Abstract
Many hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells function as place cells, increasing their firing rate when a specific place field is traversed. The dependence of CA1 place cell firing on position within the place field is asymmetric. We investigated the source of this asymmetry by injecting triangular depolarizing current ramps to approximate the spatially tuned, temporally diffuse depolarizing synaptic input received by these neurons while traversing a place field. Ramps were applied to CA1 pyramidal neurons from male rats in vitro (slice electrophysiology) and in silico (multicompartmental NEURON model). Under control conditions, CA1 neurons fired more action potentials at higher frequencies on the up-ramp versus the down-ramp. This effect was more pronounced for dendritic compared with somatic ramps. We incorporated a four-state Markov scheme for NaV1.6 channels into our model and calibrated the spatial dependence of long-term inactivation according to the literature; this spatial dependence was sufficient to explain the difference in dendritic versus somatic ramps. Long-term inactivation reduced the firing frequency by decreasing open-state occupancy, and reduced spike amplitude during trains by decreasing occupancy in the closed state, which comprises the available pool. PKC activator phorbol-dibutyrate, known to reduce NaV long-term inactivation, removed spike amplitude attenuation in vitro more visibly in dendrites and greatly reduced adaptation, consistent with our hypothesized mechanism. Intracellular application of a peptide inducing long-term NaV inactivation elicited spike amplitude attenuation during spike trains in the soma and greatly enhanced adaptation. Our synergistic experimental/computational approach shows that long-term inactivation of NaV1.6 is a key mechanism of adaptation in CA1 pyramidal cells.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The hippocampus plays an important role in certain types of memory, in part through context-specific firing of “place cells”; these cells were first identified in rodents as being particularly active when an animal is in a specific location in an environment, called the place field of that neuron. In this in vitro/in silico study, we found that long-term inactivation of sodium channels causes adaptation in the firing rate that could potentially skew the firing of CA1 hippocampal pyramidal neurons earlier within a place field. A computational model of the sodium channel revealed differential regulation of spike frequency and amplitude by long-term inactivation, which may be a general mechanism for spike frequency adaptation in the CNS.
Keywords: dendrite, hippocampus, K channel, modulation, Na channel, place cells
Introduction
Area CA1 of the hippocampus is thought to play a key role in learning and memory, specifically episodic memories (Burgess et al., 2002), sequential order (Fortin et al., 2002; Hoang and Kesner, 2008), and trace conditioning (Shors, 2004; Hunsaker et al., 2006). A type of episodic memory involving the sequential order of places along a linear track has been well studied in rodents (O'Keefe and Nadel, 1978). Many CA1 pyramidal cells function as place cells (O'Keefe and Dostrovsky, 1971); they increase their firing rate as they traverse a place field. These fields are specific to a given environment (Colgin et al., 2008), and result from the activation of a particular subset of synaptic inputs by aspects of the place field in that environment. The firing rate of individual place cells is generally symmetric around a peak in the center of the place field, as recorded in head-restrained mice navigating a virtual spatial environment on a spherical treadmill (Harvey et al., 2009). However, intracellular recordings from the same mice revealed that traversing the place field evoked a depolarizing ramp of synaptic excitation that was not symmetric, but instead peaked after three-quarters of the field had been traversed, such that the peaks of the firing rate and the underlying depolarization do not coincide. One explanation for this asymmetry is provided by the response of CA1 neurons to a temporally symmetric current ramp in anesthetized mice (Harris et al., 2002). The firing rate adapts such that the neuron fires substantially less on the down-ramp compared with the up-ramp. Such adaptation can explain how the firing rate peaks before the synaptic depolarizing ramp peaks. Although Harvey et al. (2009) observed a symmetric dependence of the firing rate on distance through the place field, this dependence becomes skewed in a predictive direction as the center of mass moves earlier in the place field as the environment becomes more familiar (Mehta et al., 2000), likely through synaptic plasticity mechanisms. Since adaptation is likely to contribute to the skew of the firing rate, here we investigate intrinsic mechanisms that determine firing rate adaptation in CA1 pyramidal cells.
Adaptation can result from the accumulation of an outward current or a decrement in an inward current active in the interspike interval (ISI). The Ca2+-activated small-conductance (SK) K+ current (ISK; Stocker et al., 1999; Pedarzani et al., 2005) and the muscarinic M-type K+ current (IM; Otto et al., 2006; Gu et al., 2008) are often implicated in firing rate adaptation in CA1 pyramidal neurons. Voltage-gated Na+ channels in CA1 pyramidal neurons have previously been shown to exhibit long-term inactivation that increases with distance from the soma along the apical dendrite (Colbert et al., 1997; Jung et al., 1997), and the resulting decrease in the available pool of sodium channels has been suggested as a mechanism for frequency adaptation in the same neurons (Fernandez and White, 2010).
We systematically tested these possible mechanisms of adaptation using a combination of in vitro slice electrophysiology and computational modeling. We applied symmetric current ramps to the soma or apical dendrite of CA1 pyramidal cells to simulate the depolarizing synaptic input these cells receive in vivo while crossing a place field. Selective blockers of ISK and IM did not decrease adaptation. To investigate the putative contribution of long-term inactivation of Na+ channels to adaptation, we calibrated a Markov model of a Na+ channel with a second inactivated state that recovered more slowly than the first, according to the spatially dependent long-term inactivation shown in the study by Mickus et al. (1999).
Experimentally, we observed that adaptation was more pronounced in the dendrites compared with the somata. The model demonstrated that the increase in occupancy in the long-term inactivated state of sodium channels along the apical dendrite was sufficient to account for this difference. Increasing or decreasing the occupancy in the long-term inactivated state through a selective peptide or protein kinase C (PKC) activator, respectively, significantly enhanced or attenuated adaptation, as predicted by the model.
Materials and Methods
Experimental methods
Slice preparation.
All the procedures described were conducted according to protocols approved by the Louisiana State University Health Sciences Center-New Orleans Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee, following guidelines on the responsible use of laboratory animals in research from the National Institutes of Health. The 7- to 11-week-old male Sprague Dawley rats were deeply anesthetized via intraperitoneal injection of ketamine and xylazine (90 and 10 mg/kg, respectively). Once the toe-pinch and palpebral reflexes ceased, rats were transcardially perfused with ice-cold oxygenated cutting solution containing the following (in mm): NaHCO3 28, KCl 2.5, NaH2PO4 1.25, MgCl2 7, CaCl2 0.5, dextrose 7, sucrose 234, l-ascorbic acid 1, and sodium pyruvate 3; then the rats were decapitated. The brains were rapidly removed and a vibratome was used to cut 400-µm-thick transverse hippocampal slices that were then transferred to a chamber filled with an oxygenated artificial CSF (ACSF) containing the following (in mm): NaCl 125, NaHCO3 25, KCl 2.5, NaH2PO4 1.25, MgCl2 1, CaCl2 2, dextrose 25, ascorbate 1, and sodium pyruvate 3. After the cutting procedure, slices were allowed to recover for 1 h at 36°C, with an additional recovery period of at least 1 h at room temperature.
Patch-clamp electrophysiology.
Individual slices were transferred to a submerged recording chamber and superfused with ACSF. All experiments were performed at 34–36°C. CA1 pyramidal cells were identified via differential interference contrast-infrared video microscopy. Whole-cell current-clamp recordings were made using amplifiers (model BVC 700A, Dagan) in the active “bridge” mode. Recording pipettes had a resistance of 1–3 MΩ (for somatic recordings) and 3–5 MΩ (for dendritic recordings) when filled with an internal solution containing the following (in mm): potassium methanesulfonate 125, KCl 20, HEPES 10, EGTA 0.5, NaCl 4, Mg2ATP 4, Tris2GTP 0.3, and phosphocreatine 14. Cells with resting membrane potentials depolarized beyond −60 mV at break-in were discarded. Series resistance was monitored throughout the recordings and was usually <20 MΩ; recordings were discarded when series resistance reached 25 or 30 MΩ, for somatic and dendritic recordings, respectively.
NBQX (10 μm), dl-APV (50 μm), gabazine (12.5 μm), and CGP55845 (1 μm) were applied in the external solution in all experiments to block glutamatergic and GABAergic neurotransmission, respectively, to isolate the contribution of the intrinsic ion channels to the ramp responses. Apamin (100 nm), XE-991 (10 μm), phorbol-2,13-dibutyrate (PDBu; 5 μm), and Androctonus mauretanicus mauretanicus toxin 3 (AmmTx3; 300 nm) were added to ACSF as needed, from stock solutions made with water or DMSO; the concentration of DMSO in the final solution was ≤0.1%. In a set of experiments to induce long-term inactivation of NaV channels, an N-terminally acetylated peptide, corresponding to residues 2–21 of the fibroblast growth factor homologous factor 2A (FHF2A; Dover et al., 2010) was added to the pipette intracellular solution. The peptide [F2A(2–21)] was purchased from Thermo Fisher Scientific, where it was custom synthesized and purified by HPLC, and its identity was confirmed by mass spectroscopy. Gabazine and CGP55845 were purchased from Abcam; dl-APV and XE-991 were purchased from HelloBio; NBQX, apamin, and AmmTx3 were purchased from Alomone Labs; and PDBu was purchased from Sigma-Aldrich.
To approximate the depolarizing input that place cells receive as an animal traverses the place field, symmetric ramp-shaped depolarizing current injections were applied via the recording electrode to CA1 pyramidal neurons at either the soma or the apical dendrite (150–250 µm from soma). Two second ramps (1 s up, 1 s down) and 10 s ramps (5 s up, 5 s down) were delivered to simulate different running speeds. Current amplitude was adjusted to evoke peak frequencies between 10 and 25 Hz that resemble place cell firing as recorded in vivo (Hargreaves et al., 2007; Resnik et al., 2012; Bittner et al., 2015).
Experimental design and statistical analyses
Experimental data were recorded and analyzed with Igor Pro software (WaveMetrics). Asymmetry of action potential (AP) firing with respect to the depolarizing ramp was quantified as a normalized spike ratio that is referred to here as the normalized adaptation ratio. While a ratio can be obtained by simply dividing the number of APs fired on the up-ramp by the number fired on the down-ramp (Harris et al., 2002), this measure cannot be applied in the case where a cell fires zero APs on the down-ramp. Here, we define the normalized adaptation ratio as follows:
This value ranges from −1 to 1. Positive values indicate asymmetry shifted toward the up-ramp, with the extreme case (a value of 1) being a cell that fired only on the up-ramp. Negative values indicate asymmetry that is shifted toward the down-ramp, with the extreme case (a value of −1) being a cell that fired only on the down-ramp. A value of 0 indicates symmetric firing. Plots of instantaneous frequency versus current (f/I) use the current value at the midpoint of the ISI. For experiments with PDBu, changes in the rate of rise of the action potentials during a spike train were assessed by comparing the time derivative of the membrane potential (dV/dt; Colbert et al., 1997) in different conditions, to estimate the degree of attenuation of sodium conductance along the train. As most ramps evoked at least seven action potentials, a ratio was obtained of the maximum dV/dt for the seventh over the first action potential during the spike trains.
A typical patch-clamp electrophysiology experiment with a difference in means of 1.3–2× and an SD of 0.2–0.3 requires n = 6–12 for p = 0.05 and a power of 0.9 (Cohen, 1977); each experimental group in this study is made up of n ≥ 8 recording sessions. The number of cells recorded for each experiment is indicated in the Results section. Recordings were from one cell per slice; in general, a maximum of three recordings were obtained for each animal.
Statistical analyses were performed in SPSS (IBM; RRID:SCR_002865), following the tutorials from Laerd Statistics [2015 (statistical tutorials and software guides, retrieved from https://statistics.laerd.com/)]. Where possible, comparisons were made in the same cell before and after pharmacological treatment; the summary plots for these data include individual points and the mean ± SEM. For these experiments, parametric analyses (paired-samples t test and repeated-measures ANOVA) were used to compare treatments in the same neurons, since data were normally distributed as determined by the Shapiro–Wilk test for normality. Significant differences revealed by repeated-measures ANOVA were followed up by post hoc analysis using Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons. When comparing somatic and dendritic responses, data were not collected within the same neurons and displayed unequal variance. For this case, the parametric independent-samples t test, modified to accommodate unequal variance (Welch's t test) was used. The summary plot in this case consists of a box-and-whisker plot, showing individual data. The mean values ± SEM for these data are reported in the text. Differences were considered to be statistically significant when p < 0.05.
Computational modeling
A multicompartmental CA1 pyramidal neuron model from our laboratory was used as a starting point (Combe et al., 2018). This model was based on the model in the study by Poirazi et al. (2003a), with subsequent changes made in studies by Shah et al. (2008) and Bianchi et al. (2012). The multicompartmental model from Combe et al. (2018) has 144 compartments in a reconstructed morphology (Megías et al., 2001; Fig. 1A), where each compartment can be represented with an equivalent circuit (Fig. 1B). Currents carried over from previous models include the leak conductance; an A-type K+ current (with different parameters for proximal and distal dendrites); a hyperpolarization-activated mixed cationic h-current; and T-type, R-type, and L-type Ca2+ currents. Consistent with previous models, a sevenfold gradient in the h-conductance (Magee, 1998), and a sixfold gradient in the A-type potassium conductance (Hoffman et al., 1997) were implemented along the apical trunk. In addition, to match the known lower amplitudes of back-propagating spikes in the dendrites compared with the somata (Gasparini and Migliore, 2015), our model has a lower sodium conductance density in the dendrites compared with the somata (75% of the somatic value). The delayed rectifier in the previously cited models was replaced by separate models of Kv1 and Kv2 currents known to be present in CA1 pyramidal cells (Kirizs et al., 2014; Liu and Bean, 2014; Morgan et al., 2019). The Kv1 model was taken from the study by Zbili and Debanne (2020), available at https://senselab.med.yale.edu/ModelDB/showmodel.cshtml?model=263053, and the Kv2 model was obtained at https://senselab.med.yale.edu/ModelDB/showModel?model=184176. An inward rectifying potassium current, IKIR, was also added, consistent with the literature (Chen and Johnston, 2005), as follows: . For the simulations in this work, IM and ISK were removed from the model for reasons explained in the text. In the previous model, intracellular calcium decayed with first-order kinetics to baseline values. Calcium dynamics are not central to this study, but we incorporated the calcium balance module from the study by Ashhad and Narayanan (2013) to make this model suitable for future extensions that will rely on calcium dynamics.
To better model long-term inactivation of NaV1.6, the dominant somatodendritic isoform in these cells (Lorincz and Nusser, 2010), the fast Hodgkin–Huxley-type Na+ current was replaced in the apical dendrites and somata with a Markov model modified from the study by Balbi et al. (2017). The original fast Hodgkin–Huxley-type Na+ currents were retained in the axon and basal dendrites in the absence of any data regarding long-term inactivation in those regions. This Markov model has the following four states: a closed state (C), a fast inactivated state (I1), a long-term inactivated state (I2) and an open state O (Fig. 1D1). Current can only flow through the open state, and transitions between initial state i and final state j are governed by voltage-dependent equations in which a Boltzmann function is multiplied by a scale factor, Rmax, specific to each transition rate, that gives the maximum transition rate, as follows: , where VH is the half-activation voltage and Vs the slope factor. The VH for the C-to-O transition was made spatially dependent along the apical dendrite, varying linearly from 0 mV at 0 µm from the soma to 6 mV at ≥200 µm from the soma. As a result, the half-activation voltage V1/2 for the NaV channels were −13 mV proximally and −20 mV distally, which are consistent with the data (Gasparini and Magee, 2002), showing that the V1/2 is more depolarized at the proximal dendrites compared with the distal dendrites.
The O–I1 and I1–O transition rates are the sum of two scaled Boltzmann functions; the parameters are reported in Table 1. To calibrate the Markov model, we simulated the experimental protocol described in the study by Mickus et al. (1999). To replicate the cell-attached patch-clamp data therein, a simulated voltage-clamp was applied to a single-compartment isopotential model containing only the Markov model of the Na+ channel. The model was held at −65 mV, and a 20 Hz train of 10 2 ms square pulses depolarized to 50 mV from rest was applied. The percentage steady-state inactivation was calculated as the difference in current amplitude between the first pulse and the 10th pulse divided by the amplitude of the first pulse. The transition rate for entry from I1 to I2 was calibrated in different spatial compartments to reproduce the observed increase in the percentage of steady-state inactivation with distance from the soma along the apical dendritic trunk, as shown in the study by Mickus et al. (1999). The following monotonically increasing expression for the maximum rate for the I1 to I2 transition produced the steady-state inactivation shown in Figure 1D3, with x representing the distance from the soma in μm: for x < 49.4, ; for 49.4 < x 124.1, ; and for x > 124.1, . Comparing the original data (reproduced with permission from the original Elsevier source, Mickus et al., 1999) in Figure 1D2 with the simulations in Figure 1D3 shows that the percentage of inactivation calculated from the simulation falls into the envelope of the experimental results.
Table 1.
C-O | O-C | O-I1 | I1-O | C-I1 | I1-C | C-I1 | I1-I2 | I2-I1 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
R max | 14 | 4 | 0.5, 2.5 | 3.75e-4, 1.875e-3 | 0.2 | 0.2 | 0.2 | * | 18e-5 |
VH (mV) | 0–6 | −48 | −42, 10 | −42, 10 | −65 | −65 | −65 | −25 | −50 |
VS (mV) | −6 | 9 | 12, −12 | 12, −12 | −11 | 10 | −11 | −5 | 12 |
The asterisk indicates a distance-dependent parameter as described in the text. Rmax is in units of ms−1, and VH and VS are in millivolts.
The passive properties of the model were adjusted consistent with the literature; the response of the model neuron to a 500-ms-long square pulse of 200 pA hyperpolarizing current injection is similar to that obtained in a typical in vitro recording (Fig. 1C). To account for the additional membrane area because of the higher density of dendritic spines at distal dendritic sites (Megías et al., 2001), a spine factor was incorporated into the model that multiplied the capacitance and divided the membrane resistance. This factor was 1.0 in the soma, the first 40 µm of the basal tree, and the first 100 µm of the apical trunk. In the apical tree, the factor stepped to 2.0 at 100 µm, then increased with a linear dependence up to 3.5 at the point where the apical dendrite branches into the tuft (394 μm in neuron pc2b). In the basal tree, the factor stepped to 3.5 at 40 µm from the soma. These values were based on spine densities reported by Megías et al. (2001). A sigmoidal decrease in membrane and axial resistance along the apical dendrites was implemented to further account for the decrease in input resistance along the apical dendrites (Magee et al., 1998; Poirazi et al., 2003b).
To replicate the experimental ramp protocol in the multicompartmental model, enough current was injected in the model to hold the membrane potential at −60 mV, and all transients were allowed to equilibrate before the application of the symmetric current ramp. These ramps, of 2 and 10 s durations, as described above, were applied in the soma, and at 219 μm from the soma in the dendrites for the morphology shown in Figure 1A. The amplitude of the ramp was adjusted to reach similar peak frequencies as in the experiments. For the simulations, spike threshold was defined as the voltage at which the second temporal derivative of the membrane potential exceeded 20 mV/ms2.
Data availability
The model code is freely available and can be downloaded from ModelDB at: http://modeldb.yale.edu/267140.
Results
In vivo patch-clamp recordings in rats on a linear track revealed that the synaptic input received at the somata of CA1 pyramidal cells while traversing the place field of the neuron resulted in a hill-shaped depolarization above the baseline (Epsztein et al., 2011) that generally closely followed the shape of the firing rate of the cell. Since there were no defined, repeatable shapes of this hill-shaped depolarization (Epsztein et al., 2011), in a first approximation to this spatially modulated synaptic input, we injected symmetric triangular-shaped ramps, 2 or 10 s in total duration, in either the soma (Fig. 2A1,A2) or the apical trunk at ∼200 µm from the soma (between 140 and 240 µm; Fig. 2B1,B2). The different durations could approximately correspond to different running speeds. For the 2 s ramp in the soma (Fig. 2A1), there are clearly more spikes on the up-ramp compared with the down-ramp, which is quantified in the summary data of the normalized adaptation ratio (see Materials and Methods) in Figure 2C1. Larger values of normalized adaptation ratio correspond to greater asymmetry. Moreover, the inset that plots the instantaneous f/I clearly shows hysteresis; the frequency on the down-ramp is always slower than that on the up-ramp at the same average level of depolarizing current. Concomitantly, firing initiates at a lower current amplitude than at which it terminates. This type of f/I plot characterizes rate adaptation (Venugopal et al., 2015). The 10 s somatic ramps (Fig. 2A2) show a similar pattern. The dendritic response to a 2 s ramp (Fig. 2B1) exhibits a much more pronounced decrease in spike height during the train as well as a more pronounced spike rate adaptation than the somatic ones. A Welch's t test was run to compare the normalized adaptation ratios between somatic and dendritic injections. The normalized adaptation ratio for 2 s dendritic ramps (0.44 ± 0.03, n = 30) was significantly higher than that for somatic ramps (0.29 ± 0.02, n = 33, t(51.096) = 4.178, p < 0.001; Fig. 2C1). As with the shorter ramps, there is a much more pronounced decline in spike height in the dendrites than in the somata during the 10 s ramps; however, in this case a substantial recovery of spike amplitude occurs on the down-ramp (Fig. 2B2). The frequency, however, does not recover on the down-ramp, indicating that these two aspects are regulated somewhat independently, as we later demonstrate. A Welch's t test was run to compare the normalized adaptation ratios between somatic and dendritic injections for 10 s durations. Again, the normalized adaptation ratio was significantly higher for dendritic ramps (0.36 ± 0.02, n = 23) than for somatic ramps (0.27 ± 0.02, n = 21, t(40.427) = 3.269, p = 0.002; Fig. 2C2). For all groups, spiking on the down-ramp ceased at a significantly higher current value than that required to elicit spiking on the up-ramp, as illustrated in Figure 2D, which shows the difference between the current needed to initiate the last spike on the down-ramp and the current needed to initiate the first spike on the up-ramp. The mean increases were as follows (in pA): somatic 2 s ramps, 22.8 ± 3.3 (t(32) = 6.937); dendritic 2 s ramps, 40.1 ± 6.6 (t(29) = 6.093); somatic 10 s ramps, 22.4 ± 3.7 (t(20) = 6.029); dendritic 10 s ramps, 29.0 ± 4.4 (t(22) = 6.628; p < 0.0005 for each comparison between current at last spike vs first spike).
Since the SK and M-type potassium currents are likely candidates to contribute to rate adaptation, we applied pharmacological blockers of each current separately and then together, and recorded responses in the apical dendrite where adaptation is most prominent (Fig. 3). We have previously shown that the SK current prevents CA1 neurons from following fast (70–100 Hz) trains of Schaffer collateral stimulation, but not 40 Hz stimulation (Combe et al., 2018). In this set of experiments with current ramp injections, the SK current blocker apamin (100 nm) did not occlude firing rate adaptation (Fig. 3A) and the normalized adaptation ratios were not significantly different (0.49 ± 0.05 under control conditions; 0.46 ± 0.06 in the presence of apamin; t(10) = −1.511; p = 0.162; n = 11; Fig. 3D). The M-type K+ channel blocker XE-991 (10 μm) caused the cells to fire faster at the onset of spiking (Fig. 3B), but did not significantly affect the normalized adaptation ratio (0.49 ± 0.06 under control conditions; 0.52 ± 0.07 in the presence of XE-991; t(8) = 0.992; p = 0.35; n = 9; Fig. 3E). When both blockers were applied simultaneously, they still failed to occlude spike rate adaptation (Fig. 3C), and the normalized adaptation ratio was not significantly different (0.45 ± 0.04 in control conditions; 0.50 ± 0.05 in the presence of apamin and XE-991; t(10) = 1.513; p = 0.161; n = 11; Fig. 3F). The voltage waveforms are different, however, in the presence of XE-991, in that the afterhyperpolarizations (AHPs) are shallower (Fig. 3E,F, inset). This may account for the initial higher firing frequency in Figure 3, B and C, which is consistent with the findings in the study by Gu et al. (2005).
The observations that adaptation is more pronounced in the dendritic than in the somatic ramps, and that dendritic ramps show a prominent decrease in spike height along the train made us hypothesize that the two features could be related. The decrease in spike height during the train in the dendrites is known to be because of long-term inactivation of Na+ channels, also called slow, cumulative inactivation (Colbert et al., 1997; Jung et al., 1997). To add this feature to our model, we replaced the fast Hodgkin–Huxley-type Na+ current in the apical dendrites and soma with a Markov model modified from the study by Balbi et al. (2017; see Materials and Methods). The previous studies in CA1 neurons often refer to slow, cumulative inactivation (Jung et al., 1997; Mickus et al., 1999). We and others (Dover et al., 2010; Navarro et al., 2020) call this process long-term inactivation instead, to differentiate it from a separate process called slow inactivation, which requires seconds to develop (Fleidervish and Gutnick, 1996; Ulbricht, 2005). In contrast, the long-term inactivated state is entered rapidly (within milliseconds) when FHFs bind to cytoplasmic domains of voltage-gated sodium channels (Goldfarb, 2012), but the recovery is slow, resulting in the longevity of occupancy in the long-term inactivated state. We therefore simulated the typical ramp responses recorded experimentally under control conditions in this modified multicompartmental model containing the Markov model for the Na+ channel; we also omitted the M-type and SK K+ currents, since the above results indicated that these currents did not substantially promote firing rate adaptation during the triangular ramp protocol.
Figure 4A shows 2 and 10 s simulated somatic ramps. The normalized adaptation ratio is ∼0.3 for both ramps, and rate-adapting f/I curves are also observed (Fig. 4A1,A2, insets); thus, both the voltage traces and the f/I plots are qualitatively similar to those experimentally observed in Figure 2A. The state diagram plots (Fig. 4A3,A4) show the fractional occupancy for each state of the Markov model for the Na+ channels (i.e., the fraction of channels that occupy a given state) as a function of time; a color diagram of the transitions is shown as an inset at the right of Figure 4A4. By definition, the sum of the fractional occupancy in all states is equal to 1. The color code at the right of Figure 4A3 orders the states in order of increasing availability to open. The least available state is the long-term inactivated (I2, magenta), since channels in this deeply inactivated state constitute a slow pool that is not readily available. Transitions from I1 to O are allowed, but at an extremely low rate (Table 1), hence the thinner arrow indicating that this rate is very small in the inset in Figure 4A4. Therefore, in general, channels must progress from the short-term inactivated state I1 (orange) through the closed state C (cyan) before opening (O, green). The occupancy in the open state during the ISI is too small to be observed when plotted on the same scale as the other states, but is critical as described below. In the same way, the I1-to-O transition rate, though small compared with the others, is responsible for the incomplete inactivation of the NaV channel that allows for a persistent current, which is active during the interspike interval. For the 2 s ramp (Fig. 4A3), occupancy in I2 accumulates throughout the spike train. For the 10 s ramp (Fig. 4A4), I2 recovers somewhat during the latter part of the spike train for reasons explained below. Occupancy in I1 shows a pronounced increase during each spike but is diminished by the end of the spike train, because a large fraction of channels become sequestered in I2.
Figure 4B1–B4 shows 2 and 10 s simulated dendritic ramps. The normalized adaptation ratio is 0.45 for the shorter ramp and 0.38 for the longer ramp, and rate-adapting f/I curves were again observed (Figs. 4B1,B2, insets). The voltage traces and f/I curves are qualitatively similar to the experimental observations in Figure 2B. For short ramps, more adaptation is observed in the dendrites than in the somata. Notably, the simulations also capture the substantially steeper decrease in spike amplitude in the dendrites compared with the soma, which is consistent with the experimental observations in Figure 2. In addition, for longer ramps, the simulations capture the partial recovery of spike height on the down-ramp, while the frequency keeps decreasing (compare Figs. 4B2, 2B2). One important contrast between the state diagram plots for somatic and dendritic current ramps is that for somatic ramps, Na+ channel occupancy in I1 is always higher than that in I2, whereas during dendritic ramps the I2 occupancy ends up exceeding that of I1. This is because of the spatial dependence of the parameter Rmax that determines the occupancy in the I2 state (see Materials and Methods; Fig. 1D3), and largely accounts for the differential effects of somatic versus dendritic current injection. Thus, the more prominent effects on both spike frequency and amplitude reduction in the dendrites are because of the greater buildup of occupancy in the I2 state in the dendrites compared with the somata (Fig. 4A,B, compare magenta traces).
During the short ramps, occupancy in I2 accumulates until the end of the spike train; in contrast, during the longer ramps, occupancy in I2 peaks and then declines before spiking ceases (Fig. 4, compare B3, B4). For these longer ramps, as the frequency slows on the down-ramp, longer interspike intervals provide more time for long-term inactivation to be removed. For sufficiently long intervals, more long-term inactivation is removed during the ISI than is added during the preceding spike, allowing occupancy in I2 to decrease. As occupancy in I2 decreases, occupancy in I1 increases, shifting a fraction of channels from the slow pool to the available pool. The spike height, which decreases during the up-ramp because long-term inactivation is accumulating, recovers to some degree on the down-ramp, as occupancy in the I2 state starts to decrease. However, the frequency does not recover; in fact, it slows down even more as the spike height recovers. Our working hypothesis was that occupancy in the I2 state is responsible for both the decrease in spike height and the decrease in frequency; therefore, it was not immediately evident how these two quantities could be differentially regulated by occupancy in the I2 state.
An explanation for these counterintuitive findings comes from the observation that, as I2 occupancy decreases and I1 occupancy increases during the down-ramp for long ramps, occupancy in C increases as well (Fig. 4B4, cyan trace). This suggests that the closed state, which constitutes the readily available pool, may play a role in the differential recovery of spike height and frequency. We looked more closely at the occupancy in various states during the longer ramps, using spike height as an additional measurement, and compared the differences between somatic and dendritic ramps by superimposing occupancy in the various states for the same levels of injected currents in the up-ramps and down-ramps (Fig. 5A–C). These plots show that occupancy in each of the states follows a history-dependent (hysteretic) trajectory. If there were no hysteresis, occupancy on the up-ramps and down-ramps at the same level of applied current would be identical. Figure 5A shows that the occupancy in the immediately available pool, consisting of channels in the closed C state (Baranauskas and Martina, 2006), is generally lower during the down-ramp than the up-ramp at comparable values of injected current. The hysteresis is more pronounced for the dendritic injections; moreover, the occupancy in the readily available state is, in general, lower in the dendrite than at the soma (Fig. 5, compare A1, A2). At spike threshold, only the Na+ channels in the available pool (C state) can be regeneratively recruited for the action potential upstroke. Therefore, in Figure 5D, we plotted the spike height versus occupancy in the C state at spike threshold. The height was measured from threshold to peak to exclude contributions of the AHP to spike amplitude. Occupancy in C accurately predicted spike height, independent of whether injected current was increasing (Fig. 5D, up-ramp, filled circles) or decreasing (Fig. 5D, down-ramp, open circles), both for the somata and for the dendrites (Fig. 5D). However, the dependence of the spike height on C occupancy was much steeper in the dendrites than in the somata, suggesting that a smaller change in C occupancy can yield a larger difference in spike height during the spike train. Together with the larger hysteresis in occupancy states in the dendrites, this largely explains the greater decrement in spike height in the dendrites.
Given the dependence of dendritic spike height on the available pool in C, it is clear that the spike height in Figure 4B2 recovers during the down-ramp because C, which is generally in equilibrium with I1, is increasing on the down-ramp [Fig. 5A2 (and also Fig. 4B2, cyan trace)]. Recall that the decrease in I1 (and consequently C) is driven by the cumulative increase of occupancy in the slow pool (I2), as shown in Figure 4. We therefore looked at the occupancy in I2 for the superimposed up-ramps and down-ramps. Again, the hysteresis is more prominent in the dendrites (Fig. 5B2) than in the soma (Fig. 5B1). The differential regulation of the spike height and frequency by occupancy in the I2 state occurs in part because the frequency does not depend directly on occupancy in C, which increases even as the frequency has yet to recover. Instead, the frequency depends on the very small fractional occupancy in the open state during the interspike interval (Fig. 5C; with values during spikes removed). Occupancy in the open state during the interspike interval is generally lower for both soma (Fig. 5C1) and dendrite (Fig. 5C2) on the down-ramp compared with the up-ramp at comparable values of injected current, but again the effect is more prominent in the dendrite. Occupancy in the open state during the ISI determines the level of persistent Na+ current; this level, together with other factors, particularly the level of injected current, determines the frequency. The net current (Fig. 5E), which is the sum of the ionic currents and the injected current and is equal to the capacitive current that charges the membrane, is quite small during the ISI, on the order of <30 pA at most, and approaches zero during flat ISIs. Although the occupancy in O is small, resulting in a small persistent Na+ current compared with the spiking currents, this current (Fig. 5F) is large relative to the net current during the ISI. Therefore, the decrease in the Na+ current on the down-ramp compared with the up-ramp accounts for the slower frequencies on the down-ramp. The currents shown in Figure 5, E and F, were summed over all compartments, in contrast to the state occupancies in Figures 4 and 5A–C, which are local to the specific somatic or dendritic compartment where the current ramp was injected, as indicated. Frequency does not recover on the down-ramp because occupancy in the open state, although it increases during the down-ramp, is still below the values on the up-ramp, so the small increase is not sufficient to compensate for the constantly decreasing level of injected current on the down-ramp. A Markov model that differentiates between a fast and a slow pool as well as differentiating between a readily available pool and a fast-inactivated pool easily accounts for this differential regulation of spike frequency and spike height, whereas we were never able to obtain these results with Hodgkin–Huxley-type models. Our model describes well both spike frequency and height changes; the differential regulation of these two quantities initially appears counterintuitive and would be difficult to tease out experimentally.
To show unequivocally that changes in occupancy in I2 are sufficient to affect asymmetry and spike rate adaptation in the f/I plots, we looked at the effect of bidirectionally manipulating the levels of occupancy in I2. We initially studied the effects of removing long-term inactivation by running simulations with entry into I2 blocked (Fig. 6A,B). These simulations confirm that firing rate adaptation in the model is indeed because of this mechanism; the f/I plots cease to show any hysteresis and become linear instead, both for somatic (Fig. 6A) and dendritic (Fig. 6B) current injections. The model strongly suggests that spike rate adaptation is because of long-term inactivation of the sodium channels, and phosphorylation by PKC has been shown to reduce long-term inactivation of dendritic Na+ channels (Colbert and Johnston, 1998). Therefore, we tested the effect of reducing occupancy in the long-term inactivated state in CA1 neurons in vitro by applying a PKC activator, the phorbol ester PDBu (Fig. 6C,D). As predicted, both somatic and dendritic ramps exhibit far less rate adaptation in the presence of PDBu compared with control. PDBu (5 μm) decreased the normalized adaptation ratio in the soma (from 0.36 ± 0.04 in control conditions to 0.13 ± 0.04; t(10) = −6.738; p < 0.001; n = 11; Fig. 6C,E). PDBu also prevented the reduction in spike amplitude during dendritic ramps, consistent with an effect exerted via reducing long-term inactivation of sodium channels (Colbert and Johnston, 1998). In addition, the normalized adaptation ratio for the dendritic recordings decreased from 0.50 ± 0.06 to 0.18 ± 0.05 (t(9) = −5.256; p < 0.001; n = 10; Fig. 6D,F).
Since there are other targets of phosphorylation by PKC, most notably the A-type potassium current (IA), carried by Kv4.2 channels, which is expressed at higher densities in the dendrites of CA1 neurons (Hoffman et al., 1997; Hoffman and Johnston, 1998), we performed an additional set of experiments to rule out a contribution of the A-type current to the occlusion of firing rate adaptation and asymmetry. Figure 7 shows that the specific Kv4 blocker AmmTx3 (300 nm; Maffie et al., 2013) by itself has little effect on spike frequency adaptation or spike height (Fig. 7, compare B, A). However, the latency to fire from the ramp onset for similar current injections was found to be significantly shorter (395.0 ± 62.0 ms) in the presence of AmmTx3 than under control conditions (474.7 ± 60.7 ms; t(8) = −5.788; p < 0.0005; Fig. 7E). Since IA is known to increase the delay to the first spike (Connor and Stevens, 1971), this result confirms a significant block of this current by AmmTx3. When added in the presence of AmmTx3, PDBu 5 μm still substantially reduced spike rate adaptation (Fig. 7C), as quantified by the normalized adaptation ratio (Fig. 7F), and prevented the decrease in spike amplitude during a train. The normalized adaptation ratio was significantly different across the three conditions according to a one-way repeated-measures ANOVA (F(2,16) = 16.100; p < 0.001). Bonferroni-corrected post hoc pairwise comparisons found no significant changes between the normalized adaptation ratios under control conditions (0.47 ± 0.07) and in the presence of AmmTx3 (0.50 ± 0.07; p = 0.541), but a significant decrease in the presence of PDBu (0.24 ± 0.04), with p = 0.015 and p = 0.006, respectively (n = 9). Figure 7D shows the profile of the rate of rise (dV/dt) of the dendritic voltage traces. The maximum rate of rise corresponds to the early part of the action potential and is dependent on the available Na+ conductance (Fleidervish et al., 1996; Colbert and Johnston, 1998); the decrease in these peaks mirrors the decrease in spike amplitude along the action potential train evoked by the ramp. The maximum rate of rise decreased significantly along the train because of the progressively decreased availability of Na+ channels during the train under control conditions and in the presence of AmmTx3, but far less in the presence of PDBu (Fig. 7G). A one-way repeated-measures ANOVA found that the ratio between the seventh and the first dV/dt was significantly different among the three conditions (F(1.102,8.819) = 134.262; p < 0.001; n = 9). Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons found no differences between control conditions (0.49 ± 0.04) and in the presence of AmmTx3 (0.49 ± 0.04; p = 1.00), but a significant increase in the presence of PDBu (0.83 ± 0.02; p < 0.001), suggesting a significant activity-dependent decrease in the fast sodium current in control and AmmTx3, that is prevented by PKC activation with PDBu. The amount of depolarization experienced during a ramp before the first spike does not induce substantially less inactivation in the presence of PDBu compared with in its absence. This explains the lack of effect on the rate of rise of the first (but not subsequent) spikes consistent with the findings of the study by Colbert and Johnston (1998).
To verify that long-term inactivation of Na+ channels is a generalizable mechanism to induce firing adaptation and asymmetric response to input ramps, we ran further simulations with two different morphologies (Fig. 8). As shown in Figure 1D3, the values in the Markov model transitions that produce a spatially dependent increase in the percentage of long-term inactivation similar to published data (Mickus et al., 1999) were determined for the c80761 morphology (Ishizuka et al., 1995; Fig. 8A) and the pc1a morphology (Megías et al., 2001; Fig. 8D). When this Markov model was used to replace the original description of the Na+ channels in the soma and apical dendrites for these morphologies, it produced adaptive responses to symmetric current injections, which were more evident in the dendrites than in the somata (Fig. 8B,E, C,F, compare left panels). Moreover, the decrease in spike height for dendritic simulations, as previously described, was also observed. Both these features, the decrease in spike height and the adaptation were removed by blocking entry into the I2 state in the Markov model (Fig. 8B,C,E,F, right panels).
As for the opposite scenario, to observe the effect of a generalized, enhanced long-term inactivation of Na+ channels in vitro, we included in the recording pipette solution a synthetic peptide corresponding to the N-terminal region (residues 2–21) of FHF2A [F2A(2–21)]. This peptide, F2A(2–21), was shown to be necessary and sufficient to selectively induce long-term inactivation in NaV1.6 channels (Dover et al., 2010). When F2A(2–21) was added to the intracellular solution, the voltage traces show a prominent decrease in the spike height during the train as well an enhanced adaptation, in a manner that is clearly concentration dependent (Fig. 9, compare A, B). F2A(2–21) increased the normalized adaptation ratio in a dose-dependent manner [Fig. 9C; the values were 0.46 ± 0.05 for 0.25 mm (n = 8); and 0.84 ± 0.07 for 0.5 mm (n = 11)]. With the higher dose, frequently the neurons did not fire at all on the down-ramp, resulting in a normalized ratio of 1.0. As the drug dialyzed rather quickly into the neurons, it was not possible to obtain full control datasets; this last value of normalized adaptation ratio, however, was almost three times higher than the one recorded for somatic ramp injection under control conditions (0.29 ± 0.02), and was even twice as large as the one found for dendritic ramp injections (0.44 ± 0.03; Fig. 2). To accomplish a similar situation in the model, we ran simulations where the transitions for the Markov model for all Na+ channels were adjusted as in the distal dendrites in Figure 1D3, therefore enhancing occupancy in I2 in the whole model neuron. When simulating injected current into the soma (Fig. 9D), the resulting trace now shows a strong decrease in spike amplitude during the train, as well as a stronger adaptation, as visible from the f/I plot (the normalized adaptation ratio went from 0.31 in Fig. 4A1 to 0.45 in Fig. 9D). Altogether, these results support our model prediction that entry of NaV channels into the long-term inactivated state promotes firing rate adaptation in response to symmetric depolarizing input.
Discussion
Summary
In this study, we have shown that (1) experimentally observed adaptation in response to symmetric depolarizing ramps is more prominent in the dendrites than in the somata, (2) adaptation is not mediated by the M-type and SK K+ currents that mediate adaptation in other conditions, (3) adaptation is likely mediated by long-term inactivation of voltage-dependent sodium channels, and (4) a phenomenological four-state Markov model of these channels, calibrated with a distance-dependent long-term inactivation, can account for the differences in adaptation between somata and dendrites, as well as for the differential regulation of spike height and frequency by long-term inactivation.
Significance
Since the excitatory inputs that mediate spatially tuned firing of place cells are likely received in the dendrites, the adaptation mediated by long-term inactivation of Na+ channels that we observe in the dendritic ramps likely affects the position of the center of mass of place fields, with implications for spatial coding. In addition to the 2 and 10 s ramps presented in the figures, a limited number of ramp heights and durations (1–4 s) were also examined in this study. In general, for constant total current injection (area under the curve), adaptation is maximal for longer ramps with smaller peaks, but occurs for all shapes.
This study was performed in the context of a sustained depolarization that is thought to underlie activation of ensembles of CA1 place cells. However, the sequential activation of place cells along a linear track likely generalizes to sequential activation encoding other types of episodic memory (Wood et al., 1999), meaning that these findings may be important for understanding episodic memory in general. Also, gain control (Chance et al., 2002), defined as modulation of the slopes of the frequency–current relationships of a neuron, is a general feature of neural processing (Salinas and Thier, 2000). The spike frequency adaptation modulated by NaV long-term inactivation is a form of divisive gain control as sustained depolarization, and the consequent adaptation, can decrease the slope of the steady-state frequency–current relationship (Fernandez and White, 2010).
Previous work on long-term inactivation of NaV channels in CA1 pyramidal cells
This work bridges studies using somatic recordings suggesting that long-term inactivation of sodium channels is responsible for frequency adaptation (Fernandez and White, 2010; Venkatesan et al., 2014) with studies showing that slow sodium channel recovery from inactivation is responsible for a decrease in amplitude during a train of dendritic back-propagating action potentials (Colbert et al., 1997; Colbert and Johnston, 1998). Our modeling work unifies these phenomena with a common mechanistic explanation of how these two processes (spike frequency and spike amplitude) are differentially regulated.
Essential aspects of the phenomenological Markov model
Much more complicated Markov models of NaV gating exist (Goldfarb, 2012; Navarro et al., 2020), but we attempted to use the simplest model possible to gain insight into long-term inactivation. We started with a published six-state model (Balbi et al., 2017) and removed one open state (Knowlton et al., 2021) and one closed state, leaving the four-state model, as in Figure 1D1. Figure 10 recapitulates the main features of the model and how the various states/pools differentially regulate adaptation and spike height during the train. The first essential aspect of the model is that the transition from I2 to I1 is very slow compared with that from I1 to I2, so long-term inactivation is entered rapidly but exited slowly, such that the slow pool (I2; Fig. 10, magenta) sequesters a fraction of the NaV channels and renders them completely unavailable for hundreds of milliseconds. Channels are recruited into this state during a spike and recover slowly during the interspike interval (Fig. 4), allowing accumulation across multiple spikes. The choice to connect I2 to the fast pool via I1 rather than O, as some models do (Goldfarb, 2012; Navarro et al., 2020), is not fundamental but phenomenological and was inherited from Balbi et al. (2017). The second essential aspect of the model is that the fast pool is not monolithic but is separated into the following three conceptual pools: the fast-inactivated (I1) pool, the readily available (C) pool, and the open (O) pool. Therefore, including I2, a minimum of four pools of NaV channels is required to explain our results. The closed state (Fig. 10, cyan) is immediately available to be recruited into a regenerative, positive-feedback loop that drives the spike upstroke, and, as shown in Figure 5D, the occupancy in this pool uniquely determines spike height. The I1 fast-inactivated state constitutes its own pool (Fig. 10, orange); it is not available immediately before a spike but can be quickly recruited into the available pool during the afterhyperpolarization and contribute to the following spike. Finally, the open pool (Fig. 10, green) has up to 20% occupancy during spiking, but on the order of 0.01% during the interspike intervals, as shown in Figure 5C. The slow pool has more occupancy on the down-ramp than on the up-ramp at comparable values of injected current, because of the history of depolarization; therefore, by a material balance, the fast pool must have less occupancy on the down-ramp. We have shown that although hysteresis in I2 drives both decreases in spike height and in frequency, these aspects are differentially regulated by the available and open pools, respectively. As shown in Figure 4, the long-term inactivated state is entered quickly during a spike, and, if the subsequent interspike interval is too short to remove the amount of long-term inactivation induced by the spike, as seen during the up-ramp, inactivation accumulates. However, for sufficiently long intervals, more long-term inactivation is removed during the ISI than is added during the preceding spike, allowing occupancy in the long-term inactivated state to decrease. As a consequence, occupancy in the readily available (C) pool increases, allowing spike height to recover somewhat on the down-ramp. In contrast to the complete dependence of spike height on the available pool, frequency depends not only on the hysteresis in the open pool, but also on the injected current. Therefore, spike height can recover to some degree on the down-ramp while the frequency is still decreasing.
Contributions of SK and M-type potassium currents
Previous work has implicated the SK and M-type K+ currents in spike frequency adaptation in CA1 pyramidal cells (Madison and Nicoll, 1984; Aiken et al., 1995; Peters et al., 2005; Otto et al., 2006). Our previous work (Combe et al., 2018) showed that the SK current limited the spiking frequency of CA1 pyramidal cells in response to trains of Schaffer collateral stimulation. Action potentials failed on average for every other stimulus for >50 Hz train in control, and blocking SK with apamin allowed the cells to follow the stimulus train more faithfully. However, at the firing frequencies (<25 Hz) examined in this study, the SK K+ current did not significantly contribute to adaptation (Fig. 3A). In addition, blocking the M-type K+ current with XE-991 allowed the initial spiking frequency to increase, but did not shift the normalized adaptation ratio in our experiments (Fig. 3B,C). Similarly, linopirdine, a specific M-type channel blocker, changed the time course of spike frequency adaptation in CA1 pyramidal cells, but not the degree of adaptation attained at steady state (Fernandez and White, 2010).
Modulation
We have shown that adaptation can be modulated by phosphorylation state (Fig. 6). Since phosphorylation states depend on the neuromodulatory milieu, adaptation may be plastic, which is relevant to spatial navigation; increasing adaptation moves the center of mass of the place field in a direction opposite the direction of motion, whereas decreasing adaptation moves it in the direction of motion. In addition, since dendritic NaV channels require seconds to fully recover from long-term inactivation (Colbert et al., 1997; Mickus et al., 1999), if a place field is traversed again within a few seconds of the first pass, the response will likely be attenuated. As stated above, there are also implications for gain control of the frequency–current relationship. Although we have modeled the transition rates between Markov states as parameters, in the context of neuromodulation the transition rates can be considered state variables whose values depend on the phosphorylation state of the channels.
Generality of this mechanism of spike rate adaptation
Adaptation because of long-term inactivation of NaV channels has been observed in serotonergic raphe neurons (Navarro et al., 2020). Long-term inactivation of NaV channels has also recently been demonstrated to control not only adaptation, but also entry into depolarization block in different subpopulations of midbrain dopamine neurons (Knowlton et al., 2021). We conclude that long-term inactivation of NaV channels may be quite generally used as a mechanism for spike frequency adaptation in the CNS.
Footnotes
This work was funded by National Institutes of Health Grant R01-MH-115832 under the CRCNS program to C.C.C. and S.G. We thank Dr. Mitchell Goldfarb for helpful discussion.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Aiken SP, Lampe BJ, Murphy PA, Brown BS (1995) Reduction of spike frequency adaptation and blockade of M-current in rat CA1 pyramidal neurones by linopirdine (DuP 996), a neurotransmitter release enhancer. Br J Pharmacol 115:1163–1168. 10.1111/j.1476-5381.1995.tb15019.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashhad S, Narayanan R (2013) Quantitative interactions between the A-type K+ current and inositol trisphosphate receptors regulate intraneuronal Ca2+ waves and synaptic plasticity. J Physiol 591:1645–1669. 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.245688 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balbi P, Massobrio P, Hellgren Kotaleski J (2017) A single Markov-type kinetic model accounting for the macroscopic currents of all human voltage-gated sodium channel isoforms. PLoS Comput Biol 13:e1005737. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005737 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baranauskas G, Martina M (2006) Sodium currents activate without a Hodgkin-and-Huxley-type delay in central mammalian neurons. J Neurosci 26:671–684. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2283-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bianchi D, Marasco A, Limongiello A, Marchetti C, Marie H, Tirozzi B, Migliore M (2012) On the mechanisms underlying the depolarization block in the spiking dynamics of CA1 pyramidal neurons. J Comput Neurosci 33:207–225. 10.1007/s10827-012-0383-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bittner KC, Grienberger C, Vaidya SP, Milstein AD, Macklin JJ, Suh J, Tonegawa S, Magee JC (2015) Conjunctive input processing drives feature selectivity in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Nat Neurosci 18:1133–1142. 10.1038/nn.4062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgess N, Maguire EA, O'Keefe J (2002) The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory. Neuron 35:625–641. 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00830-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chance FS, Abbott LF, Reyes AD (2002) Gain modulation from background synaptic input. Neuron 35:773–782. 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00820-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen X, Johnston D (2005) Constitutively active G-protein-gated inwardly rectifying K+ channels in dendrites of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci 25:3787–3792. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5312-04.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J (1977) CHAPTER 1 - the concepts of power analysis. In: Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (revised edition), pp 1–17. New York: Academic. [Google Scholar]
- Colbert CM, Johnston D (1998) Protein kinase C activation decreases activity-dependent attenuation of dendritic Na+ current in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. J Neurophysiol 79:491–495. 10.1152/jn.1998.79.1.491 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colbert CM, Magee JC, Hoffman DA, Johnston D (1997) Slow recovery from inactivation of Na+ channels underlies the activity-dependent attenuation of dendritic action potentials in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci 17:6512–6521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colgin LL, Moser EI, Moser M-B (2008) Understanding memory through hippocampal remapping. Trends Neurosci 31:469–477. 10.1016/j.tins.2008.06.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Combe CL, Canavier CC, Gasparini S (2018) Intrinsic mechanisms of frequency selectivity in the proximal dendrites of CA1 pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci 38:8110–8127. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0449-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Connor JA, Stevens CF (1971) Prediction of repetitive firing behaviour from voltage clamp data on an isolated neurone soma. J Physiol 213:31–53. 10.1113/jphysiol.1971.sp009366 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dover K, Solinas S, D'Angelo E, Goldfarb M (2010) Long-term inactivation particle for voltage-gated sodium channels. J Physiol 588:3695–3711. 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.192559 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epsztein J, Brecht M, Lee AK (2011) Intracellular determinants of hippocampal CA1 place and silent cell activity in a novel environment. Neuron 70:109–120. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez FR, White JA (2010) Gain control in CA1 pyramidal cells using changes in somatic conductance. J Neurosci 30:230–241. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3995-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleidervish IA, Gutnick MJ (1996) Kinetics of slow inactivation of persistent sodium current in layer V neurons of mouse neocortical slices. J Neurophysiol 76:2125–2130. 10.1152/jn.1996.76.3.2125 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleidervish IA, Friedman A, Gutnick MJ (1996) Slow inactivation of Na+ current and slow cumulative spike adaptation in mouse and guinea-pig neocortical neurones in slices. J Physiol 493:83–97. 10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021366 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fortin NJ, Agster KL, Eichenbaum HB (2002) Critical role of the hippocampus in memory for sequences of events. Nat Neurosci 5:458–462. 10.1038/nn834 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gasparini S, Magee JC (2002) Phosphorylation-dependent differences in the activation properties of distal and proximal dendritic Na+ channels in rat CA1 hippocampal neurons. J Physiol 541:665–672. 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.020503 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gasparini S, Migliore M (2015) Action potential backpropagation. In: Encyclopedia of computational neuroscience (Jaeger D, Jung R, eds), pp 133–137. New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Goldfarb M (2012) Voltage-gated sodium channel-associated proteins and alternative mechanisms of inactivation and block. Cell Mol Life Sci 69:1067–1076. 10.1007/s00018-011-0832-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gu N, Vervaeke K, Hu H, Storm JF (2005) Kv7/KCNQ/M and HCN/h, but not KCa2/SK channels, contribute to the somatic medium after-hyperpolarization and excitability control in CA1 hippocampal pyramidal cells. J Physiol 566:689–715. 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.086835 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gu N, Hu H, Vervaeke K, Storm JF (2008) SK (KCa2) channels do not control somatic excitability in CA1 pyramidal neurons but can be activated by dendritic excitatory synapses and regulate their impact. J Neurophysiol 100:2589–2604. 10.1152/jn.90433.2008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hargreaves EL, Yoganarasimha D, Knierim JJ (2007) Cohesiveness of spatial and directional representations recorded from neural ensembles in the anterior thalamus, parasubiculum, medial entorhinal cortex, and hippocampus. Hippocampus 17:826–841. 10.1002/hipo.20316 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris KD, Henze DA, Hirase H, Leinekugel X, Dragoi G, Czurkó A, Buzsáki G (2002) Spike train dynamics predicts theta-related phase precession in hippocampal pyramidal cells. Nature 417:738–741. 10.1038/nature00808 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harvey CD, Collman F, Dombeck DA, Tank DW (2009) Intracellular dynamics of hippocampal place cells during virtual navigation. Nature 461:941–946. 10.1038/nature08499 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoang LT, Kesner RP (2008) Dorsal hippocampus, CA3, and CA1 lesions disrupt temporal sequence completion. Behav Neurosci 122:9–15. 10.1037/0735-7044.122.1.9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman DA, Magee JC, Colbert CM, Johnston D (1997) K+ channel regulation of signal propagation in dendrites of hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Nature 387:869–875. 10.1038/43119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman DA, Johnston D (1998) Downregulation of transient K+ channels in dendrites of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons by activation of PKA and PKC. J Neurosci 18:3521–3528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunsaker MR, Thorup JA, Welch T, Kesner RP (2006) The role of CA3 and CA1 in the acquisition of an object-trace-place paired-associate task. Behav Neurosci 120:1252–1256. 10.1037/0735-7044.120.6.1252 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ishizuka N, Cowan WM, Amaral DG (1995) A quantitative analysis of the dendritic organization of pyramidal cells in the rat hippocampus. J Comp Neurol 362:17–45. 10.1002/cne.903620103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jung H-Y, Mickus T, Spruston N (1997) Prolonged sodium channel inactivation contributes to dendritic action potential attenuation in hippocampal pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci 17:6639–6646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirizs T, Kerti-Szigeti K, Lorincz A, Nusser Z (2014) Distinct axo-somato-dendritic distributions of three potassium channels in CA1 hippocampal pyramidal cells. Eur J Neurosci 39:1771–1783. 10.1111/ejn.12526 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knowlton CJ, Ziouziou TI, Hammer N, Roeper J, Canavier CC (2021) Inactivation mode of sodium channels defines the different maximal firing rates of conventional versus atypical midbrain dopamine neurons. PLoS Comput Biol 17:e1009371. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009371 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu PW, Bean BP (2014) Kv2 channel regulation of action potential repolarization and firing patterns in superior cervical ganglion neurons and hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci 34:4991–5002. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1925-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorincz A, Nusser Z (2010) Molecular identity of dendritic voltage-gated sodium channels. Science 328:906–909. 10.1126/science.1187958 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Madison DV, Nicoll RA (1984) Control of the repetitive discharge of rat CA 1 pyramidal neurones in vitro. J Physiol 354:319–331. 10.1113/jphysiol.1984.sp015378 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maffie JK, Dvoretskova E, Bougis PE, Martin-Eauclaire M-F, Rudy B (2013) Dipeptidyl-peptidase-like-proteins confer high sensitivity to the scorpion toxin AmmTX3 to Kv4-mediated A-type K+ channels. J Physiol 591:2419–2427. 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.248831 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magee J, Hoffman D, Colbert C, Johnston D (1998) Electrical and calcium signaling in dendrites of hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Annu Rev Physiol 60:327–346. 10.1146/annurev.physiol.60.1.327 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magee JC (1998) Dendritic hyperpolarization-activated currents modify the integrative properties of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci 18:7613–7624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Megías M, Emri Z, Freund TF, Gulyás AI (2001) Total number and distribution of inhibitory and excitatory synapses on hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells. Neuroscience 102:527–540. 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00496-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehta MR, Quirk MC, Wilson MA (2000) Experience-dependent asymmetric shape of hippocampal receptive fields. Neuron 25:707–715. 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81072-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mickus T, Jung H-y, Spruston N (1999) Properties of slow, cumulative sodium channel inactivation in rat hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. Biophys J 76:846–860. 10.1016/S0006-3495(99)77248-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan PJ, Bourboulou R, Filippi C, Koenig-Gambini J, Epsztein J (2019) Kv1.1 contributes to a rapid homeostatic plasticity of intrinsic excitability in CA1 pyramidal neurons in vivo. Elife 8:e49915. 10.7554/eLife.49915 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Navarro MA, Salari A, Lin JL, Cowan LM, Penington NJ, Milescu M, Milescu LS (2020) Sodium channels implement a molecular leaky integrator that detects action potentials and regulates neuronal firing. Elife 9:e54940. 10.7554/eLife.54940 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Keefe J, Dostrovsky J (1971) The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res 34:171–175. 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90358-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Keefe J, Nadel L (1978) The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Oxford UP. [Google Scholar]
- Otto JF, Yang Y, Frankel WN, White HS, Wilcox KS (2006) A spontaneous mutation involving Kcnq2 (Kv7.2) reduces M-current density and spike frequency adaptation in mouse CA1 neurons. J Neurosci 26:2053–2059. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1575-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pedarzani P, McCutcheon JE, Rogge G, Jensen BS, Christophersen P, Hougaard C, Strøbaek D, Stocker M (2005) Specific enhancement of SK channel activity selectively potentiates the afterhyperpolarizing current IAHP and modulates the firing properties of hippocampal pyramidal neurons. J Biol Chem 280:41404–41411. 10.1074/jbc.M509610200 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters HC, Hu H, Pongs O, Storm JF, Isbrandt D (2005) Conditional transgenic suppression of M channels in mouse brain reveals functions in neuronal excitability, resonance and behavior. Nat Neurosci 8:51–60. 10.1038/nn1375 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poirazi P, Brannon T, Mel BW (2003a) Arithmetic of subthreshold synaptic summation in a model CA1 pyramidal cell. Neuron 37:977–987. 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00148-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poirazi P, Brannon T, Mel BW (2003b) Pyramidal neuron as two-layer neural network. Neuron 37:989–999. 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00149-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Resnik E, McFarland JM, Sprengel R, Sakmann B, Mehta MR (2012) The effects of GluA1 deletion on the hippocampal population code for position. J Neurosci 32:8952–8968. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6460-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salinas E, Thier P (2000) Gain modulation: a major computational principle of the central nervous system. Neuron 27:15–21. 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00004-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shah MM, Migliore M, Valencia I, Cooper EC, Brown DA (2008) Functional significance of axonal Kv7 channels in hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci U|S|A 105:7869–7874. 10.1073/pnas.0802805105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shors TJ (2004) Memory traces of trace memories: neurogenesis, synaptogenesis and awareness. Trends Neurosci 27:250–256. 10.1016/j.tins.2004.03.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stocker M, Krause M, Pedarzani P (1999) An apamin-sensitive Ca2+-activated K+ current in hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci U|S|A 96:4662–4667. 10.1073/pnas.96.8.4662 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ulbricht W (2005) Sodium channel inactivation: molecular determinants and modulation. Physiol Rev 85:1271–1301. 10.1152/physrev.00024.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Venkatesan K, Liu Y, Goldfarb M (2014) Fast-onset long-term open-state block of sodium channels by A-type FHFs mediates classical spike accommodation in hippocampal pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci 34:16126–16139. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1271-14.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Venugopal S, Hsiao C-F, Sonoda T, Wiedau-Pazos M, Chandler SH (2015) Homeostatic dysregulation in membrane properties of masticatory motoneurons compared with oculomotor neurons in a mouse model for amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. J Neurosci 35:707–720. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1682-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood ER, Dudchenko PA, Eichenbaum H (1999) The global record of memory in hippocampal neuronal activity. Nature 397:613–616. 10.1038/17605 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zbili M, Debanne D (2020) Myelination increases the spatial extent of analog-digital modulation of synaptic transmission: a modeling study. Front Cell Neurosci 14:40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The model code is freely available and can be downloaded from ModelDB at: http://modeldb.yale.edu/267140.