Abstract
Background
Symptom checker apps are patient-facing decision support systems aimed at providing advice to laypersons on whether, where, and how to seek health care (disposition advice). Such advice can improve laypersons’ self-assessment and ultimately improve medical outcomes. Past research has mainly focused on the accuracy of symptom checker apps’ suggestions. To support decision-making, such apps need to provide not only accurate but also trustworthy advice. To date, only few studies have addressed the question of the extent to which laypersons trust symptom checker app advice or the factors that moderate their trust. Studies on general decision support systems have shown that framing automated systems (anthropomorphic or emphasizing expertise), for example, by using icons symbolizing artificial intelligence (AI), affects users’ trust.
Objective
This study aims to identify the factors influencing laypersons’ trust in the advice provided by symptom checker apps. Primarily, we investigated whether designs using anthropomorphic framing or framing the app as an AI increases users’ trust compared with no such framing.
Methods
Through a web-based survey, we recruited 494 US residents with no professional medical training. The participants had to first appraise the urgency of a fictitious patient description (case vignette). Subsequently, a decision aid (mock symptom checker app) provided disposition advice contradicting the participants’ appraisal, and they had to subsequently reappraise the vignette. Participants were randomized into 3 groups: 2 experimental groups using visual framing (anthropomorphic, 160/494, 32.4%, vs AI, 161/494, 32.6%) and a neutral group without such framing (173/494, 35%).
Results
Most participants (384/494, 77.7%) followed the decision aid’s advice, regardless of its urgency level. Neither anthropomorphic framing (odds ratio 1.120, 95% CI 0.664-1.897) nor framing as AI (odds ratio 0.942, 95% CI 0.565-1.570) increased behavioral or subjective trust (P=.99) compared with the no-frame condition. Even participants who were extremely certain in their own decisions (ie, 100% certain) commonly changed it in favor of the symptom checker’s advice (19/34, 56%). Propensity to trust and eHealth literacy were associated with increased subjective trust in the symptom checker (propensity to trust b=0.25; eHealth literacy b=0.2), whereas sociodemographic variables showed no such link with either subjective or behavioral trust.
Conclusions
Contrary to our expectation, neither the anthropomorphic framing nor the emphasis on AI increased trust in symptom checker advice compared with that of a neutral control condition. However, independent of the interface, most participants trusted the mock app’s advice, even when they were very certain of their own assessment. Thus, the question arises as to whether laypersons use such symptom checkers as substitutes rather than as aids in their own decision-making. With trust in symptom checkers already high at baseline, the benefit of symptom checkers depends on interface designs that enable users to adequately calibrate their trust levels during usage.
Trial Registration
Deutsches Register Klinischer Studien DRKS00028561; https://tinyurl.com/rv4utcfb (retrospectively registered).
Keywords: symptom checkers, disposition advice, anthropomorphism, artificial intelligence, urgency assessment, patient-centered care, human-computer interaction, consumer health, information technology, IT, mobile phone
Introduction
Background
Patients are increasingly searching for health information on the web before seeking medical care [1-3]. As an alternative to commercial search engines, patient-facing decision support systems called symptom checkers were developed to provide the first access point to health-related information. These tools are targeted at laypersons and ask users to enter their signs and symptoms before presenting preliminary diagnoses and an assessment of the level of care to seek [4]. The latter assessment, the so-called disposition or urgency advice, is arguably the more important function of symptom checkers, as it could prevent unnecessary visits and direct patients toward the appropriate health care facility, thus reducing the burden on the health care system [5,6].
Related Work
Symptom checkers have mostly been investigated in terms of accuracy; user characteristics; and, occasionally, their effect on user care-seeking behavior. We will report on these findings in turn. In non–industry-funded studies, their accuracy appears to be mediocre: Semigran et al [6] found that disposition advice of apps is accurate at 57% on average, Yu et al [7] identified an accuracy between 50% and 74% for emergency cases, and Hill et al [8] found appropriate disposition advice to be provided in 49% of case evaluations on average. Although the symptom checker accuracy in these studies is mediocre, the range is very broad, and some symptom checkers perform well. For example, Ceney et al [9] found a disposition accuracy of up to 90% for a system that performs best in urgency assessment. At the health system level, evidence is still inconclusive whether symptom checkers bear the potential to make patient journeys more efficient and decrease the burden on health care services, with a study on telephone triage suggesting a redistribution rather than a reduction in health care workload [4,10-12]. Given that the current reliability of symptom checkers seems rather low on average, 2 (systematic) reviews advise against using these tools in lieu of current assessment models [13,14].
Concerning user characteristics, research has found that symptom checker users are predominantly female, more often young than old, and more often have a higher than a lower level of education [15,16]. In terms of behavioral effects, one study showed that most users plan to follow the received advice [17]. Another study by Winn et al [18] found that the perceived urgency of symptoms decreased after using a symptom checker. However, the advice given by the symptom checker was not recorded in that study, and it remains unclear whether users are more prone to lower urgency advice or whether they might have overestimated the urgency in their initial assessment. A vignette-based experimental study found that on average, symptom checkers currently do not outperform laypersons in terms of disposition accuracy. However, best-in-class apps seem superior to laypersons [19,20].
In addition to a system’s accuracy, which is well known to affect behavior, subjective trust (ie, self-reported trust in automated systems; see the study by Schaefer et al [21]) is a key factor determining whether humans follow advice from decision aids or rely on automated systems (behavioral trust). Trust in automation has been shown to be influenced by several factors, which can be divided into performance based (eg, reliability) and attribute based (eg, appearance) [21]. Although symptom checker research has so far focused on performance-based factors, studies on the influence of attribute-based factors are mostly missing. In general automation research, anthropomorphism—making the automation appear human like—has been identified as one of many potential influences [22]. There are several methods for designing human-like systems or framing them as such; however, visual anthropomorphism is the easiest to include in a symptom checker (eg, using a picture of a person on the user interface). The direction of the relationship between visual anthropomorphism and trust seems to vary. In a study by de Visser et al [23], trust was lower for anthropomorphic interfaces compared with technical systems. However, this is only the case when the system’s reliability is high. With decreasing reliability, trust decreased less steeply for the anthropomorphic system than for the technical system, suggesting a resilient influence of anthropomorphism, which could be replicated in another study [24].
In contrast, in a medical decision-making task, Pak et al [25] found that trust and follow rates, with constant reliability of 67%, were higher when the decision support system’s interface included the image of a physician. These contradictory findings might be explained by Hertz and Wiese [26], who found that people preferred assistive agents that were thought to have the greatest expertise for a specific task. For medical decision-making, health care professionals are highly trusted, and patients seem to ascribe greater expertise to physicians than to self-assessment apps, whereas in other use cases, such as analytical and computational tasks, users might find assistance from a nonhuman agent more trustworthy [1,27]. In terms of symptom checkers, anthropomorphic framing could be used to increase expertise perception because of humanization (ie, making it more human like) or technological framing (eg, artificial intelligence [AI]) because of technologization (ie, emphasizing its technological nature so that it is seen as an expert system). Indeed, some symptom checkers, such as Symptomate [28], have already emphasized using AI algorithms, which are commonly used as a buzzword for machines imitating human intelligence [29]. Although transparent communication of using AI in applications will soon be required by law [30], the design of such systems often hints at displaying AI use to enhance trust because of increased expertise perception [31]. On the basis of these findings, will showing an image of a physician in symptom checkers make them more trustworthy? Could trust also be enhanced by emphasizing that symptom checkers base their recommendations on AI?
Aim of This Study
This study aimed to examine the influence of framing effects on subjective trust in symptom checkers and the behavioral consequences of trust (ie, dependence and following behavior), which are strongly related [32]. Higher trust is particularly useful when using highly accurate symptom checkers, as patient outcomes can only be improved by following correct (and safe) advice. However, when a symptom checker does not perform well, high trust can also be dangerous in the case of incorrect advice (eg, recommending self-care while emergency care is required). Thus, our study aimed to identify potential factors influencing users’ trust in these decision aids apart from system accuracy.
As trust in physicians is generally higher than in computerized decision aids [27], we were particularly interested in assessing whether anthropomorphic framing (ie, displaying an image of a physician as a human expert decision maker on the user interface) leads to increased trust in decision aids. Furthermore, we examined whether framing the symptom checker as being based on AI increases users’ trust. We hypothesized that anthropomorphism would increase participants’ subjective trust in the app and the proportion of participants following the app’s advice (behavioral trust). We expected the same effect (higher subjective and behavioral trust) when framing the symptom checker as AI. As Winn et al [18] showed that users commonly decreased their appraised urgency level after symptom checker use, we explored whether users might be more prone to follow a symptom checker when its urgency appraisal is lower than their own. Kopka and colleagues [33] found that most laypersons are certain in their urgency assessment and that in absolute numbers, laypersons make most errors when they are certain of their appraisal. For this reason, we also examined whether users tended to accept advice from such decision aids when they were already certain of their own judgment. We expected that users’ inclination to follow a decision aid’s advice would decrease with higher decisional certainty, as users tend to rely on automation when they are not confident but solve tasks manually when they are confident [34,35]. Finally, we explored the association between demographical and other interindividual variables and trust.
Methods
Ethics Approval and Consent to Participate
This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Department of Psychology and Ergonomics (Institut für Psychologie und Arbeitswissenschaft [IPA]) at Technische Universität Berlin (tracking number: FEU_9_210315). Participants volunteered to participate in the survey, and informed consent was required. On the first page, participants were told about the investigator, the study’s purpose, what data were to be collected during the study, and where and for how long they would be stored. On the second page, participants were informed about the duration of the survey (approximately 5 minutes) and received additional information regarding the scope and use of attention checks.
Participants
Yee et al [36] found that the effect size for showing a human in an interface on subjective trust was Cohen's d=0.28. On the basis of an a priori power analysis for independent t tests with an assumed Cronbach α of .05 and a power of 1−β=0.80, we aimed to sample at least 477 (n=159, n=159, and n=159 for the 3 groups, respectively) participants to detect differences between the 3 groups (2 experimental groups and 1 control group). We expected some participants to fail attention checks (items that were embedded in the survey questions and asked participants to select a particular option, eg, “Please select Disagree”); therefore, we oversampled by 10%. To avoid participants’ decisions being influenced by their residential country or ability to understand the scenario, only US residents fluent in English were eligible. They also had to participate in the web-based questionnaire on a desktop device or tablet as the survey’s graphical elements could not be reliably displayed on smartphone devices. Another requirement was not being a medical professional (ie, nurse, paramedic, and physician). We sampled participants using Prolific [37], a platform characterized by high data quality [38], starting on Saturday, May 15, 2021, at 5 PM Eastern Daylight Time and on Sunday, May 16, 2021, at 4 PM Eastern Daylight Time. We chose these days as Casey et al [39] have shown that the samples recruited via the web are more diverse during the weekend than on working days. Following Ho et al [40], participants were compensated £0.70 (US $0.91) for their participation and received an additional £0.18 (US $0.24) as an incentive for the correct decision (ie, selecting self-care in their last appraisal) to increase data quality through attentive participation.
Design
We used a 1-factorial experimental design with factor framing and factor levels of anthropomorphic framing and framing as AI along with a control group (allocation ratio 1:1:1). These were manipulated by integrating a picture of a physician, an iconographic representation of AI similar to that displayed by Symptomate [28], or a mock company logo into the mock symptom checker’s advice screen (Figure 1). Participants were automatically randomly assigned (simple randomization) to one of these levels using a randomization tool integrated into the Unipark Enterprise Feedback Suite (EFS) Survey. In every condition, they had to appraise one and the same case vignette by deciding whether the fictitious patient required health care or self-care was sufficient. Although there are other urgency levels in symptom checkers, we chose this binary decision as the question of whether to seek care at all is the first decision patients must make [15,20]. The participants were tasked to appraise the case vignette twice: once before receiving advice from the decision aid (initial stand-alone assessment) and once after receiving the advice. The decision aid’s advice was programmed to always contradict the participant’s stand-alone assessment. The dependent variables were subjective and behavioral trust (ie, whether the participant followed the advice of the decision aid).
Materials
We chose one specific case vignette, originally used by Hill et al [8] to assess symptom checker accuracy, as it showed a high item variance; that is, participants in an unpublished pretest with 56 participants were about equally split in their appraisal of whether the fictitious patient required health care. Owing to the high item variance, this case vignette allowed us to capture the potential influence of the advice’s urgency (ie, higher or lower urgency) on users’ trust. The case vignette describes the typical presentation of fungal skin infection (tinea pedis), colloquially known as athlete’s foot: “A 33-year-old male has scaly skin between the toes. The skin is a little itchy and turns soft and white when wetted. The skin has an odour.” According to the gold standard solution for this case vignette assigned by an expert panel [8], proper self-care was sufficient for this case. Although not necessary, we would also consider it appropriate to seek professional health care for the condition, as a physician could educate the patient on proper self-care options. Hence, it is neither negligent in regarding self-care as sufficient nor is it overcautious to deem the health care required when appraising this vignette. As the decision aid always disagreed with the participants’ initial assessment, it either gave higher or lower urgency advice depending on the participant’s initial stand-alone assessment. As most symptom checkers complement their disposition recommendation with a diagnostic assessment, our decision aid named a possible diagnosis congruent with the corresponding urgency level: when providing disposition advice more acute than the participant (ie, the symptom checker recommended seeking health care), the symptom checker provided the (made-up) diagnosis of psoriasis vulgaris along with the text, “These symptoms normally require medical evaluation, please see a healthcare professional!” For lower urgency, it returned the (original) diagnostic suggestion of tinea pedis along with the text, “These symptoms can normally be managed at home, self-care is sufficient!” The wording is based on a screening of different symptom checkers and represents a symbiosis between the advice given by Symptomate [28] and Ada [41].
As a decision aid, we created a mock app with a simple result presentation screen using PowerPoint (Microsoft Corporation) [42], Affinity Photo (Serif Ltd) [43], and Vectornator (Linearity) [44]. Participants could not interact with the decision aid to input information as not everyone would have entered the symptoms in the same way, and thus, the decision path would have differed. As this interaction influences trust [45], we tried to eliminate any resulting bias by presenting a results screen only. This design was inspired by Pak et al [25], who assessed anthropomorphism in a decision support system for diabetics using the picture of a physician. We designed our interfaces to include the same diagnoses and disposition advice with a picture of a mock symptom checker logo, a physician, or an icon representing AI (Figure 1). To ensure that the decision aid was displayed in the same way for all participants and that the results were not biased by different presentations on different phones, we placed the interface directly in a mock phone. The simulated phone could then be viewed on a computer or tablet. For the anthropomorphic condition, we chose the depiction of a young male physician based on the findings of a study by Pak et al [46], who found the depictions of a male physician embedded in a decision aid less susceptible to fluctuations in perceptions of trust as a function of the decision aid’s reliability, and depictions of younger agents exhibited fewer age differences in perceived trust than older agents.
Framing manipulation corresponds to the actual visual framing in a widely used symptom checker [28]. Although framing can be manipulated to a greater extent (eg, by presenting videos and stories [23]), we decided to use a picture only to represent currently applied practice. Therefore, the extent of our manipulation is similar to that of other studies that assessed the effects of anthropomorphism on trust in decision aids [25,47].
Survey
A web-based survey was developed using Unipark EFS Survey [48]. All collected data were saved on the platform, and only the authors of this study had access to the data. We evaluated the usability and technical functionality of the questionnaire and then conducted a pilot study in which test participants were asked to provide feedback on any display problems, unclear questions or statements, or other issues that might have occurred. After these were resolved, the questionnaire was rolled out as a voluntary, open survey that was only accessible via the Prolific recruitment platform (initial contact). We did not advertise the survey in any other way than presenting it on the platform. Participants were presented with 1 questionnaire on each page; hence, the items per page ranged from 1 to 13 on a sum of 19 pages. They could return using browser buttons and review their answers, which were checked for completeness using the built-in function of the Unipark EFS Survey. Although the symptom checker interfaces were adapted to the participant’s responses (see the Design section and the Materials section), we did not use adaptive questioning to reduce the number of questions.
Survey visitor numbers were assessed by assigning participants an ID when opening the questionnaire. Most participants accessing the survey completed it (completion rate: 572/607, 94.2%).
Dependent Measures
Subjective trust in the symptom checker app (primary outcome) was measured by adapting the Trust in Automated Systems Survey [49], which uses a 7-point Likert scale with 12 items; as suggested by Gutzwiller et al [50], we randomized the order in which items were presented to avoid a positive bias. Behavioral trust (secondary outcome) was measured using an adapted TNO trust task [51], as previously reported by several authors [23,24,47,52]. First, the participants had to rate the appropriate urgency level on their own. Afterward, they were shown the symptom checker app’s contradicting recommendation and had to make a final decision. We measured whether they changed their decision in favor of the decision aid’s advice and coded behavioral trust at the individual level as Boolean (true or false). We then determined the proportion of participants following the advice (follow rates) as a measure of behavioral trust at the group level.
Procedure
After participants gave consent to participate, we surveyed their age, gender, educational background, and prior medical training. Next, participants were asked about their propensity to trust using the Propensity to Trust in Technology Scale with 6 items on a 5-point Likert scale [53] and their eHealth literacy using the eHealth Literacy Scale with 8 items on a 5-point Likert scale [54].
Afterward, the 2 urgency levels (health care and self-care) between which the participants had to choose when appraising the fictitious case vignette were explained, and participants’ understanding of these definitions was assured with multiple-choice questions (3 rewordings of the urgency level definitions to which participants had to assign the correct urgency level). Next, they were shown the case vignette, and they appraised its urgency and rated their decisional certainty using a visual analogue scale with values from 0 (minimum certainty) to 100 (maximum certainty). They then saw the results screen of the mock decision aid advising the opposite of their assessment with 1 of the 3 different designs (Figure 1) and had to make a final decision on the urgency level and again state their decisional certainty.
Thereafter, they were presented with the Trust in Automated Systems Survey and had the opportunity to provide feedback or any other comments in an open text field. Finally, to ensure that our intervention was successful, participants were asked to specify which image was embedded in the decision aid presented to them previously (manipulation check).
Data Analysis
We cleaned and analyzed the data using base R (version 4.0.5) [55], the tidyverse packages [56], and aod [57]. For inferential analysis of continuous outcomes, we used a 1-way between-subjects ANOVA. For binary outcomes, we used dummy-coded binomial logistic regression and tested the coefficients using Wald chi-square tests. To test demographic and interindividual influences, we used multiple linear regression and multiple binomial logistic regression with standardized coefficients for better comparability. The effect coding scheme and results can be found in Multimedia Appendices 1-6. We used an effect coding scheme to compare each factor level to the mean of all factor levels. Thus, deviations from the mean can be quantified and tested for significance instead of performing group comparisons with a single, consistent reference category (as in dummy coding). For example, the coefficients and P values of gender 1 (Multimedia Appendix 1) represent the differences and significance tests of women compared with the mean of other genders. Similarly, the metrics of education 1 (Multimedia Appendix 2) represent differences between participants with a bachelor’s degree compared with the mean of all other education levels. Finally, we conducted sensitivity power analyses using the R package pwr [58] to estimate the population effect size for selected results that appeared statistically nonsignificant.
Results
Participant Characteristics
The survey was completed in 6 minutes and 19 seconds (Median, IQR 4 minutes, 36 seconds to 8 minutes, 46 seconds). Of the 607 individuals accessing the survey, 35 (5.8%) did not finish the questionnaire, 14 (2.3%) were excluded as they were trained medical professionals, 27 (4.4%) were excluded as they took part on a mobile phone, and 37 (6.1%) failed at least one of the attention checks. Therefore, of the 607 individuals, the total sample size was 494 (81.4%). Distributions of age, gender, level of education, propensity to trust, and eHealth literacy overall and in each of the 3 groups are reported in Table 1.
Table 1.
Characteristics | Control group (n=173) | Anthropomorphic (n=160) | Artificial intelligence (n=161) | Total | |||||
Age (years), mean (SD) | 34.5 (13.8) | 32.1 (12.5) | 31.6 (12.2) | 32.8 (12.9) | |||||
Gender, n (%) | |||||||||
|
Female | 81 (46.8) | 78 (48.8) | 77 (47.8) | 236 (47.8) | ||||
|
Male | 87 (50.3) | 80 (50) | 82 (50.9) | 249 (50.4) | ||||
|
Other | 5 (2.9) | 2 (1.3) | 2 (1.2) | 9 (1.8) | ||||
Education, n (%) | |||||||||
|
Less than high school | 0 (0) | 4 (2.5) | 3 (1.9) | 7 (1.4) | ||||
|
High school graduate | 25 (14.5) | 12 (7.5) | 20 (12.4) | 57 (11.5) | ||||
|
College or associate degree | 48 (27.7) | 50 (31.3) | 63 (39.1) | 161 (32.6) | ||||
|
Bachelor degree | 66 (38.2) | 66 (41.3) | 52 (32.3) | 184 (37.2) | ||||
|
Graduate degree or higher | 34 (19.7) | 28 (17.5) | 23 (14.3) | 85 (17.2) | ||||
Prior medical training, n (%) | |||||||||
|
No training | 141 (81.5) | 135 (84.4) | 136 (84.3) | 412 (83.4) | ||||
|
Basic first aid | 32 (18.5) | 25 (15.6) | 25 (15.5) | 82 (16.6) | ||||
Propensity to Trust scorea, mean (SD) | 4.1 (0.5) | 4.1 (0.5) | 4.0 (0.5) | 4.1 (0.5) | |||||
eHEALSb scorec, mean (SD) | 30.5 (4.91) | 30.0 (5.27) | 30.1 (5.61) | 30.2 (5.25) | |||||
Initial assessment of the case vignette, n (%) | |||||||||
|
Health care | 60 (34.7) | 62 (38.8) | 65 (40.4) | 187 (37.9) | ||||
Self-care | 113 (65.3) | 98 (61.3) | 96 (59.6) | 307 (62.1) | |||||
Completion time (minutes), median (IQR) | 6:32 (4:32-8:53) | 6:09 (4:39-8:17) | 6:18 (4:38-9:16) | 6:19 (4:36-8:46) |
aPropensity to Trust refers to the Propensity to Trust in Technology Scale, and possible scores range from 1 (low) to 5 (high).
beHEALS: eHealth Literacy Scale.
cPossible scores range from 8 (low) to 40 (high).
Almost all participants (480/494, 97.2%) correctly recollected the image embedded in the mock decision aid (manipulation check).
Influence of Framing on Participants’ Trust
Subjective Trust
Descriptively, trust in all 3 framing conditions was very similar (anthropomorphic: mean 4.503, SD 0.922; AI: mean 4.495, SD 0.817; control: mean 4.508, SD 0.921; Figure 2). Framing had no significant effect on subjective trust (F2,491=0.009; P=.99; η2=0.00). On the basis of a sensitivity power analysis (α=.05; 1−β=0.80; anthropomorphic n=160, AI n=173, and control n=161), we estimated the effect size of possible differences between the groups to not be greater than η2=0.018.
Behavioral Trust
Most participants followed the decision aid’s advice and changed their urgency appraisal (384/494, 77.7%). Behavioral trust was slightly higher for the anthropomorphic system (127/160, 79.4%) than for the control group (134/173, 77.5%); however, the difference (odds ratio [OR] 1.120, 95% CI 0.664-1.897) was not statistically significant (χ21=0.2; P=.67).
Behavioral trust was slightly lower for the AI system (123/161, 76.4%) than for the control group; however, the difference (OR 0.942, 95% CI 0.565-1.570) was not statistically significant (χ21=0.053; P=.82), either.
Influence of the Urgency Level Provided by the Decision Aid on Trust
We observed no differences in subjective trust between participants receiving advice of greater urgency (health care required) than their stand-alone initial assessment (self-care sufficient) (mean 4.5, SD 0.919) and those receiving less urgent advice (mean 4.5, SD 0.869). Concerning behavioral trust, the proportion of participants who followed more urgent advice was slightly lower (235/307, 76.5%) than the proportion who followed advice of lower urgency than their own initial stand-alone assessment (149/187, 79.7%).
Influence of Participants’ Decisional Certainty on Trust
The participants of all 3 groups were certain about their initial stand-alone assessment (median 70, IQR 60-81). Only 12.8% (63/494) were unsure (ie, indicating a certainty of <50% about their appraisal). No differences in patterns were observed between the framing conditions.
Participants’ certainty in their initial assessment was not associated with subjective trust in the decision aids (R2=0.001; Figure 3). With increasing decisional certainty, behavioral trust decreased (OR 0.966, 95% CI 0.952-0.979; χ21=25.0; P<.001; McFadden R2=0.055). However, behavioral trust was high and remained >50% (19/34, 56%), even for participants indicating maximum decision certainty (100/100; Figure 4). There were no differences between the framing conditions.
Demographic and Interindividual Influences on Trust
Neither demographic variables (age, gender, and education) nor basic first aid training was associated with subjective and behavioral trust in the symptom checker when controlling for the other variables. However, an individual’s propensity for trust and eHealth literacy increased subjective trust and were statistically significant (P<.001). However, these 2 variables did not have a statistically significant influence on behavioral trust (Multimedia Appendices 4 and 6).
Discussion
Principal Findings
Effect of Anthropomorphic or AI Framing
The aim of this study was to explore the factors influencing laypersons’ subjective and behavioral trust in symptom checkers. In particular, we examined the hypothesis that the common features of symptom checker interfaces that frame the system as either AI-based or anthropomorphic affect users’ trust in these systems. Our analysis does not support this hypothesis: we could not observe a difference in trust—neither subjective nor behavioral—between a neutral symptom checker interface (showing a mock company logo) and interfaces framed as either anthropomorphic or as using AI. This is in contrast to previous findings from other domains where anthropomorphism led to an increase or decrease in trust [23-25]. In addition, we expected that designing anthropomorphic decision aids in a clinical context would yield higher trust as symptom checker users trust physicians more than self-assessment apps [15,26,27,59]. However, our participants’ trust was unaffected by how the symptom checker was framed, indicating that users seem to perceive a symptom checker mainly as an app, regardless of whether a depiction of a physician or an AI icon is included. We did not find an effect of framing on subjective and behavioral trust in our study; however, we cannot rule out that framing might influence other variables in the trust formation process. For example, anthropomorphism has been shown to moderate the relationship between reliability and trust [23,24]: framing might moderate the impact of a decision aid’s reliability on trust; however, it might not be sufficient to build trust (in medical advice) on its own. Instead, other factors, such as explanations of the reasoning underlying the symptom checker’s advice, might help build trust more effectively [60].
Persuasive Power of Symptom Checker Apps
We found that most participants (384/494, 77.7%) followed the decision aid’s advice. This is in line with Verzantvoort et al [17], who reported a high intention of users to follow dispositional advice from a decision aid (65%). However, both findings stand in contrast to those indicating a low behavioral trust in symptom checkers; for example, Meyer et al [15] reported that only a minority of those advised by a symptom checker to visit the emergency department followed this advice, and Miller et al [61] found that most patients presenting to a primary care clinic stick to their stand-alone assessment when using a symptom checker in a primary care clinic’s waiting room. Taken together, these findings hint at symptom checker users’ behavioral trust being a function of the exact urgency decision and context of use: when users are undecided between seeking emergency or nonemergency care, they might depend less on the symptom checker’s advice compared with when choosing whether professional medical care is required at all or self-care is appropriate. Users might also be more inclined to accept guiding advice from symptom checker apps before arriving at a health care facility. Interestingly, a web search seems to change only few people's urgency level [62]; this indicates a difference in advice-taking between symptom checker use and general web search.
Another influencing factor on behavioral trust is decisional certainty; that is, whether users follow a symptom checker’s advice depends on how certain they are of their own stand-alone assessment. However, our findings hint at the high persuasive power of symptom checkers: although participants indicating maximum certainty in their own stand-alone assessment followed the advice less often than those indicating lower levels of certainty, most still changed their decision according to the decision aid’s recommendation. This finding is central as it emphasizes the impact symptom checkers may have on the urgency decision. Symptom checkers could not only assist when patients are uncertain whether and where to seek health care but also convince those who (wrongly) are very certain in their appraisal. This might prove very useful, as Kopka and colleagues [33] report that laypersons’ urgency errors are most frequent when they indicate high confidence in their stand-alone appraisal. In contrast, high dependence on symptom checkers potentially signifies laypersons using them as a replacement for decision-making rather than as a decision aid. This should be further investigated through research on the cognitive and metacognitive mechanisms with which laypersons monitor their own reasoning when confronted with advice from symptom checkers, similar to the Jussupow et al [63] study on an AI-based decision aid supporting physicians in diagnostic decisions. In addition, laypersons’ high dependence on symptom checkers emphasizes the need for a framework to identify and label those apps defying the general trend by proving them to be both accurate and safe to use, as currently, symptom checkers’ accuracy is being reported as mediocre in general, with only a few performing well [6-9,14].
Interindividual Variables' Effect on Trust in Symptom Checkers
A previous study indicated gender differences in appraising medical situations (eg, Cooper and Humphrey [64] showed that female participants assessed their urgency as more risk averse); however, we could not replicate this finding for trust. Our findings suggest that demographic and interindividual differences might be negligible when drafting recommendations on whether and how symptom checker apps should be designed. Although users who are generally more inclined to trust show higher subjective trust in symptom checkers, they do not seem to follow their advice more often, which might have methodological reasons; that is, the item terminology referencing technology too broadly [53,65]. Concerning the influence of eHealth literacy on trust, we observed that it increased subjective trust but not behavioral trust. Users with higher eHealth literacy might have more knowledge about eHealth applications and thus be more open to receiving advice from a decision aid while at the same time being more able to integrate a decision aid’s advice into their own decision-making rather than uncritically adopting the presented advice.
Limitations
First, the intervention might not have been effective in producing meaningful differences. However, nearly all participants (480/494, 97.2%) were able to recall the picture they were presented with as part of the decision aid, thus proving that they took note of the depictions used for framing. Moreover, the results remained consistent, even if participants who could not recall the presented picture were excluded from the analysis. The framing itself represents another limitation. Although we followed the current practice and manipulation extent of previous studies, other interface and framing aspects are conceivable that may not have been captured in this study. For example, it would be interesting to assess whether personalized images (eg, patients’ own physicians) could increase their trust.
In our study, participants did not interact with the decision aid as we only presented a symptom checker’s results screen instead of letting them enter the data or symptoms into an actual app. This was done to keep the survey short, avoid dropout when entering symptoms for a longer period, and avoid introducing any bias because of different algorithmic pathways resulting from participants unreliably entering information, which is a nonneglible risk, as shown by Jungmann et al [66]. As trust could be influenced by user experience throughout the interaction [45], we could not account for a potentially moderating role of that factor. This limitation applies equally to all experimental groups; thus, internal validity is not compromised. However, as symptom checkers commonly require extensive user interaction over a span of multiple minutes [67], their ecological validity might be limited. Future research should alter the existing symptom checkers to test whether our results can be replicated in practice. Our participants also only evaluated a single case vignette, whereas, in other studies, participants solved as many as 20 with the help of a symptom checker app. Hence, the duration of exposure to the intervention was low in our study. However, we consider this closer to the real use case of symptom checkers, where users seek advice on a single set of complaints rather than systematically testing the app by iteratively entering the signs and symptoms of highly heterogeneous fictitious patient descriptions. However, unlike in the real use case, participants could not change their decision at a later stage. In practice, they might decide to see a health care professional after gathering further evidence, even if they decided for self-care to be sufficient when using a symptom checker. Thus, our concept of behavioral trust only captures users’ intentions after consulting a symptom checker, not their actual behavior (ie, [not] seeking health care according to the symptom checker’s prompt).
All participants appraised only a single case vignette, which was the same across all 3 groups. We used only this specific case vignette as it has been used in previous studies and was ambiguous enough for patients to choose both self-care and health care. However, technically, many other vignettes and symptoms can be entered and should thus be investigated in the future. The gold standard for the case vignette used in this study is self-care; however, visiting a health care professional with these symptoms is not inappropriate and, in particular, not unsafe. Thus, deviation from the gold standard solution may be considered wrong but not consequential. Although the gold standard solution was assigned by a panel of physicians, the idea of absolute correct urgency may vary for different physicians. It would be interesting to see whether our findings can be replicated for a variety of cases with different gold standard urgency levels (eg, 3-tiered or 4-tiered urgency levels). Other decisions, such as whether emergency care is required, should also be examined, as this study could not provide any evidence for other urgency-related decisions. Especially concerning the decision of whether emergency care is required, we consider a further investigation into the question of whether layperson trust is unaffected by the direction of the (contradicting) advice by a decision aid worthwhile, as here, an incorrect appraisal is more consequential.
It cannot be ruled out that some participants researched the correct solution on the web to obtain a bonus. However, as this could have occurred in all groups, internal validity should not be impaired.
Participants did not assess their own symptoms but were presented with a fictitious case vignette as a proxy for a medical case. Although this arguably reductionist approach is commonly applied when evaluating symptom checkers [6,8,68,69], it remains unclear whether participants assess these symptoms in the same way they do when experiencing them. For example, in the case of real symptoms, not only might the information input change, but the patients’ mental well-being and their perceived self-efficacy in implementing an action might also have an impact. It is also conceivable that participants might not have empathized enough with the situation or that the urgency was assessed differently. However, web-based health information sources are commonly used to assess the symptoms of others [3]; thus, this use case still possesses a high degree of external validity.
As we only collected quantitative data, we could not explain why the participants changed their decisions. Future studies should conduct qualitative studies on decision-making when assisted by a symptom checker.
Finally, the participants in this study were well-educated, with 54.5% (269/494) of participants having a bachelor’s degree or higher. Although our sample is not representative of the US population, the average education level is very close to that of symptom checker users [15]. The same applies to our participants’ average age, which is very close to that of users [16] and had no impact on our exploratory analyses.
Practical Implications
Although some developers frame their symptom checkers as anthropomorphic or as an AI, there appears to be no meaningful impact on users’ trust based on our study. Although previous studies found an influence of anthropomorphism on trust in general automation [23-25], we could not extend these findings to symptom checkers. As we kept this study as true to reality as possible—by specifically using a mock symptom checker instead of other decision aids used in experimental laboratory setups and by testing an externally valid use case where users only assessed a single case vignette and could not estimate symptom checker accuracy—our results are more applicable to the specific use case of symptom checkers. The effect found by other authors presumably materializes only when users can assess a system’s accuracy. As multiple assessments in a row do not correspond to the natural use of symptom checkers, framing (as currently applied) does not seem to provide any benefit in terms of trust.
Although sociodemographic factors appear to have an impact on symptom checker use [15,16], they do not seem to alter trust. Thus, trust depends on eHealth literacy; for example, symptom checkers do not need to be customized for age, gender, or education to increase trust, although they might be customized to increase usability and user experience.
Finally, as initial trust is very high, regardless of framing and demographic factors, further increasing users’ trust in these systems may not be a priority. Instead, we suggest that it may be more worthwhile to explore ways of supporting users in their decision-making so that they do not have to rely uncritically on a symptom checker’s advice. For example, this can be achieved by providing explanations of disposition advice tailored to the individual user [60].
Conclusions
The subjective and behavioral trust of laypersons in clinical decision aids is high and is not influenced by framing such systems as anthropomorphic or using AI.
However, users are inclined to change their minds based on the symptom checker’s advice, even when they report maximum certainty in their initial and contradicting stand-alone appraisal. This indicates the high persuasive power of the symptom checker and thus demonstrates its potential to make patient journeys more efficient. At the same time, our findings hint at the danger that laypersons may use symptom checkers to substitute rather than to assist their own decision-making. Although some symptom checkers commonly provide accurate and safe advice, the range of symptom checker accuracy varies widely. Thus, before recommending symptom checkers for general use, rigorous standards for evaluating symptom checkers must be defined to ensure that only those symptom checkers are recommended that are accurate and safe enough to be worthy of the trust people have in them. Further research should investigate how to ensure that symptom checkers function as aids rather than replacements in laypersons’ decision-making.
Acknowledgments
The authors express their gratitude to the participants and Sami Gaber for the portrait pictures. This project was funded by the home institutions of the last authors (MAF and FB). The authors acknowledge the support from the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft [DFG]) and the Open Access Publication Fund of Charité, Universitätsmedizin Berlin.
Abbreviations
- AI
artificial intelligence
- EFS
Enterprise Feedback Suite
- OR
odds ratio
Effect coding scheme of gender.
Effect coding scheme of education.
Multiple linear regression of demographic and interindividual influences on subjective trust with standardized coefficients.
Multiple binomial logistic regression of demographic and interindividual influences on behavioral trust with standardized coefficients.
Multiple linear regression of demographic and interindividual influences on subjective trust with unstandardized coefficients.
Multiple binomial logistic regression of demographic and interindividual influences on behavioral trust with unstandardized coefficients.
CONSORT-eHEALTH checklist (V 1.6.1).
Footnotes
Authors' Contributions: MK and MLS conceived the study. MK created the questionnaire and interfaces, designed and conducted the analyses, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. MLS assisted in questionnaire creation and data analysis and worked on manuscript development. ER, TR, FB, and MAF provided critical input and advice on the study and questionnaire design, analysis methods, and drafts of the paper. All authors accept full responsibility for the final version of this manuscript.
Conflicts of Interest: All authors have completed the International Committee of Medical Journal Editors uniform disclosure form and declare no support from any organization for the submitted work, no financial relationships with any organizations that might have an interest in the submitted work in the previous 3 years, and no other relationships or activities that could appear to have influenced the submitted work. The lead author affirms that this manuscript is an honest, accurate, and transparent account of the study being reported; that no important aspects of the study have been omitted; and that any discrepancies from the study as planned (and, if relevant, registered) have been explained.
Editorial Notice
This randomized study was only retrospectively registered, given that the authors believed registration was unnecessary for this kind of trial and the particular outcomes which are measured. The editor granted an exception from ICMJE rules mandating prospective registration of randomized trials. However, readers are advised to carefully assess the validity of any potential explicit or implicit claims related to primary outcomes or effectiveness, as retrospective registration does not prevent authors from changing their outcome measures retrospectively.
References
- 1.Baumann E, Czerwinski F, Rosset M, Seelig M, Suhr R. Wie informieren sich die Menschen in Deutschland zum Thema Gesundheit? Erkenntnisse aus der ersten Welle von HINTS Germany. Bundesgesundheitsblatt Gesundheitsforschung Gesundheitsschutz. 2020 Sep;63(9):1151–60. doi: 10.1007/s00103-020-03192-x.10.1007/s00103-020-03192-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Cocco AM, Zordan R, Taylor DM, Weiland TJ, Dilley SJ, Kant J, Dombagolla M, Hendarto A, Lai F, Hutton J. Dr Google in the ED: searching for online health information by adult emergency department patients. Med J Aust. 2018 Oct 15;209(8):342–7. doi: 10.5694/mja17.00889.10.5694/mja17.00889 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Fox S, Duggan M. Health Online 2013. Pew Internet & American Life Project. 2013. Jan 15, [2021-07-13]. https://www.pewinternet.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/9/media/Files/Reports/PIP_HealthOnline.pdf .
- 4.Chambers D, Cantrell AJ, Johnson M, Preston L, Baxter SK, Booth A, Turner J. Digital and online symptom checkers and health assessment/triage services for urgent health problems: systematic review. BMJ Open. 2019 Aug 01;9(8):e027743. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-027743. https://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=31375610 .bmjopen-2018-027743 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Millenson ML, Baldwin JL, Zipperer L, Singh H. Beyond Dr. Google: the evidence on consumer-facing digital tools for diagnosis. Diagnosis (Berl) 2018 Sep 25;5(3):95–105. doi: 10.1515/dx-2018-0009. https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/dx-2018-0009 .dx-2018-0009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Semigran HL, Linder JA, Gidengil C, Mehrotra A. Evaluation of symptom checkers for self diagnosis and triage: audit study. BMJ. 2015 Jul 08;351:h3480. doi: 10.1136/bmj.h3480. http://www.bmj.com/lookup/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=26157077 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Yu SW, Ma A, Tsang VH, Chung LS, Leung SC, Leung LP. Triage accuracy of online symptom checkers for Accident and Emergency Department patients. Hong Kong J Emerg Med. 2020 Jul 1;27(4):217–22. doi: 10.1177/1024907919842486. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hill MG, Sim M, Mills B. The quality of diagnosis and triage advice provided by free online symptom checkers and apps in Australia. Med J Aust. 2020 Jun;212(11):514–9. doi: 10.5694/mja2.50600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ceney A, Tolond S, Glowinski A, Marks B, Swift S, Palser T. Accuracy of online symptom checkers and the potential impact on service utilisation. PLoS One. 2021 Jul 16;16(7):e0254088. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0254088. https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254088 .PONE-D-20-21627 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Campbell JL, Fletcher E, Britten N, Green C, Holt T, Lattimer V, Richards DA, Richards SH, Salisbury C, Taylor RS, Calitri R, Bowyer V, Chaplin K, Kandiyali R, Murdoch J, Price L, Roscoe J, Varley A, Warren FC. The clinical effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of telephone triage for managing same-day consultation requests in general practice: a cluster randomised controlled trial comparing general practitioner-led and nurse-led management systems with usual care (the ESTEEM trial) Health Technol Assess. 2015 Feb;19(13):1–212. doi: 10.3310/hta19130. doi: 10.3310/hta19130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Donovan E, Wilcox CR, Patel S, Hay AD, Little P, Willcox ML. Digital interventions for parents of acutely ill children and their treatment-seeking behaviour: a systematic review. Br J Gen Pract. 2020 Mar;70(692):e172–8. doi: 10.3399/bjgp20X708209. https://bjgp.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=32041764 .bjgp20X708209 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Anhang Price R, Fagbuyi D, Harris R, Hanfling D, Place F, Taylor TB, Kellermann AL. Feasibility of Web-based self-triage by parents of children with influenza-like illness: a cautionary tale. JAMA Pediatr. 2013 Feb;167(2):112–8. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.1573.1485650 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Wallace W, Chan C, Chidambaram S, Hanna L, Iqbal FM, Acharya A, Normahani P, Ashrafian H, Markar SR, Sounderajah V, Darzi A. The diagnostic and triage accuracy of digital and online symptom checker tools: a systematic review. medRxiv (forthcoming) 2021 Dec 21; doi: 10.1101/2021.12.21.21268167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Gottliebsen K, Petersson G. Limited evidence of benefits of patient operated intelligent primary care triage tools: findings of a literature review. BMJ Health Care Inform. 2020 May;27(1):e100114. doi: 10.1136/bmjhci-2019-100114. https://informatics.bmj.com/lookup/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=32385041 .bmjhci-2019-100114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Meyer AN, Giardina TD, Spitzmueller C, Shahid U, Scott TM, Singh H. Patient perspectives on the usefulness of an artificial intelligence-assisted symptom checker: cross-sectional survey study. J Med Internet Res. 2020 Jan 30;22(1):e14679. doi: 10.2196/14679. https://www.jmir.org/2020/1/e14679/ v22i1e14679 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Morse KE, Ostberg NP, Jones VG, Chan AS. Use characteristics and triage acuity of a digital symptom checker in a large integrated health system: population-based descriptive study. J Med Internet Res. 2020 Nov 30;22(11):e20549. doi: 10.2196/20549. https://www.jmir.org/2020/11/e20549/ v22i11e20549 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Verzantvoort NC, Teunis T, Verheij TJ, van der Velden AW. Self-triage for acute primary care via a smartphone application: practical, safe and efficient? PLoS One. 2018 Jun 26;13(6):e0199284. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0199284. https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199284 .PONE-D-17-00522 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Winn AN, Somai M, Fergestrom N, Crotty BH. Association of use of online symptom checkers with patients' plans for seeking care. JAMA Netw Open. 2019 Dec 02;2(12):e1918561. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.18561. https://jamanetwork.com/journals/jamanetworkopen/fullarticle/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.18561 .2757995 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Schmieding ML, Mörgeli R, Schmieding MA, Feufel MA, Balzer F. Benchmarking triage capability of symptom checkers against that of medical laypersons: survey study. J Med Internet Res. 2021 Mar 10;23(3):e24475. doi: 10.2196/24475. https://www.jmir.org/2021/3/e24475/ v23i3e24475 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Schmieding ML, Kopka M, Schmidt K, Schulz-Niethammer S, Balzer F, Feufel MA. Triage accuracy of symptom checker apps: five-year follow-up evaluation. J Med Internet Res (forthcoming) 2021 doi: 10.2196/31810. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Schaefer KE, Chen JY, Szalma JL, Hancock PA. A meta-analysis of factors influencing the development of trust in automation: implications for understanding autonomy in future systems. Hum Factors. 2016 May;58(3):377–400. doi: 10.1177/0018720816634228.0018720816634228 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hoff KA, Bashir M. Trust in automation: integrating empirical evidence on factors that influence trust. Hum Factors. 2015 May;57(3):407–34. doi: 10.1177/0018720814547570.0018720814547570 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.de Visser EJ, Krueger F, McKnight P, Scheid S, Smith M, Chalk S, Parasuraman R. The world is not enough: trust in Cognitive agents. Proc Hum Factors Ergon Soc Annu Meet. 2012 Sep 01;56(1):263–7. doi: 10.1177/1071181312561062. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.de Visser EJ, Monfort SS, McKendrick R, Smith MA, McKnight PE, Krueger F, Parasuraman R. Almost human: anthropomorphism increases trust resilience in cognitive agents. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2016 Sep;22(3):331–49. doi: 10.1037/xap0000092.2016-37452-001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Pak R, Fink N, Price M, Bass B, Sturre L. Decision support aids with anthropomorphic characteristics influence trust and performance in younger and older adults. Ergonomics. 2012;55(9):1059–72. doi: 10.1080/00140139.2012.691554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Hertz N, Wiese E. Good advice is beyond all price, but what if it comes from a machine? J Exp Psychol Appl. 2019 Sep;25(3):386–95. doi: 10.1037/xap0000205.2019-04354-001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Baldauf M, Fröehlich P, Endl R. Trust Me, I’m a Doctor – user perceptions of AI-driven apps for mobile health diagnosis. Proceedings of the 19th International Conference on Mobile and Ubiquitous Multimedia; MUM '20; November 22-25, 2020; Essen, Germany. 2020. Nov, pp. 167–78. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Symptomate - Check Your Symptoms Online. Symptomate. 2021. [2021-03-26]. https://symptomate.com/
- 29.Barragán-Montero A, Javaid U, Valdés G, Nguyen D, Desbordes P, Macq B, Willems S, Vandewinckele L, Holmström M, Löfman F, Michiels S, Souris K, Sterpin E, Lee JA. Artificial intelligence and machine learning for medical imaging: a technology review. Phys Med. 2021 Mar;83:242–56. doi: 10.1016/j.ejmp.2021.04.016.S1120-1797(21)00173-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Proposal for a regulation of the European parliament and of the council laying down harmonised rules on artificial intelligence (artificial intelligence act) and amending certain union legislative acts. European Commission. 2021. Apr 21, [2021-12-01]. https://eur-lex.europa.eu/legal-content/EN/TXT/?qid=1623335154975&uri=CELEX%3A52021PC0206 .
- 31.Lerch FJ, Prietula MJ, Kulik CT. The Turing effect: the nature of trust in expert systems advice. In: Feltovich PJ, Ford KM, Hoffman R, editors. Expertise in Context: Human and Machine. Cambridge, MA, USA: MIT Press; 1997. May, pp. 417–48. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Parasuraman R, Sheridan TB, Wickens CD. Situation awareness, mental workload, and trust in automation: viable, empirically supported cognitive engineering constructs. J Cogn Eng Decis Mak. 2008 Jun 01;2(2):140–60. doi: 10.1518/155534308x284417. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kopka M, Balzer F, Feufel MA, Schmieding ML. Triage Capability of Laypersons: Retrospective Exploratory Analysis. JMIR Human Factors (forthcoming) 2022 doi: 10.2196/38977. https://preprints.jmir.org/preprint/38977 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lee J, Moray N. Trust, control strategies and allocation of function in human-machine systems. Ergonomics. 1992 Oct;35(10):1243–70. doi: 10.1080/00140139208967392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Lee JD, Moray N. Trust, self-confidence, and operators' adaptation to automation. Int J Hum Comput Stud. 1994 Jan;40(1):153–84. doi: 10.1006/ijhc.1994.1007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Yee N, Bailenson JN, Rickertsen K. A meta-analysis of the impact of the inclusion and realism of human-like faces on user experiences in interfaces. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems; CHI '07; April 28-May 3, 2007; San Jose, CA, USA. 2007. Apr, pp. 1–10. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Online participant recruitment for surveys and market research. Prolific. 2021. [2021-04-09]. https://www.prolific.co/
- 38.Peer E, Brandimarte L, Samat S, Acquisti A. Beyond the Turk: alternative platforms for crowdsourcing behavioral research. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2017 May;70:153–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.01.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Casey LS, Chandler J, Levine AS, Proctor A, Strolovitch DZ. Intertemporal differences among MTurk workers: time-based sample variations and implications for online data collection. SAGE Open. 2017 Jun 14;7(2):215824401771277. doi: 10.1177/2158244017712774. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Ho CJ, Slivkins A, Suri S, Vaughan JW. Incentivizing high quality crowdwork. ACM SIGecom Exch. 2015 Dec;14(2):26–34. doi: 10.1145/2904104.2904108. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Ada Health Ada - Check Your Health. App Store. 2021. [2020-12-23]. https://apps.apple.com/app/id1099986434 .
- 42.Microsoft PowerPoint (Version 2019) Microsoft Corporation. 2021. [2021-04-04]. https://www.microsoft.com/de-de/microsoft-365/powerpoint .
- 43.Serif Labs Affinity Photo (Version 1.9.2) App Store. 2021. [2021-04-04]. https://apps.apple.com/de/app/affinity-photo/id824183456?mt=12 .
- 44.Linearity Vectornator - Grafikdesign (Version 4.0.1) App Store. 2021. [2021-04-04]. https://apps.apple.com/de/app/vectornator-grafikdesign/id1219074514 .
- 45.Salanitri D, Hare C, Borsci S, Lawson G, Sharples S, Waterfield B. Relationship between trust and usability in virtual environments: an ongoing study. Proceedings of the 17th International Conference on Human-Computer Interaction: Design and Evaluation; HCI '15; August 2-7, 2015; Los Angeles, CA, USA. 2015. pp. 49–59. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Pak R, McLaughlin AC, Bass B. A multi-level analysis of the effects of age and gender stereotypes on trust in anthropomorphic technology by younger and older adults. Ergonomics. 2014;57(9):1277–89. doi: 10.1080/00140139.2014.928750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Zou X, Lv C, Zhang J. The effect of group membership, system reliability and anthropomorphic appearance on user’s trust in intelligent decision support system. Proceedings of the 17th International Conference on Human-Computer Interaction: Design and Evaluation; HCII '20; July 19-24, 2020; Copenhagen, Denmark. 2020. Jul, pp. 231–42. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Questback Umfragesoftware für Studierende und Wissenschaftler. Unipark. 2021. [2021-04-04]. https://www.unipark.com/umfragesoftware/
- 49.Jian JY, Bisantz AM, Drury CG. Foundations for an empirically determined scale of trust in automated systems. Int J Cogn Ergon. 2000 Mar;4(1):53–71. doi: 10.1207/S15327566IJCE0401_04. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Gutzwiller RS, Chiou EK, Craig SD, Lewis CM, Lematta GJ, Hsiung CP. Positive bias in the ‘Trust in Automated Systems Survey’? An examination of the Jian et al. (2000) scale. Proc Hum Factors Ergon Soc Annu Meet. 2019 Nov 20;63(1):217–21. doi: 10.1177/1071181319631201. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.van Dongen K, van Maanen PP. Under-reliance on the decision aid: a difference in calibration and attribution between self and aid. Proc Hum Factors Ergon Soc Annu Meet. 2006 Oct 1;50(3):225–9. doi: 10.1177/154193120605000304. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Muralidharan L, de Visser EJ, Parasuraman R. The effects of pitch contour and flanging on trust in speaking cognitive agents. CHI '14 Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems; CHI EA '14; April 26-May 1, 2014; Toronto, Canada. 2014. pp. 2167–72. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Jessup SA. Measurement of the Propensity to Trust Automation. Wright State University. 2018. [2022-04-25]. https://corescholar.libraries.wright.edu/etd_all/2209/
- 54.Norman CD, Skinner HA. eHEALS: the eHealth Literacy Scale. J Med Internet Res. 2006 Nov 14;8(4):e27. doi: 10.2196/jmir.8.4.e27. https://www.jmir.org/2006/4/e27/ v8i4e27 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.R Core Team R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. 2021. [2021-05-14]. https://www.R-project.org/
- 56.Wickham H, Averick M, Bryan J, Chang W, McGowan LD, François R, Grolemund G, Hayes A, Henry L, Hester J, Kuhn M, Pedersen T, Miller E, Bache SM, Müller K, Ooms J, Robinson D, Seidel DP, Spinu V, Takahashi K, Vaughan D, Wilke C, Woo K, Yutani H. Welcome to the Tidyverse. J Open Source Softw. 2019 Nov 21;4(43):1686. doi: 10.21105/joss.01686. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Lesnoff M, Lancelot R. aod: Analysis of Overdispersed Data. The Comprehensive R Archive Network. 2019. [2021-05-28]. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/aod/index.html .
- 58.Champely S, Ekstrom C, Dalgaard P, Gill J, Weibelzahl S, Anandkumar A, Ford C, Volcic R, De Rosario H. pwr: Basic Functions for Power Analysis. The Comprehensive R Archive Network. 2020. [2021-05-28]. https://cran.r-project.org/package=pwr .
- 59.Using technology to ease the burden on primary care. Healthwatch Enfield. 2019. Jan, [2021-03-13]. https://www.healthwatch.co.uk/sites/healthwatch.co.uk/files/reports-library/20190122_Enfield_%20Using%20technology%20to%20ease%20the%20burden%20on%20primary%20care.pdf .
- 60.Woodcock C, Mittelstadt B, Busbridge D, Blank G. The impact of explanations on layperson trust in artificial intelligence-driven symptom checker apps: experimental study. J Med Internet Res. 2021 Nov 03;23(11):e29386. doi: 10.2196/29386. https://www.jmir.org/2021/11/e29386/ v23i11e29386 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Miller S, Gilbert S, Virani V, Wicks P. Patients' utilization and perception of an artificial intelligence-based symptom assessment and advice technology in a British primary care waiting room: exploratory pilot study. JMIR Hum Factors. 2020 Jul 10;7(3):e19713. doi: 10.2196/19713. https://humanfactors.jmir.org/2020/3/e19713/ v7i3e19713 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Levine DM, Mehrotra A. Assessment of diagnosis and triage in validated case vignettes among nonphysicians before and after Internet search. JAMA Netw Open. 2021 Mar 01;4(3):e213287. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.3287. https://jamanetwork.com/journals/jamanetworkopen/fullarticle/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.3287 .2777835 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Jussupow E, Spohrer K, Heinzl A, Gawlitza J. Augmenting medical diagnosis decisions? An investigation into physicians’ decision-making process with Artificial Intelligence. Inf Syst Res. 2021 Sep;32(3):713–35. doi: 10.1287/isre.2020.0980. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Cooper AA, Humphreys KR. The uncertainty is killing me: self-triage decision making and information availability. E J Appl Psychol. 2008 Oct 14;4(1):1–6. doi: 10.7790/ejap.v4i1.124. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Jessup SA, Schneider TR, Alarcon GM, Ryan TJ, Capiola A. The measurement of the propensity to trust automation. In: Chen JY, Fragomeni G, editors. Virtual, Augmented and Mixed Reality: Applications and Case Studies. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing; 2019. pp. 476–89. [Google Scholar]
- 66.Jungmann SM, Klan T, Kuhn S, Jungmann F. Accuracy of a Chatbot (Ada) in the diagnosis of mental disorders: comparative case study with lay and expert users. JMIR Form Res. 2019 Oct 29;3(4):e13863. doi: 10.2196/13863. https://formative.jmir.org/2019/4/e13863/ v3i4e13863 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Ćirković A. Evaluation of four artificial intelligence-assisted self-diagnosis apps on three diagnoses: two-year follow-up study. J Med Internet Res. 2020 Dec 04;22(12):e18097. doi: 10.2196/18097. https://www.jmir.org/2020/12/e18097/ v22i12e18097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Gilbert S, Fenech M, Upadhyay S, Wicks P, Novorol C. Quality of condition suggestions and urgency advice provided by the Ada symptom assessment app evaluated with vignettes optimised for Australia. Aust J Prim Health. 2021 Oct;27(5):377–81. doi: 10.1071/PY21032.PY21032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Semigran HL, Levine DM, Nundy S, Mehrotra A. Comparison of physician and computer diagnostic accuracy. JAMA Intern Med. 2016 Dec 01;176(12):1860–1. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2016.6001.2565684 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Effect coding scheme of gender.
Effect coding scheme of education.
Multiple linear regression of demographic and interindividual influences on subjective trust with standardized coefficients.
Multiple binomial logistic regression of demographic and interindividual influences on behavioral trust with standardized coefficients.
Multiple linear regression of demographic and interindividual influences on subjective trust with unstandardized coefficients.
Multiple binomial logistic regression of demographic and interindividual influences on behavioral trust with unstandardized coefficients.
CONSORT-eHEALTH checklist (V 1.6.1).