Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2022 May 18;17(5):e0266887. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0266887

Romantic partner embraces reduce cortisol release after acute stress induction in women but not in men

Gesa Berretz 1, Chantal Cebula 1, Blanca Maria Wortelmann 1, Panagiota Papadopoulou 1, Oliver T Wolf 2, Sebastian Ocklenburg 1,3,4, Julian Packheiser 1,5,*
Editor: Marta Panzeri6
PMCID: PMC9116618  PMID: 35584124

Abstract

Stress is omnipresent in our everyday lives. It is therefore critical to identify potential stress-buffering behaviors that can help to prevent the negative effects of acute stress in daily life. Massages, a form of social touch, are an effective buffer against both the endocrinological and sympathetic stress response in women. However, for other forms of social touch, potential stress-buffering effects have not been investigated in detail. Furthermore, the possible stress-buffering effects of social touch on men have not been researched so far. The present study focused on embracing, one of the most common forms of social touch across many cultures. We used a short-term embrace between romantic partners as a social touch intervention prior to the induction of acute stress via the Socially Evaluated Cold Pressor Test. Women who embraced their partner prior to being stressed showed a reduced cortisol response compared to a control group in which no embrace occurred. No stress-buffering effect could be observed in men. No differences between the embrace and control group were observed regarding sympathetic nervous system activation measured via blood pressure or subjective affect ratings. These findings suggest that in women, short-term embraces prior to stressful social situations such as examinations or stressful interviews can reduce the cortisol response in that situation.

Introduction

Embracing is one of the most prevalent forms of social touch in everyday life and across cultures [1]. They are used to greet others, convey love and affection but can also be used in negative situations to console people who feel sad or depressed [2]. Embraces have been demonstrated to be influenced by the affective state of the embracing individuals [3, 4]. Moreover, embracing your partner elicits stronger positive emotional responses both on the behavioral and neurophysiological level compared to embracing objects [5]. The positive feeling of an embrace is also associated with the duration of the embrace since longer embraces are perceived as more pleasant compared to very short, i.e. 1s long, embraces [6].

The positive emotional effects of embracing might explain why it is linked to benefits on physical and mental health. For example, embracing has been shown to reduce blood pressure [7], is associated with decreases in inflammation [8] and with increased subjective well-being [9]. Furthermore, embracing is linked to a reduced risk of infection and an accelerated recovery from viral diseases [10]. The latter study suggested that this health benefit might be due to the potential buffering effects of embraces on the stress response as this effect was especially dominant on days with interpersonal tension.

Stress is one of the leading factors associated with a variety of mental disorders such as depression, burn-out or anxiety disorders [1113]. While these disorders are generally linked to long-term stress exposure, acute stress has been, amongst other things, associated with increases in negative mood indicating the importance of regulating stress for subjective well-being as well [14]. On the physiological level, the stress response comprises two separate but interconnected branches. The activation of the sympathetic nervous system leads to a fast release of adrenalin from the adrenal medulla as well as noradrenaline from postganglionic neurons resulting in an increase of heart rate, blood pressure and breathing frequency [15]. This release is paralleled by the nucleus coeruleus leading to an increase of adrenalin and noradrenalin in the brain which is connected to the subjective feeling of stress [16]. The other branch consists of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) axis. Here, secretion of corticotropin-releasing-hormone and arginine-vasopressin from the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus causes the release of adrenocorticotropic hormone from the pituitary into the circulatory system. This leads to a release of glucocorticoid hormones from the adrenal cortex. In humans, cortisol represents the main glucocorticoid and is therefore regarded as one of the major stress hormones [17].

Major stressors that have been demonstrated to activate these two systems are pressure to perform, uncontrollability and social evaluation [18, 19]. While confrontation with other people can elicit a stress reaction, the presence and the support of others, especially through social touch, can also act as a buffer towards the bodily stress response. For example, Heinrichs et al. [20] exposed men to the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST), a stress induction paradigm exerting psychosocial stress [21], and found that a preparation for the social evaluation of TSST with social support reduced the secretion of cortisol. Furthermore, they also administered oxytocin which has been demonstrated to exert stress-attenuating effects [22] and is released especially through social touch [23]. Heinrichs et al. [20] found that social support plus oxytocin provided an even stronger buffering effect for the secretion of cortisol compared to social support by itself.

Prolonged social touch and the associated increased levels of oxytocin have been suggested to induce a shift from sympathetic to parasympathetic activation in animal studies [24] and have been demonstrated to increase psychophysiological relaxation in women [25]. Furthermore, embracing a human-shaped cushion during a phone conversation has been shown to reduce cortisol levels in women compared to a phone call without the embrace [26]. However, these studies did not perform a stress induction calling into question whether these effects apply to acute physiological stress reactions. To our knowledge, only a few selected studies investigated the role of prolonged physical contact on the acute stress response in the laboratory. For example, Ditzen et al. [27] studied the effects of different types of couple interactions on cortisol and heart rate responses elicited via the TSST in women. In three experimental groups, they tested the effects of no interactions between romantic partners, verbal social support and physical social support in form of a 10-minute-long shoulder massage on the sympathetic nervous system and the HPA axis. They found significant decreases of both heart rate and cortisol release exclusively in the physical support group suggesting that physical contact provides the most effective buffer against the bodily stress response. In line with these findings, Grewen et al. [28] compared cohabitating couples that either held hands for a prolonged period (10min) and shortly embraced each other afterwards (20s) or did not touch each other prior to a public-speaking task. They found that the mutual physical contact attenuated systolic, diastolic blood pressure and heart rate increases compared to the control group. Finally, Pauley et al. [29] subjected romantic couples or platonic friends to acute stress after they talked either about fond mutual memories for a period of 10 minutes and embraced afterwards (< 10s duration), only shared each other’s presence or waited separately for the stress induction to begin. The authors observed an attenuated cardiovascular stress response in all participants that went through the affectionate communication plus embracing condition. For HPA-axis activation, the findings were moderated by the relationship type as platonic friends showed a stronger increase in cortisol after expressing mutual affection compared to romantic partners that only shared each other’s presence.

Since the previous studies either used long-lasting touch such as massages or combined embraces with other means such as hand holding or affectionate communication, the present study investigated if short embraces by themselves can buffer against the stress reaction since embraces have been indicated to be a viable stress buffer in stressful situations [10]. To this end, we invited romantic couples and randomly attributed them to two experimental groups. One group was asked to perform a mutual embrace before undergoing a joint stress induction procedure. The other group did not embrace each other, and partners only provided support through their physical presence. We hypothesized that the embracing group demonstrates a reduced cortisol and blood pressure response to the stressor in accordance with previous findings [2529].

Methods

Participants

A total of 76 participants with an average age of 22.30 years (standard deviation: 2.24; range: 19 to 32 years) were included in the final sample. 36 participants were male and 40 were female. Participants were invited as romantic couples and therefore always came in pairs. All participants were in heterosexual relationships even though we did not exclude couples of other sexual orientations from participating in the study. The numerical imbalance in the groups resulted from post hoc exclusion of participants due to technical issues. The participants included in the final sample had no history of mental or neurodevelopmental disorders, were non-smokers, did not take any medication and had no prior experience with the experimental paradigm. Furthermore, the body mass index was between 18.5 and 27 since obesity has been linked to systematic increases in HPA-axis responsivity [30]. The study was approved by the local ethics committee at the faculty of psychology at Ruhr University Bochum. All participants gave written informed consent and were treated in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki.

Experimental procedure

All experiments were conducted between 1pm to 6pm to control for effects of the circadian rhythm on cortisol [31]. To prevent the contamination of saliva samples, the participants were explicitly asked not to eat or drink anything apart from water for one hour before the procedure. Couples arrived jointly and were then attributed randomly to one of two experimental groups. In the control condition, couples underwent a stress induction procedure (see below for detail) together. In the embrace condition, participants went through the identical procedure but also embraced each other once during the experiment. In total, 19 women and 18 men were part of the control group and 21 women and 18 men were part of the embracing group.

After being picked up by the experimenter and being seated in the experimental room, participants in both groups first provided informed written consent. After filling out demographic questionnaires, a baseline saliva sample as well as systolic and diastolic blood pressure were taken (see Fig 1). Furthermore, participants filled out the Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS, [32]). Following baseline measurement, the experimenter either left the room immediately to allow for the stress induction to commence (control condition) or instructed the participants to embrace in a standing position once they left the room and until they re-entered the room (embrace condition). No specific instruction was given to allow for a natural experience of the embrace. The experimenter then left and waited for exactly 20s after which they re-entered the room to signal the end of the embrace. As in the control group, the experimenter then left and the stress induction procedure began.

Fig 1.

Fig 1

A) In the embrace condition, participants filled out questionnaires and provided a baseline measurement for cortisol, blood pressure and affective state prior to the experimental onset. After this was concluded, a 20s embrace in the absence of the experimenter took place. Following the embrace, the couples were subjected to the SECPT in a group setting with another measurement cycle of cortisol, blood pressure and affective state. 15 and 25 minutes after the SECPT was concluded, another measurement was conducted. B) Same as in A), but the partners did not embrace prior to the SECPT.

In both groups, a previously unknown experimenter (randomly chosen to be female or male) entered the room dressed in a lab coat. This experimenter was briefed to act neutral and distanced to the participants both during the instructions and during the stress procedure itself. The participants were then informed about the stress procedure, namely the Socially Evaluated Cold Pressor Test (SECPT) which has been evaluated for group testing scenarios [33]. The SECPT requires participants to place their hand with spread fingers in an ice-cold water bath (0–4°C) for a maximum of three minutes upon which the experimenter ends the procedure. Participants were instructed that they could remove their hand if the procedure could no longer be endured at any time. During the SECPT, participants were furthermore filmed by a camera to which they were asked to constantly hold eye contact. They were also asked not to talk during the procedure. Any violations of these instructions were continuously remarked by the experimenter. After one minute of the SECPT start, the blood pressure was taken again, regardless of whether the procedure was aborted or not. After the procedure had ended, another saliva sample was immediately taken, and the participants filled out another PANAS questionnaire as well as subjective stress questionnaires about the SECPT. Here, the participants had to indicate on a scale from 0–100 how difficult, painful, stressful and unpleasant the SECPT procedure had been. The experimenter for the SECPT then left the room and the original experimenter came back.

Following the SECPT, the couples were separated from each other to fill out the Relationship Assessment Scale (RAS, [34]). This was done to prevent any influence on the rating by the romantic partners by having to fill out the questionnaire next to them. The RAS is a 7-item questionnaire that assesses the relationship quality on a 5-point Likert scale. Furthermore, both 15 minutes and 25 minutes after the conclusion of the SECPT, another saliva sample, blood pressure and affective ratings using the PANAS questionnaire were taken from the participants. Thus, participants provided four measurements in total (baseline, SECPT, 15min and 25min post SECPT).

Cortisol measurements

Saliva was collected polypropylene micro salivettes (Sarstedt, Nürmbrecht, Germany). After the end of the experimental session, the samples were stored at -20°C until further analysis. Cortisol was assessed in an in-house laboratory of the Genetic Psychology and Cognitive Psychology Department at Ruhr University Bochum by using a cortisol enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (Cortisol Saliva ELISA, IBL, Hamburg, Germany). The intra‐assay coefficients of variance (CV) were below 5% and inter-assay CVs were below 15%.

Cardiovascular measurements

Blood pressure was taken on the left arm using an M700 Intelli IT blood pressure monitor (OMRON, Kyoto, Japan). The left arm was used as the participants were instructed to submerge their right hand under water. The cuff was situated approximately 2 cm above the elbow bend. Participants were asked to rest their arm on the table and remain silent during the measurement to ensure valid results.

Statistical analyses

Statistical evaluation was performed using R version 4.1.0. Relationship satisfaction was compared between both groups using independent samples t-tests. Cortisol data was log-transformed prior to further analysis. We used a linear mixed model analysis using the lme4 and lmerTest package [35, 36] as these models can include subjects as a random factor in addition to fixed effects allowing for better generalization to the underlying population [37]. The model was run with (1) cortisol, (2) systolic blood pressure, (3) diastolic blood pressure as well as (4) positive and (5) negative affective state as dependent variables and measurement time point (levels: baseline, SECPT, 15min post SECPT, 25min post SECPT), condition (levels: control, embracing) and sex (levels: women, men) as independent variables. For cortisol, we also included the use of oral contraceptives (OCs) as a covariate due to its known effect of cortisol. Post-hoc tests were performed using the emmeans function to correct for multiple comparisons (Bonferroni method). A post-hoc sensitivity analysis was conducted to determine how much power would have been necessary given the present sample size to detect a within-between interaction effect using g*Power [38]. Here, we used four groups, four repeated measures, a repeated measures correlation of r = 0.7 and non-sphericity correction of ε = 0.62 as inputs. 80% power would have been achieved at interaction effects greater than Cohen’s f = 0.14. Data that was only obtained during the SECPT, i.e., difficulty, painfulness, stressfulness, and unpleasantness of the SECPT as well as the duration of the hand in the water were evaluated using a linear model with each of these parameters as dependent variable and group and sex as independent variables.

Results

Relationship satisfaction

Relationship satisfaction in the overall sample was high with a mean RAS score of 30.88 (SD = ± 3.99, maximum score = 35). The two experimental groups showed no difference in relationship satisfaction (t(73) = 0.54, p = .593). There was furthermore no difference in relationship satisfaction between women and men (t(73) = 0.16, p = .872).

Cortisol

We observed significant main effects of measurement time point at 15 min and 25 min post SECPT (β = 0.53 [0.38–0.67], SE = 0.07, t = 7.12, p < .001; β = 0.48 [0.33–0.62], SE = 0.07, t = 6.42 p < .001, respectively). Bonferroni corrected post hoc tests revealed that cortisol levels were increased at 15 min and 25 min post SECPT compared to baseline and the SECPT (all p’s < .001) indicating that the SECPT overall succeeded in inducing a cortisol response. We also found a significant main effect of sex with men showing a higher cortisol response overall (β = 0.37 [0.09–0.65], SE = 0.14, t = 2.59, p = .012). Finally, we observed a three-way interaction between condition, sex and the measurement time point at 15 and 25min post SECPT (β = 0.61 [0.07–1.15], SE = 0.28, t = 2.16, p = .032; β = 0.81 [0.27–1.35], SE = 0.28, t = 2.82, p = .005, respectively). For differences between the embracing and control groups, we found a significant difference only at 25min post SECPT after Bonferroni correction. Here, post-hoc tests revealed significantly less cortisol concentrations in individuals in the embracing group compared to the control group for women only (t = 2.21, p = .028, see Fig 2). For differences between sexes, we only found significant differences between women and men in the embracing group. Here, women demonstrated significantly lower cortisol levels compared to men at both 15min (t = 2.32, p = .023) and 25min post SECPT (t = 2.34, p = .022). Usage of OCs as a covariate did not reach significance in the model (β = -0.15 [-0.58–0.09], SE = 0.21, t = 0.71, p = .480). Descriptive values for cortisol measurements across groups can be found in Table 1. The effect size of the model with and without interactions can be found in S1 Table.

Fig 2.

Fig 2

Cortisol concentration for women (top) and men (bottom) during baseline, the SECPT and 15 minutes as well as 25 minutes post SECPT for the embrace and control condition. White dots represent the median value for each group. Error bars represent the upper and lower quartiles. Group differences between the conditions are marked. * = p < .05.

Table 1. Descriptive values (mean value and standard deviation) of logarithmized cortisol values across the four measurement time points broken down by condition and sex.

Time point   Women   Men  
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
Baseline Control 1.38 .60 Control 1.50 .60
  Embrace 1.32 .69 Embrace 1.40 .55
Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
SECPT Control 1.39 .62 Control 1.46 .75
  Embrace 1.16 .88 Embrace 1.49 .62
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
15min post Control 1.83 .91 Control 2.18 .76
  Embrace 1.42 .76 Embrace 2.34 .62
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
 25min post Control 1.89 .84 Control 2.06 .77
  Embrace 1.34 .76 Embrace 2.28 .77

Blood pressure

For systolic blood pressure, we found a significant main effect of measurement time point during the SECPT (β = 12.83 [9.53–16.11], SE = 1.68, t = 7.62, p < .001) and at both the 15min (β = -5.47 [-8.76 –-2.18], SE = 1.68, t = 3.25 p < .001) and 25min post SECPT measurement (β = -7.03 [-10.32– -3.74], SE = 1.68, t = 4.18, p < .001). Bonferroni corrected post-hoc test revealed a significant increase in systolic blood pressure during the SECPT compared to baseline (t = 7.62, p < .001), but significant decreases 15min (t = 3.25 p < .001) and 25min after the SECPT (t = 4.18, p < .001) compared to baseline (see S1 Fig). The model for sex also reached significance (β = 14.79 [10.07–19.52], SE = 2.41, t = 6.13, p < .001) with men showing higher systolic blood pressure compared to women. No other main effect or interaction reached significance (all p’s. > .228). Descriptive values for systolic blood pressure measurements across groups can be found in Table 2. The effect size of the model with and without interactions can be found in S1 Table.

Table 2. Descriptive values (mean value and standard deviation) of systolic blood pressure values across the four measurement time points broken down by condition and sex.

Time point   Women   Men  
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
Baseline Control 113.26 14.82 Control 129.72 14.71
  Embrace 114.95 14.60 Embrace 132.33 14.99
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
SECPT Control 125.37 12.98 Control 140.33 13.36
  Embrace 129.10 17.03 Embrace 146.61 17.22
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
15min post Control 109.58 10.17 Control 121.06 10.81
  Embrace 110.33 14.14 Embrace 127.17 15.37
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
25min post Control 106.53 10.10 Control 118.83 13.29
  Embrace 112.05 10.36 Embrace 124.06 14.73

For diastolic blood pressure, we only found a significant main effect of measurement time point during the SECPT (β = 12.55 [9.43–15.68], SE = 1.60, t = 7.86, p < .001). Bonferroni corrected post hoc test revealed a significant increase in diastolic blood pressure during the SECPT compared to all other measurement time points (all p’s < .001, see S2 Fig). No other main effect or interaction reached significance (all p’s > .068). Descriptive values for diastolic blood pressure measurements across groups can be found in Table 3. The effect size of the model with and without interactions can be found in S1 Table.

Table 3. Descriptive values (mean value and standard deviation) of diastolic blood pressure values across the four measurement time points broken down by condition and sex.

Time point   Women   Men  
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
Baseline Control 80.26 12.33 Control 77.94 12.39
  Embrace 82.67 12.93 Embrace 80.56 12.43
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
SECPT Control 91.16 8.40 Control 91.00 13.72
  Embrace 93.14 9.67 Embrace 96.78 13.26
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
15min post Control 77.90 7.56 Control 75.11 13.54
  Embrace 81.24 14.86 Embrace 79.94 10.06
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
25min post Control 78.47 8.65 Control 75.22 11.05
  Embrace 79.81 11.03 Embrace 78.50 8.21

Affective state

For positive affective state, there was a significant main effect of measurement time point at the 15min and 25min post SECPT measurement (β = -0.14 [-0.27 –-0.01], SE = 0.07, t = 2.11, p = .036; β = -0.29 [-0.42 –-0.16], SE = 0.07, t = 4.27, p < .001, respectively). Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc tests however only reached significance for the 25min post SECPT measurement (see S3 Fig). Here, positive affect decreased with respect to baseline and the SECPT (t = 4.27, p < .001; t = 3.88, p < .001, respectively). No other main effect or interaction reached significance (all p’s > .227). Descriptive values for positive affective ratings across groups can be found in Table 4. The effect size of the model with and without interactions can be found in S1 Table.

Table 4. Descriptive values (mean value and standard deviation) of positive PANAS values across the four measurement time points broken down by condition and sex.

Time point   Women   Men  
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
Baseline Control 3.21 .56 Control 3.06 .55
  Embrace 3.03 .57 Embrace 2.94 .56
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
SECPT Control 3.23 .75 Control 3.02 .81
  Embrace 2.90 .80 Embrace 3.00 .69
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
15min post Control 3.07 .74 Control 2.84 .65
  Embrace 2.99 .89 Embrace 2.76 .73
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
25min post Control 2.90 .58 Control 2.61 .81
  Embrace 2.94 .92 Embrace 2.61 .76

For negative affective state, there was a significant main effect of measurement time point at the 25min post SECPT measurement (β = -0.13 [-0.22 –-0.04], SE = 0.05, t = 2.75, p = .007, see S4 Fig). Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc tests revealed a significant decrease in negative affect at the 25min post SECPT measurement compared to the baseline and the SECPT itself (t = 2.75, p = .039; t = 4.33, p < .001). No other main effect or interaction reached significance (all p’s > .252). Descriptive values for negative affective ratings across groups can be found in Table 5. The effect size of the model with and without interactions can be found in S1 Table.

Table 5. Descriptive values (mean value and standard deviation) of negative PANAS values across the four measurement time points broken down by condition and sex.

Time point   Women   Men  
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
Baseline Control 1.26 .19 Control 1.22 .26
  Embrace 1.41 .38 Embrace 1.32 .42
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
SECPT Control 1.41 .43 Control 1.27 .40
  Embrace 1.45 .39 Embrace 1.40 .41
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
15min post Control 1.20 .31 Control 1.27 .55
  Embrace 1.23 .37 Embrace 1.24 .29
  Group Mean SD Group Mean SD
25min post Control 1.08 .18 Control 1.21 .58
  Embrace 1.24 .32 Embrace 1.18 .48

Subjective SECPT ratings and SECPT duration

We neither found any main effects of condition or sex, nor any interaction for the difficulty, painfulness, stressfulness or unpleasantness ratings of the SECPT (all p’s > .059). The same was true for the time that the participants held their hands in the ice-cold water (all p’s > .051). Descriptive values for these measures across groups can be found in Table 6.

Table 6. Descriptive values (mean value and standard deviation) of the SECPT ratings and the duration of the SECPT broken down by condition and sex.

Difficulty Painfulness Stressfulness Unpleasantness Duration in s
Embrace women 69.05 ± 27.73 55.24 ± 32.03 44.29 ± 24.61 73.81 ± 25.39 148.0 ± 57.56
Embrace men 62.78 ± 24.49 45.56 ± 29.35 41.67 ± 23.33 62.78 ± 24.92 166.50 ± 39.32
Control women 70.35 ± 33.25 61.58 ± 33.54 54.74 ± 31.33 74.74 ± 27.56 143.68 ± 58.13
Control men 51.11 ± 30.66 54.44 ± 31.29 44.44 ± 24.79 67.78 ± 17.00 169.72 ± 34.91

Discussion

In the present study, we investigated the efficacy of a short-term embrace on the physiological stress response. To this end, we invited romantic couples and subjected them to the SECPT in a joint testing environment. One group was additionally instructed to embrace each other prior to the stress procedure. We hypothesized that both responses from the sympathetic nervous system as well as from the HPA axis would be decreased through the application of social touch. We could partly confirm our hypothesis as there was a significantly reduced cortisol response in the embracing group compared to the control group. This effect was selective to women and could not be observed in men. Blood pressure as a marker of sympathetic nervous system activity and subjective ratings of affective state were not modulated by the embrace.

A noteworthy finding of our study is the sex difference in the influence of the embrace on cortisol changes in response to the SECPT. We found no evidence that men benefitted from a short-term embrace as a potential stress buffer and our results indicated that this effect is specific to women. The effect was not mediated by differences in relationship quality as there were no difference in relationship satisfaction between women and men. A conceivable explanation for this sex difference could relate to varying levels of oxytocin release between men and women following the embrace. Oxytocin is hypothesized to inhibit the synthesis of adrenocorticotropic hormone in the pituitary gland as it resembles vasopressin in its molecular structure [39]. Rising oxytocin levels are associated with decreases in vasopressin ultimately resulting in decreased cortisol secretion in the human body [39]. While we did not measure oxytocin in the present study, a meta-analytic study on sex differences in affective touch has demonstrated that women perceive affective touch as significantly more pleasant compared to men [40]. Moreover, oxytocin has been demonstrated to be directly correlated with the perceived pleasantness of gentle touch [41]. Therefore, the mutual embrace might have elicited higher levels of perceived pleasantness and thus higher levels of oxytocin release in women compared to men which could explain the observed difference. Another explanation could relate to the “tend-and-befriend” hypothesis which states that women respond differentially to stress-inducing events in the environment compared to men [42]. According to this hypothesis, women respond to stressful situations via an increase in care and protection for offspring, which in turn reduces the bodily stress response through an increase in oxytocin. This evolutionary conserved mechanism to increase offspring survival could thus generalize to a variety of stressful situations. It should be noted however that a study investigating the oxytocin release in response to stress did not find marked sex differences [43]. Thus, this sex difference requires additional research.

Our results have implications for everyday life situations since increases in glucocorticoids have been shown to impair memory retrieval and executive functioning [4447]. Memory traces show impaired retrieval under higher cortisol levels, especially if they have a negative valence [4850]. Since everyday situations like an upcoming examination are associated with a pronounced cortisol increase [51], it is important to consider potential buffers against the negative effects of cortisol secretion on memory retrieval. In contrast to long massages or prolonged hand-holding as performed in previous studies [27, 28], a short-term embrace could be considered a highly feasible method in everyday life to buffer against these effects based on the findings of our study. It remains however an open question whether this effect is limited to romantic partner embraces or generalizes also to embraces between platonic friends. A recent survey study investigating the role of social touch on mental health aspects such as feelings of anxiety found that health benefits seem to be mostly linked to non-sexual intimate touch [52]. This finding would be in line with the observed effect in the study by Pauley et al. [29] in which relationship type moderated the cortisol reactivity in the participants. Future research is needed to further investigate this issue.

In contrast to our hypotheses, we did not observe any changes in sympathetic nervous system activity indicated by the absence of group difference in blood pressure, regardless of sex. This result is not in accordance with previous findings where prolonged exposure to social touch or affectionate communication resulted in reduced sympathetic nervous system activity indicated by decreased blood pressure and heart rate or increased heart rate variability [25, 2729]. A possible explanation could relate to the pain associated with the SECPT as no other of the previously reported studies used a painful stimulus to induce stress. Pain strongly engages the sympathetic nervous system and is thus difficult to reduce [53]. Another possible explanation why we could not observe changes in blood pressure could relate to the time course of our experiment. The SECPT in our experiment took place immediately after the embrace. The very quick succession of the SECPT might have prevented the buffering effects of oxytocin to take effect as the blood concentration could have been too low at this point in time. Since this should have however also affected results for HPA-axis activation, this explanation is less likely.

Our study is subject to a few limitations that need to be acknowledged. First, while we controlled for OCs using them as covariates in the model, we did not collect data on the menstrual cycle in women that has been demonstrated to influence cortisol secretion [54]. Since our sample was randomly attributed to the experimental groups, it is however unlikely that there were systematic differences in cycle phases. Another limitation was the lack of an oxytocin measure which could have informed us about changes in oxytocin levels across the study, especially during the SECPT. It should be noted however that peripheral hematic and salivary oxytocin levels have been called into question whether they provide accurate measurements for oxytocin levels in the brain where the HPA axis is regulated [55]. A final limitation concerns the lack of an affective evaluation of the embrace since it could have illuminated whether men truly perceived the embrace as less pleasant compared to women.

In conclusion, we found a cortisol-buffering effect of embraces between romantic partners following a stress induction procedure. The effect was specific to women. This finding could have implications for stress reduction in everyday situations that often induce stress like exams, oral presentations or job interviews. Importantly, we also want to highlight our findings in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic that has substantially increased stress and depression levels in many individuals due to economic and social restrictions [56, 57]. It could be conceived that these increases in everyday stress are in part due to the lack of social affective touch through the means of for example embraces. Future research needs to be conducted to identify the long-term impact of the pandemic on stress and social touch behavior.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Effect sizes of the models with and without interactions for cortisol, diastolic and systolic blood pressure as well as positive and negative affect.

Both R2 and Cohen’s f are given as effect size measures.

(DOCX)

S1 Fig

Systolic blood pressure for women (top) and men (bottom) during baseline, the SECPT and 15 minutes as well as 25 minutes post SECPT for the embrace and control condition. White dots represent the median value for each group. Error bars represent the upper and lower quartiles.

(TIF)

S2 Fig

Diastolic blood pressure for women (top) and men (bottom) during baseline, the SECPT and 15 minutes as well as 25 minutes post SECPT for the embrace and control condition. White dots represent the median value for each group. Error bars represent the upper and lower quartiles.

(TIF)

S3 Fig

Positive affect ratings for women (top) and men (bottom) during the baseline, SECPT and 15 minutes as well as 25 minutes post SECPT for the embrace and control condition. White dots represent the median value for each group. Error bars represent the upper and lower quartiles.

(TIF)

S4 Fig

Negative affect ratings for women (top) and men (bottom) during the baseline, SECPT and 15 minutes as well as 25 minutes post SECPT for the embrace and control condition. White dots represent the median value for each group. Error bars represent the upper and lower quartiles.

(TIF)

Data Availability

Ethical approval was not provided by the participants to make the data publicly available. The authors therefore are not allowed to upload the data to a public repository as they contain demographic data that allows for potential identification of the participants. For questions regarding the ethics approval, please contact the relevant ethics committee (ethikkommission-psychologie@rub.de). To make the data accessible, we uploaded both data and code to a private repository in Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/8gyr2/). Requests for the data can be made to two of the authors (gesa.berretz@rub.de or j.packheiser@nin.knaw.nl) or to the local ethics committee of the Psychology Department at Ruhr University Bochum (ethikkommission-psychologie@rub.de).

Funding Statement

O.T.W. is financially supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (project number: 400672603; grant: WO733/18-1). S.O. is financially supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (project number: 400672603, OC127/9-1). J.P. was financially supported by the German National Academy of Sciences Leopoldina (LPDS 2021-05) and received his salary from this organization. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. We acknowledge support by the DFG Open Access Publication Funds of the Ruhr-Universität Bochum.

References

  • 1.Forsell LM, Åström JA. Meanings of Hugging: From Greeting Behavior to Touching Implications. Comprehensive Psychology. 2012; 1:02.17.21.CP.1.13. doi: 10.2466/02.17.21.CP.1.13 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Ocklenburg S, Packheiser J, Schmitz J, Rook N, Güntürkün O, Peterburs J, et al. Hugs and kisses–The role of motor preferences and emotional lateralization for hemispheric asymmetries in human social touch. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2018; 95:353–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Packheiser J, Rook N, Dursun Z, Mesenhöller J, Wenglorz A, Güntürkün O, et al. Embracing your emotions: affective state impacts lateralisation of human embraces. Psychological research. 2019; 83:26–36. doi: 10.1007/s00426-018-0985-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Packheiser J, Schmitz J, Metzen D, Reinke P, Radtke F, Friedrich P, et al. Asymmetries in social touch—motor and emotional biases on lateral preferences in embracing, cradling and kissing. Laterality. 2020; 25:325–48. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2019.1690496 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Packheiser J, Berretz G, Rook N, Bahr C, Schockenhoff L, Güntürkün O, et al. Investigating real-life emotions in romantic couples: a mobile EEG study. Scientific reports. 2020; 11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Dueren AL, Vafeiadou A, Edgar C, Banissy MJ. The influence of duration, arm crossing style, gender, and emotional closeness on hugging behaviour. Acta Psychologica. 2021; 221:103441. Epub 2021/11/02. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103441 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Light KC, Grewen KM, Amico JA. More frequent partner hugs and higher oxytocin levels are linked to lower blood pressure and heart rate in premenopausal women. Biological psychology. 2005; 69:5–21. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.van Raalte LJ, Floyd K. Daily hugging predicts lower levels of two proinflammatory cytokines. Western Journal of Communication. 2020:1–20. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ocklenburg S, Malek IM, Reichart JS, Katona L, Luhmann M, Packheiser J. Give me a hug–More frequent everyday embracing is associated with better daily mood in lonely individuals. 2021. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D, Turner RB, Doyle WJ. Does hugging provide stress-buffering social support? A study of susceptibility to upper respiratory infection and illness. Psychological science. 2015; 26:135–47. doi: 10.1177/0956797614559284 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Berretz G, Wolf OT, Güntürkün O, Ocklenburg S. Atypical lateralization in neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders: What is the role of stress. cortex. 2020; 125:215–32. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.12.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Iacovides A, Fountoulakis K, Kaprinis S, Kaprinis G. The relationship between job stress, burnout and clinical depression. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2003; 75:209–21. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(02)00101-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Shin LM, Liberzon I. The neurocircuitry of fear, stress, and anxiety disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010; 35:169–91. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.83 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Bolger N, DeLongis A, Kessler RC, Schilling EA. Effects of daily stress on negative mood. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1989; 57:808. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.57.5.808 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ulrich-Lai YM, Herman JP. Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nature reviews neuroscience. 2009; 10:397–409. doi: 10.1038/nrn2647 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Joëls M, Baram TZ. The neuro-symphony of stress. Nature reviews neuroscience. 2009; 10:459–66. doi: 10.1038/nrn2632 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.de Kloet ER, Joëls M, Holsboer F. Stress and the brain: from adaptation to disease. Nature reviews neuroscience. 2005; 6:463–75. doi: 10.1038/nrn1683 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Birkett MA. The Trier Social Stress Test protocol for inducing psychological stress. Journal of visualized experiments: JoVE. 2011. doi: 10.3791/3238 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Mitchell MS, Greenbaum RL, Vogel RM, Mawritz MB, Keating DJ. Can you handle the pressure? The effect of performance pressure on stress appraisals, self-regulation, and behavior. Academy of Management Journal. 2019; 62:531–52. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Heinrichs M, Baumgartner T, Kirschbaum C, Ehlert U. Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress. Biological psychiatry. 2003; 54:1389–98. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00465-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kirschbaum C, Pirke K-M, Hellhammer DH. The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’–a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology. 1993; 28:76–81. doi: 10.1159/000119004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Smith AS, Wang Z. Hypothalamic oxytocin mediates social buffering of the stress response. Biological psychiatry. 2014; 76:281–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Tang Y, Benusiglio D, Lefevre A, Hilfiger L, Althammer F, Bludau A, et al. Social touch promotes interfemale communication via activation of parvocellular oxytocin neurons. Nature Neuroscience. 2020; 23:1125–37. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-0674-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Uvnäs-Moberg K. Oxytocin may mediate the benefits of positive social interaction and emotions. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1998; 23:819–35. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(98)00056-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Meier M, Unternaehrer E, Dimitroff SJ, Benz ABE, Bentele UU, Schorpp SM, et al. Standardized massage interventions as protocols for the induction of psychophysiological relaxation in the laboratory: a block randomized, controlled trial. Scientific reports. 2020; 10:1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-56847-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Sumioka H, Nakae A, Kanai R, Ishiguro H. Huggable communication medium decreases cortisol levels. Scientific reports. 2013; 3:1–6. doi: 10.1038/srep03034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ditzen B, Neumann ID, Bodenmann G, Dawans B von, Turner RA, Ehlert U, et al. Effects of different kinds of couple interaction on cortisol and heart rate responses to stress in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007; 32:565–74. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.03.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Grewen KM, Anderson BJ, Girdler SS, Light KC. Warm partner contact is related to lower cardiovascular reactivity. Behav Med. 2003; 29:123–30. doi: 10.1080/08964280309596065 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Pauley PM, Floyd K, Hesse C. The stress-buffering effects of a brief dyadic interaction before an acute stressor. Health Commun. 2015; 30:646–59. Epub 2014/08/04. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2014.888385 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Incollingo Rodriguez AC, Epel ES, White ML, Standen EC, Seckl JR, Tomiyama AJ. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis dysregulation and cortisol activity in obesity: A systematic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015; 62:301–18. Epub 2015/08/21. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.08.014 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Labuschagne I, Grace C, Rendell P, Terrett G, Heinrichs M. An introductory guide to conducting the Trier Social Stress Test. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019; 107:686–95. Epub 2019/09/24. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.09.032 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1988; 54:1063. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Minkley N, Schröder TP, Wolf OT, Kirchner WH. The socially evaluated cold-pressor test (SECPT) for groups: Effects of repeated administration of a combined physiological and psychological stressor. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014; 45:119–27. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.03.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hendrick SS, Dicke A, Hendrick C. The relationship assessment scale. Journal of social and personal relationships. 1998; 15:137–42. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB, Christensen RHB. lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of statistical software. 2017; 82:1–26. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. arXiv preprint arXiv:1406.5823. 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Barr DJ, Levy R, Scheepers C, Tily HJ. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of memory and language. 2013; 68:255–78. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior research methods. 2007; 39:175–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Gimpl G, Fahrenholz F. The oxytocin receptor system: structure, function, and regulation. Physiol Rev. 2001; 81:629–83. doi: 10.1152/physrev.2001.81.2.629 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Russo V, Ottaviani C, Spitoni GF. Affective touch: A meta-analysis on sex differences. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2020; 108:445–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Portnova GV, Proskurnina EV, Sokolova SV, Skorokhodov IV, Varlamov AA. Perceived pleasantness of gentle touch in healthy individuals is related to salivary oxytocin response and EEG markers of arousal. Experimental Brain Research. 2020; 238:2257–68. doi: 10.1007/s00221-020-05891-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Taylor SE, Klein LC, Lewis BP, Gruenewald TL, Gurung RA, Updegraff JA. Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychol Rev. 2000; 107:411–29. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.107.3.411 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Floyd K, Pauley PM, Hesse C. State and Trait Affectionate Communication Buffer Adults’ Stress Reactions. Communication Monographs. 2010; 77:618–36. doi: 10.1080/03637751.2010.498792 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.McCormick CM, Lewis E, Somley B, Kahan TA. Individual differences in cortisol levels and performance on a test of executive function in men and women. Physiology & Behavior. 2007; 91:87–94. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.01.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Wolf OT. Stress and memory retrieval: mechanisms and consequences. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2017; 14:40–6. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Shields GS, Bonner JC, Moons WG. Does cortisol influence core executive functions? A meta-analysis of acute cortisol administration effects on working memory, inhibition, and set-shifting. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015; 58:91–103. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.04.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Shields GS, Sazma MA, Yonelinas AP. The effects of acute stress on core executive functions: A meta-analysis and comparison with cortisol. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2016; 68:651–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Merz CJ, Hagedorn B, Wolf OT. An oral presentation causes stress and memory impairments. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2019; 104:1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.02.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Guenzel FM, Wolf OT, Schwabe L. Stress disrupts response memory retrieval. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013; 38:1460–5. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.12.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Atsak P, Guenzel FM, Kantar-Gok D, Zalachoras I, Yargicoglu P, Meijer OC, et al. Glucocorticoids mediate stress-induced impairment of retrieval of stimulus-response memory. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016; 67:207–15. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.02.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Ringeisen T, Lichtenfeld S, Becker S, Minkley N. Stress experience and performance during an oral exam: the role of self-efficacy, threat appraisals, anxiety, and cortisol. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping. 2019; 32:50–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Mohr M von Kirsch LP, Fotopoulou A. Social touch deprivation during COVID-19: effects on psychological wellbeing and craving interpersonal touch. Royal Society Open Science. 2021; 8:210287. Epub 2021/09/08. doi: 10.1098/rsos.210287 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Schlereth T, Birklein F. The sympathetic nervous system and pain. Neuromol Med. 2008; 10:141–7. Epub 2007/11/08. doi: 10.1007/s12017-007-8018-6 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Wolfram M., Bellingrath S., & Kudielka B. M. (2011). The cortisol awakening response (CAR) across the female menstrual cycle. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36(6), 905–912 doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.12.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.McCullough ME, Churchland PS, Mendez AJ. Problems with measuring peripheral oxytocin: can the data on oxytocin and human behavior be trusted. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2013; 37:1485–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Salari N, Hosseinian-Far A, Jalali R, Vaisi-Raygani A, Rasoulpoor S, Mohammadi M, et al. Prevalence of stress, anxiety, depression among the general population during the COVID-19 pandemic: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Global Health. 2020; 16:57. Epub 2020/07/06. doi: 10.1186/s12992-020-00589-w . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Ettman CK, Abdalla SM, Cohen GH, Sampson L, Vivier PM, Galea S. Prevalence of Depression Symptoms in US Adults Before and During the COVID-19 Pandemic. JAMA Netw Open. 2020; 3:e2019686. Epub 2020/09/01. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.19686 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Marta Panzeri

9 Feb 2022

PONE-D-21-38194Romantic partner embraces reduce cortisol release after acute stress induction in women but not in menPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Packheiser,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please comment about power analysis for sample size

Discuss more in detail gender difference

Please quote pandemic restrictions effects

Please clarify the difference between romantic partner embrace and other types such as parents, friends, etc.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Mar 26 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Marta Panzeri, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

1. When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. We note that the grant information you provided in the ‘Funding Information’ and ‘Financial Disclosure’ sections do not match. 

When you resubmit, please ensure that you provide the correct grant numbers for the awards you received for your study in the ‘Funding Information’ section.

3. We noted in your submission details that a portion of your manuscript may have been presented or published elsewhere. 

( The results have been pre-published at https://psyarxiv.com/32bde/. It is not under consideration elsewhere.)

Please clarify whether this [conference proceeding or publication] was peer-reviewed and formally published. If this work was previously peer-reviewed and published, in the cover letter please provide the reason that this work does not constitute dual publication and should be included in the current manuscript.

 4. In your Data Availability statement, you have not specified where the minimal data set underlying the results described in your manuscript can be found. PLOS defines a study's minimal data set as the underlying data used to reach the conclusions drawn in the manuscript and any additional data required to replicate the reported study findings in their entirety. All PLOS journals require that the minimal data set be made fully available. For more information about our data policy, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability.

Upon re-submitting your revised manuscript, please upload your study’s minimal underlying data set as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and include the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers within your revised cover letter. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories. Any potentially identifying patient information must be fully anonymized.

Important: If there are ethical or legal restrictions to sharing your data publicly, please explain these restrictions in detail. Please see our guidelines for more information on what we consider unacceptable restrictions to publicly sharing data: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions. Note that it is not acceptable for the authors to be the sole named individuals responsible for ensuring data access.

We will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide in your cover letter.

5. Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This is a very interesting study adjudicating the effects of a brief affectionate embrace on physiological reactivity to cold pressor. In my opinion, the study is well designed and executed, and I recommend publication after attention to a few small issues.

Given that the sample comprised heterosexual couples, it is confusing that the numbers of women and men are not equal, until the authors explain that the imbalance “resulted from posthoc exclusions of participants due to failure to meet the inclusion criteria or empty salivettes.” I understand the issue of empty salivettes, but how were participants enrolled in the study in the first place if they didn’t meet inclusion criteria?

I think the authors are overlooking a few relevant studies. For instance, Grewen and colleagues used a similar experimental design but examined the effects of handholding rather than hugging, and Pauley et al. used a similar manipulation with a variety of stress inducers.

This is a fairly small N, so I wonder how the authors arrived at this sample size? Was an a-priori power analysis conducted? Even a post-hoc sensitivity analysis would be useful to justify the sample size.

Regarding the authors’ post-hoc explanation of their unexpected sex difference—and regarding their implication of cortisol, specifically—they might find value in Shelley Taylor’s tend-and-befriend hypothesis, which similarly explains why affectionate behavior may benefit women more than it benefits men (although they should also note Floyd et al.’s experiment, which did not find that sex difference in oxytocinergic reactions).

Floyd, K., Pauley, P. M., & Hesse, C. (2010). State and trait affectionate communication buffer adults’ stress reactions. Communication Monographs, 77(4), 618–636. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637751.2010.498792

Grewen, K. M., Anderson, B. J., Girdler, S. S., & Light, K. C. (2003). Warm partner contact is related to lower cardiovascular reactivity. Behavioral Medicine, 29(3), 123–130. https://doi.org/10.1080/08964280309596065

Pauley, P. M., Floyd, K., & Hesse, C. (2015). The stress-buffering effects of a brief dyadic interaction before an acute stressor. Health Communication, 30(7), 646–659. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2014.888385

Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lewis, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L., Guring, R. A. R., & Updegraff, J. A. (2000). Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: Tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological Review, 107(3), 411–429. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.107.3.411

Reviewer #2: Review of the manuscript entitled “Romantic partner embraces reduce cortisol release after acute stress induction in women but not in men” by Berretz and colleagues.

In this interesting study, the authors tested the effect of a short-term embrace between romantic partners on the subjective/objective measurements of acute stress. They divided 76 participants in a control and in an experimental group, and they induced acute stress via the Socially Evaluated Cold Pressor Test (SECPT). Stress was measured by means of: i) cortisol response, ii) sympathetic response (blood pressures), and iii) subjective affect ratings (PANAS questionnaire). All these measures were collected 1) at baseline, 2) SECPT, 3) 15 minutes after SECPT, 4) 25 minutes after SECPT. Results revealed a reduced cortisol response in the embracing group compared to the control group, but only in women. Neither blood pressure nor subjective ratings were modulated by the embrace.

The study is well-conceived and the manuscript is clear and well-written. Results are original and of interest for both specialists and for a wider audience. I have not major concerns about this manuscript, but only some minor hints that could help the authors to further improve the clarity of the text.

1) Page 5: “The other group did not embrace each other, and the partners only provided social support during the joint testing procedure”. Can the authors better explain what is meant here with “social support”? At a first glance, this is not clear.

2) In the Participants section it is stated that 36 participants were male and 40 were female, but that all participants were in heterosexual relationships. In the following paragraph this point is clarified, but at this point it sounds strange: I suggest explaining here that some participants were excluded because of technical issues.

3) In the same section, the sample’s BMI is given, but I do not understand the reason why it can be useful. Can the authors justify this information? Alternatively, maybe it can be deleted.

4) Did the authors ask to participants the duration of their relationship? Can this info alter the effect of the embrace? In other words, can we expect a different effect of the embrace in accordance with the duration of the relationship (e.g., stronger effect in long-lasting relations?).

5) Page 8: “Following the SECPT, the couples were separated from each other to fill out the Relationship Assessment Scale (RAS) in an unbiased manner”. Please, explain the “unbiased manner”: what does it mean?

6) From a theoretical point of view, can we expect different results according to the specific person involved in the embrace? Specifically, is the embrace of the romantic partner similar to that of the mother? Can we expect different cortisol effect according to the “role” of the persons? What can we expect of the embrace is carried out by an unknown person? In other words, is the embrace per se or the subjective experience of embracing the partner responsible for the effect found here? This point should be discussed, at least in the final section of the manuscript.

7) In the last sentence, I would also add a reference to the pandemic: reading the manuscript, I think to different everyday situations, such as exams, oral presentations or job interviews – as listed by the authors – but also to the importance of the physical contact in this period of social distancing. I would like to suggest inserting this crucial applied implication of this result.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes: Giulia Prete

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2022 May 18;17(5):e0266887. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0266887.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


9 Mar 2022

Response to Reviews

Comments by the editor:

Dear Dr. Packheiser,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please comment about power analysis for sample size

Discuss more in detail gender difference

Please quote pandemic restrictions effects

Please clarify the difference between romantic partner embrace and other types such as parents, friends, etc.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Marta Panzeri, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Response:

We thank the editor for her positive assessment of our manuscript. Attached you will find our response letter in which we address the concerns of the reviewer point-by-point. The changes made in accordance with the reviewers are highlighted in the marked manuscript. If longer passages have been added to the manuscript, we explicitly state them in the response letter. We explicitly also want to thank both reviewers for their helpful comments that significantly improved the manuscript conceptually and heightened its relevance with respect to the ongoing pandemic. An important note to the reviewers is that we changed the citation style in the manuscript in accordance with the PloS One guidelines. However, we chose to use non-numbered citations when we referenced the relevant sections in the response letter to facilitate what additional research has been incorporated in the manuscript.

Reviewer comments:

Reviewer #1:

This is a very interesting study adjudicating the effects of a brief affectionate embrace on physiological reactivity to cold pressor. In my opinion, the study is well designed and executed, and I recommend publication after attention to a few small issues.

Response:

We thank the reviewer for their positive evaluation and helpful comments on our study.

Point 1:

Given that the sample comprised heterosexual couples, it is confusing that the numbers of women and men are not equal, until the authors explain that the imbalance “resulted from posthoc exclusions of participants due to failure to meet the inclusion criteria or empty salivettes.” I understand the issue of empty salivettes, but how were participants enrolled in the study in the first place if they didn’t meet inclusion criteria?

Response:

We agree with the reviewer that this must have seemed confusing as both reviewers stumbled over this particular paragraph. In accordance with the second reviewer, we re-structured this paragraph so that the information about the exclusion due to technical issues appears earlier in the manuscript. With regard to the post-hoc exclusion of participants, contact was in most cases only established with one romantic partner during recruitment. Thus, on rare occasions, the other partner was not informed about all relevant inclusion criteria before arriving at the study location. In these cases, we decided to nonetheless move forward with the study protocol as at least one participant was meeting the inclusion criteria. The other partner was excluded. In accordance with the second reviewer, we simply state that the numerical imbalance was due to technical issues as this detailed explanation seems unnecessary for the reader.

It reads:

“The numerical imbalance in the groups resulted from post hoc exclusion of participants due to technical issues.”

Point 2:

I think the authors are overlooking a few relevant studies. For instance, Grewen and colleagues used a similar experimental design but examined the effects of handholding rather than hugging, and Pauley et al. used a similar manipulation with a variety of stress inducers.

Response:

We are grateful that the reviewer has pointed us towards these extremely relevant studies. We added information on both studies to the introduction and discussion. The section in the introduction reads:

“In line with these findings, Grewen, Anderson, Girdler, and Light (2003) compared cohabitating couples that either held hands for a prolonged period (10min) and shortly embraced each other afterwards (20s) or did not touch each other prior to a public-speaking task. They found that the mutual physical contact attenuated systolic, diastolic blood pressure and heart rate increases compared to the control group. Finally, Pauley, Floyd, and Hesse (2015) subjected romantic couples or platonic friends to acute stress after they talked either about fond mutual memories for a period of 10 minutes and embraced afterwards (< 10s duration), only shared each other’s presence or waited separately for the stress induction to begin. The authors observed an attenuated cardiovascular stress response in all participants that went through the affectionate communication plus embracing condition. For HPA-axis activation, the findings were moderated by the relationship type as platonic friends showed a stronger increase in cortisol after expressing mutual affection compared to romantic partners that only shared each other’s presence.”

The sections in the discussion read:

“It remains however an open question whether this effect is limited to romantic partner embraces or generalizes also to embraces between platonic friends. A recent survey study investigating the role of social touch on mental health aspects such as feelings of anxiety found that health benefits seem to be mostly linked to non-sexual intimate touch (Mohr, Kirsch, & Fotopoulou, 2021). This finding would be in line with the observed effect in the study by Pauley et al. (2015) in which relationship type moderated the cortisol reactivity in the participants. Future research is needed to further investigate this issue.

In contrast to our hypotheses, we did not observe any changes in sympathetic nervous system activity indicated by the absence of group difference in blood pressure, regardless of sex. This result is not in accordance with previous findings where prolonged exposure to social touch or affectionate communication resulted in reduced sympathetic nervous system activity indicated by decreased blood pressure and heart rate or increased heart rate variability (Ditzen et al., 2007; Grewen et al., 2003; Meier et al., 2020; Pauley et al., 2015). A possible explanation could relate to the pain associated with the SECPT as no other of the previously reported studies used a painful stimulus to induce stress. Pain strongly engages the sympathetic nervous system and is thus difficult to reduce (Schlereth & Birklein, 2008).”

Mohr, M. von, Kirsch, L. P., & Fotopoulou, A. (2021). Social touch deprivation during COVID-19: Effects on psychological wellbeing and craving interpersonal touch. Royal Society Open Science, 8(9), 210287. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.210287

Schlereth, T., & Birklein, F. (2008). The sympathetic nervous system and pain. NeuroMolecular Medicine, 10(3), 141–147. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12017-007-8018-6

Point 3:

This is a fairly small N, so I wonder how the authors arrived at this sample size? Was an a-priori power analysis conducted? Even a post-hoc sensitivity analysis would be useful to justify the sample size.

Response:

We agree with the reviewer that the sample could have been extended. The limited sample size was largely due to difficulties in data acquisition during the pandemic. While we did not conduct an a priori power analysis, we conducted a post hoc sensitivity analysis in g*Power in accordance with the reviewer’s suggestion. First, we determined the effect size of the three-way interaction in the model by subtracting the R² value of a model containing only main effects from the full interaction model. The effect size of the interaction alone was at R² = 0.042 or Cohen’s f = 0.21. A sensitivity analysis was conducted in g*Power (Faul et al., 2007) in a repeated measures within-between subject interaction model containing four groups (embracing, control, male, female) and four measurements. Correlations between measures were on average high (r = 0.7) and sphericity was violated (Greenhouse-Geisser ε = 0.62). Using these settings, 80% power would have been achieved with an effect of Cohen’s f = 0.14. We added a paragraph in the methods section that reads:

“A post hoc sensitvity analysis was conducted to determine how much power would have been necessary given the present sample size to detect within-between interaction effect using g*Power (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007). Here, we used four groups, four repeated measures, a repeated measures correlation of r = 0.7 and non-sphericity correction of ε = 0.62 as inputs. 80% power would have been achieved at interaction effects greater than Cohen’s f = 0.14.”

We added the effect size for the three-way interactions of each dependent variable that was evaluated via a mixed-model to the supplementary information.

Supplementary table 1. Effect sizes of the models with and without interactions for cortisol, diastolic and systolic blood pressure as well as positive and negative affect. Both R² and Cohen’s f are given as effect size measures.

Cortisol Systolic BP Diastolic BP Positive Affect Negative Affect

Effect size without interactions R² = 0.164

f = 0.44 R² = 0.388

f = 0.80 R² = 0.237

f = 0.56 R² = 0.038

f = 0.20 R² = 0.049

f = 0.23

Effect size with interactions

R² = 0.206

f = 0.51 R² = 0.388

f = 0.80 R² = 0.239

f = 0.56 R² = 0.055

f = 0.24 R² = 0.062

f = 0.26

Interaction effect alone R² = 0.042

f = 0.21 R² = 0

f = 0 R² = 0.002

f = 0.04 R² = 0.017

f = 0.13 R² = 0.013

f = 0.11

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.‑G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191.

We also estimated the effect size of the relevant comparison between the female embracing and control group in the linear mixed model using the eff_size function from the emmeans package. This way, random effects and covariates are accounted for in the effect size estimation. Here, a large effect size was estimated (Cohen’s f = 0.62, standard error = 0.27). Using this effect size in a simple independent sample t-test model revealed 96.6% power to detect the effect. We are therefore confident that the employed sample size was sufficient for the present study. We hope that the reviewer agrees with our assessment.

Point 4:

Regarding the authors’ post-hoc explanation of their unexpected sex difference—and regarding their implication of cortisol, specifically—they might find value in Shelley Taylor’s tend-and-befriend hypothesis, which similarly explains why affectionate behavior may benefit women more than it benefits men (although they should also note Floyd et al.’s experiment, which did not find that sex difference in oxytocinergic reactions).

Response:

We thank the reviewer for pointing this out as this hypothesis provides a further possible explanation for the observed sex difference. We added the suggested literature to the discussion. The section reads:

“Another explanation could relate to the “tend-and-befriend” hypothesis which states that women respond differentially to stress-inducing events in the environment compared to men (Taylor et al., 2000). According to this hypothesis, women respond to stressful situations via an increase in care and protection for offspring, which in turn reduces the bodily stress response through an increase in oxytocin. This evolutionary conserved mechanism to increase offspring survival could thus generalize to a variety of stressful situations. It should be noted however that a study investigating the oxytocin release in response to stress did not find marked sex differences (Floyd et al., 2010). Thus, this sex difference requires additional research.”

Floyd, K., Pauley, P. M., & Hesse, C. (2010). State and trait affectionate communication buffer adults’ stress reactions. Communication Monographs, 77(4), 618–636. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637751.2010.498792

Grewen, K. M., Anderson, B. J., Girdler, S. S., & Light, K. C. (2003). Warm partner contact is related to lower cardiovascular reactivity. Behavioral Medicine, 29(3), 123–130. https://doi.org/10.1080/08964280309596065

Pauley, P. M., Floyd, K., & Hesse, C. (2015). The stress-buffering effects of a brief dyadic interaction before an acute stressor. Health Communication, 30(7), 646–659. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2014.888385

Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lewis, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L., Guring, R. A. R., & Updegraff, J. A. (2000). Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: Tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological Review, 107(3), 411–429. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.107.3.411

Reviewer #2:

Review of the manuscript entitled “Romantic partner embraces reduce cortisol release after acute stress induction in women but not in men” by Berretz and colleagues.

In this interesting study, the authors tested the effect of a short-term embrace between romantic partners on the subjective/objective measurements of acute stress. They divided 76 participants in a control and in an experimental group, and they induced acute stress via the Socially Evaluated Cold Pressor Test (SECPT). Stress was measured by means of: i) cortisol response, ii) sympathetic response (blood pressures), and iii) subjective affect ratings (PANAS questionnaire). All these measures were collected 1) at baseline, 2) SECPT, 3) 15 minutes after SECPT, 4) 25 minutes after SECPT. Results revealed a reduced cortisol response in the embracing group compared to the control group, but only in women. Neither blood pressure nor subjective ratings were modulated by the embrace.

The study is well-conceived and the manuscript is clear and well-written. Results are original and of interest for both specialists and for a wider audience. I have not major concerns about this manuscript, but only some minor hints that could help the authors to further improve the clarity of the text.

Response:

We thank the reviewer for her positive and constructive feedback on our study.

Point 1:

Page 5: “The other group did not embrace each other, and the partners only provided social support during the joint testing procedure”. Can the authors better explain what is meant here with “social support”? At a first glance, this is not clear.

Response:

We thank the reviewer for pointing this out. The couples were not prohibited to talk to each other prior to the SECPT in the either group. We changed this sentence to provide clarification on this matter, as we did not specify how the support was constituted. It reads: “The other group did not embrace each other and partners only provided support through their physical presence.”

Point 2:

In the Participants section it is stated that 36 participants were male and 40 were female, but that all participants were in heterosexual relationships. In the following paragraph this point is clarified, but at this point it sounds strange: I suggest explaining here that some participants were excluded because of technical issues.

Response:

Both reviewers had an issue with this description. We explained the underlying technical issues under point 1 for reviewer 1. In accordance with your comments, we now state that participants were excluded due to technical issues in the participants section.

Point 3:

In the same section, the sample’s BMI is given, but I do not understand the reason why it can be useful. Can the authors justify this information? Alternatively, maybe it can be deleted.

Response:

Obesity has been linked to a systematic increase in HPA-axis responsivity. Thus, participants with considerable overweight were excluded from the study. We added this information to the manuscript. It reads:

“Furthermore, the body mass index was between 18.5 and 27 since obesity has been linked to systematic increases in HPA-axis responsivity (Rodriguez et al., 2015).”

Rodriguez, A. C. I., Epel, E. S., White, M. L., Standen, E. C., Seckl, J. R., & Tomiyama, A. J. (2015). Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis dysregulation and cortisol activity in obesity: a systematic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 62, 301-318.

Point 4:

Did the authors ask to participants the duration of their relationship? Can this info alter the effect of the embrace? In other words, can we expect a different effect of the embrace in accordance with the duration of the relationship (e.g., stronger effect in long-lasting relations?).

Response:

Information on relationship duration was unfortunately not assessed. While it could be a possible moderator in the present study, we believe that it is more likely that the relationship satisfaction would play a more direct role to the embracing effect. We explored this possibility and added the RAS score to the model as a further variable. The results were unchanged, but relationship satisfaction did not reach significance indicating that it did not play a major role in the effect of the embrace.

Point 5:

Page 8: “Following the SECPT, the couples were separated from each other to fill out the Relationship Assessment Scale (RAS) in an unbiased manner”. Please, explain the “unbiased manner”: what does it mean?

Response:

We rephrased this sentence to be clearer. We meant that the participants would have been likely influenced by the presence of their partner and would have likely avoided negative ratings of the relationship. We clarified this in the revised manuscript. It reads:

“This was done to prevent any influence on the rating by the romantic partners by having to fill out the questionnaire next to them.”

Point 6:

From a theoretical point of view, can we expect different results according to the specific person involved in the embrace? Specifically, is the embrace of the romantic partner similar to that of the mother? Can we expect different cortisol effect according to the “role” of the persons? What can we expect of the embrace is carried out by an unknown person? In other words, is the embrace per se or the subjective experience of embracing the partner responsible for the effect found here? This point should be discussed, at least in the final section of the manuscript.

Response:

The reviewer raises a very interesting point here. Indeed, the relationship between the embracing individuals is likely to play a role in these experiments as a recent survey study has demonstrated that only intimate (non-sexual) social touch can for example decreases feelings of anxiety and generally increase psychological well-being (Mohr et al., 2021). Friendly and professional touch did not provide a similar effect. Thus, the observed effect in our study seems to be limited to either romantic partners or close friends. We added a paragraph on this in the discussion.

It reads:

“It remains however an open question whether this effect is limited to romantic partner embraces or generalizes also to embraces between platonic friends. A recent survey study investigating the role of social touch on mental health aspects such as feelings of anxiety found that health benefits seem to be mostly linked to non-sexual intimate touch (Mohr, Kirsch, & Fotopoulou, 2021). This finding would be in line with the observed effect in the study by Pauley et al. (2015) in which relationship type moderated the cortisol reactivity in the participants. Future research is needed to further investigate this issue.”

Point 7:

In the last sentence, I would also add a reference to the pandemic: reading the manuscript, I think to different everyday situations, such as exams, oral presentations or job interviews – as listed by the authors – but also to the importance of the physical contact in this period of social distancing. I would like to suggest inserting this crucial applied implication of this result.

Response:

We fully agree with the reviewer that the pandemic should be mentioned given the importance of social touch in physical and mental well-being and the current restrictions and social distancing mandate. We added this to the last paragraph.

It reads:

“Importantly, we also want to highlight our findings in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic that has substantially increased stress and depression levels in many individuals due to economic and social restrictions (Ettman et al., 2020; Salari et al., 2020). It could be conceived that these increase in everyday stress are in part to the lack of social affective touch through the means of for example embraces. Future research needs to be conducted to identify the long-term impact of the pandemic on stress and social touch behavior.”

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers Hugging and Stress.docx

Decision Letter 1

Marta Panzeri

30 Mar 2022

Romantic partner embraces reduce cortisol release after acute stress induction in women but not in men

PONE-D-21-38194R1

Dear Dr. Packheiser,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Marta Panzeri, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: No

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: I would like to congratulate with the authors for their great work! They fully considered all the suggestions received and I believe the present version of the manuscript is now clearer and complete. I just want to suggest a couple minor hints which can be helpful in providing further support for the conclusions of the present study.

Firstly, concerning the explanation of the sex difference, a reference could also be made to different results showing that females are more “responsive” than males to “social stimuli” in general, which can be intended as in line with the results found here (see for instance https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/28175962/ https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19083993/ https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18461176/).

Secondly, concerning the importance of the present results in the light of the pandemic, I want to indicate a study carried out *during* the quarantine imposed by the government at the beginning of the pandemic, revealing that persons who were highly fearful for the COVID-19 (higher worry), were objectively the most anxious as measured by means of standardized tests (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33362631/). This is in line with the conclusions proposed by the authors and can be added to further support this view.

Finally, I want to express a special mention for the effort made to add all the new statistical evidence, which surely increases the completeness of the manuscript!

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Acceptance letter

Marta Panzeri

26 Apr 2022

PONE-D-21-38194R1

Romantic partner embraces reduce cortisol release after acute stress induction in women but not in men

Dear Dr. Packheiser:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Marta Panzeri

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. Effect sizes of the models with and without interactions for cortisol, diastolic and systolic blood pressure as well as positive and negative affect.

    Both R2 and Cohen’s f are given as effect size measures.

    (DOCX)

    S1 Fig

    Systolic blood pressure for women (top) and men (bottom) during baseline, the SECPT and 15 minutes as well as 25 minutes post SECPT for the embrace and control condition. White dots represent the median value for each group. Error bars represent the upper and lower quartiles.

    (TIF)

    S2 Fig

    Diastolic blood pressure for women (top) and men (bottom) during baseline, the SECPT and 15 minutes as well as 25 minutes post SECPT for the embrace and control condition. White dots represent the median value for each group. Error bars represent the upper and lower quartiles.

    (TIF)

    S3 Fig

    Positive affect ratings for women (top) and men (bottom) during the baseline, SECPT and 15 minutes as well as 25 minutes post SECPT for the embrace and control condition. White dots represent the median value for each group. Error bars represent the upper and lower quartiles.

    (TIF)

    S4 Fig

    Negative affect ratings for women (top) and men (bottom) during the baseline, SECPT and 15 minutes as well as 25 minutes post SECPT for the embrace and control condition. White dots represent the median value for each group. Error bars represent the upper and lower quartiles.

    (TIF)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers Hugging and Stress.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    Ethical approval was not provided by the participants to make the data publicly available. The authors therefore are not allowed to upload the data to a public repository as they contain demographic data that allows for potential identification of the participants. For questions regarding the ethics approval, please contact the relevant ethics committee (ethikkommission-psychologie@rub.de). To make the data accessible, we uploaded both data and code to a private repository in Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/8gyr2/). Requests for the data can be made to two of the authors (gesa.berretz@rub.de or j.packheiser@nin.knaw.nl) or to the local ethics committee of the Psychology Department at Ruhr University Bochum (ethikkommission-psychologie@rub.de).


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES