Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2022 Apr 6;36(6):346–359. doi: 10.1177/15459683221088864

A novel way of measuring dual-task interference: the reliability and construct validity of the dual-task effect battery in neurodegenerative disease

Jason K Longhurst 1, John V Rider 2, Jeffrey L Cummings 3, Samantha E John 4, Brach Poston 5, Elissa Held-Bradford 6, Merrill R Landers 7
PMCID: PMC9133058  NIHMSID: NIHMS1786484  PMID: 35387509

Abstract

Background:

Decreased automaticity is common among individuals with neurodegenerative disease and is often assessed using dual-task (DT) paradigms. However, the best methods for assessing performance changes related to DT demands remain inconclusive.

Objective:

To investigate the reliability and validity of a novel battery of DT measures (DT Effect – Battery (DTE-B)) encompassing three domains: task-specific interference, task prioritization, and automaticity.

Methods:

Data for this retrospective cross-sectional study included 125 participants with Parkinson’s disease (PD), 127 participants with Alzheimer’s disease (AD), and 84 healthy older adults. Reliability analyses were conducted using a subset of each population. DTE-B measures were calculated from single and DT performance on the Timed Up and Go test and a serial subtraction task. Construct validity was evaluated via associations within the DTE-B and with theoretically supported measures as well as known-groups validity analyses.

Results:

Good to excellent reliability was found for DTE-B measures of task interference (motor and cognitive DT effects) (ICCs≥.658) and automaticity (combined DT effect (cDTE)) (ICCs≥.938). Evidence for convergent validity was found with associations within the hypothesized constructs. Known-groups validity analyses revealed differences in the DTE-B among the healthy group and PD and AD groups (ps≤.001), excepting task prioritization (ps≥.061).

Conclusions:

This study provides evidence to support the DTE-B as a reliable measure of multiple constructs pertinent to DT performance. The cDTE demonstrated evidence to support its validity as a measure of automaticity. Further investigation of the utility of the DTE-B in both PD and AD, as well as other populations, is warranted.

Keywords: Automaticity, dual-task interference, Parkinson disease, Alzheimer disease, Neurobehavioral Manifestations, Multitasking behavior

Introduction

A decrease in automaticity - performance of task without attention directed to task execution - is common among individuals with neurodegenerative disease.1,2 It is a key deficit in Parkinson’s disease (PD) and is a crucial target of retraining and rehabilitation for individuals with PD.3 In PD, automaticity is more impaired among those with freezing of gait and cognitive impairment.46 In Alzheimer’s disease (AD), deficits in automaticity progress with disease severity.79 Automaticity is compromised in individuals with mild cognitive impairment relative to healthy individuals9 and individuals with mild cognitive impairment with impaired automaticity progress to dementia more rapidly than those with comparatively intact automaticity.10,11

Automaticity is assessed by dual-task (DT) paradigms wherein a primary task (often a motor task) and a secondary task (often a cognitive task) are done concurrently;1 however, there is no consensus on the best methods or measures for assessing DT performance.12 Yang et al. observed that there were many different methods for assessing the DT effect or the relative change in performance resulting from conducting a DT.12 DT assessment is performed by quantifying the change in primary task performance or the change in secondary task performance while completing the combined task.1214 One of the most accepted measures for assessing task specific DT interference is calculating the motor or cognitive DT effect, which relates DT performance to single-task (ST) performance.12 While this method is valuable in assessing individual task components related to DT performance, a measure assessing DT interference that quantifies the combined interference of the motor and the cognitive tasks may be a more holistic measure of DT effect and may provide a more accurate picture of automaticity.1517 Task prioritization (i.e., whether the motor or cognitive task is prioritized during DT) is most often determined by the manner of instruction for performing the DT. Recently, there have been efforts made to categorize and quanitify task prioritization.13,14,18

Few studies investigating DT performance in PD or AD have included measures that individually assess change in performance of both the primary and secondary tasks that comprise the DT (motor DT effect (mDTE) and cognitive DT effect (cogDTE)). Fewer still have utilized a measure of task prioritization. To our knowledge, only one study has previously utilized a robust measure of combined interference in these populations.15 The inclusion of measures of motor, cognitive, and combined effects of DT interference, as well as task prioritization, may help to elucidate subtle motor and cognitive deficits related to automaticity, particularly in individuals with neurodegenerative disease.19 Additionaly, utilization of a series of measures allows for assessment of other important aspects of DT performance including between task discrepancies and task prioritization and for putting them in context of each other. This method could prove useful for assessing disability, disease progression, and response to treatment.20

The purpose of this study was to investigate the reliability and validity of a novel combination of DT measures herein referred to as the DT effect battery (DTE-B) among individuals PD, AD, and healthy older adults. The DTE-B includes several DT measures falling into three related domains: 1) Task specific interference or effects (mDTE and cogDTE); 2) Task prioritization (task prioritization category and modified attention allocation index (mAAI)); and 3) Automaticity (combined DTE (cDTE) - a novel measure of combined motor and cognitive DT interference). The first aim of this study was to investigate the test-retest reliability of the DTE-B. The second aim was to investigate the construct validity (encompassing convergent validity, divergent validity, and known-groups validity) of the DTE-B. We hypothesized that evidence for the validity of the cDTE would be demonstrated through moderate to strong associations with measures of other DT metrics and more automatic tasks, as well as weak to no relationship with tasks with relatively high attentional demands (e.g., high task complexity involving many sensory, motor, and cognitive functions). We anticipated that known-groups validity analyses would provide further evidence for the validity of the DTE-B. While AD and PD have differing pathophysiology and presentation, they have many commonalities including they they are both progressive nneurodegenerative diseases the are protient misfoling disease, have prominent neuroinflammatory mechanisms, and lead to both cognitive and motor impairments.21,22 The main phenotypic difference is that PD has more prominent early motor features with later cognitive impairment while AD has more prominent early cognitive features.23 The cDTE considers cognitive performance with equal weight to motor performance, such that a relatively similar decline in either will result in a similar outcome for cDTE. Thus, we hypothesized that while automaticity (cDTE) would be least impacted in the healthy older adult group, both the PD and AD groups would be more impacted, though not different between groups. However, since PD has more prominent motor impairment than cognitive impairment, and AD is characterized by more prominent cognitive impairment than motor impairment, we predicted that these disease features would influence task specific interference (mDTE and cogDTE). Specifically we anticipated that they would be different between these two groups with the AD group showing greater cogDTE and less mDTE than the PD group. We hypothesized that mAAI would differ between groups, with the PD group prioritizing cognitive performance (less decline in cognitive performance than motor performance under DT conditions), the AD group prioritizing motor performance (less decline in motor performance than cogntive performance under DT conditions), and the healthy older group demonstrating no clear prioritization strategy.

