Skip to main content
Sage Choice logoLink to Sage Choice
. 2021 Nov 16;42(6):755–764. doi: 10.1177/0272989X211058068

Shared Decision Making in Health Care: Theoretical Perspectives for Why It Works and For Whom

Ken Resnicow 1,2,, Delwyn Catley 3,4, Kathy Goggin 5,6, Sarah Hawley 7,8, Geoffrey C Williams 9,10
PMCID: PMC9108118  NIHMSID: NIHMS1750633  PMID: 34784805

Abstract

Applying both theoretical perspectives and empirical evidence, we address 2 key questions regarding shared decision making (SDM): 1) When should SDM be more patient driven, and when should it be more provider driven? and 2) Should health care providers match their SDM style/strategy to patient needs and preferences? Self-determination theory, for example, posits a distinction between autonomy and independence. A patient may autonomously seek their health care provider’s input and guidance, perhaps due to low perceived competence, low coping resources, or high emotional arousal. Given their need state, they may autonomously require nonindependence. In this case, it may be more patient centered and need supportive to provide more provider-driven care. We discuss how other patient characteristics such as personality attributes, motivational state, and the course of illness and other parameters such as time available for an encounter may inform optimal provider decision-making style and strategy. We conclude that for some types of patients and clinical circumstances, a more provider-driven approach to decision making may be more practical, ethical, and efficacious. Thus, while all decision making should be patient centered (i.e., it should consider patient needs and preferences), it does not always have to be patient driven. We propose a flexible model of SDM whereby practitioners are encouraged to tailor their decision making behaviors to patient needs, preferences, and other attributes. Studies are needed to test whether matching decision-making behavior based on patient states and traits (i.e., achieving concordance) is more effective than simply providing all patients with the same type of decision making, which could be tested using matching/mismatching designs.

Keywords: counseling, cultural differences, motivational interviewing, smoking cessation

Introduction

Shared decision making (SDM) has become an almost universally accepted component of our health care delivery system. 1 Its endorsement and adoption across the health care system in the United States and globally is driven by both ethical considerations and empirical evidence.2,3 Yet, despite its widespread acceptance, some key questions regarding SDM remain. This article will focus on 2 related questions that have significant implications for theory, practice, and research:

  1. When should SDM be more patient driven, and when should it be more provider driven?

  2. Should health care providers match decision-making style and strategy to patient needs, preferences, and traits?

To answer these 2 questions, we bring in theoretical perspectives of patient-centeredness as well as relevant empirical evidence regarding individual preferences for and response to different provider communication styles. We then address circumstances in which a more provider-driven approach may be more practical, ethical, and efficacious. Integrating these theoretical and empirical observations, we propose a flexible model of SDM, whereby practitioners can tailor their DM communication styles to patient needs, preferences, and other attributes. We conclude with some future research priorities.

We begin by defining SDM. Although there is no universally accepted definition of SDM,35 most models of SDM consider it a collaborative process by which both the patient and health care provider contribute to medical decisions, with patients being full partners in decisions about their care. Virtually all models of SDM recommend that health care providers (at times aided by decision-making tools) explain the pros and cons and costs and benefits of treatment options and help patients choose the treatment option that best aligns with their preferences, values, beliefs, emotional state, and perceived capabilities, all of which should be elicited by the health care provider. The provider checks in periodically with the patient to evaluate what additional information, time, or input they may require, and the health care provider is transparent about any biases about a particular treatment or test they may possess, and they do not judge the patient’s decision. This is in contrast to purely physician-driven (also termed paternalistic) decision making (DM), in which the health care provider, operating from a position of authority and greater technical expertise, makes treatment recommendations and generally directs the decision-making process, with the patient playing a more passive role. In practice, encounters are rarely fully patient or provider driven and comprise a mixture of both styles with varying degrees of patient- and provider-driven interaction. The SDM process comprises several subelements, including agenda setting, eliciting patient preferences, tailoring information, encouraging patient deliberation, and delineating choices.57

Less clear is the boundary between SDM and behavior change counseling. SDM includes preference-sensitive decisions for which there is no clear “correct” clinical option, such as choosing between a stool test versus colonoscopy for colon cancer screening, whether or not to undergo PSA testing for prostate cancer screening, or having a lumpectomy versus mastectomy for early-stage breast cancer. In each case, there is more than 1 evidence-based acceptable option, and the practitioner adopts a position of equipoise. On the other hand, it is less clear whether SDM encompasses persuasive clinical encounters where there is a clear benefit for one option, such as quitting smoking, losing weight, exercising, or undergoing cancer screening (all relative to not engaging in the evidence-based behavior). Persuasive encounters necessarily require a different approach and skill set than equipoise encounters do. For example, the techniques used to persuade, such as motivational interviewing (MI), can be quite different—and more consistent with counseling—than the skills used to support patients deciding between 2 equally beneficial options (i.e., making preference-sensitive decisions). Yet, SDM has been called for in both contexts. Thus, for purposes of this article, we have assumed a broad definition of SDM that includes both persuasive and preference-sensitive encounters.

There is no single unifying theory to explicate the mechanisms of SDM. 8 Here, we will incorporate 2 models that are particularly relevant to understanding the concepts of patient-centeredness and patient-driven DM: 1) self-determination theory (SDT) and 2) the difficulty × motivation matrix. We focus on these 2 models as they specifically address patient autonomy and patient motivation as key mechanisms of the behavior-change process and because both models provide insights regarding the distinction between patient-driven and provider-driven DM.

SDT, originally proposed by Deci and Ryan in the realm of organizational psychology, conceptualizes a continuum of motivational regulation that ranges from controlled to autonomous.913 Controlled motivation can result from external pressure (e.g., health care provider) or from the person’s internal sense of internalized guilt or shame. In contrast, autonomous motivation is seen as fully volitional and concordant with patient needs, preferences, and values. In general, change that occurs through autonomous regulation is stronger than change through more controlled processes. 14 SDT proposes 3 fundamental human needs that are relevant to SDM: competence, relatedness, and autonomy. When these needs are met, SDT posits, people experience higher-quality motivation, which leads to more successful behavior change, greater psychological well-being, and higher postdecision satisfaction. 15 From an SDT perspective, behavior change that arises from an autonomous process that is volitional, aligned with patient values, and consistent with their perceived competence will be more enduring and more positively appraised than behaviors that arise from more controlled regulation. In this way, SDM can be seen as supporting these 3 basic human needs and operating thereby through autonomous processes. 16 On the other hand, controlled motivation does not address these 3 key needs and will lead to worse outcomes both in terms of the choice itself and the appraisal thereof. Thus, on the surface, a patient-driven process of DM would seem to align with the autonomous pathway proposed by SDT, whereas provider-driven DM would seemingly align with controlled motivation. However, a deeper dive into SDT suggests a more nuanced interpretation.

Understanding SDM through the lens of SDT requires delineating a distinction between autonomy and independence. 16 Autonomy is the SDT North Star. It is central to all encounters. Independence refers to how much the patient acted on their own behalf without input from others. In SDT, the opposite of independence is dependence. Dependence occurs when a patient relies on the advice and help of their provider to resolve their ambivalence and determine a course of treatment. Dependence can be seen as analogous to provider-driven DM. A smoker who decides to quit by himself or herself functions independently, whereas the smoker who wants advice from their physician to quit displays dependent functioning. However, both the self-quitter as well as the physician-dependent quitter can quit from an autonomous position. A patient may autonomously decide to seek their health care provider’s input; that is, they may decide autonomously to be nonindependent.

Consider a cancer patient who may prefer to rely on their practitioner to decide if they should obtain germline genetic testing or whether they should opt for a lumpectomy versus mastectomy to treat their early-stage breast cancer. They may relinquish their independence for many reasons, including feeling overwhelmed by their condition and other coping demands of their cancer. But they are doing so autonomously.

Consider also a patient with low perceived competence to make a health care decision. In this case, the patient may prefer and may respond positively to a more provider-driven style. Having the provider drive the encounter may bolster the patient’s competence, which in turn may shift the patient toward a more a patient-driven nexus. In each of these cases, because the patient has requested provider direction, the interaction is still patient centered and need supportive. The patient is acting autonomously, and it can be patient centered to provide more provider-driven DM. The key is to match the style of DM to patient needs and preferences for autonomy. It is important to note that according to SDT, although a provider-driven approach may be appropriate to promote behavior change for some patients, there are some techniques—some pathways to change—such as shame and guilt that are never warranted, as they are viewed as inherently unethical and/or ineffective.

A second theoretical perspective that may be useful in addressing our dialectic is the Difficulty × Motivation matrix proposed by Resnicow et al. 17 In this framework, which was informed by SDT, the choice of intervention approach for promotion of behavior change is based on 2 orthogonal dimensions: the individual’s perceived difficulty for making the behavior change (defined by how much energy they perceive is required to make the change) and their motivation to change (e.g., controlled v. autonomous).

A key component of this framework is that the type of behavior change influences the degree of patient autonomy needed. Modifying less complex or less difficult behaviors such as seat belt use or most screening tests may need different types of interventions than more complex, high-energy behavior changes such as substance misuse, obesity, or chronic disease management. For example, more external policy or “nudge” interventions such as financial incentives and regulatory mandates may be appropriate for simpler behavior changes. For more complex behavior changes, patient-driven interventions may be more important than for simpler behaviors. Thus, patient-driven DM may be more important for more complex changes such as smoking cessation (which assumes that we define SDM more broadly to include persuasive encounters) than simpler changes such as seat belt use or preference sensitive decisions.

In the parlance of SDT, working through autonomous motivation may be more important for more complex changes and health care decisions. For simpler behavior changes and medical decisions, a more external, and provider-driven approach may be warranted. This begins to formulate a framework for when patient-driven DM may be more desired.

The difficulty × motivation framework also predicts that when patients facing a medical decision feel overwhelmed or have little energy to devote to the DM process, perhaps because they just learned of their diagnosis, their condition has worsened, or other life demands have depleted their coping resources, they may be more likely to rely on their provider to guide the DM process. According to the difficulty × motivation framework, when coping resources and adaptive energy are low or patient anxiety or fear may be high, patients may be less willing and able to be actively involved in the DM process. 17 They may not be able to invest time and energy into researching their options, integrating complex information, and driving the decision, and thus in these situations they, may prefer a more directive approach.

Above, we provided some theoretical perspectives that suggest there may be benefit in matching the DM style to patient preferences and that, under certain circumstances, individuals will have better clinical and/or psychosocial outcomes from more provider-driven DM. Yet these frameworks do not elucidate all the factors that may lead a patient to prefer a certain type of DM encounter/style. Just like with coping resources and adaptive energy levels, there are other individual characteristics that may affect the DM approach that individuals prefer. For example, race/ethnicity has been associated, in both survey and intervention research, with different DM style preferences. Levinson et al., 18 in a representative national sample of 2765 US adults, queried preferences regarding 3 components of the DM process: 1) seeking information, 2) discussing options, and 3) making the final decision.

They found that self-identified Hispanic respondents were more likely than Black and White respondents to prefer physician-directed communication for seeking information, and both Hispanic and Black respondents were more likely than white respondents to prefer leaving final decisions about medical care up to their providers; that is, they reported a weaker preference for shared decision making. There were no ethnic differences regarding the discussing options variable. Overall, 91% of the sample at least moderately agreed with the options statement. Thus, racial and ethnic differences in preference for aspects of SDM may be limited to subcomponents of the process itself. It is also important to note that although some groups reported weaker preferences for elements of SDM, it is not clear from the multivariate results whether these groups had a stronger preference for provider-driven decision making or just a weaker preference for patient-driven decision making. The authors also found that women were more likely than men to prefer a patient-directed approach across all race/ethnic groups and all 3 dimensions. While educational attainment was not associated with the options preference, preferences for shared information seeking and final decision making increased as educational attainment increased.

Another study reported racial differences in preference for SDM 19 similar to Levinson et al. The authors surveyed 3177 adults and found that, overall, 62% of respondents preferred SDM, 28% preferred patient centered DM, and 9% preferred provider-centered DM. Black respondents were more likely to prefer provider-centered DM than White respondents in both univariate and adjusted analyses. Lower income was also associated with a stronger preference for provider-centered DM. Black respondents were significantly more likely than White respondents to report experiencing provider-centered care, which is consistent with several prior studies.2023

The preference for a more directive, provider-driven communication style among some Black Americans might be the result of negative personal interactions with the health care system, society at large, and historical and persistent structural racism. Weaker preferences for patient-driven DM among Black Americans may have its roots in historical and persistent disenfranchisement and maltreatment by the health care system. 24 Preferences for provider communication made be a reaction to receiving low autonomy support care. Given poor interactions with the health care system, some Black Americans may question the willingness and capability of providers to exhibit “patient-centeredness,” which may lead to doubt about how well their needs will be will elicited and respected. It may also in part reflect their own personal negative experiences with the system when they might have received poor care or their needs were not met. 25 Thus, it may be the case that Black Americans’ preference for or expectation for provider-driven DM is driven by the fact that they do not expect patient-centeredness from their providers because they have been less likely to receive it24,26 and have become accustomed to if not resigned to receiving provider-driven, low autonomy support health care. Given this, Black Americans may have lower efficacy to communicate their preference to providers, and they may have lower trust that their preferences and needs will be taken into account. 26 Together, this could engender a preference for provider-driven interactions, which perhaps more accurately should be characterized as disbelief that providers can treat Black patients with true patient-centeredness. This preference for provider-driven DM may be different if they have a provider who is of the same racial background or if they possess high trust in their provider. It is also possible that the preference for more provider-driven communication is an informed choice rather than a reaction to suboptimal provider care. More research is needed to understand the origins of these cultural differences and how best to respond to them in clinical practice.

In contrast to the 2 studies noted above, in one survey conducted among 974 patients with diabetes from 34 community health centers in 17 Midwestern and West-Central states, race was not associated with patient preference for SDM. 7 Moreover, in that study, Black Americans were actually more likely to report initiating discussions about their diabetes care with their providers. While the Levinson et al. and Murray et al, studies surveyed the general adult population, the Peek et al. study surveyed only adults with diabetes, which makes comparison between these studies somewhat difficult. 7

Although individuals may prefer different DM styles, it is important to determine whether such preferences are related to differential intervention response. It may be the case that, independent of stated preferences, individuals may still derive benefit from greater involvement in the DM process.1,8,27 In other words, independent of preference, some degree of patient-driven decision making may still be beneficial.

We could find no studies that directly assessed whether individual states or traits are associated with differential response to patient-driven or provider-driven DM. Evidence from a recent secondary analysis that explored individual response to 2 styles of smoking cessation counseling, however, can also inform this inquiry. 28 One style was based on a more directive approach, and the other used MI. MI incorporates many of the core tenets of SDT and many of the principles and strategies of SDM.16,2932

For example, a key aspect of MI entails helping individuals to work through their ambivalence or resistance about behavior change. MI providers, using techniques such as reflective listening, establish a nonconfrontational and supportive climate in which patients feel comfortable expressing both the positive and negative aspects of their current behavior. Although the MI encounter is ultimately goal driven, the MI provider often begins the process by adopting a position of equipoise, 33 acknowledging the patient’s ambivalence and avoiding the injection of the counselor’s aspirations for the patient. This helps reduce patient reactance34,35 and builds rapport. Perhaps the primary distinction between SDM and MI is that in MI, the encounter is generally focused on whether the individual will choose to change a behavior that carries a clear-cut benefit, whereas in many SDM encounters, the focus is more on which treatment or test the patient chooses. Thus, MI, although a behavior-change technique rather than a DM technique, nonetheless incorporates many of the principles and strategies of SDM. Now we return to the smoking cessation study.

This randomized clinical trial compared MI and health education (HE) as contrasting methods to encourage smoking cessation among a sample of adult smokers who were dichotomized into Black and non-Black, with the vast majority of the latter being White. The HE intervention was intended to be a more directive, provider-driven contrast to the MI counseling, which can be seen as more analogous to patient-driven DM.

The authors compared postintervention quit attempts among 138 Black and 66 non-Black smokers all with low baseline desire to quit, who were randomly assigned to 4 sessions of MI or HE. 28 They found that MI was less effective than HE in Black smokers both in terms of quit attempts and the target mediator of autonomous motivation. Moreover, this race difference was largely explained by differences in baseline preference for patient- versus provider-driven communication style. Specifically, participants were asked at baseline to choose on a scale from 1 to 10, “When it comes to my smoking, I want an expert to tell me what to do.” Black smokers scored higher on this item, and when analyses included DM-style preference in the multivariate model, the race effects were substantially diminished, indicating that differences in communication style preferences largely accounted for the race-specific effects of MI and HE.

The weaker response to MI on the part of Black smokers is consistent with qualitative studies, which found that southern Black patients and providers expressed negative reactions to viewing video MI encounters.36,37 The authors report that Black participants found more traditional paternalistic approaches (i.e., physician-led decision conversations) were more representative of “good counseling” and more familiar to them. In addition, health care providers voiced concern about the limited input of the provider during the MI consultation. It should be noted that race effects for MI reported by Grobe et al. are inconsistent with 1 meta-analysis that found no moderating effect for patient race in studies when MI was used in medical settings. 38 The meta-analysis, however, examined only whether MI was superior to control or alternate interventions, whereas the Grobe et al. study further found that African American participants showed a stronger response to the more directive HE counseling than Whites did.

Individual differences in communication style preferences have also been shown to moderate the response to tailored messages. Two studies asked participants a similar question at baseline as the one used in Grobe et al. study, “When it comes to my xx [diet in one study and/or colon cancer screening in the other], I want an expert to tell me what to do.”39,40 We view this question as analogous to asking about preference for patient-driven versus provider-driven care. Both studies had 2 intervention conditions, one that provided tailored messaging based on principles of SDT (e.g., autonomy support) and strategies from MI (e.g., reflective listening, rolling with resistance, values alignment) and a control group that received untailored, traditional HE messages. The experimental arms in these 2 studies can be seen as analogous to comparing patient-driven communication versus provider-driven communication. One study focused on changing fruit and vegetable intake and the other on increasing screening for colorectal cancer. In both studies, the response to the experimental intervention was moderated by baseline patient-driven versus provider-driven preference variable. Specifically, individuals who preferred a more patient-driven approach (i.e., SDM) showed a stronger response to the tailored MI-type messages than those who preferred a more provider-driven style.

Finally, a cross-sectional study of 1690 women including localized breast cancer patients provides further support that some individuals benefit more from autonomy support care than others do. Women were asked about their perceptions of autonomy-supportive communication from their surgeons and medical oncologists, 15 their communication style preferences (assessed by 2 questions: “When it came to getting treatment for my breast cancer, I preferred to be told what to do” and “When it came to getting treatment for breast cancer, I wanted my doctor to tell me what to do”), and their self-reported decision quality (key components of decision satisfaction). 41 Overall patient-reported decision quality scores were positively associated with higher levels of perceived autonomy-supportive communication from surgeons and medical oncologists. This was true even among those who stated a preference for directive communication. However, patient communication style preference moderated the association between physician communication style received and perceived decision quality. Specifically, the difference in decisional quality based on provider communication style was significantly greater among those who preferred a nondirective style compared with those who preferred a directive style. 42 That is, those who wanted a more autonomy-supportive communication style benefitted more from receiving autonomy support from their provider than those who preferred a more directive style. This is consistent with Lantz et al., 43 who found that concordance between preferred DM style and actual DM style received was associated with higher patient decision satisfaction and lower regret. 43 Similarly, a review by Kiesler and Auerbach 8 found that the more the information received by patients matched their preferences, the better their adjustment to treatment, the less their subsequent emotional dysphoria, and the greater their adherence to their medical recommendations.

An important caveat is that with the exception of Grobe et al., these studies do not address whether more patient-driven DM may be harmful among some groups. This is important because if there is no harm in providing patient-driven DM even to those who do not ask for it, then tailoring or matching communication style may not be necessary. Everyone should simply receive patient-driven DM. The importance of the Grobe et al. study is that it provides a signal that universal implementation of patient-driven SDM may not be indicated, as a more directive style may be superior for some individuals. 44

SDT provides a framework to help guide provider tailoring of DM behavior. The guiding principle is that patients do better when their needs are satisfied, and under varying patient conditions, different provider behaviors and communication styles can fulfill patient needs. The key is that provider DM behaviors should support patient needs for autonomy and competence, which in some cases may lead to a more provider-driven conversation. Thus, as long as the patient has autonomously chosen to seek advice from their provider or has requested a more directive tone, by providing this direction, the provider ensures the encounter is still patient centered, even if not patient driven. Under these parameters, positive behavioral and psychologic outcomes could be expected.14,16 Similarly, if the patient needs a more directive style of DM, and the provider offers a more patient-driven (and therefore mismatched) style of DM, placing too much responsibility on the part of the patient, the patient might perceive this as controlling behavior, and worse outcomes could be expected. Alternatively, if the patient does not prefer or seek directive provider input, provider-driven DM could result in feeling controlled and lead to poor health behavior outcomes and worse psychological well-being.14,16 In sum, if a patient needs and/or requests a more directive DM process, it may be patient centered to provide it. A patient may autonomously decide to seek provider-driven DM, volitionally requesting provider input and direction.

So too the difficulty × motivation framework suggests a role of matching DM style to patient needs and preferences. For example, consider a patient who may have just received an ominous diagnosis; they know little about their newly diagnosed conditioned or their treatment options. In addition, the treatment options are complex with regard to side effects, efficacy, cost, recovery, and other risks. Under these circumstances, they may feel overwhelmed by both the need to learn about their condition or what treatment to choose. They might also dread any anticipatory regret about making a decision on their own. In these cases, the patient may be relieved to have an expert tell them what they should do and why. These dynamics may also be related to the patient’s stage of illness or treatment. For example, breast cancer patients are least likely to prefer SDM immediately after their initial surgery. 27 From an energy perspective, while recovering from surgery, the patient may temporarily have insufficient emotional resources (attention capacity) to devote to SDM and may therefore prefer to rely on their provider during that time. Similarly, depressed patients (depression is a low-energy state) may be less likely to engage in treatment DM. 45 Thus, under certain conditions, the most effective type of DM may be less shared than under other conditions because of available emotional resources or the motivational state of the patient. Yet, under these circumstances, less SDM may still be more patient centered and effective. For those wanting a more patient-driven decision, practitioners should focus on reducing the difficulty of the decision and supporting patient efficacy. Another factor that can determine DM style is how much time is available for the patient encounter. When less time is available, a more provider-driven approach may be more appropriate. 46 The goal is to match the provider communication and DM style to the patient’s needs and emotional state.

Although we have focused on applying 2 theoretical models to help explain and guide the use of different DM styles for different patients, using other theories to address these issues such as decision conflict theory is encouraged. 47 For example, from a decision conflict perspective, asking a patient to drive a decision when they are not prepared to do so, do not have the energy to do, do not prefer to do so, or do not have the skills to do so could result in patient overload, insufficient deliberation, and ultimately a poor-quality decision. Application of other frameworks and theories to the DM process, such as the transtheoretical model, 48 social cognitive theory, 49 and the patient activation model, 50 while beyond the scope of this article, is encouraged.

Conclusion and Future Directions

The discussion presented herein suggests rethinking how SDM is conceptualized and implemented. A more flexible approach to delivery of SDM may be indicated. Rather than assuming all patients under all circumstances require the same degree or kind of SDM, we propose, based on integration of both empirical evidence and theoretical perspectives, that provider DM style be tailored to patients’ preferences, disease state, personality, motivation, and psychological need satisfaction (i.e., autonomy, competence, and relatedness). This means that while all SDM should be patient centered, it does not always have to be patient driven. Under some circumstances, a more provider-driven approach may be more practical, ethical, and efficacious. We acknowledge that more research is needed to support the utility of this “precision SDM” approach as well as to develop methods to implement such matching in clinical practice.

Future Research

Studies are needed to test how best to match DM style to patient states, needs, and traits; preferences; and/or personality characteristics. Evidence for the utility of matching DM style is scant, and more controlled studies are needed to address this issue. 8 This could include matching/mismatching designs in which patients are stratified by a baseline characteristic that is thought to influence response to SDM, such as preference for expert-driven communication, and then they are assigned to either their presumptive matched and mismatched interventions. The hypothesis would be that matched interventions should outperform mismatched interventions. Within intervention or observational studies, we can measure factors such as patient autonomy needs, perceived competence, and psychological well-being to help identify the psychological mechanisms and mediators by which SDM interventions work, which would lead to more tailored delivery of DM interventions. Studies to elucidate the mechanisms through which DM style matching may operate are also encouraged.

Unpacking studies to identify active ingredients of SDM are also warranted. As noted above, SDM comprises numerous subcomponents (e.g., seeking permission, eliciting values, providing choices, reflecting emotion, supporting autonomy, etc.). Approaches such as MOST that use a fractional factorial design may be particularly useful to identify active ingredients of SDM.51,52 Main effects and lower-order interactions of subelements of SDM can be identified and tested in randomized fractional factorials studies. Experiments that test hypotheses about mechanisms are essential. This could lead to the development of brief measures that clinicians can use to determine which style to employ with a particular patient.

The efficacy of matching DM to patient characteristics in clinical practice requires that providers alter their DM style based on patient needs and preferences. 43 Whether providers are able to modulate their DM style and what type of training may be needed to enable them to do so merits study. Similarly, what patient characteristics should be used to determine which style of DM to employ and how to assess these characteristics needs to be determined. Practitioners could rely on overt patient preference (i.e., “I prefer my provider just tell me what to do”) or an assessment of the patient’s perceived difficulty or coping resources related to the decision at hand. How best to measure DM preference, including whether it is a generalized trait or a decision-specific state, and the stability of this preference merits further work. 53 Further, patient-driven and provider-driven DM are not binary options but represent a continuum of DM styles. This has implications for both measurement and implementation of SDM.

While individual characteristics, such as race/ethnicity, education, age, and gender may broadly be associated with differential preference for and response to DM style, making clinical decisions based solely on these characteristics is discouraged. Providers may project their own biases on patients, 24 and such group targeting fails to account for within-group heterogeneity and could lead to exacerbating health disparities and mistrust. It is important for providers to try and elicit individual needs and preferences on a per-patient basis.

Finally, research is needed to determine what states and traits may predispose a patient to respond to different styles of DM. For example, different levels of trust with the health care system, available support, and baseline knowledge may all affect DM preferences. 54 This line of research can also address potential individual-level differences in SDM preferences and intervention response, including the potential impact of patient-provider race/ethnicity concordance. It would be helpful to elucidate whether a preference for directiveness among some groups may be a response to historical and persistent non–patient-centered experiences in the health care system. In this case, reducing health disparities in how providers communicate with their patients may lead to increased trust with the health care system and therefore a shift in SDM preferences from more provider centered to more patient driven.

Footnotes

The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The authors received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding: The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This article was supported by NIH grants 1-U01-CA232827, 1-P30-CA046592-29, 1-R01-HL128231, and 1-R01-MD-016867-01 to the first author.

Contributor Information

Ken Resnicow, Department of Health Behavior & Health Education, University of Michigan School of Public Health, Ann Arbor, MI, USA; University of Michigan Rogel Cancer Center, Ann Arbor, MI, USA.

Delwyn Catley, Center for Children’s Healthy Lifestyles & Nutrition, Children’s Mercy Kansas City, Kansas City, MO, USA; School of Medicine, University of Missouri–Kansas City, Kansas City, MO, USA.

Kathy Goggin, School of Medicine, University of Missouri–Kansas City, Kansas City, MO, USA; Division of Health Services and Outcomes Research, Children’s Mercy Kansas City, Kansas City, MO, USA.

Sarah Hawley, Department of Medicine, University of Michigan Rogel Cancer Center, Ann Arbor, MI, USA; University of Michigan Rogel Cancer Center, Ann Arbor, MI, USA.

Geoffrey C. Williams, Collaborative Science and Innovations Billings Clinic, Billings Montana Emeritus Department of Medicine and Center for Community Health & Prevention, University of Rochester, Rochester, New York.

References

  • 1. Légaré F, Thompson-Leduc P. Twelve myths about shared decision making. Patient Educ Couns. 2014;96(3):281–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Nayfield SG, Bongiovanni GC, Alciati MH, Fischer RA, Bergner L. Statutory requirements for disclosure of breast cancer treatment alternatives. J Natl Cancer Inst. 1994;86(16):1202–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Frosch DL, Kaplan RM. Shared decision making in clinical medicine: past research and future directions. Am J Prev Med. 1999;17(4):285–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Makoul G, Clayman ML. An integrative model of shared decision making in medical encounters. Patient Educ Couns. 2006;60(3):301–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Braddock CH, III, Edwards KA, Hasenberg NM, Laidley TL, Levinson W. Informed decision making in outpatient practice: time to get back to basics. JAMA. 1999;282(24):2313–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Bomhof-Roordink H, Gärtner FR, Stiggelbout AM, Pieterse AH. Key components of shared decision making models: a systematic review. BMJ Open. 2019;9(12):e031763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Peek ME, Tang H, Cargill A, Chin MH. Are there racial differences in patients’ shared decision-making preferences and behaviors among patients with diabetes? Med Decis Making. 2011;31(3):422–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Kiesler DJ, Auerbach SM. Optimal matches of patient preferences for information, decision-making and interpersonal behavior: evidence, models and interventions. Patient Educ Couns. 2006;61(3):319–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Deci E, Ryan R. Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior. New York: Plenum; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Ryan RM, Kuhl J, Deci EL. Nature and autonomy: an organizational view of social and neurobiological aspects of self-regulation in behavior and development. Dev Psychopathol. 1997;9(4):701–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Ryan RM, Deci EL. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am Psychol. 2000;55(1):68–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Markland D, Ryan RM, Tobin VJ, Rollnick S. Motivational interviewing and self-determination theory. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2005;24(6):811–31. [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Deci E, Ryan R. The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol Inquiry. 2000;11:227–68. [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Ng JYY, Ntoumanis N, Thøgersen-Ntoumani C, et al. Self-determination theory applied to health contexts: a meta-analysis. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2012;7(4):325–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Shumway D, Griffith KA, Jagsi R, Gabram SG, Williams GC, Resnicow K. Psychometric properties of a brief measure of autonomy support in breast cancer patients. BMC Med Inform Decis Making. 2015;15:51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Vansteenkiste M, Williams GC, Resnicow K. Toward systematic integration between self-determination theory and motivational interviewing as examples of top-down and bottom-up intervention development: autonomy or volition as a fundamental theoretical principle. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9:23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Resnicow K, Teixeira PJ, Williams GC. Efficient allocation of public health and behavior change resources: the “difficulty by motivation” matrix. Am J Public Health. 2017;107(1):55–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Levinson W, Kao A, Kuby A, Thisted RA. Not all patients want to participate in decision making: a national study of public preferences. J Gen Intern Med. 2005;20(6):531–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Murray E, Pollack L, White M, Lo B. Clinical decision-making: patients’ preferences and experiences. Patient Educ Couns. 2007;65(2):189–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Johnson RL, Roter D, Powe NR, Cooper LA. Patient race/ethnicity and quality of patient-physician communication during medical visits. Am J Public Health. 2004;94(12):2084–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Levinson W, Hudak PL, Feldman JJ, Frankel RM, Kuby A, Bereknyei S, Braddock C, III. “It’s not what you say …”: racial disparities in communication between orthopedic surgeons and patients. Med Care. 2008;46(4):410–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Ghods BK, Roter DL, Ford DE, Larson S, Arbelaez JJ, Cooper LA. Patient-physician communication in the primary care visits of African Americans and whites with depression. J Gen Intern Med. 2008;23(5):600–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Kaplan SH, Gandek B, Greenfield S, Rogers W, Ware JE. Patient and visit characteristics related to physicians’ participatory decision-making style: results from the Medical Outcomes Study. Med Care. 1995;33(12):1176–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. van Ryn M, Burke J. The effect of patient race and socio-economic status on physicians’ perceptions of patients. Soc Sci Med. 2000;50(6):813–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Peek ME, Odoms-Young A, Quinn MT, Gorawara-Bhat R, Wilson SC, Chin MH. Race and shared decision-making: perspectives of African-Americans with diabetes. Soc Sci Med. 2010;71(1):1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Cooper-Patrick L, Gallo JJ, Gonzales JJ, et al. Race, gender, and partnership in the patient-physician relationship. JAMA. 1999;282(6):583–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Hack TF, Degner LF, Watson P, Sinha L. Do patients benefit from participating in medical decision making? Longitudinal follow-up of women with breast cancer. Psychooncology. 2006;15(1):9–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Grobe JE, Goggin K, Harris KJ, Richter KP, Resnicow K, Catley D. Race moderates the effects of motivational interviewing on smoking cessation induction. Patient Educ Couns. 2020;103(2):350–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Resnicow K, Rollnick S. Motivational interviewing in health promotion & behavioral medicine settings. In: Cox WM, Klinger E, eds. Handbook of Motivational Counseling: Motivating People for Change. 2nd ed. Sussex (UK): Wiley and Sons; 2011. p 581–95. [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Resnicow K, McMaster F, Rollnick S. Action reflections: a client-centered technique to bridge the WHY-HOW transition in motivational interviewing. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2012;40(4):474–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Resnicow K, McMaster F. Motivational interviewing: moving from why to how with autonomy support. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9:19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Miller WR, Rollnick S. Motivational Interviewing: Helping People Change. 3rd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Zuckoff A, Dew MA. Research on MI in equipoise: the case of living organ donation. Motivational Interviewing: Training, Research, Implementation, and Practice. 2012;1(1):39–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Dillard JP, Shen L. On the nature of reactance and its role in persuasive health communication. Commun Monogr. 2005;72(2):144–68. [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Brehm SS, Brehm JW. Psychological Reactance: A Theory of Freedom and Control. New York: Academic Press; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Miller S, Khensani N, Beech B. Perceptions of physical activity and motivational interviewing among rural African-American women with type 2 diabetes. Women’s Health Issues. 2010;1–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Miller ST, Marolen KN, Beech BM. Perceptions of physical activity and motivational interviewing among rural African-American women with type 2 diabetes. Womens Health Issues. 2010;20(1):43–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Lundahl B, Moleni T, Burke BL, et al. Motivational interviewing in medical care settings: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Patient Educ Couns. 2013;93(2):157–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Resnicow K, Zhou Y, Hawley S, et al. Communication preference moderates the effect of a tailored intervention to increase colorectal cancer screening among African Americans. Patient Educ Couns. 2014;97(3):370–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Resnicow K, Davis RE, Zhang G, et al. Tailoring a fruit and vegetable intervention on novel motivational constructs: results of a randomized study. Ann Behav Med. 2008;35(2):159–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Resnicow K, Abrahamse P, Tocco RS, et al. Development and psychometric properties of a brief measure of subjective decision quality for breast cancer treatment. BMC Med Inform Decis Making. 2014;14:110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Martinez KA, Resnicow K, Williams GC, et al. Does physician communication style impact patient report of decision quality for breast cancer treatment? Patient Educ Couns. 2016;99(12):1947–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Lantz PM, Janz NK, Fagerlin A, et al. Satisfaction with surgery outcomes and the decision process in a population-based sample of women with breast cancer. Health Serv Res. 2005;40(3):745–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Hawley ST, Lantz PM, Janz NK, et al. Factors associated with patient involvement in surgical treatment decision making for breast cancer. Patient Educ Couns. 2007;65(3):387–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Simon D, Loh A, Wills CE, Härter M. Depressed patients’ perceptions of depression treatment decision-making. Health Expect. 2007;10(1):62–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Caverly TJ, Hayward RA. Dealing with the lack of time for detailed shared decision-making in primary care: everyday shared decision-making. J Gen Intern Med. 2020;35(10):3045–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Janis IL, Mann L. Decision Making: A Psychological Analysis of Conflict, Choice, and Commitment. New York: Free Press; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Velicer WF, Norman GJ, Fava JL, Prochaska JO. Testing 40 predictions from the transtheoretical model. Addict Behav. 1999;24(4):455–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Bandura A. Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs (NJ): Prentice Hall; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Hibbard JH, Stockard J, Mahoney ER, Tusler M. Development of the Patient Activation Measure (PAM): conceptualizing and measuring activation in patients and consumers. Health Serv Res. 2004;39(4 pt 1):1005–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Collins L. Optimization of Behavioral, Biobehavioral, and Biomedical Interventions: The Multiphase Optimization Strategy (MOST). New York: Springer; 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Nair V, Strecher V, Fagerlin A, et al. Screening experiments and the use of fractional factorial designs in behavioral intervention research. Am J Public Health. 2008;98(8):1354–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Smit ES, Zeidler C, Resnicow K, de Vries H. Identifying the most autonomy-supportive message frame in digital health communication: a 2x2 between-subjects experiment. J Med Internet Res. 2019;21(10):e14074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Hawley ST, Morris AM. Cultural challenges to engaging patients in shared decision making. Patient Educ Couns. 2017;100(1):18–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Medical Decision Making are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

RESOURCES