Abstract
Alzheimer disease and related dementias (ADRD) present considerable challenges to health-care and medical systems worldwide, especially in low-income and middle-income countries. In this Perspective, we leverage our experience and expertise with older Latino groups to review and discuss the need to integrate cultural factors into dementia research and care. We examine the importance of considering the effects of cultural factors on clinical presentation and diagnosis, dementia risk factors, dementia research and recruitment, and caregiving practices, with a focus on minoritized groups in the USA. We highlight critical gaps in the literature to stimulate future research aimed at improving ADRD prevention and early detection of ADRD and developing novel treatments and interventions across ethnoracially diverse populations. In addition, we briefly discuss some of our own initiatives to promote research and clinical care among Latino populations living in the USA.
Introduction
Alzheimer disease (AD) and related dementias (ADRD) are predicted to become one of the greatest challenges facing health-care and medical systems worldwide1, especially in low-income and middle-income countries, where the number of older people is predicted to increase by 185% by 20502. The demographics in countries such as the USA are also rapidly changing with the expansion of populations traditionally considered as minority ethnic and racial groups. Hispanic/Latino/a/e, – hereby referred as Latino individuals – generally include persons of Cuban, Mexican, Puerto Rican, South or Central American, or other Spanish culture or origin regardless of race, and are the fastest growing subpopulation among aged individuals in the USA3. Latino and Asian populations are expected to surpass the Black population as the largest minority groups in the U.S in the coming decades.
Ethnic and racial disparities in dementia research have been documented4. In the USA, older Black and Latino individuals are more likely than older white individuals to have ADRD5, but also have an increased risk of missed or delayed diagnosis6,7, a disproportionately high rate of misdiagnosis of cognitive disorders8 and a reduced likelihood of being informed of their dementia status by their doctor9. These factors all impede access to timely and effective early interventions and recommendations, resulting in worse outcomes for the patient, caregiver and family10,11.
Culture — a set of traditions, beliefs, values and living styles shared by a group of individuals — is a key component of a person’s life; it is integrated into people’s behaviours and passed through generations13. Culture consists of factors such as language, religion and spiritual beliefs, gender roles, geographical location or country of origin, education, upbringing and migration history. Culture also has a crucial roles in health behaviours, perception of illness and even the aetiology of dementia, all of which can contribute to delays in diagnosis and treatment14 as well as influencing the risk of and resilience to AD.
The cultural, racial and ethnic diversity in the USA and worldwide creates an urgent need to promote cultural competence and awareness by increasing multicultural education and training among clinicians and researchers in the field of ADRD4. Culture should be considered when conducting clinical evaluations to diagnose cognitive impairment or dementia, and should also inform the design of clinical and research studies, not only to increase research engagement of diverse individuals who are more representative of the larger population but also to enable equitable clinical research and care that is relevant to diverse communities. The goal of this Perspective is to highlight key considerations of multicultural research and clinical practices in the field of ADRD, with particular attention to the challenges and opportunities of working with ethnically and racially diverse populations. Specifically, we focus on the role of cultural factors in clinical evaluations, dementia risk, research and study recruitment and caregiving experiences in the Latino population living in the USA.
Clinical evaluation
Culture has an important role in the expression and manifestation of symptoms (for example, anxiety, depression or cognitive concerns), as well as reporting of symptoms, perceptions of their causes, coping mechanisms and help-seeking behaviours. Therefore, it is important to conduct a comprehensive clinical evaluation, including assessment of behavioural and psychiatric symptoms, that integrates relevant cultural factors within a biopsychosocial framework (BOX 1)15,16. For instance, individuals often believe that ageing is associated with cognitive decline and might view AD symptoms such as memory loss as part of ‘normal ageing’. Cognitive or behavioural changes can sometimes be dismissed, or people might be reluctant to report mood symptoms or seek an evaluation to assess memory concerns. In some Latino cultures, AD might be viewed as a punishment from God, witchcraft (‘brujeria’), or a sign of mental illness or going crazy (‘volverse loco’), all of which carry additional sociocultural stigma for the individual and their family17, which can also lead to delays in seeking treatment17.
Box 1 |. Clinical evaluation: key points and recommendations.
Cultural factors need to be considered in clinical evaluations to diagnose cognitive impairment or dementia, as well as to inform the design of clinical and research studies in Alzheimer disease and related dementias (ADRD).
Clinicians should follow proposed cultural guidelines, standards and clinical frameworks, such as ECLECTIC.
Cultural factors, including cultural values and identity, race, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, language, educational attainment, occupational history and immigration history, should be integrated into clinical evaluations.
Consideration of cultural and linguistic factors, including acculturation, education, literacy and bilingualism, can facilitate selection of cognitive measures and norms, assessment of functional abilities, and clinical case conceptualization and diagnosis.
Following the evaluation, culturally relevant and appropriate recommendations should be provided.
Research examining the impact of sociocultural and linguistic factors on cognitive presentations and trajectories is required to guide clinical evaluation and help to mitigate health disparities in the field of ADRD.
Neuropsychological assessment is important for detecting, diagnosing and monitoring cognitive change over time, as well as for guiding treatment and recommendations. However, most neuropsychological tests were originally developed in western, English-speaking, non-Latino White populations. Performance on these tests can be affected by a range of interacting cultural factors, including macrosocietal structures (for example, economics, sociopolitical history, government structures and educational systems) and individual characteristics, such as cultural values, race, ethnicity, racial socialization, socioeconomic status, language, educational attainment, literacy and immigration history18–20. In the USA, several sets of guidelines and standards have been developed to improve the cultural competence and awareness of clinicians when working with linguistically and culturally diverse individuals15,16,21, including the National Standards for Culturally and Linguistically Appropriate Services (CLAS) from the Office of Minority Health of the US Department of Health and Human Services, as well as specific considerations for the assessment of culturally diverse individuals22,23. Similarly, the ECLECTIC framework24 has been proposed to guide neuropsychological assessment by integrating an individual’s relevant cultural factors that affect test performance, such as education and literacy, culture and acculturation, language, economics, communication style, testing situation, how intelligence is defined and migration history.
The opportunity to learn, defined by the American Psychological Association as “the extent to which individuals have had exposure to instruction or knowledge that affords them the opportunity to learn the content and skills targeted by the test22”, is influenced by both duration and quality of education. Literacy skills and education affect neuropsychological test performance, as they shape both cognitive processes and test-taking abilities25, which might, in turn, have an impact on test fairness, for example, via content bias, testing context, test response, accessibility and/or validity22. In addition, low education levels have been identified as a risk factor for dementia1,26. Although efforts have been made to develop neuropsychological tests and normative data to assess individuals from diverse ethnic and racial groups with high rates of illiteracy and/or low levels of formal education (see review), including the Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale27, NEUROPSI28 and the Figure Memory Test29, additional tools and normative data are needed to better characterize cognitive functioning in these individuals30.
