooks JX, Cullen KE. The primate cerebellum selectively encodes unexpected self-motion. Current Biology. 2013;23:947–955. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brooks JX, Cullen KE. Early vestibular processing does not discriminate active from passive self-motion if there is a discrepancy between predicted and actual proprioceptive feedback. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2014;111:2465–2478. doi: 10.1152/jn.00600.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Carpenter J, Bithell J. Bootstrap confidence intervals: when, which, what? A practical guide for medical statisticians. Statistics in Medicine. 2000;19:1141–1164. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-0258(20000515)19:9&#x0003c;1141::aid-sim479&#x0003e;3.0.co;2-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Carriot J, Brooks JX, Cullen KE. Multimodal integration of self-motion cues in the vestibular system: active versus passive translations. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:19555–19566. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3051-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chabrol FP, Arenz A, Wiechert MT, Margrie TW, DiGregorio DA. Synaptic diversity enables temporal coding of coincident multisensory inputs in single neurons. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18:718–727. doi: 10.1038/nn.3974. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cherif S, Cullen KE, Galiana HL. An improved method for the estimation of firing rate dynamics using an optimal digital filter. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2008;173:165–181. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.05.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cullen KE, McCrea RA. Firing behavior of brain stem neurons during voluntary cancellation of the horizontal vestibuloocular reflex. I. Secondary vestibular neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1993;70:828–843. doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.70.2.828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cullen KE, Rey CG, Guitton D, Galiana HL. The use of system identification techniques in the analysis of oculomotor burst neuron spike train dynamics. Journal of Computational Neuroscience. 1996;3:347–368. doi: 10.1007/BF00161093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cullen KE, Minor LB. Semicircular canal afferents similarly encode active and passive head-on-body rotations: implications for the role of vestibular efference. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:RC226. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-11-j0002.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cullen KE. In: Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Furman JM, Lempert T, editors. Vol. 137. Elsevier; 2016. Physiology of central pathways; pp. 17–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cullen KE. Vestibular processing during natural self-motion: implications for perception and action. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2019;20:346–363. doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0153-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cullen KE, Zobeiri OA. Proprioception and the predictive sensing of active self-motion. Current Opinion in Physiology. 2021;20:29–38. doi: 10.1016/j.cophys.2020.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dahlem K, Valko Y, Schmahmann JD, Lewis RF. Cerebellar contributions to self-motion perception: evidence from patients with congenital cerebellar agenesis. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2016;115:2280–2285. doi: 10.1152/jn.00763.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dale A, Cullen KE. The Ventral Posterior Lateral Thalamus Preferentially Encodes Externally Applied Versus Active Movement: Implications for Self-Motion Perception. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y) 2019;29:305–318. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Denoth F, Magherini PC, Pompeiano O, Stanojević M. Responses of Purkinje cells of the cerebellar vermis to neck and macular vestibular inputs. Pflugers Archiv. 1979;381:87–98. doi: 10.1007/BF00582338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Diener HC, Dichgans J, Bootz F, Bacher M. Early stabilization of human posture after a sudden disturbance: influence of rate and amplitude of displacement. Experimental Brain Research. 1984;56:126–134. doi: 10.1007/BF00237448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fujishima K, Kawabata Galbraith K, Kengaku M. Dendritic Self-Avoidance and Morphological Development of Cerebellar Purkinje Cells. Cerebellum (London, England) 2018;17:701–708. doi: 10.1007/s12311-018-0984-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fujita H, Kodama T, du Lac S. Modular output circuits of the fastigial nucleus for diverse motor and nonmotor functions of the cerebellar vermis. eLife. 2020;9:e58613. doi: 10.7554/eLife.58613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gardner EP, Fuchs AF. Single-unit responses to natural vestibular stimuli and eye movements in deep cerebellar nuclei of the alert rhesus monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1975;38:627–649. doi: 10.1152/jn.1975.38.3.627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Husson Z, Rousseau CV, Broll I, Zeilhofer HU, Dieudonné S. Differential GABAergic and glycinergic inputs of inhibitory interneurons and Purkinje cells to principal cells of the cerebellar nuclei. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:9418–9431. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0401-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Ilg W, Giese MA, Gizewski ER, Schoch B, Timmann D. The influence of focal cerebellar lesions on the control and adaptation of gait. Brain : A Journal of Neurology. 2008;131:2913–2927. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Jamali M, Sadeghi SG, Cullen KE. Response of vestibular nerve afferents innervating utricle and saccule during passive and active translations. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2009;101:141–149. doi: 10.1152/jn.91066.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kennedy PM, Inglis JT. Interaction effects of galvanic vestibular stimulation and head position on the soleus H reflex in humans. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2002;113:1709–1714. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(02)00238-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kennedy A, Wayne G, Kaifosh P, Alviña K, Abbott LF, Sawtell NB. A temporal basis for predicting the sensory consequences of motor commands in an electric fish. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:416–422. doi: 10.1038/nn.3650. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kleine JF, Guan Y, Kipiani E, Glonti L, Hoshi M, Büttner U. Trunk position influences vestibular responses of fastigial nucleus neurons in the alert monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2004;91:2090–2100. doi: 10.1152/jn.00849.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Knogler LD, Markov DA, Dragomir EI, Štih V, Portugues R. Sensorimotor Representations in Cerebellar Granule Cells in Larval Zebrafish Are Dense, Spatially Organized, and Non-temporally Patterned. Current Biology. 2017;27:1288–1302. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.03.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kubin L, Magherini PC, Manzoni D, Pompeiano O. Responses of lateral reticular neurons to sinusoidal stimulation of labyrinth receptors in decerebrate cat. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1980;44:922–936. doi: 10.1152/jn.1980.44.5.922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kubin L, Magherini PC, Manzoni D, Pompeiano O. Responses of lateral reticular neurons to sinusoidal rotation of neck in the decerebrate cat. Neuroscience. 1981;6:1277–1290. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(81)90187-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kurzan R, Straube A, Büttner U. The effect of muscimol micro-injections into the fastigial nucleus on the optokinetic response and the vestibulo-ocular reflex in the alert monkey. Experimental Brain Research. 1993;94:252–260. doi: 10.1007/BF00230293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lam CK, Tokuno CD, Staines WR, Bent LR. The direction of the postural response to a vestibular perturbation is mediated by the cerebellar vermis. Experimental Brain Research. 2016;234:3689–3697. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4766-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Lanore F, Cayco-Gajic NA, Gurnani H, Coyle D, Silver RA. Cerebellar granule cell axons support high-dimensional representations. Nature Neuroscience. 2021;24:1142–1150. doi: 10.1038/s41593-021-00873-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Manzoni D, Pompeiano O, Andre P. Convergence of directional vestibular and neck signals on cerebellar purkinje cells. Pflugers Archiv. 1998;435:617–630. doi: 10.1007/s004240050562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Manzoni D, Pompeiano O, Bruschini L, Andre P. Neck input modifies the reference frame for coding labyrinthine signals in the cerebellar vermis: a cellular analysis. Neuroscience. 1999;93:1095–1107. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(99)00275-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Manzoni D, Andre P, Pompeiano O. Proprioceptive neck influences modify the information about tilt direction coded by the cerebellar anterior vermis. Acta Oto-Laryngologica. 2004;124:475–480. doi: 10.1080/00016480410016595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Marr D. A theory of cerebellar cortex. The Journal of Physiology. 1969;202:437–470. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1969.sp008820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Martin CZ, Brooks JX, Green AM. Role of Rostral Fastigial Neurons in Encoding a Body-Centered Representation of Translation in Three Dimensions. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38:3584–3602. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2116-17.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Massot C, Schneider AD, Chacron MJ, Cullen KE. The vestibular system implements a linear-nonlinear transformation in order to encode self-motion. PLOS Biology. 2012;10:e1001365. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. McCall AA, Miller DM, Yates BJ. Descending Influences on Vestibulospinal and Vestibulosympathetic Reflexes. Frontiers in Neurology. 2017;8:112. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. McCrea RA, Gdowski GT, Boyle R, Belton T. Firing behavior of vestibular neurons during active and passive head movements: vestibulo-spinal and other non-eye-movement related neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1999;82:416–428. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.1.416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Medrea I, Cullen KE. Multisensory integration in early vestibular processing in mice: the encoding of passive vs. active motion. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2013;110:2704–2717. doi: 10.1152/jn.01037.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Mergner T, Huber W, Becker W. Vestibular-Neck Interaction and Transformation of Sensory Coordinates. Journal of Vestibular Research. 1997;7:347–365. doi: 10.3233/VES-1997-7405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Mitchell DE, Della Santina CC, Cullen KE. Plasticity within excitatory and inhibitory pathways of the vestibulo-spinal circuitry guides changes in motor performance. Scientific Reports. 2017;7:853. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-00956-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mitoma H, Buffo A, Gelfo F, Guell X, Fucà E, Kakei S, Lee J, Manto M, Petrosini L, Shaikh AG, Schmahmann JD. Consensus Paper. Cerebellar Reserve: From Cerebellar Physiology to Cerebellar Disorders. Cerebellum (London, England) 2020;19:131–153. doi: 10.1007/s12311-019-01091-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Palkovits M, Mezey E, Hámori J, Szentágothai J. Quantitative histological analysis of the cerebellar nuclei in the cat. I. Numerical data on cells and on synapses. Experimental Brain Research. 1977;28:189–209. doi: 10.1007/BF00237096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Pélisson D, Goffart L, Guillaume A. Contribution of the rostral fastigial nucleus to the control of orienting gaze shifts in the head-unrestrained cat. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1998;80:1180–1196. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.3.1180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Person AL, Raman IM. Synchrony and neural coding in cerebellar circuits. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2012;6:97. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2012.00097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Peterka RJ. Sensorimotor integration in human postural control. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2002;88:1097–1118. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.3.1097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Pouget A, Snyder LH. Computational approaches to sensorimotor transformations. Nature Neuroscience. 2000;3 Suppl:1192–1198. doi: 10.1038/81469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Raymond JL, Medina JF. Computational Principles of Supervised Learning in the Cerebellum. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2018;41:233–253. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-080317-061948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Rousseau CV, Dugué GP, Dumoulin A, Mugnaini E, Dieudonné S, Diana MA. Mixed inhibitory synaptic balance correlates with glutamatergic synaptic phenotype in cerebellar unipolar brush cells. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:4632–4644. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5122-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Roy JE, Cullen KE. A neural correlate for vestibulo-ocular reflex suppression during voluntary eye-head gaze shifts. Nature Neuroscience. 1998;1:404–410. doi: 10.1038/1619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Roy JE, Cullen KE. Selective processing of vestibular reafference during self-generated head motion. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:2131–2142. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.21-06-02131.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Roy JE, Cullen KE. Dissociating self-generated from passively applied head motion: neural mechanisms in the vestibular nuclei. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:2102–2111. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3988-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sadeghi SG, Minor LB, Cullen KE. Response of vestibular-nerve afferents to active and passive rotations under normal conditions and after unilateral labyrinthectomy. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;97:1503–1514. doi: 10.1152/jn.00829.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Salinas E, Abbott LF. Coordinate transformations in the visual system: how to generate gain fields and what to compute with them. Progress in Brain Research. 2001;130:175–190. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(01)30012-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Sarnaik R, Raman IM. Control of voluntary and optogenetically perturbed locomotion by spike rate and timing of neurons of the mouse cerebellar nuclei. eLife. 2018;7:e29546. doi: 10.7554/eLife.29546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Scudder CA, Fuchs AF. Physiological and behavioral identification of vestibular nucleus neurons mediating the horizontal vestibuloocular reflex in trained rhesus monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1992;68:244–264. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.68.1.244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Shaikh AG, Meng H, Angelaki DE. Multiple reference frames for motion in the primate cerebellum. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:4491–4497. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0109-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Shaikh AG, Ghasia FF, Dickman JD, Angelaki DE. Properties of cerebellar fastigial neurons during translation, rotation, and eye movements. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2005;93:853–863. doi: 10.1152/jn.00879.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Sohn H, Meirhaeghe N, Rajalingham R, Jazayeri M. A Network Perspective on Sensorimotor Learning. Trends in Neurosciences. 2021;44:170–181. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Sullivan EV, Rose J, Pfefferbaum A. Effect of vision, touch and stance on cerebellar vermian-related sway and tremor: A quantitative physiological and MRI study. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y) 2006;16:1077–1086. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Sylvestre PA, Cullen KE. Quantitative analysis of abducens neuron discharge dynamics during saccadic and slow eye movements. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1999;82:2612–2632. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.5.2612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Tanaka H, Ishikawa T, Kakei S. Neural Evidence of the Cerebellum as a State Predictor. Cerebellum (London, England) 2019;18:349–371. doi: 10.1007/s12311-018-0996-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Thach WT, Goodkin HP, Keating JG. The cerebellum and the adaptive coordination of movement. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1992;15:403–442. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.15.030192.002155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Thier P, Dicke PW, Haas R, Barash S. Encoding of movement time by populations of cerebellar Purkinje cells. Nature. 2000;405:72–76. doi: 10.1038/35011062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Thomson DB, Isu N, Wilson VJ. Responses of neurons of the cat central cervical nucleus to natural neck and vestibular stimulation. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1996;76:2786–2789. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.4.2786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Tokita T, Ito Y, Takagi K. Modulation by head and trunk positions of the vestibulo-spinal reflexes evoked by galvanic stimulation of the labyrinth. Observations by labyrinthine evoked EMG. Acta Oto-Laryngologica. 1989;107:327–332. doi: 10.3109/00016488909127516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Tokita T, Miyata H, Takagi K, Ito Y. Studies on Vestibulo-spinal Reflexes by Examination of Labyrinthine-evoked EMGs of Lower Limbs. Acta Oto-Laryngologica. 2009;111:328–332. doi: 10.3109/00016489109131414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Tomatsu S, Ishikawa T, Tsunoda Y, Lee J, Hoffman DS, Kakei S. Information processing in the hemisphere of the cerebellar cortex for control of wrist movement. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2016;115:255–270. doi: 10.1152/jn.00530.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Voogd J, Jaarsma D, Marani E. Chapter 1 The cerebellum: chemoarchitecture and anatomy. Handb Chem Neuroanatomy. 1996;12:1–369. doi: 10.1016/s0924-8196(96)80003-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  77. Wolpert DM, Miall RC, Kawato M. Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 1998;2:338–347. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(98)01221-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Wu Y, Raman IM. Facilitation of mossy fibre-driven spiking in the cerebellar nuclei by the synchrony of inhibition. The Journal of Physiology. 2017;595:5245–5264. doi: 10.1113/JP274321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Yamada J, Noda H. Afferent and efferent connections of the oculomotor cerebellar vermis in the macaque monkey. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1987;265:224–241. doi: 10.1002/cne.902650207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Zipser D, Andersen RA. A back-propagation programmed network that simulates response properties of A subset of posterior parietal neurons. Nature. 1988;331:679–684. doi: 10.1038/331679a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Editor's evaluation

Megan R Carey 1

This paper addresses the important question of how the cerebellum transforms multiple streams of sensory information into an estimate of the motion of the body in the world. The authors find that Purkinje cells, the inhibitory principal neurons of the cerebellar cortex, have multimodal and highly diverse responses to vestibular and neck proprioceptive inputs. Notably, this information is combined in a way that is different from what is seen in downstream fastigial neurons, which reflect either head or body motion, but not both.

Decision letter

Editor: Megan R Carey1
Reviewed by: Stephen H Scott2

Our editorial process produces two outputs: i) public reviews designed to be posted alongside the preprint for the benefit of readers; ii) feedback on the manuscript for the authors, including requests for revisions, shown below. We also include an acceptance summary that explains what the editors found interesting or important about the work.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Distinct representations of body and head motion are dynamically encoded by Purkinje cell populations in the macaque cerebellum" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Andrew King as the Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Stephen H Scott (Reviewer #2).

The reviewers have discussed their reviews with one another, and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this to help you prepare a revised submission.

Essential revisions:

1) All Reviewers had concerns about the rFN model, in particular (a) the absence of mossy fiber inputs to fastigial neurons, and (b) whether it's just easiest to get this fastigial response by down-weighting the PCs with ipsilaterally-signed vestibular and proprioceptive signals, which would make it less interesting. More information about the model is required. At a minimum, please add (1) additional discussion of the assumptions and limitations of the model, and (2) analysis of the distribution of model weights over different classes of PCs (especially linear vs v-shaped or rectifying). Please also see the more detailed comments from the individual reviews below.

2) There was concern during the consultation phase that the focus of the paper is the difference in sensitivity to vestibular vs proprioceptive stimuli, and yet different stimuli are being presented in the two conditions (e.g., compare body velocity in Figure 1A Cell 1, and Figure 2A, Cell 1). It was not clear how these differences might impact the regression coefficients and model performance. The velocity differences between conditions should be addressed directly in the manuscript. Specifically, the reason for the discrepancy (and, if relevant, the experimental constraints on matching the velocities) should be explained in the Methods and discussed explicitly in the text. The reviewers further agreed that the authors should address this issue by (1) providing the total variance-accounted-for for all neurons (not just the normalized values in Supp 1and2), (2) plotting the residuals vs predicted firing rate for all neurons, and (3) providing additional justification that the model is not overfit, if possible.

Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

– The writing, particularly in the Results section, is very abrupt. The reader has to go to the Methods section immediately to understand what stimulus is even being provided, let alone what the motivation for the experiments is. Even as someone with reasonable experience in the vestibular field, I had difficulty following the logic of the experimental design.

– The authors have used a "naturalistic" rotational stimulus, about which I have several questions. First, I didn't understand the motivation for using this. Second, based on the figures, this rotational stimulus varied significantly across experiments. E.g., the head and velocity traces in Figure 1A are quite different for Cell 1, Cell 2, and Cell 3. Why deliver different stimuli if the goal is to make comparisons across cells? Even more confusing, the body velocity traces delivered to those exact same cells (Figure 2) appear again quite different. Why deliver different body velocity stimuli in the proprioceptive versus vestibular experiments? Since the entire manuscript focuses on the comparison between these stimuli, it would seem important to hold the parameters constant across the experiments. Head velocity traces are also different, or appear different, across the rotations at different head-on-body angles (Figure 5; e.g. the head velocity stimulus in 5A for +15 vs +30 is different in amplitude).

– In Figure 5, the authors make the interesting observation that vestibular sensitivity of PCs actually varies depending on the head angle relative to the body. I was expecting that they would then test whether the neck proprioceptive "position" term (that they had derived from the experiments in Figure 2) could be used to predict this shift in sensitivity. Instead, the authors directly jump to comparisons with the rostral fastigial population recorded in a prior study. Can they calculate whether their proprioceptive position weights (c_a,2 from equation 1) explain the measured difference in vestibular sensitivity?

– In Figure 6, the authors show modeling evidence that linear summation of Purkinje cells can produce the tuning results of fastigial neurons. They don't address whether an alternate explanation, that some of the proprioceptive or vestibular tuning derives from direct mossy fiber afferents to fastigial neurons, is possible. Furthermore, they don't seem to address the basic surprise here, that although the PCs show much less "cancellation" between vestibular and proprioceptive inputs (p. 11, last paragraph) than fastigial neurons do, the summation of PC inputs can produce fastigial responses. One assumes that the only way to obtain this result is preferential summation of the small set of PCs that do show cancellation (oppositely signed proprioceptive and vestibular sensitivities) but this is never discussed. If that is the underlying explanation of their model's success, then it seems quite weak.

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

I think the first experiment is strong and the paper should emphasize even more the comparison between Purkinje cells and the downstream fastigial neurons. I think some example fastigial neurons could be added to contrast with the Purkinje cell responses. Importantly, Figure 4C shows a red and orange cloud to denote the rFN neurons, but this is very hard to see in the figure. I think the individual neurons should be plotted to highlight the substantive difference between these Purkinje cells and fastigial neurons.

I'm not sure the model is that insightful given that it assumes that there is no other input to the fastigial nucleus. I assume that it would have substantive vestibular and neck proprioceptive input from mossy fibers. As stated in the comment above, is the point of the Purkinje cell activity to counter the sensory input from the mossy fibers in order to create two distinct signals, one related to head and one to body motion in the fastigial population? Thus, a variant for the model would be to show how the population model could create distinct head and body motion signals to counter the presence of a random pattern of vestibular and neck proprioceptive input (assumed to be generated by mossy fiber input). A more advanced model might be interesting in which weights of the parallel fibers onto Purkinje neurons are altered given random mossy fiber inputs to granule cells and fastigial neurons leading to distinct head and body motion signals in the fastigial neurons, but this is certainly beyond the scope of this manuscript.

The cells in Figure 1 and 2 show the same patterns (linear, v-shaped, rectified) across the two conditions. I assume this was common for bimodal cells? Was there any analyses done to support or refute this?

Figure 1 shows that all neurons were sensitive to vestibular input, and no unimodal proprioceptive neurons. Is this an order effect of the experiment in that you always tested vestibular first, and if responsive, then you completed the rest of the experiments? Thus, could there be proprioceptive only neurons, but they were not examined in this study due to the protocol. This is important and needs to be clearly stated in the manuscript, one way or another.

The small sample size makes conclusions regarding head-on-body position a bit of a challenge. Notably, there are only 4 unimodal neurons for Purkinje cells in Figure 5C. The number of neurons is only noted in the figure legend and the number of neurons for the fastigial nucleus is not stated at all and looks to be 11 for bimodal and 10 for unimodal. I think the main text needs to clearly state the actual numbers and recognize the very small sample size for this analysis. These limited number of neurons to characterize tuning properties also weakens the modelling results and conclusions about 40 neurons necessary to predict fastigial neuron responses.

In Figure 4C it looks like the positive body sensitivity ratio neurons tend to have larger values than the negative sensitivity ratio neurons. Is this just a random sampling issue?

Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

1 – "All non-significant coefficients were set to zero." In the Methods, the authors should briefly describe the rationale for using this approach instead of L1 regularization.

2 – The acceleration term appears to dominate in the non-preferred direction for vestibular stimulation (Figure S1A); why?

3 – Typo: "Albus and Marrs" -> "Albus and Marr".

eLife. 2022 Apr 25;11:e75018. doi: 10.7554/eLife.75018.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) All Reviewers had concerns about the rFN model, in particular (a) the absence of mossy fiber inputs to fastigial neurons, and (b) whether it's just easiest to get this fastigial response by down-weighting the PCs with ipsilaterally-signed vestibular and proprioceptive signals, which would make it less interesting. More information about the model is required. At a minimum, please add (1) additional discussion of the assumptions and limitations of the model, and (2) analysis of the distribution of model weights over different classes of PCs (especially linear vs v-shaped or rectifying). Please also see the more detailed comments from the individual reviews below.

We thank the reviewers for their attention to our manuscript and for raising these important points. We have address each of the reviewers’ main concerns.

First, to consider the influence of mossy fiber inputs, we followed Reviewer #2’s suggestion and modeled mossy fiber input using random patterns of vestibular and neck proprioceptive input. Prior studies have shown that the dynamics of vestibular nuclei neuron responses strongly resemble those of unimodal fastigial neurons in rhesus monkeys (i.e., they encode vestibular input and are insensitive to neck proprioceptive inputs, Roy and Cullen, 2001). In contrast, reticular formation neurons responses to such yaw head and/or neck rotations have not yet been described. We therefore simulated mossy fiber input first as a summation of vestibular and neck proprioceptive inputs, for which the gains and phases were randomly drawn from a distribution, comparable to that previously reported (Mitchell er al. 2017) in the vestibular nuclei (Figure 7—figure supplement 3). We then further explored the effect of systematically altering this simulated mossy fiber input – relative to the reference distribution of mossy fiber inputs – by i) doubling the gain, ii) reducing the gain by half, iii) doubling the phase, and iv) reducing the phase by half (Figure 7—figure supplement 4). Overall, we found that the addition of such simulated mossy fiber did not dramatically alter our estimate of the population Purkinje cell population size required to generate rFN neurons responses (~50 versus 40; Figure 7—figure supplement 3 and 4).

Second, to address the reviewers’ concerns regarding the Purkinje cell weights, we have added a new inset to Figure 7C. As can be seen, model weights are well distributed across different Purkinje cells. Further, to confirm that the distribution of the weights of Purkinje cells inputs are distributed over different classes of PCs we now illustrate the weight distributions for (a) linear vs. v-shaped vs. rectifying Purkinje cells, (b) bimodal vs. unimodal Purkinje cells, (c) Type I vs. Type II Purkinje cells and (d) Purkinje cells with agonistic vs. antagonistic vestibular and proprioceptive sensitivities. These results are shown in Figure 7-supplemental figures 1 and 2.

2) There was concern during the consultation phase that the focus of the paper is the difference in sensitivity to vestibular vs proprioceptive stimuli, and yet different stimuli are being presented in the two conditions (e.g., compare body velocity in Figure 1A Cell 1, and Figure 2A, Cell 1). It was not clear how these differences might impact the regression coefficients and model performance. The velocity differences between conditions should be addressed directly in the manuscript. Specifically, the reason for the discrepancy (and, if relevant, the experimental constraints on matching the velocities) should be explained in the Methods and discussed explicitly in the text. The reviewers further agreed that the authors should address this issue by (1) providing the total variance-accounted-for for all neurons (not just the normalized values in Supp 1and2), (2) plotting the residuals vs predicted firing rate for all neurons, and (3) providing additional justification that the model is not overfit, if possible.

We thank the reviewers for bringing this to our attention. The apparent discrepancy was due to an error in finalizing Figure 2. The left panel of Figure 2A illustrated a different cell than that shown in Figures1A and 3B, plotted at different time/amplitude scales than indicated by the bars. We have corrected this figure in the revised manuscript. Indeed, movement trajectories were well matched across all of our conditions, and we have revised the METHODS to more clearly explain how velocities were matched across conditions. Additionally, as requested, we now present the total variance-accounted-for for all neuron responses during whole-body and body-under-head stimulation in Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and Figure 2—figure supplement 2, respectively. Finally, we have also revised the METHODS to explain our linear regression method more clearly. Briefly, as stated in our response to Reviewer #3, a bootstrapping approach was used to prevent overfitting instead of lasso L1 regularization, since we used a non-negative/non-positive criterion in the least-square linear regression and to the best of our knowledge, there is no commonly used package for non-negative lasso regularization.

Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

– The writing, particularly in the Results section, is very abrupt. The reader has to go to the Methods section immediately to understand what stimulus is even being provided, let alone what the motivation for the experiments is. Even as someone with reasonable experience in the vestibular field, I had difficulty following the logic of the experimental design.

We appreciate the reviewer’s concern and have revised the Results section to more clearly introduce both the stimuli that were applied, as well as the logic that motivates each experiment.

– The authors have used a "naturalistic" rotational stimulus, about which I have several questions. First, I didn't understand the motivation for using this. Second, based on the figures, this rotational stimulus varied significantly across experiments. E.g., the head and velocity traces in Figure 1A are quite different for Cell 1, Cell 2, and Cell 3. Why deliver different stimuli if the goal is to make comparisons across cells? Even more confusing, the body velocity traces delivered to those exact same cells (Figure 2) appear again quite different. Why deliver different body velocity stimuli in the proprioceptive versus vestibular experiments? Since the entire manuscript focuses on the comparison between these stimuli, it would seem important to hold the parameters constant across the experiments. Head velocity traces are also different, or appear different, across the rotations at different head-on-body angles (Figure 5; e.g. the head velocity stimulus in 5A for +15 vs +30 is different in amplitude).

We have revised the Methods and Results sections to more clearly introduce the stimuli that were applied. Specifically, we chose these rotational stimuli for 2 main reasons: (1) First they mimic typical head movements generated by monkeys during orienting gaze shifts in the head-unrestrained condition. Thus, use of these stimuli facilitates future comparisons with neuronal responses during active head rotations. (2) Second, comparable stimuli have been used to assess the responses of both rFN and vestibular nuclei neurons to applied vestibular, proprioceptive, and combined stimulation in rhesus monkeys (see for example Brooks et al., 2015). Thus, the use of these stimuli also facilitates the direct comparison between rFN and vestibular nuclei neurons and the Purkinje cells in the present study. To improve clarity, we have revised the text and now refer to these stimuli as “active-like” motion profiles in the revised METHODS, consistent with the terminology used by Brooks et al., 2015.

We also agree that it is important to apply comparable stimuli in all three paradigms (whole-body, body-under-head, and head-on-body). Indeed, this was the case in our study. As explained in the response to the reviewers’ consensus comments the apparent inconsistency across the example cells in old Figure 2 was due to a scaling error that has now been corrected. We apologize for this oversight. A similar issue has also been corrected in figure 5A, and we have confirmed that the scaling is now correct for all figures.

– In Figure 5, the authors make the interesting observation that vestibular sensitivity of PCs actually varies depending on the head angle relative to the body. I was expecting that they would then test whether the neck proprioceptive "position" term (that they had derived from the experiments in Figure 2) could be used to predict this shift in sensitivity. Instead, the authors directly jump to comparisons with the rostral fastigial population recorded in a prior study. Can they calculate whether their proprioceptive position weights (c_a,2 from equation 1) explain the measured difference in vestibular sensitivity?

We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We note, however, that it is not theoretically possible to predict a neuron’s “gain field” tuning via the linear addition its response to the static head-on-body position, which was estimated during body-under-head rotation (i.e., Figure 2A). This is because the estimated position term is a fixed value which would not contribute to a head position-dependent change in dynamic firing rate modulation. Put another way, the interaction between vestibular responses and head-on-body position that underlies the tuning shown in Figure 6 (old Figure 5) is inherently nonlinear. We have revised the RESULTS to discuss this point.

– In Figure 6, the authors show modeling evidence that linear summation of Purkinje cells can produce the tuning results of fastigial neurons. They don't address whether an alternate explanation, that some of the proprioceptive or vestibular tuning derives from direct mossy fiber afferents to fastigial neurons, is possible. Furthermore, they don't seem to address the basic surprise here, that although the PCs show much less "cancellation" between vestibular and proprioceptive inputs (p. 11, last paragraph) than fastigial neurons do, the summation of PC inputs can produce fastigial responses. One assumes that the only way to obtain this result is preferential summation of the small set of PCs that do show cancellation (oppositely signed proprioceptive and vestibular sensitivities) but this is never discussed. If that is the underlying explanation of their model's success, then it seems quite weak.

As noted in our response to the reviewers’ consensus feedback above, we have performed additional modeling to consider the influence of mossy fiber inputs and present these new results at the end of the revised RESULTS. Briefly, following Reviewer #2’s suggestion, we modeled mossy fiber input using random patterns of vestibular and neck proprioceptive input drawn from the experimentally distribution observed in the vestibular nuclei. As shown in Figure 7-supplemental figure 3, our new analysis demonstrates that any of these simulated mossy fiber inputs can be combined with Purkinje cell inputs to create two distinct signals, one related to head and one to body motion – consistent with the responses of target unimodal and bimodal rFN neurons. respectively. We then further explored the effect of systematically altering this simulated mossy fiber input – relative to the reference distribution of mossy fiber inputs – by i) doubling the gain, ii) reducing the gain by half, iii) doubling the phase, and iv) reducing the phase by half (Figure 7—figure supplement 4). Overall, we found that the addition of such simulated mossy fiber did not dramatically alter our estimate of the population Purkinje cell population size required to generate rFN neurons responses (~50 versus 40; Figure 7—figure supplement 3 and 4).

Finally, regarding the comment that “the PCs show much less "cancellation" between vestibular and proprioceptive inputs”, we believe that the reviewer is referring to the fact that because vestibular vs. proprioceptive sensitives are typically of opposite sign for bimodal rFN neurons they effectively cancel during combined stimulation (i.e., head-on-body), whereas this is not observed for Purkinje cells. We agree that it is interesting to understanding the relative contribution of agonistic vs. antagonistic Purkinje cells. Thus, in the revised manuscript we now report the weight distributions of Purkinje cells with agonistic vs. antagonistic vestibular and proprioceptive sensitivities. These results are shown in Figure 7-supplemental figures 1and2. Notably, our new findings confirm that both classes have similar weights, such that the heterogeneity that we observed Purkinje cell’s response is represented in the input to the model of fastigial responses.

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

I think the first experiment is strong and the paper should emphasize even more the comparison between Purkinje cells and the downstream fastigial neurons. I think some example fastigial neurons could be added to contrast with the Purkinje cell responses. Importantly, Figure 4C shows a red and orange cloud to denote the rFN neurons, but this is very hard to see in the figure. I think the individual neurons should be plotted to highlight the substantive difference between these Purkinje cells and fastigial neurons.

We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We have revised Figure 5A (old Figure 4C) to include plots of example rFN neurons from our previous study (i.e., Brooks and Cullen 2009) to emphasize the difference between Purkinje cells and fastigial neurons responses across paradigms.

I'm not sure the model is that insightful given that it assumes that there is no other input to the fastigial nucleus. I assume that it would have substantive vestibular and neck proprioceptive input from mossy fibers. As stated in the comment above, is the point of the Purkinje cell activity to counter the sensory input from the mossy fibers in order to create two distinct signals, one related to head and one to body motion in the fastigial population? Thus, a variant for the model would be to show how the population model could create distinct head and body motion signals to counter the presence of a random pattern of vestibular and neck proprioceptive input (assumed to be generated by mossy fiber input). A more advanced model might be interesting in which weights of the parallel fibers onto Purkinje neurons are altered given random mossy fiber inputs to granule cells and fastigial neurons leading to distinct head and body motion signals in the fastigial neurons, but this is certainly beyond the scope of this manuscript.

As noted above, we have performed additional modeling to directly address the reviewer’s concern. These new results are presented at the end of the revised RESULTS. Briefly, following the reviewer’s suggestion, we modeled mossy fiber input using random patterns of vestibular and neck proprioceptive input drawn from the experimentally distribution observed in the vestibular nuclei. As shown in Figure 7-supplemental figure 3, our new analysis demonstrates that any of these simulated mossy fiber inputs can be combined with Purkinje cell inputs to create two distinct signals, one related to head and one to body motion – consistent with the responses of target unimodal and bimodal rFN neurons. respectively. We then further explored the effect of systematically altering this simulated mossy fiber input – relative to the reference distribution of mossy fiber inputs – by i) doubling the gain, ii) reducing the gain by half, iii) doubling the phase, and iv) reducing the phase by half (Figure 7—figure supplement 4). Overall, we found that the addition of such simulated mossy fiber did not dramatically alter our estimate of the population Purkinje cell population size required to generate rFN neurons responses (~50 versus 40; Figure 7—figure supplement 3 and 4).

The cells in Figure 1 and 2 show the same patterns (linear, v-shaped, rectified) across the two conditions. I assume this was common for bimodal cells? Was there any analyses done to support or refute this?

We thank the reviewer for this observation and suggestion. We performed an additional analysis (Figure 2—figure supplement 3) and found that the category (i.e., linear, v-shaped, rectified, etc.) of most of the Purkinje cells is generally not the same across the two conditions. Thus, Purkinje cells show considerable heterogeneity in their simple spike responses dynamics to vestibular versus proprioceptive stimulation. We now note this in the RESULTS.

Figure 1 shows that all neurons were sensitive to vestibular input, and no unimodal proprioceptive neurons. Is this an order effect of the experiment in that you always tested vestibular first, and if responsive, then you completed the rest of the experiments? Thus, could there be proprioceptive only neurons, but they were not examined in this study due to the protocol. This is important and needs to be clearly stated in the manuscript, one way or another.

The reviewer is correct that we generally first tested the vestibular sensitivity of individual neurons. We however did check whether neurons that were insensitive to vestibular stimulation responded to neck proprioceptive stimulation. We found that only a minority of Purkinje cells (~10%) fell into this latter category – a result is similar to that of Manzoni and colleagues in anesthetized cat (12%, Manzoni et al., 1998). We now provide this information in the revised Methods.

The small sample size makes conclusions regarding head-on-body position a bit of a challenge. Notably, there are only 4 unimodal neurons for Purkinje cells in Figure 5C. The number of neurons is only noted in the figure legend and the number of neurons for the fastigial nucleus is not stated at all and looks to be 11 for bimodal and 10 for unimodal. I think the main text needs to clearly state the actual numbers and recognize the very small sample size for this analysis. These limited number of neurons to characterize tuning properties also weakens the modelling results and conclusions about 40 neurons necessary to predict fastigial neuron responses.

As noted above, we have revised the Results to clarify the numbers of Purkinje cells that were tested (13 bimodal and 4 unimodal Purkinje cells). For comparison, in our Brooks and Cullen study, tuning curves were computed for 10 bimodal and 12 unimodal rFN. We note that i) unimodal Purkinje cells make up a relatively small percentage of anterior vermis Purkinje cells and ii) similar to unimodal rFN, our small sample of unimodal Purkinje cells did not demonstrate significant tuning. In contrast, all bimodal Purkinje cells in our sample demonstrated significant tuning. To simulate responses for the bimodal Purkinje cells that were not held long enough to test during gain-field paradigm (i.e., Figure 6), we generated tuning curves drawn from a normal distribution estimated from 13 bimodal Purkinje cells. We appreciate this was not clear in the original submission and have revised the Methods section to clarify our approach. Overall, while we recognize that our sample size is small, we nevertheless found it interesting that including this our results from this protocol did not increase the estimated population size relative to that estimated using our other dynamic protocols.

In Figure 4C it looks like the positive body sensitivity ratio neurons tend to have larger values than the negative sensitivity ratio neurons. Is this just a random sampling issue?

We thank the reviewer for this comment and performed additional analyses, but did not find difference between the amplitude of negative vs. positive values of body sensitivity ratios (-1.0 ± 0.5 vs. 1.1 ± 0.6; p=0.39).

Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

1 – "All non-significant coefficients were set to zero." In the Methods, the authors should briefly describe the rationale for using this approach instead of L1 regularization.

A non-negative/non-positive least-square linear regression used to assign a sign to each neuron’s response sensitivity. A bootstrapping approach was used instead of lasso L1 regularization as a non-parametric to eliminate the effect of nonsignificant coefficients, since to the best of our knowledge there is no commonly used package for non-negative lasso regularization (see revised Methods).

2 – The acceleration term appears to dominate in the non-preferred direction for vestibular stimulation (Figure S1A); why?

We agree that this is an interesting feature of the non-preferred response. It indicates that Purkinje cell responses generally led stimulation velocity. While our studies do not address why this is the case, we now emphasize this point in the revised caption in reference to the relevant Figure (Figure 1-supplementary figure 1).

3 – Typo: "Albus and Marrs" -> "Albus and Marr".

Thank you, we have corrected this typo.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    MDAR checklist

    Data Availability Statement

    All data and codes to generate figures are available on Figshare under the URL: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.19362239.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES