Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Oct 4.
Published in final edited form as: J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2021 Oct 19;28(8):781–789. doi: 10.1017/S1355617721001041

Partial Least Squares Analysis of Alzheimer’s Disease Biomarkers, Modifiable Health Variables, and Cognition in Older Adults with Mild Cognitive Impairment

Jessica Stark 1, Daniela J Palombo 3, Jasmeet P Hayes 1,2, Kelly J Hiersche 1, Alexander N Hasselbach 1, Scott M Hayes 1,2; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative
PMCID: PMC9094430  NIHMSID: NIHMS1789350  PMID: 34664547

Abstract

Objectives:

To identify novel associations between modifiable physical and health variables, Alzheimer’s disease (AD) biomarkers, and cognitive function in a cohort of older adults with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI).

Methods:

Metrics of cardiometabolic risk, stress, inflammation, neurotrophic/growth factors, AD, and cognition were assessed in 154 MCI participants (Mean age = 74.1 years) from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Partial Least Squares analysis was employed to examine associations among these physiological variables and cognition.

Results:

Latent variable 1 revealed a unique combination of AD biomarkers, neurotrophic/growth factors, education, and stress that were significantly associated with specific domains of cognitive function, including episodic memory, executive function, processing speed, and language, representing 45.2% of the cross-block covariance in the data. Age, body mass index, and metrics tapping basic attention or premorbid IQ were not significant.

Conclusions:

Our data-driven analysis highlights the significant relationships between metrics associated with AD pathology, neuroprotection, and neuroplasticity, primarily with tasks tapping episodic memory, executive function, processing speed, and verbal fluency rather than more basic tasks that do not require mental manipulation (basic attention and vocabulary). These data also indicate that biological metrics are more strongly associated with episodic memory, executive function, and processing speed than chronological age in older adults with MCI.

Keywords: Healthy Aging, Neuropsychology, Neuroprotection, Neuroplasticity, Memory, Executive Function

INTRODUCTION

Age and Alzheimer’s disease (AD)-related neural decline negatively affect multiple aspects of cognition, such as episodic memory (memory for specific personal past events), executive function (ability to plan, inhibit responses, and sustain attention), and processing speed (Buckner, 2004; Salthouse, 2010; Tromp, Dufour, Lithfous, Pebayle, & Després, 2015). Yet, there is substantial variability in aging, and multiple factors have been shown to accelerate or mitigate cognitive decline. For instance, studies have shown that cardiometabolic variables such as body mass index (BMI; Farooqui, Farooqui, Panza, & Frisardi, 2012; Yaffe et al., 2004), inflammatory indicators such as c-reactive protein (CRP; Yaffe et al., 2004), neurotrophic/growth factors such as brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF; Lista & Sorrentino, 2010; Miranda, Morici, Zanoni, & Bekinschtein, 2019), and blood and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarkers such as plasma tau and CSF Aß1–42 are associated with cognition and AD risk (Chiu et al., 2014; Diniz, Pinto, & Forlenza, 2008; Matura et al., 2019).

These variables and their link to cognition are typically examined in isolation, rather than simultaneously within the same study. Therefore, there is limited knowledge of the relative strength of associations between cognition and cardiometabolic, inflammation, neurotrophic/growth factors, and AD biomarkers. One exception is a study conducted by Meyer and colleagues (2019), which assessed neurotrophic/growth factors, inflammatory indicators, and AD biomarkers in cognitively normal, Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI), and AD participants. Machine learning techniques were used to create predictor weights for both CSF proteins and AD biomarkers, which were subsequently used in three separate regression models predicting general cognitive function. They found that CSF protein and AD biomarkers accounted for 31% and 26% of the variance in cognitive scores, respectively (P.-F. Meyer, Savard, Poirier, Morgan, & Breitner, 2019). However, this study did not examine metrics associated with cardiometabolic risk, which are linked to cognition and dementia risk, and did not assess specific domains of cognitive function, which are known to be differentially impacted by aging and AD.

The goal of the current study was to address a gap in the literature by using a multivariate analysis to map associations between domain-specific cognitive function and multiple AD biomarkers, neurotrophic/growth factors, inflammatory markers, and cardiometabolic metrics in older adults with MCI. The Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative Phase 1 (ADNI1) was used to obtain data on modifiable health factors (such as those associated with cardiometabolic health: BMI and cholesterol), stress (e.g., cortisol), inflammation (e.g., CRP), neuroprotection (e.g., BDNF), and AD biomarkers (e.g., CSF Aß1–42, plasma tau) in a cohort of older adults diagnosed with MCI. We implemented a Partial Least Squares Correlational (PLSC) analysis, an unbiased and flexible multivariate technique for defining latent variables in a dataset, that does not require assigned predictor and outcome variables, but rather maps shared covariance between two sets of data (Abdi & Williams, 2013). Broadly, latent variables are linear combinations of variables from a data table optimized for a specific goal. In the case of PLSC, latent variables are optimized to maximize covariance between two sets of data with the goal of finding shared information between them (Abdi & Williams, 2013). PLSC analysis was preferred to other multivariate or data-driven statistical approaches because it does not attempt to predict an outcome, making it an ideal fit for this cross-sectional data and the exploratory nature of our research question. Additionally, unlike multiple linear regression, PLSC analysis is well equipped to deal with a large number of variables or with multiple collinear variables (Van Roon, Zakizadeh, & Chartier, 2014) and data do not need to be normally distributed (Van Roon et al., 2014). Thus, PLSC analysis was employed to identify and parse novel relationships across these identified physiological domains and cognition in a cohort of older adults with MCI.

METHODS

Participants

Participants with a diagnosis of MCI from the ADNI1 cohort were included in the current study. Full participant inclusion/exclusion criteria are available in the ADNI Procedures Manual, 2010, and are summarized here: 6th grade or higher education, fluent in English or Spanish, Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) ≥ 24, Clinical Dementia Rating of .5, subjective memory complaint by subject or study partner, impaired episodic memory, and sufficiently preserved general cognition and functional performance not meeting criteria for AD. Participants with missing data for any variables of interest were excluded, as complete data were necessary for PLSC analysis. One participant classified as MCI with an MMSE score of 23 and one participant with an extremely high and improbable triglycerides value (2084.0 mg/dL) were excluded. The final analysis sample included 154 MCI participants (age: 54.4–88.3 years; Mean = 74.1 years; SD = 7.5 years; education: 6–20 years; Mean = 16.0 years; SD = 2.9 years; 51 females; 150 White, 2 Asian, 2 Black; 67 APOE ε4 negative). Other ADNI cohorts (ADNIGO, ADNI2, and ADNI3) were excluded from the analysis as these cohorts did not assess neurotrophic and growth factors. Study procedures were approved by site-specific Institutional Review Boards, and all participants and/or authorized representatives provided written informed consent consistent with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Neuropsychological Assessment

Neuropsychological data were obtained from screening (WMS-R Logical Memory and MMSE) and baseline visits (all other tests). Average time between appointments was 41.3 days. Nineteen raw scores from the assessment were included in the PLS analysis (see Table 1).

Table 1.

Participant Demographic, Neuropsychological, Physical, Health, and AD Biomarker Data Entered into the PLS Analysis. KEY = ADAS-COG = Alzheimer’s disease Assessment Scale, Cognitive Subsection; ANART = American National Adult Reading Test; MMSE = Mini Mental State Exam; CSF = Cerebrospinal Fluid; RAVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; WMS-R = Weschler Memory Scale – Revised

Demographic Variables Physical, Health, and AD Variables
Unit Mean (SD) Unit Mean (SD)
Age Years 74.1 (7.5) Body Mass Index kg/m2 25.8 (3.7)
Education Years 16.0 (2.9) Seated Systolic Blood Pressure mmHg 134.1 (17.8)
Neuropsychological Variables Seated Diastolic Blood Pressure mmHg 74.2 (9.7)
WMS-R Logical Memory Immediate Recall Number of story details correctly recalled 6.9 (3.2) Seated Pulse Rate Per minute 64.2 (9.5)
WMS-R Logical Memory Delay Recall Number of story details correctly recalled 3.7 (2.7) Serum Glucose mg/dL 101.8(31.7)
RAVLT List 1 Number of words correctly recalled 4.0 (1.4) Triglycerides mg/dL 151.0 (102.5)
RAVLT List 6 Number of words correctly recalled 3.4 (3.0) Cholesterol mg/dL 198.7 (39.8)
RAVLT List B Number of words correctly recalled 3.6 (1.4) Insulin* + uIU/mL 0.3 (0.3)
RAVLT Delay Recall Number of words correctly recalled 2.5 (3.0) C-reactive Protein * + ug/mL 0.1 (0.5)
Longest Digit Span Forward Length of longest digit span correctly recalled 6.5 (1.1) Cortisol * + ng/mL 2.2 (0.1)
Longest Digit Span Backward Length of longest digit span correctly recalled 4.6 (1.1) Interleukin-6 Receptor* + ng/mL 1.5 (0.1)
Trail Making Test, Trail B Score ** Seconds to correctly complete the trail 135.8 (73.5) Insulin-like Growth Factor Binding Protein * + ng/mL 2.0 (0.2)
Trail Making Test, Trail A Score ** Seconds to correctly complete the trail 46.3 (24.7) Brain-derived Neurotrophic Factor * + ng/mL 0.3 (0.4)
Digit Symbol Substitution Score Number of correctly drawn symbols 36.7(10.8) Vascular Endothelial Growth Factor* + pg/mL 2.8 (0.1)
Clock Drawing Score Number of clock details correctly drawn based on verbal command 4.1 (1.1) Epidermal Growth Factor* + pg/mL 1.6 (0.6)
Clock Copy Score Number of clock details correctly drawn when visual clock stimulus is present 4.6 (0.8) Heparin-binding Epidermal-Growth-F actor-like Growth Factor *+ pg/mL 1.9 (0.4)
Category Fluency: Animals Number of words produced in the correct category 15.7 (4.7) Hepatocyte Growth Factor * + ng/mL 0.6 (0.1)
Category Fluency: Vegetables Number of words produced in the correct category 10.7 (3.4) Platelet-derived Growth Factor BB * + pg/mL 3.2 (0.5)
Boston Naming Test Total Correct Number of drawings correctly named 25.8 (3.9) Apolipoprotein E* + ug/mL 1.7 (0.2)
ANART Errors ** Number of words incorrectly pronounced 14.5 (9.9) Tau + pg/mL 2.8 (1.7)
MMSE Total score 26.9 (1.8) 1–42 (CSF) pg/mL 742.0 (337.3)
ADAS-COG ** Total Score 11.7(4.6) Total tau (CSF) pg/mL 311.6(129.7)
P-tau181 (CSF) pg/mL 31.1 (15.1)
*

these values are normalized

**

higher scores = poorer performance

+

plasma levels

Episodic Memory –

WMS-R Logical Memory I (immediate recall; number of story details correctly recalled), WMS-R Logical Memory II (delayed recall; number of story details correctly recalled), Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test List 1 (RAVLT; number of words correctly recalled), RAVLT List B (number of words correctly recalled on the interference list); RAVLT List 6 (number of words correctly recalled on the original list after interference); and RAVLT 30-minute delay recall (number of words correctly recalled on the original list).

Working Memory –

Digit Span Forward (length of the longest digit span correctly recalled).

Executive Function –

Digit Span Backward (length of the longest digit span correctly recalled), Trail Making Test (Trail B; number of seconds to correctly complete the trail).

Processing Speed –

Trail Making Test (Trail A; number of seconds to correctly complete the trail), Digit Symbol Substitution Test (number of correctly drawn symbols).

Visuospatial Ability –

Clock Drawing Test (Clock Drawing [number of details correctly drawn based on verbal command] and Clock Copy [number of clock details correctly drawn/copied when a visual clock stimulus is present]).

Language –

Category Fluency (number of words produced in the correct category for animals and vegetables), The Boston Naming Test (number of drawings correctly named).

Premorbid IQ –

American National Adult Reading Test (number of words incorrectly pronounced).

Global Cognition –

MMSE (total score), Alzheimer’s disease Assessment Scale (ADAS-COG; total score).

Cardiometabolic, Stress, and Inflammation Variables

BMI (kg/m2), systolic blood pressure (mmHg), diastolic blood pressure (mmHg), pulse rate (per minute), cholesterol (mg/dL), triglycerides (mg/dL), and serum glucose (mg/dL) data were obtained. Insulin (uIU/mL), cortisol (ng/mL), CRP (ug/mL), and interleukin-6 receptor (ng/mL) data were assessed from fasting plasma blood samples; these data were normalized and checked for the defined least detectable dose during the quality control process.

Growth Factors and Neurotrophic Factors

Insulin-like growth factor binding protein (ng/mL), epidermal growth factor (pg/mL), heparin-binding epidermal-growth-factor-like growth factor (HB-EGF-like-GF; pg/mL), hepatocyte growth factor (ng/mL), platelet-derived growth factor BB (PDGF; pg/mL), BDNF (ng/mL), and vascular-endothelial growth factor (pg/mL) were analyzed. Data were normalized and checked for the defined least detectable dose during the quality control process.

AD Biomarkers

Plasma Apolipoprotein E (apoE; ug/mL), plasma tau (pg/mL), CSF total tau (t-tau; pg/mL), CSF phospho-tau (181; p-tau181; pg/mL), and CSF Aß1–42 (pg/mL) were also examined. Only values within the given ranges were included for analyses: Aß1–42 200–1700 pg/mL, p-tau181 8–120 pg/mL, and t-tau 80–1300 pg/mL, as these are the reported technical limits.

Data Processing and Analysis

ADNI1 data were scrubbed using RStudio (Version 1.2.5001; R version 3.6.1). Raw data files for all blood- and CSF-based biomarkers were checked for imputed values. To ensure data integrity, all analytes with >10% imputed values were removed. Participants who had missing data or invalid data as indicated by the ADNI manual were excluded.

The PLS Command line package (Version 6, 2013) was downloaded from the open-source PLS User Guide: http://pls.rotman-baycrest.on.ca/source/ and run in MATLAB (Version 2019b). A cross-covariance matrix between demographic (age and education), physical, health, and AD data (matrix 1) and cognitive data (matrix 2) was created and factorized using singular value decomposition into mutually orthogonal singular vectors (Abdi & Williams, 2013). The PLSC algorithm uses these singular vectors to create latent variables that express the largest amount of information common to both input matrices (Krishnan, Williams, McIntosh, & Abdi, 2011). Thus, these latent variables refer to the pattern of covariance between physical, health, and AD variables (matrix 1) and cognitive function (matrix 2). The PLSC command line outputs as many latent variables as there are behavioral variables (19), the sum of which total to 100% of the cross-block covariance.

The p-value for all 19 latent variables was determined using permutation analyses. Permutation samples are created using our input dataset. Matrix 2 variables are randomly shuffled within participants, while matrix 1 variables remain intact. The PLSC model is re-run on each of these permutation samples, creating a distribution that can be used to determine a p-value for each latent variable (Krishnan et al., 2011). Latent variables were determined as statistically significant if the latent variable had a p-value of < .05 after 1500 permutations of the data (Abdi & Williams, 2013). Reliability of a latent variable was assessed through split-half resampling, a procedure that determines the reliability of the associations described between the two matrices of data (physical, health, AD, and cognitive) within a given latent variable (Kovacevic, Abdi, Beaton, & McIntosh, 2013). To compute this, the full study sample was randomly split into half, and each half was independently analyzed. Latent variables were considered reliable if both sides of the data met criteria for significance (p < .05; Kovacevic et al., 2013). Physical, health, and AD variables with a bootstrap ratio (BSR) with an absolute value greater than or equal to 1.96 (corresponding to p < .05), determined by 1000 resamplings with replacement of the data, were considered reliable contributors to the latent variables. Cognitive measures were considered to significantly contribute to the latent variable if their correlation with the latent variable was significantly different than zero (p < .05). Using these cutoffs, patterns of physical, health, AD variables, and cognitive scores that account for significant amounts of covariance in the data were determined (see Table 1 for variables included in the analysis).

RESULTS

Three significant latent variables (all values p < .01) were identified. Latent variable 1 (LV1; Figure 1) accounted for 45.2% of the cross-block covariance. LV1 was considered reliable, as it met criteria for split-half reliability (all values p < .05; Kovacevic et al., 2013). For LV1, neurotrophic/growth factors, AD biomarkers, a stress biomarker, and education were significantly associated with performance across multiple cognitive domains. Specifically, HB-EGF-like-GF, PDGF, BDNF, plasma tau, CSF t-tau, CSF p-tau181, CSF Aß1–42, cortisol, and education were significantly associated with performance on measures of episodic memory (WMS-R Logical Memory immediate and delayed recall, RAVLT List 1 and List B), processing speed (Trail A and Digit Symbol Substitution Test), executive function (Trail B), visuospatial ability (Clock Drawing and Copy), verbal fluency (Category Fluency Animals, Category Fluency Vegetables), language (Boston Naming Test), and global cognition (MMSE, ADAS-COG). All significant cognitive tests had statistically equal contributions to LV1 as all had error bars (representing 95% confidence intervals) overlapping with one another (Figure 1). Better performance on this subset of cognitive measures was associated with increased neurotrophic/growth factor levels, less AD pathology, lower levels of stress, and higher education (for individual BSRs, see Figure 1). Of these variables, CSF AD biomarkers and education had the highest BSRs, revealing that these variables had the strongest associations with cognition, followed by growth factors such as HB-EGF-like-GF, PDGF, and BDNF, as well as a stress biomarker, cortisol. To further visualize LV1,we plotted the relationship between the physical/health/AD scores for LV1 (representing how well an individual’s physical/health/AD variables contribute to the LV1 pattern) and two cognitive tests (raw scores on Digit Symbol Substitution (Figure 2A) and WMS-R Logical Memory delayed recall (Figure 2B)).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Correlation profile and bootstrap ratios for Latent variable 1. (A) The correlation between each cognitive test variable to the identified physical, health, and AD variables listed in panel B. Significant variables have 95% confidence intervals (error bars) that do not cross the x-axis (0). (B) Each Physical, Health, and AD Variable’s contribution to LV1 represented by their bootstrap ratios, indicating directionality with significant cognitive tests represented in A (for instance, performance on logical memory immediate was positively correlated with levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor, and had a negative correlation with CSF tau). Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Variables with bootstrap ratios > |1.96| (equivalent to a p-value of < .05) are considered significant contributors to the LV and are indicated by *. (KEY PANEL A: ADAS-COG = Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale, Cognitive Subsection; ANART = American National Adult Reading Test; MMSE = Mini Mental State Examination; RAVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; KEY PANEL B: APOE = apolipoprotein E; BDNF = brain-derived neurotrophic factor; BMI = body mass index; CSF = cerebrospinal fluid; HB-EGF-like-GF = heparin-binding epidermal growth factor-like growth factor; IGF = insulin-like growth factor; p-tau = phospho-tau 181; VEGF = vascular endothelial growth factor).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

For display purposes, scatterplots of each participant’s Digit Symbol Substitution raw score (Panel A) and WMS-R Logical Memory Delayed Recall raw score (Panel B) are plotted against their individual physical/health/AD score (representing how well an individual’s physical/health/AD variables contribute to the overall pattern in LV1).

LV2 and LV3 were also significant, accounting for 17.2% and 11.9% of the cross-block covariance, respectively. For LV2, IL-6 receptor, neurotrophic/growth factors, and AD biomarkers were significantly associated with performance in measures of delayed episodic memory. Specifically, lower IL-6 receptor, BDNF, PDGF, CSF t-tau, and CSF p-tau181, and higher hepatocyte growth factor and CSF Aß1–42 were associated with better performance on WMS-R Logical Memory delayed recall and RAVLT 30-minute delayed recall (see Supplemental Figure 1 for details). However, LV2 did not meet criteria for split-half reliability (matrix 2 had a value of p > .05) and thus should be interpreted with caution. For LV3, lower levels of cardiometabolic variables and higher insulin-like growth factor binding protein, age, and education were associated with better performance in executive function (longest digit span backward length), basic attention (longest digit span forward length), premorbid IQ (ANART), and worse performance in episodic memory (RAVLT List B; see Supplemental Figure 2 for details). LV3 met criteria for split-half reliability (all values p < .05). However, the three significant cognitive outcomes in LV3 have correlations with 95% confidence intervals close to crossing the x-axis (0), and LV3 accounts for a relatively low percentage of the overall cross-block covariance; thus, the relationships reported within LV3 should be interpreted with abundant caution (see Supplemental Figure 2 for details).

DISCUSSION

To summarize, we identified a latent variable (LV1) that accounted for a large amount of cross-block covariance, revealing a pattern in the data suggesting that increased neurotrophic/growth factor levels, less AD pathology, lower stress, and higher education are associated with better performance largely on tasks associated with episodic memory, executive function, processing speed, language as well as metrics of global cognition (Figure 1). Basic attention (e.g., longest sequence recalled for digit span forward) and premorbid IQ were not significantly associated with this pattern. This pattern suggests that markers of neuroprotection, neuroplasticity, stress, and AD pathology may hold relatively less importance for cognitive metrics that do not require mental manipulation. Interestingly, modifiable cardiometabolic risk factors (such as BMI, cholesterol, etc.), which are often associated with cognition in older adults (Farooqui et al., 2012; Yaffe et al., 2004), did not contribute to the pattern described. Chronological age also did not significantly contribute to the pattern described for LV1. This discussion mainly focuses on LV1, which accounted for the most cross-block covariance and met criteria for split-half reliability.

Our results extend the literature by showing a novel association between HB-EGF-like-GF and cognition. Specifically, better cognitive performance was associated with higher levels of plasma HB-EGF-like-GF in LV1. Previous research has shown that this growth factor may have neuroprotective properties, as infusions of HB-EGF-like-GF in rats 1-day post-stroke were associated with neuroprotection against cell death (Shim & Madsen, 2018). Moreover, in rats with cypermethrin exposure (a pesticide associated with AD neuropathology), exogenous administration of HB-EGF-like-GF inhibited cypermethrin-induced accumulation of Aß1–42 and p-tau in the frontal cortex and hippocampus and led to decreases in learning and memory deficits caused by cypermethrin exposure (Maurya, Mishra, Abbas, & Bandyopadhyay, 2016). Our finding is consistent with animal models demonstrating HB-EGF-like-GF’s potential neuroprotective role (Maurya et al., 2016; Shim & Madsen, 2018). To our knowledge, a link between HB-EGF-like-GF and human cognition has not been reported. However, in one study exploring older adults who were cognitively normal or had MCI, higher levels of CSF HB-EGF-like-GF were associated with decreased levels of CSF Aß1–42 and increased levels of CSF t-tau, which is contrary to our finding (P. F. Meyer et al., 2018), as our results suggest that HB-EGF-like-GF may be associated with potential cognitive benefits. This unique finding necessitates additional research in order to clarify the role of HB-EGF-like-GF in human cognition.

Our LV1 findings for two other neurotrophic/growth factors, BDNF and PDGF, support previous research showing positive associations between these two neurotrophic/growth factors and cognition. Higher levels of BDNF were associated with better cognitive performance (on WMS-R Logical Memory immediate and delayed recall, RAVLT List 1 and List B, Trail A and Digit Symbol Substitution Test, Trail B, Clock Drawing, Category Fluency Animals and Vegetables, Boston Naming Test, MMSE, and ADAS-COG), consistent with the putative role of BDNF in neuroprotection (Lista & Sorrentino, 2010; Miranda et al., 2019). For LV1, higher PDGF was also related to better cognitive performance, which is consistent with studies showing higher levels of PDGF were associated with reduced cognitive decline (Taipa et al., 2019). However, it should be noted that the relationship between PDGF and cognition was not entirely consistent. For LV2, lower levels of PDGF were associated with better performance on WMS-R Logical Memory delayed recall and RAVLT delayed recall (although LV2 should be interpreted with caution).

AD biomarkers exhibited some of the strongest associations with cognition in LV1. Lower levels of plasma tau were associated with higher cognitive performance, supporting previous research demonstrating that plasma tau levels in those with MCI were negatively associated with episodic memory and verbal fluency performance (Chiu et al., 2014). Lower levels of CSF t-tau and CSF p-tau181 were associated with better cognitive performance, consistent with recent work (Nathan et al., 2017). Lower levels of CSF Aß1–42 were correlated with lower cognitive scores, a pattern similar to recent findings revealing that low CSF Aß1–42 levels were associated with cognitive impairment in participants with MCI (Matura et al., 2019). Increased levels of CSF t-tau and CSF p-tau181 and decreased levels of CSF Aß1–42 in those with MCI are all associated with increased risk of converting to dementia (Diniz et al., 2008). These associations between AD biomarkers and cognition contribute to our understanding of cognitive decline in MCI, demonstrating that AD biomarkers are associated with a broad range of cognitive domains (see Figure 1).

Our results also demonstrate that cortisol, a marker of stress or hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal–axis activity, had negative associations with cognition. Lower levels of cortisol were associated with higher cognitive performance, which is consistent with recent research demonstrating that cognitively normal older adults with elevated cortisol and CSF Aß1–42 were at a higher risk of clinical progression to MCI or AD (Udeh-Momoh et al., 2020). Interestingly, this relationship remained even when controlling for cognitive reserve (Udeh-Momoh et al., 2020). Our results align with this finding and contribute to the literature by demonstrating that cortisol was significantly associated with positive cognitive outcomes in an MCI group.

Regarding demographic variables, higher education was associated with superior cognitive performance in LV1, consistent with the well-documented role of education as a source of cognitive reserve (Stern, 2013). Surprisingly, chronological age did not significantly contribute to the pattern of covariance in LV1. This null finding was unexpected as multiple episodic memory and executive function tasks significantly contributed to LV1, and these cognitive domains typically decline with age (Buckner, 2004; Tromp et al., 2015). However, cognitive aging studies often do not include any array of physiological and health metrics. These data suggest neurotrophic/growth factors, AD biomarkers, a marker of stress, and education may better predict performance on tasks of episodic memory, executive function, processing speed, visuospatial ability, verbal fluency, language, and global cognition than chronological age.

The current study had limitations. Some variables such as Insulin-like Growth Factor 1 and Interleukin-6 did not pass ADNI’s internal quality control processes, precluding inclusion in our analysis. Other variables, such as sex or APOE ε4 status, were not included in the analysis because it is generally not recommended to include binarized variables (sex) or those with limited variability (APOE ε4 genotype: only 3 possible values: 0, 1, or 2 alleles) in a PLS analysis (a variable with low variance is problematic for the calculation of correlation coefficients and the unequal distribution of these measures across the sample can lead to instability in the BSR estimates, which could lead to misinterpretation of the data, making the findings less generalizable).

Overall, these findings emphasize that markers of neuroprotection, neuroplasticity, stress, and AD significantly contribute to episodic memory, executive function, and processing speed in older adults with MCI. Our results suggest that modifiable variables, such as BDNF and cortisol, which research has shown can be changed with physical exercise, may serve as potential targets for future interventions to slow cognitive impairment and progression to dementia (Lista & Sorrentino, 2010; Baker et al., 2011).

Supplementary Material

Stark_JINS_SupplementaryMaterial

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We would like to thank Jena Moody for her contributions including sharing her knowledge of the ADNI database, as well as Randy McIntosh, for sharing his valuable insight and expertise in PLS methods.

FUNDING

This research was supported by the National Institute on Aging (NIA) of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) R21AG056921 (awarded to SMH), NIA R01AG068882 (awarded to SMH), NIA R01AG058822 (awarded to JPH); and The Ohio State University Discovery Themes Chronic Brain Injury Initiative (SMH and JPH).

Data collection and sharing for this project was funded by the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) (National Institutes of Health Grant U01 AG024904) and DOD ADNI (Department of Defense award number W81XWH-12-2-0012). ADNI is funded by the National Institute on Aging, the National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering, and through generous contributions from the following: AbbVie, Alzheimer’s Association; Alzheimer’s Drug Discovery Foundation; Araclon Biotech; BioClinica, Inc.; Biogen; Bristol-Myers Squibb Company; CereSpir, Inc.; Cogstate; Eisai Inc.; Elan Pharmaceuticals, Inc.; Eli Lilly and Company; EuroImmun; F. Hoffmann-La Roche Ltd and its affiliated company Genentech, Inc.; Fujirebio; GE Healthcare; IXICO Ltd.; Janssen Alzheimer Immunotherapy Research & Development, LLC.; Johnson & Johnson Pharmaceutical Research & Development LLC.; Lumosity; Lundbeck; Merck & Co., Inc.; Meso Scale Diagnostics, LLC.; NeuroRx Research; Neurotrack Technologies; Novartis Pharmaceuticals Corporation; Pfizer Inc.; Piramal Imaging; Servier; Takeda Pharmaceutical Company; and Transition Therapeutics. The Canadian Institutes of Health Research is providing funds to support ADNI clinical sites in Canada. Private sector contributions are facilitated by the Foundation for the National Institutes of Health (www.fnih.org). The grantee organization is the Northern California Institute for Research and Education, and the study is coordinated by the Alzheimer’s Therapeutic Research Institute at the University of Southern California. ADNI data are disseminated by the Laboratory for Neuro Imaging at the University of Southern California.

Footnotes

*

Data used in preparation of this article were obtained from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) database (adni.loni.usc.edu). As such, the investigators within the ADNI contributed to the design and implementation of ADNI and/or provided data but did not participate in analysis or writing of this report. A complete listing of ADNI investigators can be found at: http://adni.loni.usc.edu/wp-content/uploads/how_to_apply/ADNI_Acknowledgement_List.pdf

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617721001041

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors have nothing to disclose.

REFERENCES

  1. Abdi H & Williams LJ (2013). Partial least squares methods: partial least squares correlation and partial least square regression. In Reisfeld B & Mayeno AN (Eds.), Methods in molecular biology (Vol. 930, pp. 549–570). Clifton, NJ: Humana Press. doi: 10.1007/978-1-62703-059-5_21 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baker LD, Frank LL, Foster-schubert K, Pattie S, Wilkinson CW, Mctiernan A, … Craft S (2011). Effects of Aerobic exercise on mild cognitive impairment. 67, 71–79. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.307.Effects [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Buckner RL (2004). Memory and executive function in aging and ad: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron, 44, 195–208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Chiu MJ, Chen YF, Chen TF, Yang SY, Yang FPG, Tseng TW, … Horng HE (2014). Plasma Tau as a window to the brain — negative associations with brain volume and memory function in mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer’s disease. Human Brain Mapping, 35, 3132–3142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Diniz BSO, Pinto JA, & Forlenza OV (2008). Do CSF total tau, phosphorylated tau, and b-amyloid 42 help to predict progression of mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease? A systematic review and meta-analysis of the literature. World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 9, 172–182. doi: 10.1080/15622970701535502 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Farooqui AA, Farooqui T, Panza F, & Frisardi V (2012). Metabolic syndrome as a risk factor for neurological disorders. Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences, 69, 741–762. doi: 10.1007/s00018-011-0840-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Kovacevic N, Abdi H, Beaton D, & McIntosh AR (2013). Revisiting PLS resampling: comparing significance versus reliability across range of simulations. In Abdi Herve, Chin WW, Esposito Vinzi V, Russolillo G, & Trinchera L (Eds.), New perspectives in partial least squares and related methods (pp. 159–170). New York: Springer New York. [Google Scholar]
  8. Krishnan A, Williams LJ, McIntosh AR, & Abdi H (2011). Partial Least Squares (PLS) methods for neuroimaging: a tutorial and review. NeuroImage, 56, 455–475. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Lista I & Sorrentino G (2010). Biological mechanisms of physical activity in preventing cognitive decline. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology, 30, 493–503. doi: 10.1007/s10571-009-9488-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Matura S, Köhler J, Reif A, Fusser F, Karakaya T, Scheibe M, … Pantel J (2019). Intrinsic functional connectivity, CSF biomarker profiles and their relation to cognitive function in mild cognitive impairment. Acta Neuropsychiatrica. doi: 10.1017/neu.2019.49 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Maurya SK, Mishra J, Abbas S, & Bandyopadhyay S (2016). Cypermethrin stimulates GSK3β-dependent Aβ and p-tau proteins and cognitive loss in young rats: reduced HB-EGF signaling and downstream neuroinflammation as critical regulators. Molecular Neurobiology, 53, 968–982. doi: 10.1007/s12035-014-9061-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Meyer PF, Savard M, Poirier J, Labonté A, Rosa-Neto P, Weitz TM, … Breitner J (2018). Bi-directional association of Cerebrospinal Fluid Immune markers with stage of Alzheimer’s disease pathogenesis. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 63, 577–590. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170887 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Meyer PF, Savard M, Poirier J, Morgan D, & Breitner J (2019). Hypothesis: cerebrospinal fluid protein markers suggest a pathway toward symptomatic resilience to AD pathology. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 15, 1160–1171. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2019.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Miranda M, Morici JF, Zanoni MB, & Bekinschtein P (2019). Brain-derived Neurotrophic factor: a key molecule for memory in the healthy and the pathological brain. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience, 13, 1–25. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2019.00363 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Nathan PJ, Lim YY, Abbott R, Galluzzi S, Marizzoni M, Babiloni C, … Frisoni GB (2017). Association between CSF biomarkers, hippocampal volume and cognitive function in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Neurobiology of Aging, 53, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.01.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Salthouse TA (2010). Selective review of cognitive aging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 754–760. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710000706 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Shim JW & Madsen JR (2018). VEGF signaling in neurological disorders. International Journal of Molecular Sciences. doi: 10.3390/ijms19010275 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Stern Y (2013). Cognitive reserve in ageing. Lancet Neurology, 11(11), 1006–1012. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6.Cognitive [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Taipa R, das Neves SP, Sousa AL, Fernandes J, Pinto C, Correia AP, … Sousa N (2019). Proinflammatory and anti-inflammatory cytokines in the CSF of patients with Alzheimer’s disease and their correlation with cognitive decline. Neurobiology of Aging, 76, 125–132. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.12.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Tromp D, Dufour A, Lithfous S, Pebayle T, & Després O (2015). Episodic memory in normal aging and Alzheimer disease: Insights from imaging and behavioral studies. Ageing Research Reviews, 24, 232–262. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2015.08.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Udeh-Momoh CT, Su B, Evans S, Zheng B, Sindi S, Tzoulaki I, … Middleton L (2020). Cortisol, Amyloid-Beta, and reserve predicts Alzheimer’s disease progression for cognitively normal older adults. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 1–10. doi: 10.3233/JAD-181030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Van Roon P, Zakizadeh J, & Chartier S (2014). Partial Least Squares tutorial for analyzing neuroimaging data. The Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 10, 200–215. doi: 10.20982/tqmp.10.2.p200 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Yaffe K, Kanaya A, Lindquist K, Simonsick EM, Harris T, Shorr RI, … Newman AB (2004). The metabolic syndrome, inflammation, and risk of cognitive decline. Journal of the American Medical Association, 292, 2237–2242. doi: 10.1001/jama.292.18.2237 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Stark_JINS_SupplementaryMaterial

RESOURCES