Culture also influences performance on neuropsychological tests31, as these tests generally measure learned abilities that are influenced by an individual’s lived experiences and learning opportunities. Moreover, neuropsychological evaluations rely on procedures and values that are inherently culture-dependent, such as the examinee–examiner relationship, the use of stereotyped language in standardized testing instructions and familiarity with the testing materials, which can influence various aspects of the testing approach, including speed, strategy and task engagement.
Acculturation — that is, the process of assimilation to another culture — is another factor that has been shown to affect neuropsychological test performance32. The level of acculturation can be ascertained during the clinical interview by gathering information regarding age at the time of immigration, exposure to culture, and cultural identity, behaviour, language and values, and can also be assessed more formally through validated questionnaires, such as the Abbreviated Multidimensional Acculturation Scale33, the Short Acculturation Scale for Hispanics34 or the Asian American Multidimensional Scale35).
Taking all of these factors into account, careful selection of cognitive measures is paramount to ensure the construct validity of instruments within a culture or population. In addition, the reference population to which an individual’s performance on cognitive testing will be compared, termed the normative data, must be from an appropriate culturally representative sample. To this end, a growing number of initiatives in cross-cultural neuropsychology are being established27,36–38, including work to develop culturally appropriate measures for Spanish-speaking individuals, such as the Brain Health Assessment39,40 and the Latin American Spanish version of the Face–Name Associative Memory Exam (LAS-FNAME)41, and to obtain normative data from several Spanish-speaking countries, including Colombia42, Ecuador43, the USA29, Mexico44, the US–Mexico border region8 and Spain45. Similarly, the US National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center has worked on translating and adapting the Uniform Data Set 3 (UDS3), which is used in the standardized annual evaluations conducted at the National Institute on Aging (NIA)-funded Alzheimer’s Disease Research Centers (ADRCs) across the USA46, for use in diverse Spanish-speaking Latino populations, and efforts are underway to include social determinants of health measures. Despite recent strides, however, tools and normative data to assess culturally and linguistically diverse individuals continue to be scarce, and culturally tailored interventions are lacking. Further research is needed to examine cognitive ageing and ADRD risk factors in diverse, minoritized and underrepresented individuals in the USA.
Cultural diversity is also linked to linguistic diversity. Thousands of languages are spoken worldwide, and in the latest US census, 22% of residents reported speaking a language other than English at home47. Neuropsychological evaluation of bilingual and multilingual individuals poses key challenges,21,48 a lack of competency and training among clinicians, and limited tools and normative data to assess these individuals. Despite the availability of tests such as the Multi-Lingual Naming Test (MINT)49, the Bilingual Verbal Abilities Test50 and the Receptive and Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Tests — Bilingual Edition51, patients often experience delayed diagnosis or misdiagnosis11. Research into bilingualism and multilingualism offers unique opportunities to explore mechanisms of resilience and reserve in ADRD52–55 and to examine the impact of linguistic factors on cognitive presentations and trajectories11,56.
Neuropsychological evaluations should preferrably be conducted in the patient’s primary language by a clinician who is proficient in that language. However, in the USA, few neuropsychologists have the necessary proficiency in non-English languages and competency in cultural neuropsychology21. If an appropriate referral is not available, interpreters can be used to conduct the evaluation57; though, clinicians would still require comprehensive training and expertise in cultural neuropsychology to understand the cultural factors that may influence neuropsychological performance23,48,58. Language and culture influence communication and interpersonal style (for example, interactions between various social roles, self-disclosure, and establishing rapport and trust), as well as non-verbal communication (for example, eye contact, expressions, gestures and interpersonal distance), all of which are integral aspects of behavioural observations during a neuropsychological evaluation.
Cultural factors, together with sociodemographic and linguistic factors, are important when assessing functional abilities to determine a clinical diagnosis. This process requires careful review of an individual’s development, educational and occupational history within a cultural context. Alongside presenting concerns, evaluation of functional abilities is important to avoid bias in the assessment process. For example, individuals who perform below average on cognitive tests might be independent in performing activities of daily living, whereas individuals with high cognitive reserve might report subtle difficulties managing activities of daily living while continuing to perform above average on cognitive tests.
One of the main goals of clinical evaluation and neuropsychological assessment is to provide recommendations regarding issues such as driving, supervision, compensatory strategies, referrals to specialty services (for example, neurology, psychiatry or psychology), speech and language therapy, cognitive rehabilitation and educational resources59. However, marked disparities exist in the use and availability of neuropsychological services, as well as specialty services such as neurology, psychiatry, speech and language therapy and cognitive rehabilitation, among minoritized groups owing to systemic, cultural and language barriers. These barriers include racism and discrimination, as well as inequalities with regard to health-care access, financial and time resources, language proficiency, and access to linguistically and culturally congruent providers11. Moreover, minoritized groups experience marked disparities in access to culturally relevant and appropriate recommendations.
Dementia risk factors
The aforementioned racial and ethnic disparities in dementia prevalence raise the possibility that, in addition to differences in genetic predisposition60, disparities in sociocultural factors place individuals from certain groups at an increased risk of dementia. Lifestyle habits and genetic risk are reported to be independently associated with the risk of dementia and cardiovascular disease (CVD), and a healthy lifestyle might substantially decrease dementia risk regardless of genetic risk (BOX 2)61–63. The few published studies that have addressed differences in risk factors in culturally diverse groups consistently suggest that a plethora of sociocultural factors could account for disparities in dementia prevalence between groups. Fortunately, many of these sociocultural risk factors are modifiable and, thus, represent a promising avenue for creating public policy and interventions that can help decrease dementia risk across racially and ethnically diverse groups.
Box 2 |. Risk factors: key points and recommendations.
Healthy lifestyle factors can decrease dementia risk independently of genetic risk.
Cardiovascular disease, smoking, sedentary lifestyles, poor diet, low socioeconomic status and greater levels of acculturation are associated with an increased risk of dementia in the USA.
Multiple risk factors, including cardiovascular disease and financial hardship, are particularly prevalent among ethnic and racial minority groups in the USA.
Exercise, a healthy diet, engaging in stimulating cognitive activities, and bilingualism or multilingualism can all help to mitigate dementia risk.
Culturally informed interventions and public health policies that promote healthy lifestyles (for example, greater physical activity and healthier diets) represent a promising avenue to substantially reduce dementia risk.
CVD, which encompasses coronary artery disease and stroke, as well as obesity, hypertension, diabetes, tobacco use and hypercholesterolaemia, is one of the most notable risk factors for dementia64. These conditions are highly prevalent in the USA, particularly among Black, South Asian and Latino individuals5,65,66. Within the US Latino population, differences in CVD risk factors have been reported among individuals from different backgrounds65. Specifically, Puerto Rican individuals have significantly higher rates of CVD risk factors than people from Mexican, Cuban, Dominican, Central American and South American backgrounds65. Studies have also shown that South Asian individuals have the highest CVD-associated mortality rates in the USA67,68 In addition, within the US Latino population, CVD and related risk factors were found to be more prevalent among US-born individuals who had low education levels, low socioeconomic status and/or high levels of acculturation than among individuals who were foreign-born or first-generation immigrants, had lived in the USA for less than 10 years and/or for whom Spanish was the preferred language65,69. In fact, greater acculturation has been associated with worse health outcomes and behaviours detrimental to health, including greater substance use and less nutritious diets in some groups, particularly those experiencing financial hardships69–71.
Poor diets and sedentary lifestyles also contribute to cognitive decline and dementia risk, as they increase inflammation levels, AD pathology burden and CVD risk72–76. Consequently, individuals from cultures and environments that promote diets low in saturated fat and high in plant-based products, and that foster physical activity, might have greater protection against dementia77,78. Individuals from various ethnic and racial groups who report leading sedentary lifestyles have identified multiple barriers to engaging in physical activity. These barriers include the perception that exercise is expensive and unimportant, which is also highly associated with lower socioeconomic status, low literacy, depression, insufficient time, and limited access to recreational centres, sidewalks, parks or other facilities79,80. Thus, policies that aim to improve the infrastructure of cities, access to affordable fitness facilities and healthy foods, and cultural values surrounding diet and physical activity could substantially help individuals to lead healthier lives. A 2020 Lancet Commission report identified additional factors that contribute to dementia risk, including excessive alcohol use, untreated depression, limited social contact, hearing loss, traumatic brain injury, air pollution, smoking and poor sleep, some of which are more prevalent in low-income populations1. By contrast, factors have been identified that protect against dementia, including attaining a high level of education, engaging in stimulating cognitive activities, and bilingualism or multilingualism. As such, countries that provide free and quality access to education and learning and encourage the use of two or more languages might help to confer protection against dementia81–83.
Taken together, the evidence suggests that multiple modifiable sociocultural factors influence dementia risk (BOX 2). Consideration of cultural values and countries’ and individuals’ economic resources, and implementation of public health policies could potentially contribute to reducing the incidence of dementia worldwide. The differences in risk factors and perceptions of health between ethnically and racially diverse groups also highlight the need to tailor interventions to an individual’s cultural background.
Dementia research
Racial and ethnic disparities in recruitment and participation in AD trials are a persistent problem84,85. Raman and colleagues86 examined data from the Anti-amyloid Treatment in Asymptomatic Alzheimer’s Disease (A4) trial, a study of an anti-amyloid-β monoclonal antibody, and reported racial and ethnic differences in recruitment sources, demographic and clinical characteristics, and reasons for exclusions, all of which have important implications for research and therapeutic development in AD87,88. Dementia research studies require careful consideration of cultural factors that are important to the participants and of potential barriers with recruitment and retention that could arise owing to cultural differences. A qualitative study of Chinese–American dementia caregivers revealed that barriers to participating in research included cultural stigmatization of a dementia diagnosis and the perception that research studies are invasive and offer little to no direct benefit to the participant or their community89. Among Black individuals in the USA mistrust towards research institutions persists owing to racist, unethical and involuntary research practices in studies as recent as the 20th century90. In the Latino population, lower levels of formal education, inequitable health-care access and perceptions about the brain and ageing are some of the common barriers to recruitment for research studies91,92.
To improve recruitment and retention, investigators must culturally diversify their research teams to reflect the communities that they intend to serve. More ethnically and racially diverse research teams can better understand and be better equipped to partner with communities in addressing the systemic barriers that affect recruitment of culturally diverse research participants. In addition, they can actively serve as liaisons between these communities and ongoing research studies through community partnerships, such as tabling events and speaking engagements at community centers (for example, churches or community fairs). Community events can provide excellent opportunities to disseminate information regarding brain health, cognitive decline and dementia, gauge attitudes and identify barriers to participating in research, and even offer brief assessments of memory concerns93, which can ultimately facilitate recruitment in dementia research. Offering culturally and linguistically appropriate memory screenings at community centres in diverse neighbourhoods, and advertising these events through varied sources and in multiple languages, can help mitigate disparities in study recruitment and participation. Interpretation of data from studies of diverse groups also needs to be done in a culturally competent fashion90.
Given that dementia affects not only the person with the condition but also their family and community, culture impinges on dementia research in many ways. One aspect of cultural identity that is often forgotten when designing a study recruitment protocol is the patient’s geographical history, that is, where they were born and raised and have lived for most of their life. A person’s culture reflects not only their customs but also the broader community in which they were raised, so it is important to consider potential geographical links to dementia risk. For example, research has shown that exposure to air pollution is related to an increased incidence of dementia94, elevated levels of neurodegenerative disease biomarkers in people with cognitive impairment95, and accelerated rates of cognitive decline, such as in memory encoding96. In the USA, Black and Latino individuals are more likely than white individuals to live in neighbourhoods with elevated levels of air pollution97,98. However, some communities in which White individuals tend to predominate, such as the coal mining communities of West Virginia, also have increased levels of dementia mortality99. The Area Deprivation Index (ADI)100 is a validated, neighbourhood-level composite index that captures data from 17 social determinants of health and can be used as a useful proxy to examine neighbourhood disadvantage in ADRD research studies.
Several US organizations have provided guidance on the recruitment of more diverse samples101. The NIA, for example, has published recommendations for increasing enrolment and retention of culturally diverse research participants102, including the Alzheimer’s & Dementia Outreach, Recruitment & Engagement (ADORE)103 resources. Similarly, the NIH actively encourages researchers to collect information on the enrolment of racially diverse participants104, and the Institute on Methods and Protocols for Advancement of Clinical Trials in ADRD (IMPACT-AD) has developed a course to help increase diversity among ADRD clinical triallists105. In addition, groups such as the Alzheimer’s Clinical Trial Consortium106 are providing training and cross-institutional support for the recruitment of diverse individuals in clinical trials.
Researchers should be aiming to create meaningful, appropriate partnerships with community organizations and leaders such that they are viewed not solely as people working on abstract scientific questions but rather as individuals who can provide direct benefits to the community, for example, through speaking engagements to promote healthy aging and sharing findings and new study goals in plain language (BOX 3). The experience of our group and collaborators working with Latino populations in Boston (MA, USA) and surrounding areas indicates that creating and nurturing these community partnerships requires considerable investment on the part of the research team and identification of key community partners (for example, community activists, clergy or politicians) to reach communities that have been historically marginalized and excluded from research. Building these partnerships takes intentional efforts and dedicated time to liaise with communities and understand their needs, which in turn can be used to shape culturally informed research studies. In addition, community engagement could allow greater transparency when reporting research findings, thereby providing a valuable service to participants by conveying clinically relevant information.
Box 3 |. Dementia research: key points and recommendations.
Racial and ethnic disparities in recruitment and participation in research and clinical trials creates concerns about the generalizability of research findings, and limits research advances and development of clinical treatments and interventions in Alzheimer disease and related dementias (ADRD).
Increasing cultural diversity in ADRD research requires intention and dedication by researchers to recruit and retain culturally diverse participants, research teams and leaders.
Partnerships between researchers with trusted community hubs and leaders (for example, churches, community fairs and cultural events) can promote recruitment and research engagement of historically marginalized and excluded communities.
Anecdotally, the experience of our research group and others suggests that building meaningful partnerships with diverse communities not only bolsters recruitment efforts but also informs the design of research studies that are meaningful to participants and communities.
Formal guidance for increasing enrolment and retention of culturally diverse research participants is available, such as the Alzheimer’s & Dementia Outreach, Recruitment & Engagement (ADORE) resources curated by the National Institute on Aging.
Dementia caregiving
Evidence highlights the presence of cultural, ethnic and racial differences in the physical and mental health of dementia caregivers. For instance, in the USA, Black and Latino caregivers tend to report greater adverse physical health outcomes than non-Latino white caregivers107,108. Across studies108,109, Black caregivers report better psychological well-being when compared with non-Latino white caregivers, whereas Latino and Asian American caregivers tend to experience higher levels of emotional stress and depression. As emphasized in a 1997 paper by Aranda and Knight110, multiple factors underlie discrepancies in caregiver outcomes across cultural, ethnic and racial groups (BOX 4).
Box 4 |. Caregiving: key points and recommendations.
Cultural, racial and ethnic differences in the health and emotional outcomes of dementia caregivers have been reported.
Multiple sociocultural factors, including faith, cultural norms, beliefs and values, influence perceptions and attitudes toward caregiving.
Caregivers from diverse cultural, ethnic and racial groups might have limited resources and face substantial obstacles accessing adequate health-care services.
A need exists to develop culturally specific research and effective interventions to appropriately address the needs of the culturally and ethnically diverse caregiving populations for patients with dementia.
In the USA, ethnic and racial minoritized groups, including indigenous groups, engage in more caregiving hours than non-minoritized groups111 but make less frequent use of formal services112–114. This disparity in service use has been linked to several factors, including unavailability of health insurance; limited knowledge or understanding of dementia; language or other communication barriers; limited knowledge by health professionals on cultural differences in the expression of mental illness; and mistrust of Western medicine114,115. Compounding this issue, Latino and Black caregivers report lower household incomes, on average, than non-Latino White or Asian American caregivers and devote a greater proportion of their income to caregiving activities, leading to a greater overall financial burden107. Caregivers from diverse cultural, ethnic and racial groups might, therefore, lack resources and face substantial obstacles to accessing adequate health-care services.
Multiple sociocultural factors influence perceptions and attitudes toward caregiving, including faith, cultural norms, beliefs and values (for example, sense of obligation or devotion toward family members, often referred to as ‘familism’) and perceptions of the ageing process116,117. Compared with non-Latino White and Asian American caregivers, Latino and Black caregivers more often report that caregiving provides them with a sense of purpose. Black and Latino individuals also report more positive caregiving experiences118 and stronger cultural motivations (for example, sense of duty, expectations and values) for providing care119. These positive conceptualizations of the caregiving experience might act as a buffer to reduce adverse emotional impacts114,119. However, other forms of sociocultural conceptualization seem to be detrimental. For instance, sociocultural stigma and social embarrassment associated with a diagnosis of dementia or patients’ behavioural disturbances are important stressors that increase the risk of adverse emotional outcomes120,121. Thus, sociocultural values and expectations could both enhance and impair the emotional well-being of dementia caregivers.
The scarcity of culturally specific research and interventions in the field of dementia caregiving is an important factor to consider when interpreting differences in caregiver outcomes across cultural groups122. Interventions that integrate specific sociocultural characteristics could be more effective in terms of outcomes, satisfaction, adherence and costs than interventions that do not take these characteristics into account123. Although caregivers share many common stressors, culture must be recognized as an important component in any efforts to explore differences in caregiver well-being or in the implementation of interventions124,125. Initiatives are being introduced to address this issue, including Resources for Enhancing Alzheimer’s Caregiver Health II (REACH II) — a multicomponent, evidence-based programme that offers specialized educational and counselling services to culturally diverse caregivers of people with dementia126,127 in the USA, including Non-Latino white, Black and Latino as well as American Indian and Alaska Native communities128. Despite such efforts, however, an estimated two-thirds of published intervention studies in the field fail to stratify results by racial or ethnic groups112, highlighting the limited attention to racial and ethnic differences and cultural factors. As discussed in greater detail in the next section, the lack of representation of diverse cultural groups in dementia research, including dementia caregiving, is an important limitation to current knowledge and clinical practices122.
To develop effective interventions, an urgent need exists to characterize factors that influence the physical and emotional well-being of dementia caregivers. Importantly, research into dementia caregiving should recognize the intersectionality and evolving nature of sociocultural factors, health-care systems and dementia care needs in relation to caregiver outcomes124. For example, caregiver individuals who self-identify as lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender and/or queer (LGBTQ) might face additional challenges that result in greater caregiver strain129. Therefore, future research studies must apply culturally relevant theoretical models and adapted measurements and recruit diverse and inclusive samples, recognizing the presence of culturally distinct subgroups within larger ethnic and racial groups. This effort is crucial to appropriately address the needs of the culturally and ethnically diverse caregiving populations of patients with dementia and inform the development of effective interventions.
New initiatives
Our group in the Multicultural Alzheimer’s Prevention Program at Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH) in Boston (MA, USA) has developed several initiatives and programmes to diversify research into ADRD. With the support of the NIA, we recently launched the Boston Latino Aging Study (BLAST). The goal of this study is to investigate the impact of various AD risk factors on cognition, brain function and molecular markers of AD pathology in older Latino individuals living in Boston and the surrounding areas. As part of the study, participants will undergo comprehensive cognitive assessments and biomarker testing. In addition, dementia risk factors, including physical inactivity, low social contact, sleep disturbances, depression, stress and obesity, will be assessed. This study will allow us to better characterize psychosocial factors, lived experiences and social determinants of health in this population. Of note, we carefully selected cognitive measures that have been validated in Latino individuals as culturally appropriate means to evaluate cognitive functioning in this population. Moreover, to ensure that the cohort is representative of the community and to maximize the generalizability of the findings, individuals will not be excluded on the basis of psychiatric or medical conditions (for example, depression or hypertension) that are well controlled.
We also recently launched the Multicultural Assessment and Research Center (MARC) at MGH to offer culturally and linguistically appropriate neuropsychological services, including clinical evaluations and cognitive training, to a diverse range of adults, including monolingual and bilingual Spanish speakers and individuals who speak neither English nor Spanish. This centre aims to integrate clinical and research work and to advance cultural neuropsychology research and training, for example, by developing novel measures and normative data. Importantly, the administrative staff, clinical leadership and staff neuropsychologists at MARC are multicultural and multilingual and have received specialized training in multicultural neuropsychology. A major objective of the centre, beyond facilitating access to high-quality clinical services, is to provide training opportunities in multicultural neuropsychology for individuals at different training stages, so as to enhance diversity in the field of ADRD.
MARC’s goals also include expansion of patient access to research and clinical studies by using novel tools, such as remote cognitive assessment. Our remote cognitive assessment work includes understanding the psychometric properties of existing cognitive measures administered with the patient in their own home130, as well as ongoing work focusing on the development and norming of several new digital cognitive measures that individuals will be able to self-administer on personal electronic devices131. Careful consideration of culture is central to this line of work, and we are currently assessing the cultural utility and validity of the new digital measures in English, Spanish and Korean speakers. We aim to make these remotely administered cognitive tests available on a variety of electronic devices, including computers, smartphones and tablets, to facilitate use in individuals with varying levels of technological literacy and familiarity. Having a range of methods to remotely administer cognitive tests should enable researchers to provide equitable access to these tests.
Conclusions and future directions
In this article, we have argued that cultural factors — together with socioenvironmental factors — should be integrated into dementia research. Research from the USA shows that older Black and Latino individuals are disproportionately more likely to have ADRD than older non-Latino white individuals. However, most ADRD studies conducted in this region have not included a good representation of diverse participants, thereby limiting our knowledge of normal ageing, cognitive decline and AD biomarkers among diverse and/or minoritized groups. Therefore, an urgent need exists to promote recruitment and retention of diverse individuals in ADRD research.
Beyond recruitment strategies to engage diverse individuals in ADRD research, cultural factors also need to be integrated into the design and implementation of clinical studies. For instance, common inclusion and exclusion criteria, such as basic literacy skills and exclusion of participants with major medical, neurological and/or psychiatric conditions, limit the generalizability of the results, as findings from a highly selected research sample might not be representative of the general population. Language barriers to study participation also need to be addressed: although approximately 22% of US residents report speaking a language other than English at home, most studies conducted in the USA require proficiency in English to participate.
Cognitive performance is commonly a major variable of interest or outcome measure in ADRD research, and culture influences performance on neuropsychological tests8. Most neuropsychological tests were originally developed in western, English-speaking, non-Latino white populations, which might affect recruitment and measurement of cognition in diverse populations. Furthermore, we need to explore how sociocultural and environmental factors, along with social determinants of health and lived experiences, such as racism, discrimination, acculturation, bilingualism, immigration history and social support, influence dementia risk and progression. In addition, recruitment, support and retention of diverse research teams and leaders is paramount to establishing partnerships with communities and provides the foundation for diversifying research in ADRD.
In conclusion, the goal of this Perspective was to highlight the need to integrate cultural factors in dementia research. We reviewed the importance of considering the effects of cultural factors on clinical presentation and diagnosis, dementia risk factors and caregiving practices in diverse populations in the USA. We highlighted crucial gaps in the literature to prompt future research that advances the field towards improving prevention, early detection and development of novel treatments and interventions in diverse populations. In addition, we hope that the discussion of some of our own research and clinical initiatives will guide other investigators who wish to expand their research or clinical work to more diverse populations.
Competing interests
The authors received grants from the NIH, the Alzheimer’s Association and the Massachusetts General Hospital Executive Committee on Research.
References
- 1.Livingston G et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. The Lancet 396, 413–446 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Prince MJ et al. World Alzheimer Report 2015-The Global Impact of Dementia: An analysis of prevalence, incidence, cost and trends. (2015).
- 3.U.S. Census Bureau. American Community Survey 5-year estimates., <https://www.census.gov/programs-surveys/acs/news/data-releases/2017.html> (2018).
- 4.Babulal GM et al. Perspectives on ethnic and racial disparities in Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias: update and areas of immediate need. Alzheimer’s & Dementia 15, 292–312 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Alzheimer’s Association. Alzheimer’s Disease Facts and Figures. Alzheimer’s & Dementia 17 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Tsoy E et al. Assessment of Racial/Ethnic Disparities in Timeliness and Comprehensiveness of Dementia Diagnosis in California. JAMA neurology (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lin P-J et al. Dementia diagnosis disparities by race and ethnicity. Medical Care 59, 679–686 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Rivera Mindt M et al. Demographically-adjusted norms for the processing speed subtests of the WAIS-III in a Spanish-speaking adult population: Results from the Neuropsychological Norms for the U.S.-Mexico Border Region in Spanish (NP-NUMBRS) project. The Clinical Neuropsychologist 35, 293–307, doi: 10.1080/13854046.2020.1723707 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Lin PJ et al. Dementia diagnosis disparities by race and ethnicity: Health services research: Cost of care and implications for intervention. Alzheimer’s & Dementia 16, e043183 (2020). [Google Scholar]
- 10.Gomez LR, Jain FA & D’Orazio LM Assessment of the hispanic cognitively impaired elderly patient. Neurologic clinics 35, 207–229 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Díaz-Santos M, Yáñez J & Suarez PA Alzheimer’s Disease in Bilingual Latinos: Clinical Decisions for Diagnosis and Treatment Planning. Journal of Health Service Psychology, 1–9 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Henrich J & McElreath R The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews: Issues, News, and Reviews 12, 123–135 (2003). [Google Scholar]
- 14.Rovner BW, Casten RJ & Harris LF Cultural diversity and views on Alzheimer disease in older African Americans. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 27, 133–137, doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e3182654794 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.American Psychological Association. Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. (https://www.apa.org/ethics/code/, 2017a).
- 16.American Psychological Association. Multicultural guidelines: An ecological approach to context, identity, and intersectionality. (http://www.apa.org/about/policy/multicultural-guidelines.pdf, 2017b). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 17.Mahoney DF, Cloutterbuck J, Neary S & Zhan L African American, Chinese, and Latino family caregivers’ impressions of the onset and diagnosis of dementia: cross-cultural similarities and differences. Gerontologist 45, 783–792, doi: 10.1093/geront/45.6.783 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Brickman AM, Cabo R & Manly JJ Ethical issues in cross-cultural neuropsychology. Applied Neuropsychology 13, 91–100 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Flores I et al. Performance of Hispanics and Non-Hispanic Whites on the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery: The roles of ethnicity and language backgrounds. The Clinical Neuropsychologist 31, 783–797 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Graves LV et al. Diagnostic accuracy and differential associations between ratings of functioning and neuropsychological performance in non-Hispanic Black and White older adults. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 1–24 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Rivera Mindt M, Byrd D, Saez P & Manly J Increasing culturally competent neuropsychological services for ethnic minority populations: A call to action. The Clinical Neuropsychologist 24, 429–453 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.American Educational Research Association, A. P. A. A., & National Council on Measurement in Education. Standards for educational and psychological testing. (American Educational Research Association, 2014). [Google Scholar]
- 23.Judd T et al. Professional considerations for improving the neuropsychological evaluation of Hispanics: a National Academy of Neuropsychology education paper. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 24, 127–135 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Fujii DEM Developing a cultural context for conducting a neuropsychological evaluation with a culturally diverse client: the ECLECTIC framework. The Clinical Neuropsychologist 32, 1356–1392, doi: 10.1080/13854046.2018.1435826 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ardila A et al. Illiteracy: The Neuropsychology of Cognition Without Reading. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 25, 689–712, doi: 10.1093/arclin/acq079 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Gauthier S R.-N. P, Morais JA, & Webster C. World Alzheimer Report 2021: Journey through the diagnosis of dementia., (Alzheimer’s Disease International, London, England, 2021). [Google Scholar]
- 27.Nielsen T & Jørgensen K Cross-cultural dementia screening using the Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale: a systematic review and meta-analysis. International psychogeriatrics 32, 1031–1044 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Ostrosky-Solís F, Ardila A & Rosselli M NEUROPSI: A brief neuropsychological test battery in Spanish with norms by age and educational level. Journal of the international Neuropsychological Society 5, 413–433 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Nitrini R, Bucki SMD, Yassuda MS, Fichman HC & Caramelli P The Figure Memory Test: diagnosis of memory impairment in populations with heterogeneous educational background. Dementia & Neuropsychologia 15, 173–185 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Franzen S et al. A systematic review of neuropsychological tests for the assessment of dementia in non-western, low-educated or illiterate populations. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 26, 331–351 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Ardila A Cross-cultural neuropsychology: History and prospects. RUDN Journal of Psychology and Pedagogics 17, 64–78 (2020). [Google Scholar]
- 32.Manly JJ, Byrd DA, Touradji P & Stern Y Acculturation, reading level, and neuropsychological test performance among African American elders. Applied neuropsychology 11, 37–46 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Zea MC, Asner-Self KK, Birman D & Buki LP The Abbreviated Multidimentional Acculturation Scale: Empirical validation with two Latino/Latina samples. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology 9, 107 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Marin G, Sabogal F, Marin BV, Otero-Sabogal R & Perez-Stable EJ Development of a short acculturation scale for Hispanics. Hispanic journal of behavioral sciences 9, 183–205 (1987). [Google Scholar]
- 35.Gim Chung RH, Kim BS & Abreu JM Asian American multidimensional acculturation scale: development, factor analysis, reliability, and validity. Cultural diversity and ethnic minority psychology 10, 66 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Dick MB, Teng EL, Kempler D, Davis DS & Taussig IM in Minority and cross-cultural aspects of neuropsychological assessment. Studies on neuropsychology, development, and cognition. 17–41 (Swets & Zeitlinger Publishers, 2002). [Google Scholar]
- 37.Nielsen TR et al. Performance of middle-aged and elderly European minority and majority populations on a Cross-Cultural Neuropsychological Test Battery (CNTB). The Clinical Neuropsychologist 32, 1411–1430 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Franzen S, Papma JM, van den Berg E & Nielsen TR Cross-cultural neuropsychological assessment in the European Union: a Delphi expert study. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 36, 815–830, doi: 10.1093/arclin/acaa083 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Possin KL et al. The brain health assessment for detecting and diagnosing neurocognitive disorders. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 66, 150–156 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Rodríguez-Salgado AM et al. A Brief Digital Cognitive Assessment for Detection of Cognitive Impairment in Cuban Older Adults. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 1–10 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Vila-Castelar C et al. Validation of the Latin American Spanish version of the face-name associative memory exam in a Colombian Sample. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 1–12 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Torres VL et al. Normative data stratified by age and education for a Spanish neuropsychological test battery: Results from the Colombian Alzheimer’s prevention initiative registry. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult 28, 230–244 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Rodríguez-Lorenzana A et al. Normative data for test of learning and memory in an ecuadorian adult population. The Clinical Neuropsychologist 34, 54–69 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Ostrosky-Solís F et al. Neuropsi attention and memory: A neuropsychological test battery in Spanish with norms by age and educational level. Applied neuropsychology 14, 156–170 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Bonete-López B et al. Validation and Norms for a Recognition Task for the Spanish Version of the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 36, 954–964, doi: 10.1093/arclin/acaa117 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Besser L et al. Version 3 of the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center’s Uniform Data Set. Alzheimer disease and associated disorders (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.U.S. Census Bureau. Detailed Languages Spoken at Home and Ability to Speak English for the Population 5 Years and Over: 2009–2013, 2015).
- 48.Mindt MR et al. Neuropsychological, cognitive, and theoretical considerations for evaluation of bilingual individuals. Neuropsychology review 18, 255–268 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Gollan TH, Weissberger GH, Runnqvist E, Montoya RI & Cera CM Self-ratings of spoken language dominance: A Multilingual Naming Test (MINT) and preliminary norms for young and aging Spanish–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15, 594–615 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Muñoz-Sandoval AF, Cummins J, Alvarado CG & Ruef ML Bilingual verbal ability tests. (Riverside Publishing; Itasca, IL, 1998). [Google Scholar]
- 51.Brownell R (Riverside Publishing, 2012).
- 52.Suárez PA et al. Native Spanish-speaker’s test performance and the effects of Spanish-English bilingualism: results from the neuropsychological norms for the U.S.-Mexico Border Region in Spanish (NP-NUMBRS) project. The Clinical Neuropsychologist 35, 453–465, doi: 10.1080/13854046.2020.1861330 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Gollan TH, Salmon DP, Montoya RI & Galasko DR Degree of bilingualism predicts age of diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease in low-education but not in highly educated Hispanics. Neuropsychologia 49, 3826–3830 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Craik FI, Bialystok E & Freedman M Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease: Bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology 75, 1726–1729 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Arce Rentería M et al. The Contributions of Active Spanish-English Bilingualism to Cognitive Reserve among Older Hispanic Adults Living in California. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 34, 1235–1235, doi: 10.1093/arclin/acz029.02 (2019). [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Rosselli M et al. Effects of bilingualism on verbal and nonverbal memory measures in mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 25, 15–28 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Paradis M Bilingualism and neuropsychiatric disorders. Journal of Neurolinguistics 21, 199–230 (2008). [Google Scholar]
- 58.Casas R et al. Interpreter-mediated neuropsychological testing of monolingual Spanish speakers. The Clinical Neuropsychologist 26, 88–101 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Meth MZ, Bernstein JP, Calamia M & Tranel D What types of recommendations are we giving patients? A survey of clinical neuropsychologists. The Clinical Neuropsychologist 33, 57–74 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Beydoun MA et al. Race, APOE genotypes, and cognitive decline among middle-aged urban adults. Alzheimer’s research & therapy 13, 1–16 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Kivipelto M et al. Apolipoprotein E ɛ4 magnifies lifestyle risks for dementia: a population-based study. Journal of cellular and molecular medicine 12, 2762–2771 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Lourida I et al. Association of lifestyle and genetic risk with incidence of dementia. Jama 322, 430–437 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Said MA, Verweij N & van der Harst P Associations of combined genetic and lifestyle risks with incident cardiovascular disease and diabetes in the UK Biobank Study. JAMA cardiology 3, 693–702 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Hakim A, Ng & Turek. Heart disease as a risk factor for dementia. Clinical Epidemiology, 135, doi: 10.2147/clep.s30621 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Daviglus ML et al. Prevalence of Major Cardiovascular Risk Factors and Cardiovascular Diseases Among Hispanic/Latino Individuals of Diverse Backgrounds in the United States. JAMA 308, 1775, doi: 10.1001/jama.2012.14517 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Schargrodsky H et al. CARMELA: Assessment of cardiovascular risk in seven Latin American cities. The American Journal of Medicine 121, 58–65, doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2007.08.038 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Kanaya AM et al. Comparing coronary artery calcium among U.S. South Asians with four racial/ethnic groups: The MASALA and MESA studies. Atherosclerosis 234, 102–107, doi: 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2014.02.017 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Jose PO et al. Cardiovascular Disease Mortality in Asian Americans. Journal of the American College of Cardiology 64, 2486–2494, doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2014.08.048 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Lara M, Gamboa C, Kahramanian MI, Morales LS & Hayes Bautista DE Acculturation and latino health in the United States: A Review of the Literature and its Sociopolitical Context. Annual Review of Public Health 26, 367–397, doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144615 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Fox M, Thayer ZM & Wadhwa PD Acculturation and Health: The Moderating Role of Sociocultural Context. American Anthropologist 119, 405–421, doi: 10.1111/aman.12867 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Allen JD et al. Pathways between acculturation and health behaviors among residents of low-income housing: the mediating role of social and contextual factors. Soc Sci Med 123, 26–36, doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.10.034 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Elahi M et al. High-fat diet-induced activation of SGK1 promotes Alzheimer’s disease-associated tau pathology. Human Molecular Genetics, doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddab115 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Wiȩckowska-Gacek A et al. Western Diet Induces Impairment of Liver-Brain Axis Accelerating Neuroinflammation and Amyloid Pathology in Alzheimer’s Disease. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience 13, doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.654509 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Thériault P, Elali A & Rivest S High fat diet exacerbates Alzheimer’s disease-related pathology in APPswe/PS1 mice. Oncotarget 7, 67808–67827, doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.12179 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Brown BM et al. Physical activity and amyloid-beta plasma and brain levels: results from the Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle Study of Ageing. Mol Psychiatry 18, 875–881, doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.107 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Di Battista AP et al. High-Intensity Interval Training Is Associated With Alterations in Blood Biomarkers Related to Brain Injury. Front Physiol 9, 1367, doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.01367 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Morris MC Nutrition and risk of dementia: overview and methodological issues. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1367, 31–37, doi: 10.1111/nyas.13047 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Ozawa M et al. Dietary patterns and risk of dementia in an elderly Japanese population: the Hisayama Study. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 97, 1076–1082, doi: 10.3945/ajcn.112.045575 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Justine M, Azizan A, Hassan V, Salleh Z & Manaf H Barriers to participation in physical activity and exercise among middle-aged and elderly individuals. Singapore Medical Journal 54, 581–586, doi: 10.11622/smedj.2013203 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Pham KL, Harrison GG & Kagawa-Singer M Perceptions of diet and physical activity among California Hmong adults and youths. Prev Chronic Dis 4, A93 (2007). [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Guzmán-Vélez E & Tranel D Does bilingualism contribute to cognitive reserve? Cognitive and neural perspectives. Neuropsychology 29, 139 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Sharp ES & Gatz M The relationship between education and dementia an updated systematic review. Alzheimer disease and associated disorders 25, 289 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Sajeev G et al. Late-life cognitive activity and dementia: a systematic review and bias analysis. Epidemiology (Cambridge, Mass.) 27, 732 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Barnes LL Biomarkers for Alzheimer Dementia in Diverse Racial and Ethnic Minorities—A Public Health Priority. JAMA Neurology 76, 251–253, doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.3444 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Mooldijk SS, Licher S & Wolters FJ Characterizing Demographic, Racial, and Geographic Diversity in Dementia Research: A Systematic Review. JAMA neurology 78, 1255–1261 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Raman R et al. Disparities by race and ethnicity among adults recruited for a preclinical Alzheimer disease trial. JAMA Network Open 4, e2114364–e2114364 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Manly JJ, Gilmore-Bykovskyi A & Deters KD Inclusion of Underrepresented Groups in Preclinical Alzheimer Disease Trials—Opportunities Abound. JAMA Network Open 4, e2114606–e2114606 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Manly JJ & Glymour MM What the Aducanumab Approval Reveals About Alzheimer Disease Research. JAMA neurology (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Hinton L, Guo Z, Hillygus J & Levkoff S Working with culture: A qualitative analysis of barriers to the recruitment of Chinese–American family caregivers for dementia research. Journal of cross-cultural gerontology 15, 119–137 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Ighodaro ET et al. Challenges and considerations related to studying dementia in Blacks/African Americans. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease 60, 1–10 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Ceballos RM et al. Latino beliefs about biomedical research participation: A qualitative study on the US–Mexico border. Journal of Empirical Research on Human Research Ethics 9, 10–21 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Gelman CR Learning from recruitment challenges: Barriers to diagnosis, treatment, and research participation for Latinos with symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Gerontological Social Work 53, 94–113 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Peavy GM et al. Community memory screening as a strategy for recruiting older adults into Alzheimer’s disease research. Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy 12, 1–12 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Chen H et al. Exposure to ambient air pollution and the incidence of dementia: A population-based cohort study. Environment international 108, 271–277 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Iaccarino L et al. Association between ambient air pollution and amyloid positron emission tomography positivity in older adults with cognitive impairment. JAMA neurology 78, 197–207 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Younan D et al. Particulate matter and episodic memory decline mediated by early neuroanatomic biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 143, 289–302 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Chakraborty J & Zandbergen PA Children at risk: measuring racial/ethnic disparities in potential exposure to air pollution at school and home. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health 61, 1074–1079 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Perera FP Multiple threats to child health from fossil fuel combustion: impacts of air pollution and climate change. Environmental health perspectives 125, 141–148 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Salm AK & Benson MJ Increased Dementia Mortality in West Virginia Counties with Mountaintop Removal Mining? International journal of environmental research and public health 16, 4278 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Kind AJ & Buckingham WR Making neighborhood-disadvantage metrics accessible—the neighborhood atlas. The New England journal of medicine 378, 2456 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Elliott CL, Ryan L & Silverberg N Building Inclusive and Open Alzheimer Disease and Alzheimer Disease–Related Dementias Research Programs. JAMA neurology 78, 1177–1178 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.National Institute on Aging. <https://www.nia.nih.gov/research/recruitment-strategy), > (
- 103.National Institute on Aging. Alzheimer’s & dementia outreach, recruitment & engagement resources, <https://www.nia.nih.gov/research/alzheimers-dementia-outreachrecruitment-engagement-resources> (
- 104.National Institutes of Health. <https://grants.nih.gov/grants/guide/notice-files/not-od-15-089.html#:~:text=The%20revised%20standards%20contain%20five,%22Not%20Hispanic%20or%20Latino.%22> (
- 105.Berkness T et al. The Institute on Methods and Protocols for Advancement of Clinical Trials in ADRD (IMPACT-AD): A Novel Clinical Trials Training Program. The journal of prevention of Alzheimer’s disease, 1–6 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Alzheimer’s Clinical Trials Consortium. <https://www.actcinfo.org/committees-page/#inclusion-diversity-and-education-in-alzheimers-disease-clinical-trials> (
- 107.Pinquart M & Sörensen S Ethnic differences in stressors, resources, and psychological outcomes of family caregiving: A meta-analysis. The Gerontologist 45, 90–106 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Liu C et al. Systematic review and meta-analysis of racial and ethnic differences in dementia caregivers’ well-being. The Gerontologist (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Moon HE, Haley WE, Rote SM & Sears JS Caregiver Well-Being and Burden: Variations by Race/Ethnicity and Care Recipient Nativity Status. Innovation in aging 4, igaa045 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Aranda MP & Knight BG The influence of ethnicity and culture on the caregiver stress and coping process: A sociocultural review and analysis. The Gerontologist 37, 342–354 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Weiss CO, Gonzalez HM, Kabeto MU & Langa KM Differences in amount of informal care received by non-Hispanic whites and Latinos in a nationally representative sample of older Americans. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 53, 146–151 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Gilmore-Bykovskyi A, Johnson R, Walljasper L, Block L & Werner N Underreporting of gender and race/ethnicity differences in NIH-funded dementia caregiver support interventions. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease & Other Dementias® 33, 145–152 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Rote SM, Angel JL, Moon H & Markides K Caregiving across diverse populations: New evidence from the national study of caregiving and Hispanic EPESE. Innovation in aging 3, igz033 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Scharlach AE et al. Cultural attitudes and caregiver service use: Lessons from focus groups with racially and ethnically diverse family caregivers. Journal of gerontological social work 47, 133–156 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.La Fontaine J, Ahuja J, Bradbury NM, Phillips S & Oyebode JR Understanding dementia amongst people in minority ethnic and cultural groups. Journal of Advanced Nursing 60, 605–614 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Powers SM & Whitlatch CJ Measuring cultural justifications for caregiving in African American and White caregivers. Dementia 15, 629–645 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Sayegh P & Knight BG The effects of familism and cultural justification on the mental and physical health of family caregivers. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 66, 3–14 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Roth DL, Dilworth-Anderson P, Huang J, Gross AL & Gitlin LN Positive aspects of family caregiving for dementia: Differential item functioning by race. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 70, 813–819 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Dilworth-Anderson P et al. Effect of race on cultural justifications for caregiving. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 60, S257–S262 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Montoro-Rodríguez J, Kosloski K, Kercher K & Montgomery RJ The impact of social embarrassment on caregiving distress in a multicultural sample of caregivers. Journal of Applied Gerontology 28, 195–217 (2009). [Google Scholar]
- 121.Sun F, Ong R & Burnette D The influence of ethnicity and culture on dementia caregiving: A review of empirical studies on Chinese Americans. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease & Other Dementias® 27, 13–22 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Napoles AM, Chadiha L, Eversley R & Moreno-John G Reviews: developing culturally sensitive dementia caregiver interventions: are we there yet? American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease & Other Dementias® 25, 389–406 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Castro FG, Barrera M Jr & Holleran Steiker LK Issues and challenges in the design of culturally adapted evidence-based interventions. Annual review of clinical psychology 6, 213–239 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Gallagher-Thompson D, Solano N, Coon D & Areán P Recruitment and retention of Latino dementia family caregivers in intervention research: Issues to face, lessons to learn. The Gerontologist 43, 45–51 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Gallagher-Thompson D et al. Conceptual framework to guide intervention research across the trajectory of dementia caregiving. The Gerontologist 60, S29–S40 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Elliott AF, Burgio LD & DeCoster J Enhancing caregiver health: findings from the resources for enhancing Alzheimer’s caregiver health II intervention. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 58, 30–37 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Meyer OL et al. Acculturation level and caregiver outcomes from a randomized intervention trial to enhance caregivers’ health: evidence from REACH II. Aging & mental health 22, 730–737 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Martindale-Adams J et al. Implementation of the REACH model of dementia caregiver support in American Indian and Alaska Native communities. Translational behavioral medicine 7, 427–434 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Anderson JG & Flatt JD Characteristics of LGBT caregivers of older adults: Results from the national Caregiving in the US 2015 survey. Journal of Gay & Lesbian Social Services 30, 103–116 (2018). [Google Scholar]
- 130.Fox-Fuller JT et al. Initial investigation of test-retest reliability of home-to-home teleneuropsychological assessment in healthy, English-speaking adults. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 1–15, doi: 10.1080/13854046.2021.1954244 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.García-Magariño I, Fox-Fuller JT, Palacios-Navarro G, Baena A & Quiroz YT Visual working memory for semantically related objects in healthy adults. Rev Neurol 71, 277–284, doi: 10.33588/rn.7108.2019479 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]