Methods

Design

A retrospective cross-sectional analysis of data collected from Cleveland Clinic Lou Ruvo Center for Brain Health (CCLRCBH) clinical medical records (PD, AD) and research (healthy older adults) data sets was conducted.24 The work was approved by the Cleveland Clinic Institutional Review Board, number 16-101, and informed consent was waived. Demographic data, DT performance, PD symptoms, cognition and depression, balance and falls, and gait measures in healthy older adults and individuals with PD and AD were captured. Participants that completed repeat DT measurement at least 7 days but no more than 28 days apart (mean 8.9±3.7) were included in the reliability analyses. For Aim 1, test-retest reliability of the DTE-B was investigated in a subset of 105 participants made up of individuals from the PD (n=37), AD (n=34), and healthy (n=34) groups. For Aim 2, we explored the construct validity of the DTE-B by comparing its components to measures of other constructs (PD symptoms (PD group only), cognition and depression, balance and falls, and gait). Convergent validity was assessed by comparing the DTE-B with measures of the same or similar constructs, and divergent validity was evaluated by comparing the DTE-B to dissimilar constructs, including depression (Patient Health Questionnaire-9; PHQ-9) and non-motor symptoms of PD (Movement Disorder Society-Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale; MDS-UPDRS part I). Known groups validity was assessed by comparing DTE-B performance between AD, PD, and healthy groups.

Participants

Parkinson’s disease and Alzheimer’s disease cohorts.

All patients with an initial physical therapy evaluation at CCLRCBH from July 2017 to June 2019 were identified from medical records. Clinical diagnosis of PD or AD was completed by a neurologist using contemporary diagnostic criteria.2527 Patients were included in either the AD or PD cohort based on their clinical diagnosis. The AD cohort included individuals ranging in symptomatic presentation from moderate dementia to mild cognitive impairment due to AD. Inclusion criterion for this study was a referral to physical therapy for primary treatment of PD or AD and completion of the dual task assessment (described below). All participants included in the analyses had a standardized physical therapy assessment conducted by one of four licensed physical therapists at one outpatient facility. All physical therapists underwent annual training for systematic collection and assessment of the outcome measures used in this study. Patients were excluded if they were referred to physical therapy for treatment of any condition that was not a result of PD or AD, including vestibular dysfunction, significant osteoarthritis, or lower extremity injury (fractures, strains, sprains) or surgery. Data from 252 records were extracted - 125 individuals with PD, and 127 individuals with AD (Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Study data sources flow diagram.

Healthy older adult cohort.

This cohort consisted of individuals ages 55-85 who were neurologically healthy and community-dwelling. Individuals were excluded from this group if they had significant orthopedic conditions that affected their gait or if they had any evidence of cognitive impairment (Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) <26).

There were group differences observed in demographic characteristics (Table 1). Specifically, the healthy group was younger and the PD group more predominantly male compared to other groups. Additionally, the racial composition of the groups differed.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics and p-values for between group differences (Quade’s ANCOVA, or Chi square test as appropriate) for the PD, AD, and healthy groups. Benjamini-Hochberg correction applied to control for family wise error. Significant group main effect p-values after correction are marked in red.

PD (n=125) AD (n=127) Healthy (n=84) p-value
DEMOGRAPHICS
  Age 74.3 (±8.6) 75.3 (±9.3) 70.3 (±5.8) <.001 *
  Sex (female %) 37 (29.6%) 59 (46.5%) 46 (54.8%) .001
  Race White: 113 White: 102 White: 75
Black: 1 Black: 12 Black: 4
Multiracial: 4 Multiracial: 4 Multiracial: 0 .036 **
Asian: 7 Asian: 3 Asian: 5
Pacific Islander: 0 Pacific Islander: 1 Pacific Islander: 0
  Ethnicity Hispanic: 2 Hispanic: 2 Hispanic: 4 .261
Non-Hispanic: 123 Non-Hispanic: 125 Non-Hispanic: 80
  Years since onset 5.9 ± 4.9 4.8 ±3 .6 NA .217
DUAL TASK PERFORMANCE
  ST TUG 12.8 ± 10.9 12.8 ± 6.4 7.1 ± 1.8 <.001 *
  ST correct response rate 3.0 ± 1.9 4.4 ± 3.0 2.1 ± .8 <.001
  DT TUG (TUGcog) 18.7 ± 20.8 19.4 ± 12.1 8.7 ± 2.7 <.001
  DT correct response rate 6.5 ± 9.9 9.1 ± 9.2 2.9 ± 2.5 <.001
  mDTE −42.0 ± 45.3 −52.1 ± 51.8 −24.6 ± 27.3 <.001 *
  cogDTE −103.6 ± 252.7 −112.6 ± 156.5 −35.6 ± 71.6 <.001 *
  Task prioritization category MPT – 4 MPT – 10 MPT – 8 .061
CPT – 21 CPT – 18 CPT – 16
MI – 100 MI – 97 MI – 56
MF – 0 MF – 2 MF – 4
  mAAI 61.5 ± 247.1 60.6 ± 150.8 11.0 ± 69.3 .413
  cDTE −213.3 ± 421.9 −245.5 ± 320.1 −74.2 ± 131.3 <.001 *
PD SYMPTOMS
  MDS-UPDRS Part I 8.2 ± 5.0
  MDS-UPDRS Part II 13.6 ± 6.2
  MDS-UPDRS Part III 27.4 ± 13.2
COGNITION & DEPRESSION
  MoCA 26.7± 5.0 18.3 ± 5.4 27.4 ± 2.0 <.001 §
  PHQ-9 6.8 ± 5.6 7.5 ± 6.1 .341
BALANCE & FALLS
  MiniBESTest
 Anticipatory 4.2 ± 1.0 3.9 ± .8 5.5 ± .7 <.001 *
 Reactive 4.0 ± 1.8 3.7 ± 1.4 5.7 ± .7 <.001 *
 Sensory Organization 5.1 ± 1.2 5.1 ± 1.0 5.8 ± .5 .001 *
 Dynamic Gait 6.7 ± 2.3 6.1 ± 1.9 9.0 ± .9 <.001
 MiniBESTest total score 20.1 ± 5.4 18.8 ± 4.0 25.9 ± 1.9 <.001
  Falls in last 30 Days .88 ± 3.2 .59 ± 1.4 .00 ± .00 <.001
  Falls in last Year 9.6 ± 35.0 2.5 ± 7.7 .3 ± .6 <.001
  Injurious Falls .39 ± .83 .37 ± .68 .06 ± .238 .003 *
  mFFABQ 16.6 ± 12.1 18.4 ± 14.2 .3 ± 1.2 <.001 *
GAIT
  6 minute walk test 341.1 ± 140.3 312.1 ± 124.1 508.5 ± 70.9 <.001 *

ST – Single Task, TUG – Timed Up and Go, DT – Dual task, TUGCog – Timed Up and Go Cognitive, mDTE – motor dual task effect, cogDTE – cognitive dual task effect, MPT – motor priority trade-off, CPT – cognitive priority trade-off, MI – mutual interference, MF- mutual facilitation, mAAI – modified attention allocation index, cDTE – combined dual task effect, MDS-UPDRS – Movement Disorder Society – Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale, MoCA – Montreal Cognitive Assessment, PHQ-9 – Patient Health Questionnaire, mFFABQ- modified Fear of Falling Avoidance Behavior Questionnaire.

*

Healthy group statistically different from the PD and AD groups

All groups statistically different from each other

§

AD group statistically different from PD and Healthy groups

PD group statistically different from AD and Healthy groups

**

PD group statistically different from AD group.

Measures

DTE-B.

The DTE-B includes measures of task specific interference (mDTE and cogDTE), task prioritization (task prioritization category and mAAI), and automaticity (cDTE) which can be derived from performance of a single DT assessment (Figure 2). Times for the following measures were extracted from physical therapy records: Timed Up and Go (TUG) and TUG-Cognitive (TUGcog). The TUG exhibits good test-retest (ICCs>0.80) reliability in individuals with PD and AD.2831 The TUGcog has excellent test-retest reliability (ICC=0.94) for community-dwelling older adults.32 ST cognitive performance during serial subtraction by three was captured by recording the number of correct responses during 20 seconds and the inverse correct response rate calculated as time in seconds divided by number of correct responses.3335 DT cognitive performance was obtained during the TUGcog using the same method beginning from a different number between 80 and 100 and the inverse correct response rate calculated as the time to complete the TUGcog divided by the number of correct responses during the TUGcog. The resultant ST and DT correct response rates were entered into the DTE equations. The serial subtraction task was selected as it is the most common clinically utilized dual task paradigm and one of the most commonly studied. Instructions for the DT were designed to encourage neutral prioritization between the two tasks (“Walk as quickly as you can safely while completing the subtraction task as quickly as you can accurately”). These variables were then used to calculate mDTE and cogDTE using the equation:14

DTE(%)=DTSTST×100%
Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Dual task effect battery.

The cDTE equation was designed based on the DTE equation and expanded to assess the combined interference of both mDTE and cDTE (Supplemental Material 1). The cDTE was calculated using the following equation:

cDTE(%)=(motorDT×CognitiveDT)(motorST×cognitiveST)(motorST×cognitiveST)×100%

For both the DT effect equations in this study, higher values indicate poorer performance, so a negative sign was inserted into the formula to be consistent with the established operational definition of DT effects:13,14

DTE(%)=DTSTST×100%
cDTE(%)=(motorDT×CognitiveDT)(motorST×cognitiveST)(motorST×cognitiveST)×100%

This approach creates the convention that all negative DT effect values are indicative of performance that deteriorated under DT conditions compared to ST conditions (DT cost). A positive DT effect value is indicative of a relative improvement on performance under DT conditions (DT facilitation).13,14 While mDTE and cogDTE are measures of task specific interference, it is proposed that cDTE is a measure that quantifies automaticity while performing a DT.

In addition to the measures of DT effect, task prioritization category (based on the criteria established by Plummer et al)13 and mAAI were utilized to assess task prioritization during DT performance. Task prioritization category was determined by plotting the mDTE against the cogDTE with each quadrant representative of one of the following categories: mutual interference (decline in motor and cognitive performance), cognitive priority trade-off (improved or unchanged cognitive performance accompanied by decline in motor performance), motor priority trade-off (improved or unchanged motor performance accompanied by decline in cognitive performance), and mutual facilitation (improved motor and cognitive performance).13 mAAI was calculated utilizing mDTE and cogDTE values to assess for trade-offs within the task as follows:14,18

mAAI=mDTEcogDTE

Positive values represent a shift in attention toward the motor task, whereas negative values indicate a shift in attention toward the cognitive task.

PD symptoms.

The MDS-UPDRDS parts I-III is a standardized measure of the impact of PD on non-motor symptoms, activities of daily living, and disease specfici motor skills, respectively. The MDS-UPDRS parts I-III were used to characterize PD symptoms.36 The MDS-UPDRS was administered by a trained movement disorders neurologist.

Cognition and Depression.

Cognition was measured using the MoCA, which was designed to differentiate normal cognition, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia due to AD. It has excellent test-retest reliability (correlation coefficient = 0.92)37, with good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.83). Additionally, it has evidence for its validity in early PD.38 The PHQ-9 was used to measure depression in the PD and AD groups and excellent reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = 089).39 The MoCA was administered by a trained PD/AD nurse or neurologist.

Balance and falls.

Scores from the Mini Balance Evaluation Systems Test (MiniBESTest) were included to describe balance performance. The MiniBESTest measures anticipatory postural control, reactive balance, sensory orientation, and dynamic gait and exhibits excellent inter-rater reliability (ICC = 0.98).40 Falls in the last 30 days, falls in the last year, and fall-related injuries in the last year were extracted. Lastly, scores for the modified fear of falling avoidance behavior questionnaire was extracted. The modified fear of falling avoidance behavior questionnaire is a self report questionnaire that quantifies avoidance behavior due to fear falling. There is evidence for the reliability of the modified fear of falling avoidance behavior questionnaire among individuals with PD and other neurologic conditions (ICC=.812).41 These measures were all administered by a trained physical therapist.

Gait.

Scores from the Six Minute Walk Test (6MWT) were included.42,43 The 6MWT has excellent test-retest reliability (ICC = 0.95-0.96) for individuals with PD.30 It has excellent test-retest (ICC = 0.982-0.987), interrater (ICC = 0.97-0.99), and intrarater reliability (ICC = 0.76-0.9) for individuals with AD.28,44 The 6MWT was administered by a trained physical therapist.

Sample size estimation

Sample size was estimated using effect size and standard deviation for cDTE obtained from pilot data using PASS 20.0.3 (NCSS, LLC. Kaysville, Utah, USA, www.ncss.com/software/pass) for both aims. For the reliability analysis (aim 1), confidence intervals for interclass correction module was utilized. The estimate revealed that a sample of 34 participants, who were each measured twice, would produce a two-sided 95% confidence interval with a width of .200 when the estimated interclass correlation is .850 utilizing a two-way random-effects ANOVA model (ICC 3,2). For construct validity (aim 2), a sample of 84 achieves 80% power to detect a difference of .3 between the null hypothesis correlation and the alternative hypothesis correlation using a two-sided hypothesis test with a significance level set at α=.05.

Data Analysis

All analyses were conducted using SPSS 24.0 (IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Armonk, NY: IBM Corp) with α = 0.05. Descriptive statistics and between group comparisons were conducted using chi square for nominal data and nonparametric (Quade’s) ANCOVA, due to lack of homogeneity of variance between groups and to include age as a covariate. Statistical corrections for multiple comparisons were completed using Benjamini-Hochberg corrections.

For Aim 1 (test-retest reliability and minimal detectable change (MDC)), a subset of participants from each group (PD (n=37), AD (n=34), healthy (n=34)) completed DT assessment twice. These data were analyzed using the ICC model 3 for continuous data and Cohen’s kappa for categorical data (task prioritization category). ICC conventions were defined as poor (<.4), fair (.4 to .59), good (.6 to .74), and excellent (.75 to 1.00).45 Kappa values were interpreted according to the following criteria: poor (<.00), slight (.00 to .20), fair (.21 to .40), moderate (.41 to .60), substantial (.61 to .80), and almost perfect (.81 to 1.00).46 To determine the MDC, standard error of measurement (SEM) was calculated using the ICC test-retest reliability statistic, where SD=standard deviation and rxx= ICC test-retest reliability statistic:

SEM=SD1rxx

Once the SEM was determined, the minimal detectable change at a 95% confidence level (MDC95) for cDTE was calculated by multiplying the SEM by 1.96 (representing 95% of the area under the curve of a normal distribution) and 1.41 (the square root of 2 to control for possible error associated with calculating the coefficient from 2 time points).

MDC=SEM×Z×2

For Aim 2 (construct validity), convergent validity, divergent validity, and known-groups analyses were completed. In this study, we compared the DTE-B to four domains of outcomes (PD symptoms, cognition and depression, balance and falls, and gait) using correlational statistics (Pearson product moment correlations or Spearman’s rho). We anticipated evidence for convergent relationship between cDTE and measures in the DTE-B, and more automatic tasks that require comparatively less attentional resources, such as steady state gait47 (6MWT) and balance reactions48 (MiniBESTest reactive balance subscale). Conversely, we anticipate that tasks demanding a moderate or greater degree of attentional resources such as anticipatory balance control49 (MiniBESTest anticipatory balance subscale), activities of daily living50 (MDS-UPDRS part II), and disease specific motor skills1,51 (MDS-UPDRS part III) will correlate more strongly with either task specific interference measures (mDTE and cogDTE) or mAAI, a measure of attention allocation. We anticipated that tasks that require high levels of attentional resource allocation such as falls and modified Fear of Falling Avoidance Behavior Questionnaire would be more closely related to mAAI specifically.52,53 Conversely, we expected divergent validity, for cDTE specifically, to be demonstrated by minimal to no relationship between cDTE and the MDS-UPDRS part I and PHQ-9, and relationships with measures that require attentional resources that are weaker than their counterparts in the task interference domain (mDTE and/or cogDTE) and task prioritization domain (mAAI) as described above.

The groups for the known-groups validity analysis were created based on neurologic diagnosis as follows: PD group (more motor impairment than cognitive impairment), AD group (more cognitive impairment than motor impairment), and a healthy older adult group (no impairment). Due to the age differences between groups, non-parametric (Quade’s) ANCOVA was conducted comparing performance on DTE-B measures (mDTE, cogDTE, mAAI, and cDTE) across groups entering age as a covariate.

RESULTS

There were several between group differences on the outcomes of interest. These are presented in Table 1.

Aim 1 (Reliability analysis).

The results of the test-retest reliability analysis for mDTE, cogDTE, task prioritization category, mAAI, and cDTE are summarized in Table 1. Briefly, mDTE, cogDTE, and cDTE all exhibited good-to-excellent reliability in all three groups. Among these DT metrics, cDTE was the most reliable. Task prioritization category was found to have poor reliability in the PD and AD groups as kappa values were not found to be significantly different from zero. As task prioritization category was found to have poor reliability, it will not be further discussed. Lastly, mAAI had excellent reliability in the PD group, good reliability in the AD group, and poor reliability in the healthy group.

Aim 2 (Convergent validity analysis).

Many relationships between DTE-B and measured variables were observed and are reported in Table 3. Briefly, correlational analyses revealed strong associations between mDTE and cDTE, cogDTE and cDTE, cogDTE and mAAI, and mAAI and cDTE for each of the three groups, with moderate relationships observed between mDTE and cogDTE. In examing hypothesized relationships within the automaticity domain, cDTE demonstrated moderate relationships with MiniBESTest reactive balance subscale and 6MWT (all groups). Among measures hypothesized to relate to domains impacted by attentional resources, in the task specific interference domain, mDTE demonstrated minimal relationship with outcomes across all groups while cogDTE was found to be moderately related to MDS-UPDRS part III (PD), MiniBESTest anticipatory balance subscale (PD), falls the last year (healthy), and modified fear of falling avoidance behavior questionnaire (PD). Within the task prioritization domain, mAAI had moderate relationships with MDS-UPDRS part II (PD), and MiniBESTest anticipatory balance subscale (PD).

Table 3.

Pearson product moment correlation coefficient or Spearman’s rho for mDTE, cogDTE, mAAI, and cDTE on hypothesized measures that require attentional control (task specific interference and task prioritization), hypothesized measures that require automaticity, and measures hypothesized to have no relationship with DTE-B among the PD, AD, and healthy groups. Benjamini-Hochberg corrections were applied to control for family wise error. Significant p-values after correction are marked in red.

PD AD HEALTHY
VARIABLE (STATISTIC) mDTE cogDTE mAAI cDTE mDTE cogDTE mAAI cDTE mDTE cogDTE mAAI cDTE
DTE-B
  mDTE (r) X .212 * −.034 .498 ** X .275 * .058 .604 ** X .272 * .113 .498 **
  cogDTE (r) .212* X −.984 ** 929 ** .275 * X −.944 ** .882 ** .272* X −.925 ** .951 **
  mAAI (r) −.034 −.984 ** X −.859 ** .058 −.944 ** X −.708 ** .113 −.925 ** x −.785 **
  cDTE (r) .498 ** .929 ** −.859 ** X .604 ** .882 ** −.708 ** X .498 ** .951 ** −.785 ** X
MEASURES THAT REQUIRE ATTENTIONAL RESOURCES
  MDS-UPDRS Part II (ρ) −.047 −.337 .384 * −.275
  MDS-UPDRS Part III (ρ) −.014 −.189 * .260* −.149
  MiniBESTest-Anticipatory (ρ) .053 .273 * −.281 * .239 * .013 .008 −.097 .033 −.005 .269* −.229 .230
  Falls in last 30 Days (r) .061 .033 −.023 .055 .028 −.105 .188 −.044 .000 .000 .000 .000
  Falls in last Year (r) .094 .060 −.044 .072 −.044 −.038 −.024 −.037 −.259* −.349 ** .259* −.399 **
  Injurious Falls (r) .022 −.021 .025 −.048 −.081 −.117 .095 −.153 .066 .064 −.043 .069
  mFFABQ (ρ) −.139 −.287 * .303* −.269 * −.048 −.120 .168 −.053 .331 .229 −.025 .306
MEASURES THAT REQUIRE AUTOMATICITY (MINIMAL ATTENTIONAL RESOURCES)
  MiniBESTest-Reactive (ρ) .249* .298 ** −.207 * .327 ** .169 .099 −.184 .010 .139 .034 −.114 .076
  6 minute walk test (r) .189 .281 * −.244 * .298 * .124 .352 ** −.313 * .347 ** .057 .294 * −.282 * .282 *
MEASURS HYPOTHESIZED TO HAVE NO RELATIONSHIP WITH DTE-B
  MDS-UPDRS Part I (ρ) .117 −.045 .174 .002
  PHQ-9 (ρ) −.151 −.175 .146 −.166 .125 −.028 .123 .039
*

=p value <.05

**

=p value<.001

PD – Parkinson’s disease, AD – Alzheimer’s disease, DTE-B – dual task effect battery, mDTE – motor dual task effect, cogDTE – cogntivie dual task effect, mAAI – modified attention allocation index, cDTE – combined dual task effect, MDS-UPDRS – movement disorder society – Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale, mFFABQ- modified Fear of Falling Avoidance Behavior Questionnaire, PHQ-9 – Patient Health Questionnaire.

Aim 2 (Divergent validity analysis).

The details of the results can be found in Table 3. No DTE-B measures were associated with MDS-UPDRS part I or with PHQ-9. In observing relative strengths of relationships between DTE-B and other outcomes based on hypothesized relationships, measures of task specific interference or task prioritization domains, which rely on attentional resources, were more strongly associated with MiniBESTest anticipatory balance subscale (PD), MDS-UPDRS part II (PD), MDS-UPDRS part III (PD), and modified fear of falling avoidance behavior questionnaire (PD) than cDTE. cDTE, in the automaticity domain, was found to be more strongly related to miniBESTest reactive balance subscale (PD), 6MWT (PD), and falls in the last year (healthy). Additionally cDTE was found to have minimal relationships among hypothesized outcomes related to task specific interference and task prioritization (PD, AD, and Healthy).

Aim 2 (Known-groups validity analysis).

The known-groups validity analyses revealed significant between group differences for mDTE, cogDTE, and cDTE (ps<.001), while showing no difference on mAAI (p=.245) (Figure 3). Schefe post hoc tests revealed that both the PD and AD groups differed from the healthy group (ps≤.033) but not from each other on all DTE-B measures (ps≥.295).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Means and standard errors of the motor dual task effect (mDTE), cognitive dual task effect (cogDTE), modified attention allocation index (mAAI), and combined dual task effect (cDTE) among Parkinson’s disease (PD), Alzheimer’s disease (AD), and healthy groups.Statistically significant difference between groups on Schefe post hoc tests indicate with brackets and the p value reported.

Discussion

Reliability.

This study demonstrated that measures of task specific interference (mDTE and cogDTE) and automaticity (cDTE) exhibit excellent test-retest reliability. Of these measures, the cDTE exhibited the highest level of reliability in each group. This is consistent with our hypothesis that cDTE is a measure of a comparatively more stable construct, automaticity. Compared to the other measures of DT performance, which can be highly influenced or biased by allocation of attention, cDTE incorporates measurement of both tasks reducing the influence of attention allocation. As task prioritization is greatly influenced by volitional attention allocation, it was unsurprising that task prioritization category and mAAI were found to be the least reliable of the DTE-B. This was particularly true of mAAI in the healthy group and is likely a result of this group having no distinct deficit in either cognition or motor control for which they need to actively compensate. This allows their task prioritization strategy to be more fluid than individuals with cognitive or motor deficits. MDCs found in this study were substantially smaller than those reported by Venema et al.54 These differences may be primarily attributable to smaller ICC values found by Venema et al., which may be due to insufficient power due to an inadequate sample. In summary, this study provides evidence for the reliability of the DTE-B with good to excellent reliability for measures of task specific interference and automaticity, while measures of task prioritization demonstrated poor to good reliability.

Convergent and divergent validity.

In investigating the construct validity of the DTE-B, and following our hypothesis, there were moderate to strong relationships with cDTE and mDTE, cogDTE, and mAAI in all groups. This is not surprising as these measures share many common elements and variables in their formulation; however, it does indicate the highly related nature of these measures of task specific interference, task prioritization, and automaticity, as they relate to DT performance. Among individuals with PD, there were moderate relationships identified between cDTE and measures of gait and reactive balance, such that poorer automaticity was related to poorer gait and reactive balance. These outcomes were anticipated to rely on automaticity, thus providing evidence for the validity of the cDTE as a measure of automaticity. Among the other variables in the DTE-B (mDTE, cogDTE, and mAAI), cDTE was most strongly associated with mDTE, cogDTE, and mAAI. Additionally, the pattern observed in the PD group was partially replicated in the AD and healthy groups, with both groups showing moderate associations between cDTE and the 6MWT, indicating that individuals with greater impairments in automaticity ambulated less distance. Overall, there is substantial evidence for cDTE as a measure of automaticity in the PD group, while in the AD and healthy groups this is less conclusive and would benefit from further investigation.

In contrast to cDTE which was hypothesized to be related to automatic functions, we anticipated that measures of task specific interference and task prioritization would be more related to tasks requiring attentional resources. The findings in the PD group support this hypothesis while the findings in the AD and healthy groups are less conclusive. Within the healthy group, this may be attributable to floor (falls, modified fear of falling avoidance behavior questionnaire) or ceiling effects (miniBESTest anticipatory balance subscale) and minimal variability on the outcomes in these domains limiting the possibility for relationships to be identified statistically. Within the AD group, it is possible that cognitive impairments may have resulted in inaccurate reporting of falls and recognition of fear of falling avoidance behavior. Conversely, we may accept that there is no relationship between these variables in the AD and healthy groups; however, prior research has shown relationship between falls and mDTE in healthy older adults55 and those with cognitive impairment.56 Further research investigating DTE-B measures relationship to psychological variables is warranted to further support the proposed domains.

Additionaly, this study provides evidence supporting the divergent validity of the DTE-B through a lack of association seen between it and dissimilar constructs (i.e., DTE-B and MDS UDPRS I and PHQ 9). The pattern of associations in the PD group was clearly delineated and in line with the a priori hypotheses and support the distinct but relatedness of the domains proposed in the DTE-B. The pattern of findings in the AD group is less clear; however, they are somewhat consistent with finding previously reported among individuals with cognitive impairment,15 indicating that cognitive specific interference (cogDTE) may play a leading role in driving DT-related performance in those with AD. We found mDTE was not strongly related to gait, balance and falls, but cogDTE was, highlighting the importance of considering multiple outcomes when evaluating DT performance. In summary, the findings of this study support the DTE-B as a measure of the multiple DT dimensions. This provides opportunities for future work that extends knowledge of DT mechanisms and applications to multiple DT related domains.

Known-groups validity.

In the known-groups validity analyses, consistent with our hypothesis, we found that DT performance was uniformly better in the healthy group compared to the PD and AD groups. In contrast to our hypothesis, we found no difference in the presentation of DT performance among the PD and AD groups on task specific interference, which we anticipated would be influenced by the difference in phenotypic disease features, with AD having more prominent cognitive involvement and PD more prominent motor involvement. This appears to be primarily driven by task prioritization during DT, which was motor prioritized in both groups; this may be a strategy that prioritizes safety over cognition during a functional task.57 These results are supported by the literature showing that both individuals with PD and AD tend to self-prioritize motor activities over cognitive activities during DT.58,59 One possible explanation for our results is that task prioritization may be more a function of the task involved.57,6062 This can occur when the attributes of one task are inherently more salient, novel, or challenging. In the case of the DT paradigm utilized in this study, it is possible that the motor task (TUG) was more salient than the cognitive task. Conversely, it is also possible that the cognitive task (serial subtraction) was more challenging or novel than the motor task. Either situation could resulti in an increased likelihood for individuals to utilize a motor prioritization streategy. Alternatively, this lack of difference may be due to the instructional language used, which encouraged neutral prioritization.63 Our results show that DT performance may be different between healthy and neurodegenerative populations; however, the patterns of DT performance may not be related to disease pathology. Future research should utilize multiple DT paradigms across participants with different diseases to assess how task and individual characteristics influence task prioritization.

Dual-task performance and falls.

In contrast to previous findings, this study found little association between DT performance and falls. Among all groups, only the healthy older adult group was found to have an association between falls and cogDTE and cDTE. This finding is consistent with previous research indicating the relationship between DT performance and falls in the healthy older adult population.64 Suprisingly, in the PD and AD groups, no relationship between DT performance and falls was identified. While this finding diverges from much of the literature, Heinzel et al. also found that mDTE and cogDTE of a cognitive-motor DT which consisted of serial subtraction and walking, similar to this study, were not associated with future falls in PD.65 However, in the PD group there was a relationship between DT performance and modified fear of falling avoidance behavior questionnaire, indicating that when DT performance was poorer, individuals had more avoidance behavior related to fear of falling. This may support the hypothesis that poor DT performance is related to falls; however, a sufficiently large portion of participants in the PD group may have recognized this risk and adapted by avoiding risky behaviors, thereby reducing their falls. This would most likely occur in the PD group given the higher fall history and greater need to avoid risky behaviors.66

Interpretation of the DTE-B.

The DTE-B is a tool that can be utilized by clinicians and researchers to characterize DT performance, and can provide clinicians information for better informed decision making. As the DTE-B includes measures of task specific interference, task prioritization, and automaticity, it allows for interpretation of results relating to these domains and their interactions. Generally speaking, poorer cDTE is indicative of a decreased capacity for automaticity which is not able to be sufficiently compensated for by attentional strategies. Changes in cDTE consequently indicate changes in this capacity. mAAI, on the otherhand is a measure of task prioritization, which is more flexible, and can be interpreted as appropriate or maladaptive in the context of the individual and the task. For example, if an individual with a history of falls demonstrates poor motor DT performance during a walking or balance paradigm and mAAI indicates significant cognitive prioritization, then this individual is likely demonstrating a mal adaptive task prioritization strategy, whereas an individual with MCI without fall history or other risks may be demonstrating an appropriate accommodative task prioritization strategy.

Limitations

This study utilized data collected during routine physical therapy practice for the PD and AD participants, and as such, it is possible that these samples represent unique subpopulations with uncertain generalizability. Additionally, while this study did utilize consistent instructional language encouraging neutral task prioritization, it is possible that the instructional language influenced task performance, resulting in changes in attentional resource allocation from usual performance. It is also possible that the DT paradigm utilized in this study may not be most representative of daily life activities that require dual-tasking and, thus, our results may not be generalizable to these DT activities. As such, different DT paradigms with differing task components, may have resulted in different results. As the mechanisms for all aspects of DT performance are not fully understood in especially in the context of PD and AD. Thus, the analyses between the PD and AD groupsbe interpreted cautiously. Research regarding DT mechanisms is warranted. The cognition of the PD group in this study was largely within normal limits; subsequently, our results may not be generalizable to individuals with PD-dementia. Additionally, the falls data collected was retrospective in nature and subject to several potential biases and our results regarding falls should be interpreted cautiously. This study utilized a restrospective design and as such some variables that may be related to the DTE-B were not able to be included. Participants in this study were included based on clinical diagnosis, which could occur at different stages of disease between individuals with AD and PD. It is possible that while our sample appears to have similar disease duration in the AD and PD group, it is possible that disease severity could be different.

Conclusion

This study provides evidence of the reliability of the DTE-B, novel tool that may improve the consistency of measurement and reporting of DT-related performance and abilities. The DTE-B may improve the characterization of multiple domains within DT performance, specifically attention allocation and automaticity, which are underutilized both clinically and in research. The novel measure of combined interference, cDTE, demonstrated adequate evidence to support its validity as a possible measure of automaticity, as shown in analyses of convergent, divergent, and known-groups validity. Further investigation of the utility of the DTE-B is warranted.

Supplementary Material

1

Table 2.

Test-retest reliability (ICC(3,2) with 95% confidence interval) and minimal detectable change (MDC95) for the motor dual task effect, cognitive dual task effect, modified attention allocation index, and combined dual task effect for each groups (PD, AD, and healthy). Test-retest reliability (Cohen’s κ with 95% confidence interval) for task prioritization category for each group.

PD (n=37) AD (n=34) Healthy (n=34)
mDTE ICC=.825 (.687 to .906)** MDC= 21.0 ICC=.841 (.705 to .917)** MDC= 18.9 ICC=.815 (.710 to .885)* MDC= 8.9
CogDTE ICC=.887 (.792 to .940)** MDC= 42.5 ICC=.827 (.681 to .910)** MDC= 40.9 ICC=.658 (.416 to .813)** MDC= 10.9
Task prioritization category κ=.236 (−.089 to.561) κ=.171 (−.201 to.543) κ=.457 (.126 to.788)**
mAAI ICC=.776 (.607 to .878)** MDC= 42.1 ICC=.690 (.462 to .832)** MDC= 40.8 ICC=.353 (.022 to .641)* MDC= 13.1
cDTE ICC=.968 (.940 to .984)** MDC= 92.4 ICC=.938 (.880 to .969)** MDC= 80.5 ICC=.945 (.892 to .972)** MDC= 18.2
*

=p value <.05

**

=p value<.001

PD – Parkinson’s disease, AD – Alzheimer’s disease, mDTE – motor dual task effect, cogDTE – cognitive dual task effect, mAAI – modified attention allocation index, cDTE – combined dual task effect

Acknowledgements:

The parent study from which the health older adult cohort was obtained was funded by an Institutional Development Award (IDeA) from the National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health: #P20GM109025.

Footnotes

Declaration of Conflicting Interests: The Authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.

Contributor Information

Jason K. Longhurst, Department of Physical Therapy and Athletic Training, Saint Louis University, St. Louis, Missouri, USA; Department of Neurorehabilitation, Cleveland Clinic Lou Ruvo Center for Brain Health, Las Vegas, Nevada, USA; Department of Physical Therapy, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, USA.

John V. Rider, School of Occupational Therapy, Touro University Nevada, Henderson, Nevada, USA; Department of Physical Therapy, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, USA.

Jeffrey L. Cummings, Department of Brain Health, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, USA.

Samantha E. John, Department of Brain Health, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, USA.

Brach Poston, Department of Kinesiology and Nutrition Sciences, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, USA.

Elissa Held-Bradford, Department of Physical Therapy and Athletic Training, Saint Louis University, Saint Louis, Missouri, USA.

Merrill R. Landers, Department of Physical Therapy, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, USA.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Wu T, Hallett M, Chan P. Motor automaticity in Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiol Dis. 2015;82:226–234. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2015.06.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Christofoletti G, Andrade LP de, Beinotti F, et al. Cognition and dual-task performance in older adults with Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Gen Med. 2014;7:383–388. doi: 10.2147/IJGM.S65803 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Rochester L, Baker K, Hetherington V, et al. Evidence for motor learning in Parkinson’s disease: acquisition, automaticity and retention of cued gait performance after training with external rhythmical cues. Brain Res. 2010;1319:103–111. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.01.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Amboni M, Barone P, luppariello L, et al. Gait patterns in parkinsonian patients with or without mild cognitive impairment. Mov Disord. 2012;27(12):1536–1543. doi: 10.1002/mds.25165 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Spildooren J, Vercruysse S, Desloovere K, Vandenberghe W, Kerckhofs E, Nieuwboer A. Freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease: The impact of dual-tasking and turning. Mov Disord. 2010;25(15):2563–2570. doi: 10.1002/mds.23327 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Vervoort G, Heremans E, Bengevoord A, et al. Dual-task-related neural connectivity changes in patients with Parkinson’ disease. Neuroscience. 2016;317:36–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.12.056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ansai JH, Andrade LP, Rossi PG, Takahashi ACMM, Vale FACC, Rebelatto JR. Gait, dual task and history of falls in elderly with preserved cognition, mild cognitive impairment, and mild Alzheimer’s disease. Braz J Phys Ther. 2017;21(2):144–151. doi: 10.1016/j.bjpt.2017.03.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.König A, Klaming L, Pijl M, Demeurraux A, David R, Robert P. Objective measurement of gait parameters in healthy and cognitively impaired elderly using the dual-task paradigm. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2017;29(6):1181–1189. doi: 10.1007/s40520-016-0703-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Muir SW, Speechley M, Wells J, Borrie M, Gopaul K, Montero-Odasso M. Gait assessment in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: the effect of dual-task challenges across the cognitive spectrum. Gait Posture. 2012;35(1):96–100. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.08.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Montero-Odasso MM, Sarquis-Adamson Y, Speechley M, et al. Association of Dual-Task Gait With Incident Dementia in Mild Cognitive Impairment: Results From the Gait and Brain Study. JAMA Neurol. 2017;74(7):857–865. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.0643 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gillain S, Warzee E, Lekeu F, et al. The value of instrumental gait analysis in elderly healthy, MCI or Alzheimer’s disease subjects and a comparison with other clinical tests used in single and dual-task conditions. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2009;52(6):453–474. doi: 10.1016/j.rehab.2008.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Yang L, Lam FMH, Liao LR, Huang MZ, He CQ, Pang MYC. Psychometric properties of dual-task balance and walking assessments for individuals with neurological conditions: A systematic review. Gait Posture. 2017;52:110–123. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.11.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Plummer P, Eskes G. Measuring treatment effects on dual-task performance: a framework for research and clinical practice. Front Hum Neurosci. 2015;9:225. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00225 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kelly VE, Janke AA, Shumway-Cook A. Effects of instructed focus and task difficulty on concurrent walking and cognitive task performance in healthy young adults. Exp Brain Res. 2010;207(1-2):65–73. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2429-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Longhurst JK, Wise MA, Krist DJ, Moreland CA, Basterrechea JA, Landers MR. Brain volumes and dual-task performance correlates among individuals with cognitive impairment: a retrospective analysis. J Neural Transm. 2020;127(7). doi: 10.1007/s00702-020-02199-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Wahn B, Sinnett S. Shared or Distinct Attentional Resources? Confounds in Dual Task Designs, Countermeasures, and Guidelines. Multisens Res. 2019;32(2):145– 163. doi: 10.1163/22134808-20181328 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Brauner FO, Balbinot G, Figueiredo AI, Hausen DO, Schiavo A, Mestriner RG. The Performance Index Identifies Changes Across the Dual Task Timed Up and Go Test Phases and Impacts Task-Cost Estimation in the Oldest-Old. Front Hum Neurosci. 2021;15(September):1–12. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.720719 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Siu KC, Woollacott MH. Attentional demands of postural control: the ability to selectively allocate information-processing resources. Gait Posture. 2007;25(1):121–126. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.02.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Belghali M, Chastan N, Cignetti F, Davenne D, Decker LM. Loss of gait control assessed by cognitive-motor dual-tasks: pros and cons in detecting people at risk of developing Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases. Geroscience. 2017;39(3):305–329. doi: 10.1007/s11357-017-9977-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.McIsaac TL, Fritz NE, Quinn L, Muratori LM. Cognitive-motor interference in neurodegenerative disease: A narrative review and implications for clinical management. Front Psychol. 2018;9(OCT). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02061 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Belghali M, Chastan N, Davenne D, Decker LM. Improving Dual-Task Walking Paradigms to Detect Prodromal Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s Diseases. Front Neurol. 2017;8(MAY). doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00207 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Compta Y, Revesz T. Neuropathological and Biomarker Findings in Parkinson’s Disease and Alzheimer’s Disease: From Protein Aggregates to Synaptic Dysfunction. J Parkinsons Dis. 2021;11(1):107–121. doi: 10.3233/JPD-202323 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Noe E, Marder K, Bell KL, Jacobs DM, Manly JJ, Stern Y. Comparison of dementia with Lewy bodies to Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease with dementia. Mov Disord. 2004;19(1):60–67. doi: 10.1002/mds.10633 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ritter A, Cummings J, Nance C, Miller JB. Neuroscience learning from longitudinal cohort studies of Alzheimer’s disease: Lessons for disease-modifying drug programs and an introduction to the Center for Neurodegeneration and Translational Neuroscience. Alzheimer’s Dement Transl Res Clin Interv. 2018;4(1):350–356. doi: 10.1016/J.TRCI.2018.06.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: A clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1992;55(3):181–184. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.55.3.181 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Albert MS, DeKosky ST, Dickson D, et al. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2011;7(3):270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Jack CR, Albert MS, Knopman DS, et al. Introduction to the recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2011;7(3):257–262. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Ries JD, Echternach JL, Nof L, Gagnon Blodgett M. Test-retest reliability and minimal detectable change scores for the timed “up & go” test, the six-minute walk test, and gait speed in people with Alzheimer disease. Phys Ther. 2009;89(6):569–579. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20080258 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Huang SL, Hsieh CL, Wu RM, Tai CH, Lin CH, Lu WS. Minimal detectable change of the timed “up & go” test and the dynamic gait index in people with Parkinson disease. Phys Ther. 2011;91(1):114–121. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20090126 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Steffen T, Seney M. Test-retest reliability and minimal detectable change on balance and ambulation tests, the 36-item short-form health survey, and the unified Parkinson disease rating scale in people with parkinsonism. Phys Ther. 2008;88(6):733–746. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20070214 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Morris S, Morris ME, Iansek R. Reliability of measurements obtained with the Timed “Up & Go” test in people with Parkinson disease. Phys Ther. 2001;81(2):810–818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hofheinz M, Schusterschitz C. Dual task interference in estimating the risk of falls and measuring change: a comparative, psychometric study of four measurements. Clin Rehabil. 2010;24(9):831–842. doi: 10.1177/0269215510367993 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Brustio PR, Magistro D, Zecca M, Rabaglietti E, Liubicich ME. Age-related decrements in dual-task performance: Comparison of different mobility and cognitive tasks. A cross sectional study. PLoS One. 2017;12(7). doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0181698 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hall CD, Echt KV., Wolf SL, Rogers WA. Cognitive and motor mechanisms underlying older adults’ ability to divide attention while walking. Phys Ther. 2011;91(7):1039–1050. doi: 10.2522/PTJ.20100114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Yang L, He C, Pang MY. Reliability and Validity of Dual-Task Mobility Assessments in People with Chronic Stroke. PLoS One. 2016;11(1):e0147833. doi: 10.1371/joumal.pone.0147833 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Goetz CG, Tilley BC, Shaftman SR, et al. Movement Disorder Society-sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPdRs): scale presentation and clinimetric testing results. Mov Disord. 2008;23(15):2129–2170. doi: 10.1002/mds.22340 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bedirian V, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53(4):695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kletzel SL, Hernandez JM, Miskiel EF, Mallinson T, Pape TL. Evaluating the performance of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment in early stage Parkinson’s disease. Park Relat Disord. 2017;37:58–64. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2017.01.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Intern Med. 2001;16(9):606–613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Godi M, Franchignoni F, Caligari M, Giordano A, Turcato AM, Nardone A. Comparison of reliability, validity, and responsiveness of the mini-BESTest and Berg Balance Scale in patients with balance disorders. Phys Ther. 2013;93(2):158–167. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20120171 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Landers MR, Durand C, Powell DS, Dibble LE, Young DL. Development of a scale to assess avoidance behavior due to a fear of falling: The fear of Falling Avoidance Behavior Questionnaire. Phys Ther. 2011;91(8):1253–1265. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20100304 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Crapo RO, Casaburi R, Coates AL, et al. ATS statement: Guidelines for the six-minute walk test. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2002. doi: 10.1164/ajrccm.166.1.at1102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bohannon RW. Comfortable and maximum walking speed of adults aged 20-79 years: Reference values and determinants. Age Ageing. 1997;26(1):15–19. doi: 10.1093/ageing/26.1.15 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Tappen RM, Roach KE, Buchner D, Barry C, Edelstein J. Reliability of physical performance measures in nursing home residents with Alzheimer’s disease. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 1997;52(1):M52–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Cicchetti DV. Guidelines, Criteria, and Rules of Thumb for Evaluating Normed and Standardized Assessment Instruments in Psychology. Psychol Assess. 1994. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.6.4.284 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Landis JR, Koch GG. The Measurement of Observer Agreement for Categorical Data. Biometrics. 1977;33(1):159. doi: 10.2307/2529310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.A M, S S, I M, JM H Gait. Handb Clin Neurol. 2018;159:119–134. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-63916-5.00007-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Peterson DS, Van Liew C, Stuart S, Carlson-Kuhta P, Horak FB, Mancini M. Relating Parkinson freezing and balance domains: A structural equation modeling approach. Park Relat Disord. 2020;79(April 2020):73–78. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2020.08.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Sakamoto S, Iguchi M. The degree of postural automaticity influences the prime movement and the anticipatory postural adjustments during standing in healthy young individuals. Hum Mov Sci. 2018;60(April):131–138. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2018.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Fitri FI, Fithrie A, Rambe AS. Association between working memory impairment and activities of daily living in post-stroke patients. Med Glas. 2020;17(2):384–389. doi: 10.17392/1135-20 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Wang YX, Zhao J, Li DK, et al. Associations between cognitive impairment and motor dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Behav. 2017;7(6):1–7. doi: 10.1002/brb3.719 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Santos LO dos, Carvalho de Abreu DC, Moraes R. Performance of Faller and Nonfaller Older Adults on a Motor–Motor Interference Task. J Mot Behav. 2018;50(3):293–306. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2017.1341380 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Pelosin E, Ogliastro C, Lagravinese G, et al. Attentional control of gait and falls: Is cholinergic dysfunction a common substrate in the elderly and Parkinson’s disease? Front Aging Neurosci. 2016;8(MAY):1–7. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Venema DM, Hansen H, High R, Goetsch T, Siu K-CC. Minimal Detectable Change in Dual-Task Cost for Older Adults With and Without Cognitive Impairment. J Geriatr Phys Ther. 2019;42(4):E32–E38. doi: 10.1519/JPT.0000000000000194 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Asai T, Oshima K, Fukumoto Y, Yonezawa Y, Matsuo A, Misu S. Does dual-tasking provide additional value in timed “up and go” test for predicting the occurrence of falls? A longitudinal observation study by age group (young-older or old-older adults). Aging Clin Exp Res. 2021;33(1):77–84. doi: 10.1007/s40520-020-01510-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Gonçalves J, Ansai JH, Masse FAA, Vale FAC, Takahashi AC de M, Andrade LP de. Dual-task as a predictor of falls in older people with mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease: a prospective cohort study. Brazilian J Phys Ther. 2018;22(5):417–423. doi: 10.1016/j.bjpt.2018.03.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Yogev-Seligmann G, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N. Do we always prioritize balance when walking? Towards an integrated model of task prioritization. Mov Disord. 2012;27(6):765–770. doi: 10.1002/mds.24963 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Simieli L, Barbieri FA, Orcioli-Silva D, Lirani-Silva E, Stella F, Gobbi LTB. Obstacle crossing with dual tasking is a danger for individuals with Alzheimer’s disease and for healthy older people. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2015;43(2):435–441. doi: 10.3233/JAD-140807 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Yogev-Seligmann G, Rotem-Galili Y, Dickstein R, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM. Effects of explicit prioritization on dual task walking in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Gait Posture. 2012;35(4):641–646. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.12.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Rapp MA, Krampe RT, Baltes PB. Adaptive task prioritization in aging: selective resource allocation to postural control is preserved in Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14(1):52–61. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000192490.43179.e7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Canning CG. The effect of directing attention during walking under dual-task conditions in Parkinson’s disease. Park Relat Disord. 2005;11(2):95–99. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2004.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Verghese J, Kuslansky G, Holtzer R, et al. Walking While Talking: Effect of Task Prioritization in the Elderly. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2007;88(1):50–53. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2006.10.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Plummer P, Altmann L, Feld J, Zukowski L, Najafi B, Giuliani C. Attentional prioritization in dual-task walking: Effects of stroke, environment, and instructed focus. Gait Posture. 2020;79:3–9. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2020.03.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Menant JC, Schoene D, Sarofim M, Lord SR. Single and dual task tests of gait speed are equivalent in the prediction of falls in older people: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev. 2014;16(1):83–104. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2014.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Heinzel S, Maechtel M, Hasmann SE, et al. Motor dual-tasking deficits predict falls in Parkinson’s disease: A prospective study. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2016;26:73–77. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Landers MR, Lopker M, Newman M, Gourlie R, Sorensen S, Vong R. A Cross-sectional Analysis of the Characteristics of Individuals With Parkinson Disease Who Avoid Activities and Participation Due to Fear of Falling. J Neurol Phys Ther. 2017;41(1):31–42. doi: 10.1097/NPT.0000000000000162 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

RESOURCES