Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2022 May 19;17(5):e0267580. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0267580

A randomized controlled trial of a video intervention shows evidence of increasing COVID-19 vaccination intention

Leah S Witus 1,‡,*, Erik Larson 2,‡,*
Editor: Ali B Mahmoud3
PMCID: PMC9119500  PMID: 35587462

Abstract

Increasing acceptance of COVID-19 vaccines is imperative for public health. Previous research on educational interventions to overcome vaccine hesitancy have shown mixed effects in increasing vaccination intention, although much of this work has focused on parental attitudes toward childhood vaccination. In this study, we conducted a randomized controlled trial to investigate whether vaccination intention changes after viewing an animated YouTube video explaining how COVID-19 mRNA vaccines work. We exposed participants to one of four interventions–watching the video with a male narrator, watching the same video with a female narrator, reading the text of the transcript of the video, or receiving no information (control group). We found that participants who watched the version of the video with a male narrator expressed statistically significant increased vaccination intention compared to the control group. The video with a female narrator had more variation in results. As a whole, there was a non-significant increased vaccination intention when analyzing all participants who saw the video with a female narrator; however, for politically conservative participants there was decreased vaccination intention for this intervention compared to the control group at a threshold between being currently undecided and expressing probable interest. These results are encouraging for the ability of interventions as simple as YouTube videos to increase vaccination propensity, although the inconsistent response to the video with a female narrator demonstrates the potential for bias to affect how certain groups respond to different messengers.

Introduction

Even before COVID-19 became a pandemic in 2020, the World Health Organization considered vaccine hesitancy a top global health threat [1]. At the time this study was performed, in February 2021, Emergency Use Authorization had been issued by the United States Food and Drug Administration for two mRNA vaccines (Pfizer-BioNTech and Moderna) for the prevention of COVID-19, representing a tremendous public health success in their high efficacy and in the speed of their development. While vaccine production and access rapidly increased in the United States and worldwide throughout the spring of 2021, overcoming vaccine hesitancy to achieve high rates of vaccine uptake became increasingly important for public health [2,3]. Although studies found that COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy declined during the initial stages of vaccine roll out [4], rates of vaccine hesitancy still remain concerningly high, particularly when unvaccinated individuals may pose a risk to members of society who are unable to be vaccinated [5]. Therefore, research to collect empirical evidence on the effect of interventions to increase vaccination intention is needed.

This study examines an eight-minute, animated, educational YouTube video on the COVID-19 mRNA vaccines that we made called “COVID mRNA vaccines explained.” The video explains how the mRNA vaccine platforms work, highlights some of the positive features of the COVID-19 mRNA vaccines such as high efficacy and rarity of serious side effects, and emphasizes the altruism of vaccination. While the lack of editorial oversight of health information on YouTube and other social media platforms has contributed to the spread of vaccine misinformation and disinformation [68], YouTube videos could constitute an attractive tool for public health campaigns as they are easily disseminated through existing websites and social media platforms [9]. Given the novelty of the COVID-19 vaccine platforms, and the urge to help end the pandemic through promoting vaccination, there are many health professionals, professors, and others who have been making and posting vaccine explainer videos for YouTube and other social media platforms such as Tiktok. However, there is little evidence about whether creating and sharing such videos is worthwhile: do these efforts actually affect vaccination propensity? Previous research on web-based educational interventions to increase vaccine acceptance, focused largely on parental attitudes toward childhood vaccinations, has found mixed results [1012]–some studies found evidence of increased intention to vaccinate [13,14], some found no effect or effects that were not statistically significant [15,16], and some even reported a backfire effect wherein the intervention decreased vaccination intention [17,18]. The prior literature highlights the need to continue such studies to identify effective interventions for overcoming vaccine hesitancy [11,1921].

Herein we present the results of a randomized controlled trial conducted with 1,184 Mechanical Turk participants to investigate the effect on vaccination intention of an animated YouTube video explaining how the COVID-19 mRNA vaccines work. We investigated whether the exact same information presented in different formats would alter the efficacy of the communication by exposing treatment groups to either: a) watching the video with a male narrator (Male-Narr-Video https://youtu.be/Fv5bs4SPiYE), b) watching the same video with a female narrator (Female-Narr-Video https://youtu.be/j3hTeDyvgPs), c) reading a blog post containing the text of the transcript of the video, or d) receiving no information to serve as a control group. The visuals and scripts for both versions of the videos were identical; only the voice of the narrator differed. The results showed statistically significant higher vaccination intention in the group that watched the Male-Narr-Video compared to the control group, a robust association for a variety of alternative specifications. However, despite participants rating the quality of instruction of both the Male-Narr-Video and Female-Narr-Video equally highly, we found less consistent associations on intention to vaccinate for participants exposed to the Female-Narr-Video intervention. As a whole, the Female-Narr-Video produced a non-significant increase in vaccination intention compared to the control group. However, the impact on vaccination intention of the Female-Narr-Video varied by political identity of the participants. Political conservatives who were exposed to the Female-Narr-Video were less likely to express probable or definite interest in getting vaccinated than conservative respondents exposed to no information in the control group and other respondents exposed to the Female-Narr-Video (political liberals or those who identified as neither liberal nor conservative). Adjusting for this conditional association showed that for respondents who did not identify as politically conservative, both videos associated with increased vaccine intention at much closer rates, no matter the narrator. No such conditioning among political conservatives was observed for participants exposed to the Male-Narr-Video. Although further research is warranted to investigate the variation in efficacy based on the gender of the narrator, overall, this evidence supports the idea that educational YouTube videos, such as the Male-Narr-Video, may serve as an easy to share, simple way to increase vaccination intention.

Background

Video development

The YouTube video was made by LSW in December 2020, shortly after the issuance of Emergency Use Authorizations by the United States Food and Drug Association for the Pfizer-BioNTech and Moderna vaccines for COVID-19, which are both mRNA vaccines. The programs Biorender and Vyond were used to create the video, which uses animations and cartoon characters to illustrate educational points about how the COVID-19 mRNA vaccines work. The full text of the video can be found in the Supplementary Information.

The Female-Narr-Video was the original version created in December 2020 for educational purposes (although the title slide and link on YouTube were changed for this study), and the Male-Narr-Video was created later as an additional experimental condition. In other words, the creation of the Female-Narr-Video was independent of the idea of testing whether narrator gender might associate with its effectiveness; only after receiving positive anecdotal feedback on the original video did we decide to study the efficacy of such variations. Given findings that audiences draw on negative stereotypes about women who communicate scientific findings, and rate women teaching about science lower than men, [22,23], we wondered whether reactions to the video could vary due to the gender of the narrator’s voice.

The video begins with an explanation of the flow of genetic information, then explains how weakened virus vaccines work before explaining what an mRNA vaccine means. The video then emphasizes that the Pfizer-BioNTech (referred to as Pfizer in the video) and Moderna vaccines have been tested for safety and efficacy, and ends with an explanation of how getting vaccinated can protect others in society.

Previous research on vaccine hesitancy interventions has identified common features for successful communication. This video incorporates many of these features, including: i) The video emphasizes the messages that approved vaccines are safe and go through evaluations [9], and that immunization also protects others, which has been found to activate positive emotions [2]. ii) The video does not focus on messages that have been reported to impede efforts to diminish vaccine hesitancy such as myth-busting, fear-based, or numerically focused messages [10]. iii) The video creates clear bottom-line (gist) messages through: explanations of concepts in simple language rather than scientific jargon; the use of animated illustrations to visually reinforce the concepts explained verbally; and the use of text on the screen to underscore take-home messages. Research in medical decision making has found that gist messages contribute to decision-making in a meaningful way relative to statistics and verbatim information [6,7,10], and previous research on vaccine communication has found that articles with gist were shared on social media more than those without [24].

At the time of making the video, there was not yet much literature on recommendations for interventions specific to the COVID-19 vaccines. In some respects, the unique characteristics of the COVID-19 vaccine may alleviate some of the attitudes that have contributed to parental vaccine hesitancy for childhood vaccinations. For instance, it has been hypothesized that the benefits of some vaccinations may not seem tangible due to unfamiliarity with the harm of the preventable disease [9,25], which may be less likely for COVID-19. However, there are new concerns particular to COVID-19 vaccines that have been reported [2,21], and the video does address some of these, including unease over the new mRNA vaccine platform that had not been used before, and concern over the speed of the vaccine development and approval (the video does not go into all of the details that contributed to the rapid vaccine development but does explain that the mRNA platform may allow quicker vaccine development than weakened virus platforms). Although previous research has shown that vaccine hesitancy is often more complex than a simple knowledge deficit [10,19], the overall tone of the video is an educational approach to influencing vaccination attitudes because in the case of the COVID-19 vaccines, the lack of knowledge about the new vaccine platform may contribute to hesitancy [26].

Previous research has indicated that the messaging matters in addition to the message—that is, it is not just what one says, but how it is said that influences the effectiveness of video communication [27,28]. This research has recommended that vaccine interventions use an enthusiastic tone [10] and establish the public health educator as an expert [7]. Although the video does not specify the credentials of the creator, the survey participants were told that the video was created by a college biochemistry professor. This study tests the effectiveness of a video that draws upon the best evidence in the vaccine hesitancy intervention literature for the message (information content) and messaging (style of communication) of vaccine communications, and additionally allows us to study whether there may be different patterns of effectiveness based on the messenger (the person who delivers the message).

Because the narrator is never depicted in the video, we saw the video as a platform to isolate and test the effects of a single variable related to the messenger: the gender of the narrator’s voice. Therefore, two versions were used in the study, identical but for the voice of the narrator: the Female-Narr-Video version had a female voice reading the transcript and the Male-Narr-Video version had a male voice. In both cases the timing of the narration was matched to the animations (which led to a 3 second difference in length of the videos). The videos used in the survey were posted on unlisted YouTube sites, and reposted on the following links for others beyond the study participants to see: Male-Narr-Video https://youtu.be/Fv5bs4SPiYE Female-Narr-Video https://youtu.be/j3hTeDyvgPs.

These videos have different title pages than the original video posted before the study was conceived.

Methods

Survey design

Participants completed a survey to measure COVID-19 vaccination intention, attitudes, and understanding. Many of the survey items were similar to validated items from the literature, with some adaptations since many of the previous scales of vaccine hesitancy have been used to measure parental attitudes towards childhood vaccinations [29]. Using the Qualtrics survey platform, participants were randomly equally distributed into one of 4 paths: In paths 1, 2, and 3 the consent information page was followed by a page where participants watched the Male-Narr-Video, the Female-Narr-Video, or read the text of the video transcript presented as a blog post. On each of these pages, the time the participants spent on the page was recorded and advancing to the next page was disabled for a few minutes (approximately for half of the length of the video and two minutes for the approximately 1,200 word text) to increase the likelihood that participants engaged the material. After the information was presented in the video or text format, the participants encountered a section with questions that first asked about their intent to get vaccinated and then asked about their attitudes concerning the COVID-19 vaccines. The participants in the 4th path did not receive any information and went directly to the vaccine intention and attitudes section.

The vaccination intention item was based on the Imperial College London “Global attitudes towards a COVID-19 vaccine” survey [4] and asked “If a COVID-19 vaccine were made available to you this week, would you get it?” Response categories were: (1) Definitely no; (2) Probably no; (3) Undecided as of now; (4) Probably yes; (5) Definitely yes; and (6) I already got one or more dose of a COVID19 vaccine. This section also included questions on multiple domains related to vaccine hesitancy attitudes: a) perceptions of vaccine safety and efficacy, b) trust in medical, scientific, governmental and pharmaceutical authorities, c) common vaccine misconceptions and d) understanding of elements of vaccination (see Supplementary Information for full survey questions). The next section of the survey asked about characteristics of the video or blog post, which included an attention check question, questions about the enjoyment of the video/blog post, and how likely participants would be to share it on social media, and was only shown to paths 1–3. This section also asked participants to rate the quality of instruction by the narrator and on the narrator’s trustworthiness, comfort and knowledge. All paths of the survey then went to a block of demographic questions and ended with a code to enter for payment on Amazon Mechanical Turk (As the most direct measure of vaccine hesitancy is the vaccination intent survey item, the analyses in this paper focus on the responses to that question. Subsequent research may examine the other survey items in greater depth).

Experimental set up

The survey was administered to 1,632 participants on Amazon Mechanical Turk on February 25th and 26th, 2021 when the two approved COVID-19 vaccines in the United States were the Pfizer-BioNTech and Moderna mRNA vaccines. Guided by contemporary findings that around 40% of people would get a vaccine if offered one [4] and a desire to detect approximately a 10 percentage point difference at a power of 0.8 at a 95% confidence level, we selected a sample size of 400 for each group. Although not targeted for the analytical method we use, a z test for difference between two independent proportions with these parameters yields group sample sizes of 388, which is higher than the sample size suggested for similar parameters in logistic regression [30]. The only worker qualification required was that the Turk worker be located in the United States. Mechanical Turk is well-validated and considered a reliable source for survey data [3133]. The pay was $5, based on a $15/h wage and a 20-minute estimate of the average completion time. The study was approved by the Macalester College Institutional Review Board (Approval #022103) and all research was performed in accordance with relevant guidelines/regulations of the Macalester Institutional Review Board. Informed consent was obtained by providing participants with written consent information at the beginning of the study, and participants indicated their consent by continuing forward with the survey. Signed consent forms were not collected to preserve the anonymity of the participants. The dataset used for this analysis is available in the S1 Appendix.

Unless otherwise noted, the exclusion criteria that were applied were: exclusion of participants who had indicated they had already been vaccinated, exclusion of participants who answered the attention check question incorrectly, and exclusion of participants who spent less than 7 min on the video page (for a 7 min 37 sec video) or who spent less than 2 min 5 sec on the blog post page. These criteria resulted in exclusion of 321 (28.6%) of respondents in one of the experimental conditions, consisting of 65 excluded for both reasons, 184 excluded for the attention check alone, and 62 excluded on the basis of time on task alone. This exclusion rate is consistent with estimates of untrustworthy MTurk workers [34]. The resulting sample consists of n = 270 for the Male-Narr-Video group, n = 255 for the Female-Narr-Video group, n = 282 for the text group, and n = 393 for the control group.

Data treatment and planned analysis

Since vaccination attitudes exist on a continuum with vaccine refusal on one end, active demand on the other, and uncertainty and hesitation in the middle [35,36], we considered vaccine intention as a continuum rather than a simple hesitant/intent dichotomy. Additionally, following previous research, we considered the possibility that the effects of an intervention may differ at distinct parts of the continuum [36]. This presumption of asymmetric effects is also consistent with the idea that vaccine intent could be conceived as a series of stages, separated by thresholds, as noted in health research [37]. Given this understanding, we modeled how exposure to each intervention associated with vaccine intention by using partial proportional odds models (generalized ordered logit) with the gologit2 program in Stata using the program’s autofit option and robust standard errors [38,39]. These models preserve the order across the categories of vaccine intent while also allowing for either a single coefficient for a variable across the different levels of intent or for separate coefficients for each level of intent. Thus, some variables may associate with a consistent change in moving from one level to the next level of vaccine intention across different levels of the continuum, while other variables may have asymmetric associations, such as varying levels of intensity or direction at different levels of vaccine intention. The findings discussed below were robust to a variety of alternative specifications, as described in the Supplementary Information.

Although participants were randomly assigned to treatment conditions, we included a series of control variables (respondents’ race/ethnicity, gender, political ideology, age, and education) in our models that may associate with vaccine intention. Mechanical Turk respondents are a diverse, but not nationally representative group [32] (see S1 Table in S1 Appendix for the frequency of these variables). We excluded variables for which there were no significant associations in the multivariate models for the sake of parsimony. The race/ethnicity variables are Black and Native American compared to all others who identify as neither. While some studies suggest that Hispanic/Latinx individuals may exhibit lower vaccine intent [40,41], and while Black, Indigenous, and Hispanic/Latinx populations have all experienced histories of medical abuse that may decrease current medical trust [4042], we found no association between Hispanic/Latinx and vaccine intent in our analysis. (This finding of no association between Latinx/Hispanic respondents and vaccine intent may be due to within-group variability that both associates with intent and decreases the likelihood of performing Mechanical Turk work tasks [42]. Additionally, the included variables may account for some of the Latinx/Hispanic within-group variation, since 9% of in-sample respondents who identified at Latinx/Hispanic also identified as Black or Native American; previous research finds of visible markers of African and indigenous ancestry [43] can associate with health-related differences among Latinx/Hispanic people.)

In addition, we included separate variables for people who described themselves as politically conservative and people who described themselves as politically liberal, comparing them to those identified politically as neither conservative nor liberal (political moderates and those who identify as something else). While trust in science is politicized, studies find differences in how political identities relate to such trust [44,45]. Although political liberals may generally trust science more than others [45], conservatives may express greater distrust than others, linked more to cultural division and collective identity [46,47], suggesting that both groups should be examined in relation to moderates and others. Political conservatives in the US have been found to have lower vaccine propensity, linked in part to patterns of trust in expertise [48]. Specifically, conservatives often see science as needing to conform to beliefs about religious authority and common sense [47], which also provide similar bases for beliefs about hierarchical, traditional gender relations [49]. Given the persistence of beliefs about gender and scientific ability [22,5052] and findings that violating traditional gender beliefs can induce negative reactions, particularly among political conservatives [53], we test the possibility that there may be conditioning of how political conservatives receive messages delivered by female experts by using an interaction term between these variables.

Results and discussion

We first estimated a model with the treatment variables and control variables (Model 1), and then estimated a model testing whether the association between Female-Narr-Video and vaccine intent varied based on respondents identifying as conservative (Model 2). Table 1 shows the results from these two models.

Table 1. Partial proportional odds ordered logit models for intention to vaccinate.

Model 1 Model 2
Coef. s.e. p Coef. s.e. p

Male-Narr-Video intervention

0.440
0.160 .006 0.440 0.159 .006

Female-Narr-Video
intervention
0.312a
0.069b
-0.144c
0.304d
0.276
0.209
0.181
0.169
.258
.741
.427
.072
0.374 0.194 .054

Female-Narr-Video intervention x Conservative
-0.444a
-0.508b
-0.971c
-0.278d
0.395
0.338
0.314
0.318
.262
.133
.002
.382

Text intervention

0.168
0.158 .287 0.175 0.157 .262

Education: B.A. or Higher
0.911a
1.088b
1.160c
0.643d
0.209
0.166
0.147
0.134
< .001
< .001
< .001
< .001
0.909a
1.100b
1.186c
0.638d
0.209
0.166
0.148
0.133
< .001
< .001
< .001
< .001

Black

-0.452
0.186 .015 -0.465 0.187 .013

Native American

-0.986
0.536 .066 -1.017 0.535 .057

55 years or older
0.022a
0.278b
0.100c
0.601d
0.297
0.242
0.213
0.196
.940
.250
.639
.002
0.083a
0.334b
0.148c
0.574d
0.296
0.241
0.215
0.196
.779
.165
.492
.003

Female respondent

-0.326
0.118 .006 -0.323 0.118 .006

Liberal

1.251
0.148 < .001 1.260 0.150 < .001

Conservative

-0.225
0.154 .143 -0.119 0.167 .475

Constant
1.472a
0.588b
0.056c
-0.943d
0.218
0.197
0.186
0.185
< .001
.003
.765
< .001
1.438a
0.506b
-0.037c
-0.969d
0.216
0.197
0.187
0.186
< .001
.010
.843
< .001
Wald chi-square 233.27
df 19
Wald chi-square 243.19
df 20

N = 1,184.

s.e. reports robust standard errrors.

Dependent variable coding: (1) Definitely no; (2) Probably no; (3) Undecided as of now; (4) Probably yes; (5) Definitely yes.

For variables that meet the proportional odds assumption, there is one coefficient for all comparisons between levels of vaccination intention.

For variables that violate the proportional odds assumption and constant.

aCoefficient for any response with higher intention to vaccinate than “definitely no” response.

bCoefficient for any response with higher intention to vaccinate than “probably no” response.

cCoefficient for either yes response compared to no or undecided responses.

dCoefficient for “definitely yes” response compared to any lower intention to vaccinate responses.

Findings

Model 1 shows a positive association between having been exposed to the Male-Narr-Video and vaccination intent, consistent across all levels of intent. Compared to the control group given no information, respondents who watched the Male-Narr-Video were more likely to express vaccination intention as measured by their response to the question how likely they would be to receive a COVID-19 vaccine if it were available to them this week (coefficient = 0.440, p = 0.005). Fig 1 illustrates the substantive meaning of these coefficients by displaying predicted probabilities for a respondent who is white, male, politically moderate, under 55, and with at least a B.A. degree in each of the four experimental conditions [54]. These characteristics are the modal responses for all but political ideology (which is a characteristic that we examine separately). Such a respondent exposed to the Male-Narr-Video would have a predicted probability of 53.5% of replying “definitely yes” for their intention to be vaccinated, compared to a predicted probability of 42.6% for the control group. Those exposed to the text intervention had a much smaller, statistically insignificant increase in vaccination intention compared to the control group (coefficient = 0.168, p = 0.278). The pattern for respondents exposed to the Female-Narr-Video intervention, however, had a more nuanced pattern, as there were asymmetric associations across the different levels of vaccine intent. Although outside of the standard cut-off for statistical significance, those exposed to the Female-Narr-Video had a greater likelihood of answering “definitely yes” to getting the vaccine compared to the control group (coefficient = 0.304, p = 0.073), albeit to a lesser degree than those exposed the Male-Narr-Video. Again, although not statistically significant, the range of coefficients at different levels of vaccine intent suggests that there may have been heterogeneous effects of being exposed to the female video, which we tested in Model 2.

Fig 1. Model 1 predicted probabilities.

Fig 1

Predicted probabilities for a participant who is white, male, political moderate, under 55, and with at least a B.A. degree upon exposure to each of the four interventions, illustrating the substantive meaning of the coefficients of Model 1. The horizontal dotted lines are the category cutoffs for the control group for ease of visual comparison.

In Model 2, we added a variable testing for a conditional association between respondents who are politically conservative and viewed the Female-Narr-Video. The interaction term tests whether exposure to the video with the female narrator had different effects for people who identify as conservative than for those who do not identify as conservative. The addition of the politically conservative x Female-Narr-Video interaction variable showed that the main effect of the Female-Narr-Video for non-conservative respondents was a positive association consistent across levels of intent on the margin of statistical significance (coefficient = 0.374, p = 0.051). The magnitude of the coefficient is closer to the Male-Narr-Video (coefficient = 0.440, p = 0.005) than it was in Model 1 for any of the levels of intent, although the larger standard error for the Female-Narr-Video does suggest more heterogeneity remaining in the effect of the Female-Narr-Video. The interaction term shows a notable significant negative association for conservatives exposed to the Female-Narr-Video being less likely to express a vaccination intention greater than the “uncertain” response (coefficient = -0.971, p = .002) when compared to either conservatives in the control condition or to others exposed to the Female-Narr-Video. In other words, among political conservatives, exposure to the video with a female narrator seemed to decrease the propensity of getting to a yes response (whether “probably yes” or “definitely yes”) for the vaccine when compared to political conservatives in the control group.

These patterns are illustrated in Fig 2, which compares the predicted probabilities for a white, male participant, under the age of 55, with at least a BA, varying both the treatment and political ideology (comparing conservative to moderate). The figure shows that there is a negligible difference in predicted probabilities for a moderate or conservative exposed to the Male-Narr-Video of having either a “definitely yes” or “probably yes” response (83.0% and 81.4% respectively). The same comparison for the Female-Narr-Video shows a substantially wider gap (82.1% for moderates, 60.6% for conservatives). Notably, the predicted probability for a conservative exposed to the Female-Narr-Video to reply to the vaccination intention question with “definitely yes” or “probably yes” (60.6%) is also substantially lower than for a conservative in the control group that was not exposed to any information (73.7%). Tests for similar conditional effects for Male-Narr-Video on political conservatives showed no such conditional association (see Supplementary Information). For example, adding an additional interaction term for conservative x Male-Narr-Video to the variables in Model 2 produced a coefficient with a near zero association (coefficient = -0.005, p = 0.987), while not changing any substantive patterns reported in Table 1.

Fig 2. Model 2 predicted probabilities.

Fig 2

Predicted probabilities from Model 2 upon exposure to each of the four interventions for a participant who is white, male, under 55, with at least a B.A. degree, and comparing whether they identify as politically moderate or conservative. The horizontal dotted lines are the category cutoffs for the control group for ease of visual comparison.

Perceptions of narrator quality

We might anticipate that the differences in the patterns in how the Female-Narr-Video and Male-Narr-Video associate with vaccination intent reflect a difference in how respondents perceived the quality of the two videos; however, we found that participants as a whole rated the quality of instruction in the two versions of the video equally highly. This similarity in rating runs contrary to expectations based on previous research that has found that when quality of instruction is measured on a 10-point scale, gender gaps in favor of men are wider, particularly in male-dominated fields [23]. In contrast, we find an insignificant difference in the other direction among respondents included in the analysis. In a bivariate model, respondents who saw the Female-Narr-Video rated the quality of instruction 0.08 points higher, on average, on a 10-point scale than those who saw the Male-Narr-Video (p = 0.406, see S2 Table in S1 Appendix). Although conservatives rated the video about a quarter point lower than other respondents, there is no evidence of conditioning of conservatives’ rating based on which version of the video they saw—the nonsignificant coefficient for the interaction term between conservative and the Female-Narr-Video condition has a positive sign. The survey items on likelihood to share the video on social media, and the trustworthiness, comfort, and knowledge of the narrator (S2-S3 Figs in S1 Appendix) likewise do not show evidence of explicit bias between the male and female-narrated versions of the video. Yet, the consistent persuasive power of the Male-Narr-Video as seen in the increased intention to vaccinate, particularly for political conservatives, may be evidence of bias against women as sources of scientific authority, and is worthy of continued study.

Limitations

Design and analysis decisions suggest a degree of caution in interpreting the results that we report here. In this section, we consider the implications of some of these design and analysis decisions.

Foremost, because we excluded some respondents from analysis on the basis of time devoted to experimental conditions or failing to pass attention checks, there is a possibility that this exclusion could introduce a post-treatment bias [55]. If some unmeasured characteristic both increased the likelihood of exclusion and was associated with decreased vaccination intent, the experimental condition groups would have a lower distribution of low vaccination intent respondents than the control group and any finding of increased vaccination intent could, therefore, be an artifact of the exclusion, rather than a result of any treatment condition.

We addressed this possibility by comparing models using ordinary least squares (OLS) regression between the analytic sample reported in the main text and the full sample (with no cases excluded). OLS models allow more direct comparison of effect sizes across models than generalized ordered logit models. (We discuss these analyses in greater detail in the Supplemental Information, while summarizing key insights here.)

These analyses show that the effect associated with the Male-Narr Video treatment condition is robust across both the limited and full sample. Although there is a small change (.025 on a 5 point scale, or about 10% of the coefficient magnitude), the effect still remains statistically significant. Furthermore, both the Female-Narr Video and text treatment coefficients are statistically insignificant in both the limited and full samples and neither coefficient shows a similar change as the coefficient for Male-Narr Video when moving from the limited to full samples. This approach to eliminating the possibility of post-treatment bias based on exclusion comes with two trade-offs. First, it alters the nature of the explanatory variable from treatment to an intent to treat with the condition, which is not a direct test of the main hypothesis that the video treatment can alter vaccination intent [55]. Second, this approach increases the risk that the full sample data misestimate treatment effects, a particularly salient concern when using MTurk data. Recent analyses of MTurk data estimate that around 25% of responses could be untrustworthy, which could attenuate treatment effects by around 10% [30]. We note that our exclusion rate and the change in magnitude of the Male-Narr Video from the analytic and full sample approximate these two estimates. Finally, because our models include control variables that could correlate both with the likelihood of exclusion and change in vaccination intent, some of the risk of post-treatment bias may be controlled for in the models. We see the balance of evidence reviewed here favoring the analytic sample. That is, the available evidence is less consistent with post-treatment bias than with the full sample containing nontreatment associated noncompliance that could affect estimates. Nonetheless, we cannot rule out the possibility that some unmeasured characteristic introduced some degree of post-treatment bias. Yet, even if there were some post-treatment bias, our main finding about the robust effect of the Male-Narr Video still receives support in the full sample analysis.

The conditioning of the Female-Narr Video among politically conservative respondents does not appear to be as robust to the inclusion in the full sample when examining the OLS models. While this outcome suggests that some caution is warranted in interpreting the specific finding, it does reinforce the general conclusion that the Female-Narr Video condition has greater heterogeneity in association with vaccination intent. That is, if the finding in the models based on the analytic sample is the result of post-treatment bias, it would be a post-treatment bias unique to just the Female-Narr Video treatment. Future research could address concerns about both post-treatment bias and data quality by instituting some of the recommendations for ensuring MTurk, such as using IP addresses as a screening tool [30].

In addition, our analysis is limited in identifying the precise mechanisms that might account for variation and possible mediators of the interventions that we tested. Although we provided both video and text interventions and altered one characteristic of the video (the narrator’s gender), we have little information about the precise qualities of the video that may have been effective and how those qualities might relate to gendered expectations, suggesting many avenues for future research. For example, the use of an animated video does not provide visual cues about the narrator, which could accentuate, attenuate, or interact with vocal characteristics. Similarly, other vocal qualities, such as pitch, accent, or volume could affect how viewers receive messages. These vocal qualities could vary across or within genders, and their association with message effects might vary. For instance, the differences between lower and higher pitches could vary for male and female narrators. Since our study used just one male and one female narrator voice, follow up work can interrogate the role of these other factors on perceived scientific authority and legitimacy.

Finally, our study has focused on communication of scientific information in relation to vaccine intent. While it is suggestive of ways that members of the public may react to scientific authority, there may be contextual influences that shape responses to the video in ways that limit generalizability to vaccination intent specifically and scientific authority more generally. The widespread reach of COVID-related hospitalization, death, and economic and social disruption may shape peoples’ openness to scientific information, particularly when vaccines were first introduced and could be seen as the most likely way to end the pandemic. More generally, it could be that people react to scientific authority differently based on the salience of issues and the extent to which they are politicized. Future research on such communication could examine both more and less political and more and less personally experienced scientific topics. Such research could give more insight about whether gender, political ideology, and scientific authority interrelate across topics. Increasing the visibility of women’s roles in scientific advances may contribute to longer-term change in gendered reactions to scientific information, although as others have argued, addressing questions of inclusion more broadly may also be necessary [22].

Conclusions

Widespread uptake of the COVID-19 vaccines worldwide is crucial for ending the COVID-19 pandemic. Previous work on vaccine hesitancy interventions have recommended that health organizations use social media channels such as YouTube to spread pro-vaccination messages, but caution that vaccine communications should first be tested [7,1921]. Herein, we gathered empirical evidence on the effect of an 8-minute animated educational COVID-19 mRNA vaccine explainer video. We found that this video demonstrated a statistically significant association with increasing vaccination propensity, but that the messenger matters–the video with a male narrator had a robust association with increased vaccination intention, while the identical video with a female narrator had much more uneven associations, part of which could be accounted for by conservatives uniquely expressing uncertainty in vaccination intention after viewing that version of the video. The video studied herein only addresses the mRNA vaccines and does not explain many of the other vaccine questions and concerns people have. Future studies that follow up on the theoretical implications of these data may further examine the ways that gender beliefs influence how members of the public respond to communication about science and medicine, and help guide interventions promoting vaccine uptake.

Supporting information

S1 Appendix. Supplementary information.

(PDF)

S1 Dataset. Data.

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dennis Cao for providing the voice of the male narrator and Adrienne Christiansen, Amy Damon, Lisa Mueller, and Shantee Rosado for their feedback and suggestions.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

LSW is supported by a Cottrell Scholar Award from the Research Corporation for Science Advancement (ID No: 27499). LSW and EL were supported by Macalester College Funds.

References

  • 1.World Health Organization. Ten threats to global health in 2019. 2019 [Cited 2021 Feb 14] Available from: https://www.who.int/news-room/spotlight/ten-threats-to-global-health-in-2019.
  • 2.Chou WYS, Budenz A. Considering emotion in COVID-19 vaccine communication: addressing vaccine hesitancy and fostering vaccine confidence. Health Commun. 2020; 35: 1718–1722. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2020.1838096 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kaplan RM, Milstein A. Influence of a COVID-19 vaccine’s effectiveness and safety profile on vaccination acceptance. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2021; 118. e2021726118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2021726118 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Mega ER. Trust in COVID vaccines is growing. Nature News. 2021. Feb 10 [Cited 2021 Feb 15] 10.1038/d41586-021-00368-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Oduwole EO, Pienaar ED, Mahomed H, Wiysonge CS. Current tools available for investigating vaccine hesitancy: a scoping review protocol. BMJ Open. 2019; 9: e033245. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2019-033245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Puri N, Coomes EA, Haghbayan H, Gunaratne K. Social media and vaccine hesitancy: new updates for the era of COVID-19 and globalized infectious diseases. Hum Vaccin Immunother. 2020; 16: 2586–2593. doi: 10.1080/21645515.2020.1780846 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Betsch C, Brewer NT, Brocard P, Davies P, Gaissmaier W, Haase N, et al. Opportunities and challenges of Web 2.0 for vaccination decisions. Vaccine. 2012; 30: 3727–3733. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2012.02.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Jamieson KH, Albarracín D. The relation between media consumption and misinformation at the outset of the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic in the US. Harv Kennedy Sch Misinformation Rev. 2020; 1. doi: 10.37016/mr-2020-38 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Arede M, Bravo-Araya M, Bouchard É, Singh Gill G, Plajer V, Shehraj A, et al. Combating vaccine hesitancy: teaching the next generation to navigate through the post truth era. Front Public Health. 2018; 6: 381. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2018.00381 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Olson O, Berry C, Kumar N. Addressing parental vaccine hesitancy towards childhood vaccines in the United States: a systematic literature review of communication interventions and strategies. Vaccines. 2020; 8: 590. doi: 10.3390/vaccines8040590 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Dubé E, Gagnon L, MacDonald NE. Strategies intended to address vaccine hesitancy: review of published reviews. Vaccine. 2015; 33: 4191–4203. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2015.04.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Sadaf A, Richards JL, Glanz J, Salmon DA, Omer SB. A systematic review of interventions for reducing parental vaccine refusal and vaccine hesitancy. Vaccine. 2013; 31: 4293–4304. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2013.07.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Horne Z, Powell D, Hummel JE, Holyoak KJ. Countering antivaccination attitudes. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015; 112: 10321–10324. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1504019112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Daley MF, Narwaney KJ, Shoup JA, Wagner NM, Glanz JM. Addressing parents’ vaccine concerns: a randomized trial of a social media intervention. Am J Prev Med. 2018; 55: 44–54. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2018.04.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Robichaud P, Hawken S, Beard L, Morra D, Tomlinson G, Wilson K, et al. Vaccine-critical videos on YouTube and their impact on medical students’ attitudes about seasonal influenza immunization: a pre and post study. Vaccine. 2021; 30: 3763–3770. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2012.03.074 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Gowda C, Schaffer SE, Kopec K, Markel A, Dempsey AF. A pilot study on the effects of individually tailored education for MMR vaccine-hesitant parents on MMR vaccination intention. Hum Vaccines Immunother. 2013; 9: 437–445. doi: 10.4161/hv.22821 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Nyhan B, Reifler J. Does correcting myths about the flu vaccine work? an experimental evaluation of the effects of corrective information. Vaccine. 2015; 33: 459–464. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2014.11.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Nyhan B, Reifler J, Richey S, Freed GL. Effective messages in vaccine promotion: a randomized trial. Pediatrics. 2014; 133: e835–e842. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-2365 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Thomson A, Vallée-Tourangeau G, Suggs LS. Strategies to increase vaccine acceptance and uptake: From behavioral insights to context-specific, culturally-appropriate, evidence-based communications and interventions. Vaccine. 2018; 26: 6457–6458. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2018.08.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Chevallier C, Hacquin AS, Mercier H. COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy: shortening the last mile. Trends Cogn Sci. 2021; 25: 331–33. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2021.02.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Dubé E, MacDonald NE. How can a global pandemic affect vaccine hesitancy? Expert Rev of Vaccines. 2020; 19: 899–901. doi: 10.1080/14760584.2020.1825944 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.McKinnon M, C. O’Connell C. Perceptions of stereotypes applied to women who publicly communicate their STEM work. Humanit Soc Sci Commun. 2020; 7: 1–8. doi: 10.1057/s41599-020-00654-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Rivera LA, Tilcsik A. Scaling down inequality: rating scales, gender bias, and the architecture of evaluation. Am Sociol Rev. 2019; 84: 248–274. doi: 10.1177/0003122419833601 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Broniatowski DA, Hilyard KM, Dredze M. Effective vaccine communication during the disneyland measles outbreak. Vaccine. 2016; 34: 3225–3228. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2016.04.044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ophir Y, Jamieson KH. Intentions to use a novel Zika vaccine: the effects of misbeliefs about the MMR vaccine and perceptions about Zika. J. Public Health. 2018; 40: e531–e537. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fdy042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Raja AS, Niforatos JD, Anaya N, Graterol J, Rodriquez RM. Vaccine hesitancy and reasons for refusing the COVID-19 vaccination among the U.S. public: a cross-sectional survey. medRxiv [Preprint] 2021. [cited 2021 March 3] Available from: https://www.medrxiv.org/content/10.1101/2021.02.28.21252610v1 [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Penţa MA, Băban A. Message framing in vaccine communication: a systematic review of published literature. Health Commun. 2018; 33: 299–314. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2016.1266574 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Young DG, Jamieson KH, Poulsen S, Goldring A. Fact-checking effectiveness as a function of format and tone: evaluating factcheck.org and flackcheck.org. Journal Mass Commun Q. 2018; 95: 49–75. doi: 10.1177/1077699017710453 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Larson HJ, Jarrett C, Schulz WS, Chaudhuri M, Zhou Y, Dube E, et al. Measuring vaccine hesitancy: the development of a survey tool. Vaccine. 2015; 33: 4165–4175. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2015.04.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG, Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. 2009; 41: 1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Mortensen K, Hughes TL. Comparing Amazon’s Mechanical Turk platform to conventional data collection methods in the health and medical research literature. J Gen Intern Med. 2018; 33: 533–538. doi: 10.1007/s11606-017-4246-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Paolacci G, Chandler J. Inside the Turk: understanding Mechanical Turk as a participant pool. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2014; 23: 184–188. doi: 10.1177/0963721414531598 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Weinberg J, Freese J, McElhattan D. Comparing data characteristics and results of an online factorial survey between a population-based and a crowdsource-recruited sample. Sociol Sci. 2014; 1: 292–310. doi: 10.15195/v1.a19 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Ahler D, Roush C, Sood G. The micro-task market for lemons: data quality on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. Political Sci Res Methods. 2021. doi: 10.1017/psrm.2021.57 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Dubé E, Laberge C, Guay M, Bramadat P, Roy R, Bettinger J. Vaccine hesitancy: an overview. Hum Vaccines Immunother. 2013; 9: 1763–1773. doi: 10.4161/hv.24657 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Betsch C, Korn L, Holtmann C. Don’t try to convert the antivaccinators, instead target the fence-sitters. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015; 112: E6725–E6726. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1516350112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Hedeker D, Mermelstein RJ. A multilevel thresholds of change model for analysis of stages of change data. Multivar Behav Res. 1998; 33: 427–455. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3304_1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Williams R. Generalized ordered logit/partial proportional odds models for ordinal dependent variables. The Stata Journal. 2006; 6: 58–82. doi: 10.1177/1536867X0600600104 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Understanding Williams R. and interpreting generalized ordered logit models. J Math Sociol. 2016; 40: 7–20. doi: 10.1080/0022250X.2015.1112384 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Galbraith KV, Lechuga J, Jenerette CM, Moore LA, Plamer MH, Hamilton JB. Parental acceptance and uptake of the HPV vaccine among African-Americans and Latinos in the United States: a literature review. Soc Sci Med. 2016; 159: 116–126. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.04.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Pacheco CM, Daley SM, Brown T, Filippi M, Greiner KA, Daley CM. Moving forward: breaking the cycle of mistrust between American Indians and researchers. Am J Public Health. 2013; 103: 2152–2159. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301480 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Noe-Bistamante L, Flores A. Facts on Latinos in the U.S. Pew Research Center. 2019. Sep 16 [cited 2021 Mar 24]. Available from https://www.pewresearch.org/hispanic/fact-sheet/latinos-in-the-u-s-fact-sheet/. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Candelario GEB. Color matters: Latina/o racial identities and life chances. In: Flores J, Ronaldo R, editors. A companion to Latina/o studies. Malden MA: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2017. pp. 337–350. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Gauchat G. Politicization of science in the public sphere: a study of public trust in the United States, 1974 to 2010. Am. Sociol. Rev. 2012; 77: 167–187. doi: 10.1177/0003122412438225 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Evans JH, Hargittai E. Who doesn’t trust Fauci? the public’s belief in the expertise and shared values of scientists in the COVID-19 pandemic. Socius 2020; 6. doi: 10.1177/2378023120947337 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Whitehead AL, Perry SL. How culture wars delay herd immunity: Christian nationalism and anti-vaccine attitudes. Socius. 2020; 6. doi: 10.1177/2378023120977727 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Gauchat G. The political context of science in the United States: public acceptance of evidence-based policy and science funding. Social Forces 2015; 94: 723–746. doi: 10.1093/sf/sov040 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Baumgaertner B, Carlisle JE, Justwan F. The influence of political ideology and trust on willingness to vaccinate. PLoS One. 2018; 13: e0191728. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0191728 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Whitehead AL, Perry SL. Is a “Christian America” a more patriarchal America? Religion, politics, and traditionalist gender ideology. Can Rev Sociol. 2019; 56: 151–177. doi: 10.1111/cars.12241 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Breda T, Jouini E, Napp C, Thebault G. Gender stereotypes can explain the gender-equality paradox. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2020; 117: 31063–31069. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2008704117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Nosek BA, Smyth FL, Sriram N, Linder NM, Devos T, Ayala A, et al. National differences in gender–science stereotypes predict national sex differences in science and math achievement. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009; 106: 10593–10597. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809921106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Moss-Racusin CA, Dovidio JF, Brescoll VL, Graham MJ, Handelsman J. Science faculty’s subtle gender biases favor male students. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012; 109: 16474–16479. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1211286109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Salerno JM, Phalen HJ. Traditional gender roles and backlash against female attorneys expressing anger in court. J Empir Leg Stud. 2019; 16: 909–932. doi: 10.1111/jels.12238 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Long JS, Freese J. Regression models for categorical dependent variables using Stata. 3rd ed. College Station: Stata Press; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Montgomery JM, Nyhan B, Torres M. How conditioning on posttreatment variables can ruin your experiment and what to do about it. American Journal of Political Science 2018;. Am J Pol Sci. 2018; 62: 760–775. doi: 10.1111/ajps.12357 [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 Appendix. Supplementary information.

(PDF)

S1 Dataset. Data.

(XLSX)

Data Availability Statement

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES