Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Dec 15.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Med. 2021 Jun 15;53(3):731–740. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721002075

Autonomic activity, posttraumatic and nontraumatic nightmares, and PTSD after trauma exposure

Thomas Mäder 1,2, Katelyn I Oliver 3,4, Carolina Daffre 3,4, Sophie Kim 3,4, Scott P Orr 3,4,5, Natasha B Lasko 3,4,5, Jeehye Seo 3,4,5,6, Birgit Kleim 1,2,7, Edward Franz Pace-Schott 3,4,5
PMCID: PMC9121310  NIHMSID: NIHMS1803857  PMID: 34127168

Abstract

Background.

Nightmares are a hallmark symptom of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). This strong association may reflect a shared pathophysiology in the form of altered autonomic activity and increased reactivity. Using an acoustic startle paradigm, we investigated the interrelationships of psychophysiological measures during wakefulness and PTSD diagnosis, posttraumatic nightmares, and nontraumatic nightmares.

Methods.

A community sample of 122 trauma survivors were presented with a series of brief loud tones, while heart rate (HRR), skin conductance (SCR), and orbicularis oculi electromyogram (EMGR) responses were measured. Prior to the tone presentations, resting heart rate variability (HRV) was assessed. Nightmares were measured using nightmare logs. Three dichotomous groupings of participants were compared: (1) current PTSD diagnosis (n = 59), no PTSD diagnosis (n = 63), (2) those with (n = 26) or without (n = 96) frequent posttraumatic nightmares, and (3) those with (n = 22) or without (n = 100) frequent nontraumatic nightmares.

Results.

PTSD diagnosis was associated with posttraumatic but not with nontraumatic nightmares. Both PTSD and posttraumatic nightmares were associated with a larger mean HRR to loud tones, whereas nontraumatic nightmare frequency was associated with a larger SCR. EMGR and resting HRV were not associated with PTSD diagnosis or nightmares.

Conclusions.

Our findings suggest a shared pathophysiology between PTSD and posttraumatic nightmares in the form of increased HR reactivity to startling tones, which might reflect reduced parasympathetic tone. This shared pathophysiology could explain why PTSD is more strongly related to posttraumatic than nontraumatic nightmares, which could have important clinical implications.

Keywords: Acoustic startle procedure, autonomic activity, heart rate, idiopathic, nightmares, pathophysiology, posttraumatic, PTSD

Introduction

Events such as being directly or indirectly exposed to actual or threatened death, serious injury, or sexual violence are commonly experienced, with about 70% of individuals experiencing at least one event during their lifetime (e.g. Benjet et al., 2016; Dückers, Alisic, & Brewin, 2016). Although trauma exposure is common, the majority of survivors will be resilient (Galatzer-Levy, Huang, & Bonanno, 2018). A significant subgroup of individuals does, however, develop trauma-related disorders, such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD; DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). According to the DSM-5, PTSD is defined as a heterogeneous syndrome that lasts more than a month and includes intrusion and avoidance symptoms, hyperarousal, and changes in cognitions and mood. PTSD is a major public health issue, with potential negative consequences including difficulties with intimate relationships, decreased academic or job performance, substance use problems, physical illness, and elevated risk of suicidal ideation and attempts (e.g. Boscarino, 2004; Boyraz, Granda, Baker, Tidwell, & Waits, 2016; Hawn, Cusack, & Amstadter, 2020; LeBouthillier, McMillan, Thibodeau, & Asmundson, 2015; Loya, 2015; Taft, Watkins, Stafford, Street, & Monson, 2011).

Nightmares are a highly prevalent symptom of PTSD and they also often occur as an acute stress symptom following a potentially traumatic event (PTE), with up to 50% of trauma survivors reporting replicative (very close to exact replays of the PTE) or mixed (partly similar to the PTE) posttraumatic nightmares and 17–30% reporting nontraumatic nightmares (content unrelated to the PTE). Notably, mixed and especially replicative posttraumatic nightmares following a PTE seem to be associated with a greater risk for developing PTSD and more severe psychopathology (Davis, Byrd, Rhudy, & Wright, 2007; David & Mellman, 1997; de Dassel, Wittmann, Protic, Höllmer, & Gorzka, 2018; Langston, Davis, & Swopes, 2010; Phelps, Forbes, Hopwood, & Creamer, 2011; Schreuder, Igreja, van Dijk, & Kleijn, 2001; Schreuder, Kleijn, & Rooijmans, 2000; Wittmann, Zehnder, Schredl, Jenni, & Landolt, 2010).

The reason for this close relationship between posttraumatic nightmares and PTSD could lie in a common pathophysiology. In this context, elevated arousal is discussed as a central pathophysiological factor in PTSD as well as in nightmare etiology (Gieselmann et al., 2019; Levin & Nielsen, 2007). Studies have found associations between PTSD and decreased heart rate variability (HRV; e.g. Campbell, Wisco, Silvia, & Gay, 2019) and an elevated autonomic startle response to a series of sudden loud tones in the form of an increased eye blink response, a larger skin conductance response (SCR) and, most robustly, a larger heart rate response (HRR; e.g. Carson et al., 2007; Orr, Lasko, Shalev, & Pitman, 1995; Orr et al., 2003; Pole, 2007). For nightmares, studies have found associations with altered HRV (Nielsen et al., 2010) and elevated mean heart rate (HR) during REM sleep, elevated HR acceleration during nightmare sleep (Nielsen & Zadra, 2000), and more leg movements and frequent awakenings than healthy controls, while this last association seems to be stronger for posttraumatic than for nontraumatic nightmares (Germain & Nielsen, 2003; Simor, Horváth, Gombos, Takács, & Bódizs, 2012). During wakefulness, nightmare distress is negatively associated with baseline resting HRV (vagal tone) in children (Secrist, Dalenberg, & Gevirtz, 2019) and using an acoustic startle paradigm, Tanev et al. (2017) found a positive correlation between posttraumatic nightmare severity and HRR, but not the SCR or the eye blink startle response, to sudden loud tones in a sample of women with PTSD. SCR primarily reflects sympathetic activity (e.g. Critchley & Nagai, 2013) and the eye blink startle response is a reflex circuit that can be modulated by limbic input, such as amygdala activity, as well as sympathetic activation (e.g. Koch & Schnitzler, 1997; Liu, Amey, Magerman, Scott, & Forbes, 2020; Szabadi, 2012). HR, on the other hand, is regulated by both the sympathetic and the parasympathetic nervous systems. Sympathetic stimulation increases HR, while parasympathetic stimulation decreases HR (e.g. Gordan, Gwathmey, & Xie, 2015). Changes in HR within a few seconds of a low intensity stimulus, typically show an initial deceleration (orienting response) followed by subsequent recovery of HR (Chen, Aksan, Anderson, Grafft, & Chapleau, 2014; Orr et al., 1995; Vila et al., 2007). However, as the stimulus intensity increases, the decelerative feature is reduced and HR acceleration instead occurs (e.g. Turpin, Schaefer, & Boucsein, 1999; Vossel & Zimmer, 1992). Therefore, one might expect that heart rate acceleration following a startling stimulus with a very short latency could occur due to rapid parasympathetic withdrawal. Tanev et al. (2017) concluded that posttraumatic nightmares might be associated with reduced parasympathetic rather than increased sympathetic activity. This altered autonomic activity might be associated with both PTSD and posttraumatic nightmares and could explain their close relationship.

In summary, there is a growing body of evidence that suggests that posttraumatic nightmares are predictive of a later PTSD diagnosis. However, the role of nontraumatic nightmares in the development of PTSD as well as the potentially shared pathophysiology of these phenomena is not well understood (Levin & Nielsen, 2007). In this study, we aimed to investigate the associations between PTSD and nontraumatic and posttraumatic nightmares, as well as the specific associations between different psychophysiological measures during wakefulness and PTSD, posttraumatic nightmares, and nontraumatic nightmares. Psychophysiological measures included resting HRV as well as HR, SC, and eye blink startle responses to a series of sudden loud tones. The current study was primarily of an exploratory nature. However, we expected: (1) a stronger association between posttraumatic nightmares and PTSD than between nontraumatic nightmares and PTSD, (2) an association between PTSD and an increased HRR to a series of loud tones, and (3) negative relationships between resting HRV and PTSD, posttraumatic nightmares, and nontraumatic nightmares.

Methods

Participants and procedure

As part of a larger project, participants were recruited from the greater Boston area via online and posted advertisements. The sample consisted of 122 participants who had experienced a DSM-5 Criterion A index trauma in the past 2 years but not within the past month. Due to significant effects of age on some of the sleep physiology variables included in the larger project (Ohayon, Carskadon, Guilleminault, & Vitiello, 2004), the age range for study participation was chosen to be between 18 and 40 years. Participants reported various types of trauma, such as sexual or physical assaults or traffic accidents. Individuals eligible to participate in the study were invited for psychiatric and sleep interviews (see online Supplementary materials for exclusion criteria). After the interviews, participants took part in an acoustic startle paradigm, during which psychophysiological measures were assessed. After the acoustic startle paradigm, participants received nightmare logs, which they were instructed to fill out for 14 consecutive days. The Partners Healthcare Institutional Review Board approved all study procedures. All participants provided written informed consent and were paid for their participation.

Structured diagnostic interview for PTSD diagnosis

PTSD diagnosis was assessed by a highly experienced interviewer using the Clinician-Administered PTSD scale for DSM-5 (CAPS-5; Weathers et al., 2013a), a 30-item structured interview, which queries DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for PTSD and is considered the gold standard in PTSD diagnosis. We created ‘PTSD’ and trauma-exposed ‘No-PTSD’ groups based on whether or not participants met the diagnostic criteria.

PTSD symptom severity

The PTSD Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5; Weathers et al., 2013b) was used to quantify the severity of DSM-5 PTSD symptoms over the past month. The PCL-5 is a self-report questionnaire containing 20 items with 5-point Likert scales. A total symptom severity score was obtained (range: 0–100). For our analyses, we excluded item 2 (‘In the past month, how much were you bothered by repeated, disturbing dreams of the stressful experience?’), since we used diary-based nightmare assessments.

Nightmare log

A nightmare log that was filled out every morning upon awakening, defined nightmares as very disturbing or unpleasant dreams that may cause awakening or are remembered in the morning. Including nightmares (leading to awakening) and bad dreams (not leading to awakening) is in line with previous findings suggesting that nightmares and bad dreams are expressions of the same phenomenon, differing in their intensity (e.g. Robert & Zadra, 2014) as well as with the operational definitions of nightmares in previous studies (e.g. Lemyre, Bastien, & Vallières, 2019; Wittmann et al. 2010). The nightmare log also queried whether nightmares were exactly like (replicative), similar to (mixed), possibly related to, or unrelated to the PTE. Replicative or mixed nightmares were categorized as posttraumatic and unrelated nightmares as nontraumatic. Possibly related nightmares were not included in either nightmare category. To account for differences in the number of days the nightmare log was filled out, weighted nightmare frequency scores were calculated (total nightmare frequency divided by the number of log entries). Based on these nightmare categories and frequency scores, we created two dichotomous groupings of participants in addition to the PTSD/No-PTSD diagnosis dichotomy: (1) ‘Posttraumatic nightmares’ (n = 26, at least one replicative or mixed nightmare/week), ‘No posttraumatic nightmares’ (n = 96, less than one replicative or mixed nightmare/week), (2) ‘Nontraumatic nightmares’ (n = 22, at least one trauma-unrelated nightmare/week), and ‘No nontraumatic nightmares’ (n = 100, less than one trauma-unrelated nightmare/week or at least one posttraumatic nightmare/week). Participants qualified for the nightmare groups if they had a total average of one nightmare per week, which did not require them to have a nightmare in each of the 2 weeks. These groupings allowed overlap when participants experienced both PTSD and nightmares or posttraumatic and nontraumatic nightmares, but participants were assigned to the more severe posttraumatic nightmare group when reporting at least one posttraumatic nightmare a week, regardless of their nontraumatic nightmare frequency.

Acoustic startle paradigm: HR, SC, and EMG responses and resting HRV

For the acoustic startle paradigm, we used the same stimuli, procedures, and response score calculations as in previous studies (e.g. Buhlmann et al., 2007; Carson et al., 2007; Mueller-Pfeiffer et al., 2014; Pace-Schott et al., 2014). Participants were asked to sit upright, quietly, with their eyes open, and with headphones placed over both ears. They were informed that they would hear a series of loud tones. After a 5-min baseline period with full physiological recording, the first startle tone was presented without warning. They were presented with 16 loud tones (500 ms, 100–102 dB, 1000 Hz) with random intervals between them (27 to 52 s). Mean startle response scores for the HR acceleration responses (HRR), skin conductance responses (SCR), and orbicularis oculi electromyogram responses (EMGR) were calculated as the mean of the responses to each of the first 15 tones. The greater the response scores, the greater the physiological reactivity. Resting HRV for the 5-min baseline phase was calculated using previously published guidelines and recommendations (e.g. Malik, 1996; Shaffer & Ginsberg, 2017). We used, for time domain, measurement of the root of the mean square of successive differences (RMSSD) between normal heartbeats and, for frequency domain, the power in the high frequency band (0.15–0.40 Hz; HF power). In line with previous research, the raw HF power scores were log-transformed before analysis to normalize the relevant distributions. RMSSD and HF power are highly correlated (e.g. Kleiger, Stein, & Bigger, 2005) and higher RMSSD and HF power scores reflect more parasympathetic activity (Laborde, Mosley, & Thayer, 2017; Shaffer & Ginsberg, 2017; Shaffer, McCraty, & Zerr, 2014). See online Supplementary materials for a more detailed description of the physiological measurements used in this study.

Statistical analyses

To examine the relationships between the psychophysiological measures (HRR, SCR, EMGR, RMSSD, and HF power), PTSD, posttraumatic nightmares, and nontraumatic nightmares, we performed bivariate correlation analyses using Spearman’s rank correlation, since quantile–quantile plots and Shapiro–Wilk tests showed that most variables did not follow a normal distribution. Due to our large sample size, we employed Welch’s t tests to investigate potential group differences in our psychophysiological variables, despite the non-normality of our variables (according to the central limit theorem; e.g. Kwak & Kim, 2017). The t tests were conducted with the three previously described dichotomous groupings for ‘PTSD/No PTSD’, ‘posttraumatic nightmares/no posttraumatic nightmares’, and ‘nontraumatic nightmares/no nontraumatic nightmares’. Cohen’s d effect sizes were calculated for the Welch’s t tests. Based on the correlation analyses and t tests, we further investigated potential associations using logistic regression analyses. We created separate models for PTSD diagnosis, posttraumatic nightmares, and nontraumatic nightmares as the categorical outcome variables, while accounting for the simultaneous contribution of psychophysiological measures and control variables (sex, age, and months since trauma). Visual inspection of scatter plots suggested that the assumption of a linear relationship between the predictors and the logits of the outcome variables was sufficiently met. Additionally, variance inflation factors (VIFs) were within an acceptable range (VIF < 3), suggesting that the assumption of the absence of multicollinearity was met. Outliers were excluded based on the visual inspections of scatter plots and the ±3 S.D. cutoff for very likely erroneous data points and missing data were handled using listwise deletion. Cook’s distances for residuals in our logistic regression analyses were in an acceptable range. All statistical tests were performed two-tailed, with an alpha level of 0.05 using the statistical software R (version 3.6.1).

Results

Descriptive statistics

Sample demographics and characteristics are displayed in Table 1. See online Supplementary materials for more detailed sample of nightmare characteristics.

Table 1.

Sample demographics and characteristics.

M ± SD 95% CI Range
Age (years) 24.16 ± 4.94 [23.28, 25.05] 18, 40
Months since trauma 11.94 ± 6.59 [10.75, 13.13] 1, 24
Nightmare log days 13.52 ± 1.16 [13.31, 13.72] 8, 14
PCL-5 30.02 ± 16.12 [27.12, 32.93] 0, 69
Sex
Female: 83 (68.0%), male: 39 (32.0%)
Education
Unknown/Not reported: 10 (8.2%) High school or equivalent: 4 (3.3%)
College: 46 (37.7%) Bachelor's degree: 40 (32.8%)
Associate's degree: 4 (3.3%) Graduate degree: 18 (14.8%)

Note. M = mean, SD = standard deviation, CI = confidence interval, PCL-5 = PTSD Checklist for DSM-5 (total score, including item 2).

Nightmares were reported with an average of 2.6 nightmares in 13.5 days. At least one posttraumatic nightmare was reported by 26 participants (21.3%), while 22 participants (18.0%) reported at least one nontraumatic nightmare per week with no frequent posttraumatic nightmares. Spearman correlation showed that prospective posttraumatic nightmare frequency measures (nightmare log) and retrospective posttraumatic nightmare frequency measures (item 2 of the PCL-5) were significantly correlated (rs(121) = 0.45, p < 0.001). Most of the posttraumatic nightmares were mixed; 18 participants (14.8%) reported at least one mixed and seven participants (5.7%) at least one replicative posttraumatic nightmare per week. One participant of the posttraumatic nightmare group reported one mixed and one replicative posttraumatic nightmare. Overall mean nightmare frequency was higher among participants in the posttraumatic nightmare group (5.7 of total diaries; posttraumatic: 3.3, nontraumatic: 1.3) compared to participants in the nontraumatic nightmare group (3.5; posttraumatic: 0.2, nontraumatic: 2.8). In the PTSD group, 23 participants (39%) reported at least one posttraumatic and 10 participants (17%) at least one nontraumatic nightmare a week. Consequently, PTSD criteria were met by 88% of the posttraumatic nightmare group and 45% of the nontraumatic nightmare group.

PTSD diagnosis, symptom severity, and nightmares

Results of the correlation analyses are shown in Table 2. Group mean responses to each of the 15 tone presentations are displayed in Fig. 1, and group means, standard deviations, and t test results of the main variables are presented in Table 3. Results of the logistic regression analyses are displayed in Table 4.

Table 2.

Results of Spearman correlation analyses.

HRR SCR EMGR PTNM NTNM RMSSD HF power
SCR 0.42***
EMGR 0.37*** 0.29**
PTNM 0.20* 0.05 0.11
NTNM 0.13 0.19* 0.07 −0.03
RMSSD 0.10 0.00 −0.06 −0.01 −0.01
HF power 0.16 −0.02 −0.03 −0.01 0.01 0.94***
PCL-5 0.20* −0.03 0.01 0.41*** 0.00 −0.08 −0.11

Note. Table shows correlation coefficients (Spearman’s rho). PTNM = frequency of posttraumatic nightmares (weighted by number of diary nights), NTNM = frequency of nontraumatic nightmares (weighted by number of diary nights), HRR = heart rate mean response, SCR = skin conductance mean response, EMGR = electromyogram mean response (left orbicularis oculi), RMSSD = Root mean square of the successive differences (resting heart rate variability), HF power = log-transformed high frequency power (resting heart rate variability), PCL-5 = PTSD Checklist for DSM-5 (without item 2)

*

= p < .05

**

= p < .01

***

< .001.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Plots of the group mean responses to the 15 loud tone presentations. The groups shown in this figure represent alternate ways of dichotomizing the total sample and the PTSD and nightmare groups are not mutually exclusive. Participants who reported both frequent posttraumatic and nontraumatic nightmares were assigned to the more severe PTNM group. A = Responses for ‘PTSD’ (solid line) vs. ‘No PTSD’ (dotted line) groups, B = responses for the ‘PTNM’ (solid line) vs. ‘No PTNM’ (dotted line) groups (PTNM = at least one posttraumatic nightmare/week), C = responses for the ‘NTNM’ (solid line) vs. ‘No NTNM’ (dotted line) groups (NTNM = at least one nontraumatic nightmare/week). HRR = heart rate mean response, SCR = skin conductance mean response, EMGR = electromyogram mean response (left orbicularis oculi).

Table 3.

Group sizes, means, standard deviations, and results of t-tests.

PTSD (n = 59) Non-PTSD (n = 63)
Measure M SD M SD t p d
HRR 2.44 0.93 2.03 0.73 −2.69 .008** −0.50
SCR 0.93 0.64 0.96 0.61 0.25 .806 0.05
EMGR 0.20 0.11 0.17 0.10 −1.63 .105 −0.30
RMSSD 51.66 22.68 58.35 30.69 1.30 .197 0.25
HF power 6.83 0.76 6.94 1.03 0.64 .525 0.12
PTNM freq. 0.10 0.13 0.03 0.10 −2.98 .004** −0.54
NTNM freq. 0.09 0.11 0.06 0.08 −1.52 .132 −0.28
PTNM (n = 26) Non-PTNM (n = 96)
Measure M SD M SD t p d
HRR 2.63 1.11 2.11 0.74 −2.24 .032* −0.55
SCR 1.00 0.68 0.93 0.61 −0.52 .608 −0.12
EMGR 0.21 0.12 0.18 0.10 −1.21 .233 −0.28
RMSSD 52.64 22.40 55.96 28.67 0.61 .547 0.13
HF power 6.89 0.81 6.88 0.94 −0.05 .957 −0.01
NTNM (n = 22) Non-NTNM (n = 100)
Measure M SD M SD t p d
HRR 2.22 0.69 2.22 0.89 0.02 .985 0.00
SCR 1.13 0.63 0.91 0.61 −1.42 .166 −0.35
EMGR 0.17 0.11 0.19 0.11 0.48 .634 0.12
RMSSD 61.09 29.14 54.01 26.91 −0.95 .350 −0.25
HF power 6.99 0.85 6.86 0.93 −0.58 .568 −0.15

Note. The groups shown in this table represent alternate ways of dichotomizing the total sample and the PTSD and nightmare groups are not mutually exclusive. Participants who reported both frequent posttraumatic and nontraumatic nightmares were assigned to the more severe PTNM group. PTNM = at least one posttraumatic nightmare / week, NTNM = at least one nontraumatic nightmare / week, M = mean, SD = standard deviation, t = t-value, d = Cohen’s d, PTNM freq. = weighted posttraumatic nightmare frequency, NTNM freq. = weighted nontraumatic nightmare frequency, HRR = heart rate mean response, SCR = skin conductance mean response, EMGR = electromyogram mean response (left orbicularis oculi), RMSSD = Root mean square of the successive differences (resting heart rate variability), HF pow. (ln) = log-transformed high frequency power (resting heart rate variability)

*

= p < .05

**

= p < .01

***

p < .001.

Table 4.

Logistic regression analysis results.

PTSD
yes: n = 57, no: n = 60
PTNM
yes: n = 26, no: n = 91
NTNM
yes: n = 20, no: n = 97

OR [95% CI] p OR [95% CI] p OR [95% CI] p
HRR 2.13 [1.18, 3.88] .013* 1.97 [1.04, 3.74] .038* 0.86 [0.43, 1.68] .661
SCR 0.58 [0.28, 1.18] .134 0.69 [0.29, 1.64] .399 2.07 [0.87, 4.92] .098
PTNM freq. 1.70 [1.18, 2.45] .005**
NTNM freq. 1.25 [0.91, 1.71] .171

Note. The groups shown in this table represent alternate ways of dichotomizing the total sample and the PTSD and nightmare groups are not mutually exclusive. Participants who reported both frequent posttraumatic and nontraumatic nightmares were assigned to the more severe PTNM group. PTNM = at least one posttraumatic nightmare / week, NTNM = at least one nontraumatic nightmare / week, PTNM freq. = posttraumatic nightmare frequency, NTNM freq. = nontraumatic nightmare frequency, OR = odds ratios, CI = confidence intervals, HRR = heart rate mean response, SCR = skin conductance mean response.

*

= p < .05

**

= p < .01.

Spearman correlations showed significant positive associations between PTSD symptom severity and posttraumatic nightmare frequency (rs(121) = 0.41, p < 0.001) and a t test showed that posttraumatic nightmare frequency was significantly higher for participants with a PTSD diagnosis compared to those without a PTSD diagnosis (t(104.35) = −2.98, p = 0.004, d = −0.54). Nontraumatic nightmare frequency was not significantly correlated with the PCL-5 score (rs(121) = 0.00, p = 0.986) and t tests showed no significant difference in nontraumatic nightmare frequency for participants with or without a PTSD diagnosis (t(99.91) = −1.52, p = 0.132, d = −0.28). Logistic regression analysis with posttraumatic and nontraumatic nightmare frequency as the main predictors, while also controlling for sex, age, and months since trauma, showed that PTSD diagnosis was significantly predicted by posttraumatic nightmare frequency [odds ratio (OR) 1.70, confidence interval (CI) 1.18–2.45, p = 0.005] but not by nontraumatic nightmare frequency (OR 1.25, CI 0.91–1.71, p = 0.171). In this model, women were more likely to meet PTSD criteria (OR 0.36, CI 0.15–0.87, p = 0.023).

Psychophysiological measures and PTSD, posttraumatic nightmares, and nontraumatic nightmares

Positive correlations were observed between the HRR and SCR (rs(119) = 0.42, p < 0.001), HRR and EMGR (rs(119) = 0.37, p < 0.001), and SCR and EMGR (rs(119) = 0.29, p = 0.002). RMSSD and HF power were significantly correlated as well (rs(108) = 0.94, p < 0.001). There were no significant correlations or trends between loud tone responses and the resting HRV variables.

Psychophysiological measures and PTSD symptom severity and diagnosis

Spearman correlations showed a significant positive association between the HRR and the PCL-5 score (rs(118) = 0.20, p = 0.030) and a t test showed that the HRR was significantly larger for participants with a PTSD diagnosis than for participants without a PTSD diagnosis (t(106.18) = −2.69, p = 0.008, d = −0.50). None of the other psychophysiological measures showed a significant association with PTSD symptom severity or diagnosis. A logistic regression analysis with the HRR and SCR as the main predictors, while also controlling for age, sex, and months since trauma showed that the HRR was a significant predictor of PTSD diagnosis (OR 2.13, CI 1.18–3.88, p = 0.013), while the SCR did not significantly predict PTSD diagnosis (OR 0.58, CI 0.28–1.18, p = 0.134). In this model, women were more likely to meet PTSD criteria (OR 0.40, CI 0.16–0.96, p = 0.040).

Psychophysiological measures and nightmares

Spearman correlations showed a significant positive association between posttraumatic nightmare frequency and the HRR (rs(119) = 0.20, p = 0.027) and a t test showed that the HRR was significantly larger for participants in the posttraumatic nightmare group compared to participants without frequent posttraumatic nightmares (t(31.38) = −2.24, p = 0.032, d = −0.55). Nontraumatic nightmare frequency was significantly correlated with the SCR (rs(119) = 0.19, p = 0.040), although a t test did not show a significant difference in the SCR between participants with frequent nontraumatic nightmares and participants without frequent nontraumatic nightmares or with frequent posttraumatic nightmares (t(26.69) = −1.42, p = 0.166, d = −0.35). A logistic regression analysis with the HRR and SCR as the main predictors, while also controlling for age (grand-mean centered), sex, and months since trauma, showed that the HRR significantly predicted posttraumatic nightmares (OR 1.97, CI 1.04–3.74, p = 0.038), but not nontraumatic nightmares (OR 0.86, CI 0.43–1.68, p = 0.661). The SCR, on the other hand, did not significantly predict posttraumatic nightmares (OR 0.69, CI 0.29–1.64, p = 0.399), but it showed a trend for nontraumatic nightmares (OR 2.07, CI 0.87–4.92, p = 0.098). None of the other psychophysiological measures showed an association with either posttraumatic or nontraumatic nightmares.

Discussion

Using an acoustic startle paradigm in a civilian sample of trauma survivors, we found that PTSD diagnosis and symptom severity were strongly associated with posttraumatic nightmares. PTSD diagnosis and posttraumatic nightmares were significantly associated with a larger mean HRR to a series of loud tones. Nontraumatic nightmare frequency, on the other hand, was associated with a larger mean SCR. The mean EMGR of the left orbicularis oculi and baseline resting HRV were not associated with any of our PTSD or nightmare measures.

Our results are in line with previous findings suggesting a strong association between posttraumatic nightmares and the development of PTSD (Davis et al., 2007; de Dassel et al., 2018; Wittmann et al., 2010). Furthermore, since the rapid changes in HR in our acoustic startle paradigm seem to be primarily driven by withdrawal of parasympathetic activity (Chen et al., 2014; Orr et al., 1995; Vila et al., 2007), our findings suggest that the close relationship between posttraumatic nightmares and PTSD might be due to a shared pathophysiology in the form of reduced activity of the parasympathetic nervous system. These findings are consistent with the only other study investigating posttraumatic nightmares in an acoustic startle paradigm (Tanev et al., 2017). Our finding that nontraumatic nightmare frequency was associated with an elevated SCR is partly consistent with a study by Nielsen et al. (2010), which found an elevated sympathetic drive during sleep in individuals with frequent nontraumatic nightmares following REM deprivation. However, sympathetic drive was most prominent during REM sleep compared to stage 2 sleep or wakefulness. Therefore, future research should investigate differences in autonomic activity between posttraumatic and nontraumatic nightmares in different sleep stages. It is possible that these differences might be more pronounced during sleep than during wakefulness.

Although some studies suggest a role of sympathetic activity in the development of PTSD (e.g. Geracioti et al. 2001; Mellman, Kumar, Kulick-Bell, Kumar, & Nolan, 1995; Southwick et al. 1999), elevated HR seems to be one of the most reliable correlates of PTSD, while the SCR and EMG features of the startle response have not consistently discriminated between PTSD and non-PTSD (e.g. Orr et al., 2003; Pole, 2007). Such findings imply the potential importance of the parasympathetic nervous system in psychophysiological abnormalities of PTSD. Parasympathetic influence might also explain the somewhat inconsistent findings for current pharmacological treatments of PTSD and nightmares, which mainly rely on medication designed to reduce sympathetic activity, such as prazosin (Gieselmann et al., 2019; Morgenthaler et al., 2018; Raskind et al., 2018; Reist et al., 2020; Yücel, van Emmerik, Souama, & Lancee, 2020). Although patients may still respond well to these medications, it seems important for future clinical trials to investigate the efficacy of medications that target parasympathetic activity as well as to compare the effects of these substances on posttraumatic and nontraumatic nightmares.

These previous findings are also in line with a recent study using an acoustic startle and fear conditioning paradigm in trauma-exposed women, which found that reduced parasympathetic activity may play an important role in impaired fear extinction (Seligowski et al., 2020). Impaired fear extinction has not only been suggested to be an important risk factor for PTSD, but also as a potential etiological factor of nightmare disorder (Levin & Nielsen, 2007). Nightmares might impair the consolidation of extinction memory by activating and reinforcing arousing fear memories, which cannot be integrated into the associative memory network and this might be further reinforced by sleep disruption, which in turn might further exacerbate stress responses and elevate sympathetic activity during sleep (Gieselmann et al., 2019; Levin & Nielsen, 2007; Nielsen, 2017; Pace-Schott, Germain, & Milad, 2015). Although impaired fear extinction may play an important role in both nontraumatic and posttraumatic nightmares, these associations could be especially strong for nightmares that activate and reinforce trauma-related fear memories.

Contrary to previous studies (e.g. Campbell et al., 2019), we did not find a significant association between resting HRV and PTSD. However, results are mixed and the associations are likely complex. Notably, these associations might be more pronounced during sleep (e.g. stage 2 or REM sleep; Nielsen et al. 2010; Simor et al. 2014; Ulmer, Hall, Dennis, Beckham, & Germain, 2018).

Our findings should be interpreted in the context of some limitations. We did not control for nightmare intensity or distress in our analyses. It could be assumed that frequent posttraumatic nightmares are high in intensity and distress, whereas for nontraumatic nightmares these measures might vary more significantly. Therefore, our findings are not able to clarify whether posttraumatic and nontraumatic nightmares are categorically distinct phenomena or whether they differ along a continuum of nightmare severity (Blaskovich, Reichardt, Gombos, Spoormaker, & Simor, 2020; Mysliwiec et al., 2018; Nielsen, 2017; Phelps, Forbes, & Creamer, 2008). Furthermore, a previous study suggests that it might be important to take prior adversity into account when comparing posttraumatic and nontraumatic nightmares (e.g. Nielsen et al., 2019). Due to the close relationship between posttraumatic nightmares and PTSD, there was a significant overlap between the members of the PTSD and posttraumatic nightmare groups. As part of the structured diagnostic interview, frequent trauma-related nightmares also contributed to the PTSD diagnosis. Although about 61% of the PTSD group did not report frequent posttraumatic nightmares and we also found an association between elevated HRR and posttraumatic nightmares when using continuous frequency measures to operationalize these constructs, future studies should try to specifically investigate the pathophysiology of posttraumatic nightmares in trauma survivors suffering from posttraumatic nightmares, but not from PTSD. Our cross-sectional design does not allow our findings to be interpreted as direct evidence for causal associations. Future studies should use longitudinal designs, in order to further unmask the complex and potentially bi-directional associations between these phenomena. Decreased parasympathetic activity may be both a symptom as well as a shared pathophysiological factor for posttraumatic nightmares and PTSD. Additionally, as a hypothesis generating exploratory first analysis of findings, and so as to avoid type II error, we did not adjust for multiple comparisons. Future replications of these findings should take into account both the possible inflation of type I error by multiple comparisons as well as the fact that the psychophysiological measures were intercorrelated. However, it is important to note that overall, our findings, relative to their respective dichotomized comparators, of a significantly larger mean HRR in individuals with a PTSD diagnosis and in individuals with frequent posttraumatic nightmares as well as a potentially larger mean SCR in individuals with frequent nontraumatic nightmares were fairly consistent, supporting the validity of our results. Finally, following the Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDoC) paradigm (Lang, McTeague, & Bradley, 2016), our community sample included sub-clinical as well as entirely resilient trauma-exposed individuals, which placed our sample, on average, toward the lower end of posttraumatic symptom severity. There are also indications, that the relatively young age of our sample might have contributed to the lower symptom severity of our sample (e.g. Kobayashi, Sledjeski, & Delahanty, 2019).

Our findings have important implications. Despite the strong association between posttraumatic nightmares following a PTE and the development of PTSD, nightmares are often underreported and undertreated, with the majority of nightmare sufferers believing that their nightmares cannot be treated (Nadorff, Nadorff, & Germain, 2015; Schredl, 2013; Thünker, Norpoth, von Aspern, Özcan, & Pietrowsky, 2014). It is therefore of great importance that health care providers are trained to thoroughly assess nightmares during initial encounter with trauma survivors and to be able to offer evidence-based treatment as soon as possible (e.g. Augedal, Hansen, Kronhaug, Harvey, & Pallesen, 2013; Gieselmann et al., 2019). Our findings suggest that psychological and pharmacological treatments that positively influence parasympathetic nervous system activity might be especially effective. Since posttraumatic nightmares following a PTE might also interact strongly with other acute symptoms (Haag, Robinaugh, Ehlers, & Kleim, 2017), such interventions might have a far-reaching impact.

In conclusion, our findings point to a shared pathophysiology between PTSD and posttraumatic nightmares in the form of reduced parasympathetic activation. This is of particular importance given that augmented startle response is one diagnostic criterion in the hyperarousal cluster of PTSD symptoms (DSM-5) and has been noted in numerous experimental studies of PTSD (Glover et al., 2011; Robison-Andrew et al., 2014). For nontraumatic nightmares the influence of the sympathetic nervous system might be more pronounced. Further research is warranted to confirm whether posttraumatic and nontraumatic nightmares are indeed two categorically distinct phenomena that may require specific and potentially different treatment approaches.

Supplementary Material

Mader et al. 2021 supplemental

Financial support.

This study was supported by the National Institute of Health/National Institute of Mental Health (EPS, grant number R01MH109638) and the Swiss National Science Foundation (TM, grant number P0ZHP1_168869).

Footnotes

Conflict of interest. None.

Supplementary material. The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721002075.

Ethical standards. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. DSM-5. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Augedal AW, Hansen KS, Kronhaug CR, Harvey AG, & Pallesen S (2013). Randomized controlled trials of psychological and pharmacological treatments for nightmares: A meta-analysis. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 17(2), 143–152. 10.1016/j.smrv.2012.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Benjet C, Bromet E, Karam EG, Kessler RC, McLaughlin KA, Ruscio AM, … Koenen KC (2016). The epidemiology of traumatic event exposure worldwide: Results from the world mental health survey consortium. Psychological Medicine, 46(2), 327–343. 10.1017/S0033291715001981. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Blaskovich B, Reichardt R, Gombos F, Spoormaker VI, & Simor P (2020). Cortical hyperarousal in NREM sleep normalizes from pre- to post- REM periods in individuals with frequent nightmares. Sleep, 43(1), 1–11. 10.1093/sleep/zsz201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Boscarino JA (2004). Posttraumatic stress disorder and physical illness: Results from clinical and epidemiologic studies. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1032(1), 141–153. 10.1196/annals.1314.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Boyraz G, Granda R, Baker CN, Tidwell LL, & Waits JB (2016). Posttraumatic stress, effort regulation, and academic outcomes among college students: A longitudinal study. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 63(4), 475–486. 10.1037/cou0000102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Buhlmann U, Wilhelm S, Deckersbach T, Rauch SL, Pitman RK, & Orr SP (2007). Physiologic responses to loud tones in individuals with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69(2), 166–172. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31802f2799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Campbell AA, Wisco BE, Silvia PJ, & Gay NG (2019). Resting respiratory sinus arrhythmia and posttraumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis. Biological Psychology, 144, 125–135. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Carson MA, Metzger LJ, Lasko NB, Paulus LA, Morse AE, Pitman RK, & Orr SP (2007). Physiologic reactivity to startling tones in female Vietnam nurse veterans with PTSD. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 20(5), 657–666. 10.1002/jts.20218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chen K-H, Aksan N, Anderson SW, Grafft A, & Chapleau MW (2014). Habituation of parasympathetic-mediated heart rate responses to recurring acoustic startle. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–10. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Critchley H, & Nagai Y (2013). Electrodermal activity (EDA). In Gellman MD., & Turner JR J. R. (Eds), Encyclopedia of behavioral medicine (pp. 666–669). New York, NY: Springer. 10.1007/978-1-4419-1005-9_13. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. David D, & Mellman TA (1997). Dreams following hurricane Andrew. Dreaming, 7(3), 209–214. 10.1037/h0094475. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Davis JL, Byrd P, Rhudy JL, & Wright DC (2007). Characteristics of chronic nightmares in a trauma-exposed treatment-seeking sample. Dreaming, 17(4), 187–198. 10.1037/1053-0797.17.4.187. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. de Dassel T, Wittmann L, Protic S, Höllmer H, & Gorzka RJ (2018). Association of posttraumatic nightmares and psychopathology in a military sample. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 10(4), 475–481. 10.1037/tra0000319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dückers MLA, Alisic E, & Brewin CR (2016). A vulnerability paradox in the cross-national prevalence of post-traumatic stress disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 209(4), 300–305. 10.1192/bjp.bp.115.176628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Galatzer-Levy IR, Huang SH, & Bonanno GA (2018). Trajectories of resilience and dysfunction following potential trauma: A review and statistical evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 63, 41–55. 10.1016/j.cpr.2018.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Geracioti TD, Baker DG, Ekhator NN, West SA, Hill KK, Bruce AB, … Kasckow JW (2001). CSF Norepinephrine concentrations in posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(8), 1227–1230. 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.8.1227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Germain A, & Nielsen TA (2003). Sleep pathophysiology in posttraumatic stress disorder and idiopathic nightmare sufferers. Biological Psychiatry, 54 (10), 1092–1098. 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00071-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gieselmann A, Aoudia MA, Carr M, Germain A, Gorzka R, Holzinger B, … Pietrowsky R (2019). Aetiology and treatment of nightmare disorder: State of the art and future perspectives. Journal of Sleep Research, 28(4), e12820. 10.1111/jsr.12820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Glover EM, Phifer JE, Crain DF, Norrholm SD, Davis M, Bradley B, … Jovanovic T (2011). Tools for translational neuroscience: PTSD is associated with heightened fear responses using acoustic startle but not skin conductance measures. Depression and Anxiety, 28(12), 1058–1066. 10.1002/da.20880. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gordan R, Gwathmey JK, & Xie L-H (2015). Autonomic and endocrine control of cardiovascular function. World Journal of Cardiology, 7(4), 204–214. 10.4330/wjc.v7.i4.204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Haag C, Robinaugh DJ, Ehlers A, & Kleim B (2017). Understanding the emergence of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder through acute stress symptom networks. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(6), 649–650. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.0788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hawn SE, Cusack SE, & Amstadter AB (2020). A systematic review of the self-medication hypothesis in the context of posttraumatic stress disorder and comorbid problematic alcohol use. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 33(5), 699–708. 10.1002/jts.22521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kleiger RE, Stein PK, & Bigger JT (2005). Heart rate variability: Measurement and clinical utility. Annals of Noninvasive Electrocardiology, 10(1), 88–101. 10.1111/j.1542-474X.2005.10101.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kobayashi I, Sledjeski EM, & Delahanty DL (2019). Gender and age interact to predict the development of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms following a motor vehicle accident. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 11(3), 328–336. 10.1037/tra0000366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Koch M, & Schnitzler H-U (1997). The acoustic startle response in rats - circuits mediating evocation, inhibition and potentiation. Behavioural Brain Research, 89(1–2), 35–49. 10.1016/S0166-4328(97)02296-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kwak SG, & Kim JH (2017). Central limit theorem: The cornerstone of modern statistics. Korean Journal of Anesthesiology, 70(2), 144–156. 10.4097/kjae.2017.70.2.144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Laborde S, Mosley E, & Thayer JF (2017). Heart rate variability and cardiac vagal tone in psychophysiological research - recommendations for experiment planning, data analysis, and data reporting. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1–18. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lang PJ, McTeague LM, & Bradley MM (2016). RDoC, DSM, and the reflex physiology of fear: A biodimensional analysis of the anxiety disorders spectrum. Psychophysiology, 53(3), 336–347. 10.1111/psyp.12462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Langston TJ, Davis JL, & Swopes RM (2010). Idiopathic and posttrauma nightmares in a clinical sample of children and adolescents: Characteristics and related pathology. Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma, 3(4), 344–356. 10.1080/19361521.2010.523064. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. LeBouthillier DM, McMillan KA, Thibodeau MA, & Asmundson GJG (2015). Types and number of traumas associated with suicidal ideation and suicide attempts in PTSD: Findings from a U.S. Nationally representative sample. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 28(3), 183–190. 10.1002/jts.22010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lemyre A, Bastien C, & Vallières A (2019). Nightmares in mental disorders: A review. Dreaming, 29(2), 144–166. 10.1037/drm0000103. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Levin R, & Nielsen TA (2007). Disturbed dreaming, posttraumatic stress disorder, and affect distress: A review and neurocognitive model. Psychological Bulletin, 133(3), 482–528. 10.1037/0033-2909.133.3.482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Liu M, Amey RC, Magerman A, Scott M, & Forbes CE (2020). The role of startle fluctuation and non-response startle reflex in tracking amygdala dynamics [preprint]. bioRxiv. 10.1101/2020.01.12.903526. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Loya RM (2015). Rape as an economic crime: The impact of sexual violence on survivors’ employment and economic well-being. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 30(16), 2793–2813. 10.1177/0886260514554291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Malik M (1996). Heart rate variability: Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Circulation, 93(5), 1043–1065. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Mellman TA, Kumar A, Kulick-Bell R, Kumar M, & Nolan B (1995). Nocturnal/daytime urine noradrenergic measures and sleep in combat-related PTSD. Biological Psychiatry, 38(3), 174–179. 10.1016/0006-3223(94)00238-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Morgenthaler TI, Auerbach S, Casey KR, Kristo D, Maganti R, Ramar K, … Kartje R (2018). Position paper for the treatment of nightmare disorder in adults: An American academy of sleep medicine position paper. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 14(06), 1041–1055. 10.5664/jcsm.7178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Mueller-Pfeiffer C, Zeffiro T, O’Gorman R, Michels L, Baumann P, Wood N, … Orr SP (2014). Cortical and cerebellar modulation of autonomic responses to loud sounds. Psychophysiology, 51(1), 60–69. 10.1111/psyp.12142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Mysliwiec V, Brock MS, Creamer JL, O’Reilly BM, Germain A, & Roth BJ (2018). Trauma associated sleep disorder: A parasomnia induced by trauma. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 37, 94–104. 10.1016/j.smrv.2017.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Nadorff MR, Nadorff DK, & Germain A (2015). Nightmares: Under-reported, undetected, and therefore untreated. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 11(07), 747–750. 10.5664/jcsm.4850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Nielsen TA (2017). The stress acceleration hypothesis of nightmares. Frontiers in Neurology, 8, 201. 10.3389/fneur.2017.00201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Nielsen TA, Carr M, Picard-Deland C, Marquis L-P, Saint-Onge K, Blanchette-Carrière C, & Paquette T (2019). Early childhood adversity associations with nightmare severity and sleep spindles. Sleep Medicine, 56, 57–65. 10.1016/j.sleep.2019.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Nielsen TA, Paquette T, Solomonova E, Lara-Carrasco J, Colombo R, & Lanfranchi P (2010). Changes in cardiac variability after REM sleep deprivation in recurrent nightmares. Sleep, 33(1), 113–122. 10.1093/sleep/33.1.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Nielsen TA, & Zadra A (2000). Dreaming disorders. In Kryger M, Roth N & Dement WC (Eds.), Principles and practice of sleep medicine (pp. 753–772). Philadelphia, WB: Saunders. [Google Scholar]
  46. Ohayon MM, Carskadon MA, Guilleminault C, & Vitiello MV (2004). Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: Developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep, 27(7), 1255–1273. 10.1093/sleep/27.7.1255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Orr SP, Lasko NB, Shalev AY, & Pitman RK (1995). Physiologic responses to loud tones in Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104(1), 75–82. 10.1037/0021-843X.104.1.75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Orr SP, Metzger LJ, Lasko NB, Macklin ML, Hu FB, Shalev AY, & Pitman RK (2003). Physiologic responses to sudden, loud tones in monozygotic twins discordant for combat exposure: Association with posttraumatic stress disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(3), 283–288. 10.1001/archpsyc.60.3.283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Pace-Schott EF, Germain A, & Milad MR (2015). Sleep and REM sleep disturbance in the pathophysiology of PTSD: The role of extinction memory. Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders, 5(1), 3. 10.1186/s13587-015-0018-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Pace-Schott EF, Tracy LE, Rubin Z, Mollica AG, Ellenbogen JM, Bianchi MT, … Orr SP (2014). Interactions of time of day and sleep with between-session habituation and extinction memory in young adult males. Experimental Brain Research, 232(5), 1443–1458. 10.1007/s00221-014-3829-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Phelps A, Forbes D, & Creamer M (2008). Understanding posttraumatic nightmares: An empirical and conceptual review. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(2), 338–355. 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Phelps AJ, Forbes D, Hopwood M, & Creamer M (2011). Trauma-related dreams of Australian veterans with PTSD: Content, affect and phenomenology. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 45(10), 853–860. 10.3109/00048674.2011.599314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Pole N (2007). The psychophysiology of posttraumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 133(5), 725–746. 10.1037/0033-2909.133.5.725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Raskind MA, Peskind ER, Chow B, Harris C, Davis-Karim A, Holmes HA, … Huang GD (2018). Trial of prazosin for post-traumatic stress disorder in military veterans. New England Journal of Medicine, 378(6), 507–517. 10.1056/NEJMoa1507598. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Reist C, Streja E, Tang CC, Shapiro B, Mintz J, & Hollifield M (2020). Prazosin for treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. CNS Spectrums, Online ahead of print, May 4, 1–7. 10.1017/S1092852920001121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Robert G, & Zadra A (2014). Thematic and content analysis of idiopathic nightmares and bad dreams. Sleep, 37(2), 409–417. 10.5665/sleep.3426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Robison-Andrew EJ, Duval ER, Nelson CB, Echiverri-Cohen A, Giardino N, Defever A, … Rauch SAM (2014). Changes in trauma-potentiated startle with treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder in combat veterans. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28(4), 358–362. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Schredl M (2013). Seeking professional help for nightmares: A representative study. The European Journal of Psychiatry, 27(4), 259–264. [Google Scholar]
  59. Schreuder BJN, Igreja V, van Dijk J, & Kleijn W (2001). Intrusive re-experiencing of chronic strife or war. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 7(2), 102–108. 10.1192/apt.7.2.102. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Schreuder BJN, Kleijn WC, & Rooijmans HGM (2000). Nocturnal re-experiencing more than forty years after war trauma. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 13(3), 453–463. 10.1023/A:1007733324351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Secrist ME, Dalenberg CJ, & Gevirtz R (2019). Contributing factors predicting nightmares in children: Trauma, anxiety, dissociation, and emotion regulation. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 11 (1), 114–121. 10.1037/tra0000387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Seligowski AV, Merker JB, Swiercz AP, Park J, Marvar PJ, Ressler KJ, & Jovanovic T (2020). Examining the cardiovascular response to fear extinction in a trauma-exposed sample. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 124, 85–90. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.02.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Shaffer F, & Ginsberg JP (2017). An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Frontiers in Public Health, 5, 1–17. 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Shaffer F, McCraty R, & Zerr CL (2014). A healthy heart is not a metronome: An integrative review of the heart’s anatomy and heart rate variability. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–19. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Simor P, Horváth K, Gombos F, Takács KP, & Bódizs R (2012). Disturbed dreaming and sleep quality: Altered sleep architecture in subjects with frequent nightmares. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 262(8), 687–696. 10.1007/s00406-012-0318-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Simor P, Körmendi J, Horváth K, Gombos F, Ujma PP, & Bódizs R (2014). Electroencephalographic and autonomic alterations in subjects with frequent nightmares during pre-and post-REM periods. Brain and Cognition, 91, 62–70. 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Southwick SM, Bremner JD, Rasmusson A, Morgan CA, Arnsten A, & Charney DS (1999). Role of norepinephrine in the pathophysiology and treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 46(9), 1192–1204. 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00219-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Szabadi E (2012). Modulation of physiological reflexes by pain: Role of the locus coeruleus. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6. 10.3389/fnint.2012.00094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Taft CT, Watkins LE, Stafford J, Street AE, & Monson CM (2011). Posttraumatic stress disorder and intimate relationship problems: A meta-analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79(1), 22–33. 10.1037/a0022196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Tanev KS, Orr SP, Pace-Schott EF, Griffin M, Pitman RK, & Resick PA (2017). Positive association between nightmares and heart rate response to loud tones: Relationship to parasympathetic dysfunction in PTSD nightmares. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 205(4), 308–312. 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000641. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Thünker J, Norpoth M, von Aspern M, Özcan T, & Pietrowsky R (2014). Nightmares: Knowledge and attitudes in health care providers and nightmare sufferers. Journal of Public Health and Epidemiology, 6(7), 223–228. [Google Scholar]
  72. Turpin G, Schaefer F, & Boucsein W (1999). Effects of stimulus intensity, risetime, and duration on autonomic and behavioral responding: Implications for the differentiation of orienting, startle, and defense responses. Psychophysiology, 36(4), 453–463. 10.1111/14698986.3640453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Ulmer CS, Hall MH, Dennis PA, Beckham JC, & Germain A (2018). Posttraumatic stress disorder diagnosis is associated with reduced parasympathetic activity during sleep in US veterans and military service members of the Iraq and Afghanistan wars. Sleep, 41(12), 1–9. 10.1093/sleep/zsy174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Vila J, Guerra P, Munoz M, Vico C, Viedmadeljesus M, Delgado L, … Rodriguez S (2007). Cardiac defense: From attention to action. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 66(3), 169–182. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Vossel G, & Zimmer H (1992). Stimulus rise time, intensity and the elicitation of unconditioned cardiac and electrodermal responses. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 12(1). 41–51. 10.1016/01678760(92)90041-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Weathers FW, Blake DD, Schnurr PP, Kaloupek DG, Marx BP, & Keane TM (2013a). The Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-5 (CAPS-5). Retrieved from https://www.ptsd.va.gov. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Weathers FW, Litz BT, Keane TM, Palmieri PA, Marx BP, & Schnurr PP (2013b). The PTSD Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5). Retrieved from https://www.ptsd.va.gov. [Google Scholar]
  78. Wittmann L, Zehnder D, Schredl M, Jenni OG, & Landolt MA (2010). Posttraumatic nightmares and psychopathology in children after road traffic accidents. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 23(2), 232–239. 10.1002/jts.20514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Yücel DE, van Emmerik AAP, Souama C, & Lancee J (2020). Comparative efficacy of imagery rehearsal therapy and prazosin in the treatment of trauma-related nightmares in adults: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 50, 1–9. 10.1016/j.smrv.2019.101248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Mader et al. 2021 supplemental

RESOURCES