Abstract
Sprinting is often seen in a variety of sports. Focusing one’s attention externally before sprinting has been demonstrated to boost sprint performance. The present study aimed to systematically review previous findings on the impact of external focus (EF), in comparison to internal focus (IF), on sprint performance. A literature search was conducted in five electronic databases (APA PsycINFO, PubMed, Scopus, SPORTDiscus, and Web of Science). A random-effects model was used to pool Hedge’s g with 95% confidence intervals (CIs). The meta-analysis included six studies with a total of 10 effect sizes and 166 participants. In general, the EF condition outperformed the IF condition in sprint performance (g = 0.279, 95% CI [0.088, 0.470], p = 0.004). The subgroup analysis, which should be viewed with caution, suggested that the benefits associated with the EF strategy were significant in low-skill sprinters (g = 0.337, 95% CI [0.032, 0.642], p = 0.030) but not significant in high-skill sprinters (g = 0.246, 95% CI [−0.042, 0.533], p = 0.094), although no significant difference was seen between these subgroups (p = 0.670). The reported gain in sprint performance due to attentional focus has practical implications for coaches and athletes, as making tiny adjustments in verbal instructions can lead to significant behavioral effects of great importance in competitive sports.
Keywords: external focus, internal focus, sprint performance, meta-analysis, qualitative interaction
1. Introduction
One of the most crucial motor skills in sports is sprinting. Sprint performance enhancement is a critical component of training programs for several individual and team sports, including short-track speed skating [1], soccer [2], American football [3], and ice hockey [4]. According to the mechanics and technique, sprinting can be divided into five stages: starting, acceleration, drive phase, recovery phase, and deceleration [5]. Despite accounting for only 5% of the total 100 m race time [6], the sprint start is one of the determining components of high performance in sprinting [7], as the margins of wins in short sprints can be as nuanced as a few milliseconds. In addition, a well-executed sprint start is conducive for athletes generating sufficient acceleration, optimizing their stride length, and outperforming others when sprinting [8]. Additionally, the shorter the sprint distance, the more critical the sprint start [9]. Furthermore, athletes who perform fast sprint starts have a psychological advantage over their competitors, which can be significant in many races [10,11]. Given that sprinting performance, especially the sprint start, is a critical skill that underpins performance in many sports, there is a large amount of scientific literature on sprint training, such as plyometric intervention programs [12], resisted sled training [13,14], and lower-limb wearable resistance training [15].
Over recent years, the role of attentional focus has emerged as a significant modulator in sprint performance. Attentional focus refers to an individual’s intentional attempt to direct their attention through explicit thoughts for the purpose of executing a motor skill [16,17]. An individual can pay attention to an IF (i.e., concentration on the body movements) or an EF (i.e., concentration on the intended movement effect). For example, a coach advising a sprinter on the push phase can give an internal cue such as, “Concentrate on exploding through your hips,” or an external cue such as, “Concentrate on exploding off the ground.” Both attentional cues call attention to the power output from an explosive driving force, but the IF emphasizes the body movement (i.e., apply force with the hips) and the EF underlines the movement effect (i.e., apply force to the ground).
Although there is often a one- or two-word difference between EF (e.g., “focus on driving the ground back”) and IF (“focus on driving your legs back”) instructions, available evidence suggests that tiny adjustments in verbal cues are enough to trigger significant behavioral effects in sprinting. Performers receiving an EF direction perform better at sprinting than those receiving an IF one [18,19]. For example, Kovacs et al. [19] examined the attentional focus effect during a sprint task where participants were instructed to accelerate as fast as possible to the finish line placed 6 m from the start line. In the study, all participants finished the sprint task under the EF and IF conditions using a counterbalanced within-subjects design. The results revealed that instructing participants to concentrate on pushing the blocks away (EF) resulted in a faster start reaction relative to instructing them to concentrate on fully extending their knees (IF). The result was similar to that reported by Ille et al. [18], who found that performers displayed more efficient sprint performance when focusing on exploding from the starting blocks (EF) compared with focusing on pushing quickly with their legs (IF). In addition, relevant reviews show that the EF advantage is not only embodied in sprint tasks but also robust in other sports tasks (e.g., soccer, golf, and swimming); for reviews, see [20,21,22]. Moreover, one recent meta-analysis [23] suggests an EF exceeds IF in motor performance with a small effect size (Hedges’ g = 0.264).
However, there has been no comprehensive evaluation specifically on the attentional focus effect on sprint performance, but the available evidence shows the benefit of the EF strategy in sprinting. Given the findings of Porter et al. [24], a comprehensive evaluation of the experimental evidence would have significant practical use. In that study, unexpectedly, 84.6% of surveyed athletes competing at the USA Track and Field Outdoor National Championships reported their coaches utilized verbal instructions that prompted an IF during authentic athletic practice [24]. According to the field investigations, there appears to be a divergence between experimental evidence and practical applications. As we know, a systematic review is an opportunity to narrow the evidence–practice gap [25], and a meta-analytic review is characterized by its comprehensiveness, quantified evidence, and guidance for practice [26]. Therefore, this divergence calls for a systematic evaluation and meta-analysis of these experimental findings to give evidence-based recommendations and reinforce the translation of knowledge into action.
In addition to providing an overall evaluation of the impact of attentional focus on sprinting, another issue requiring an answer is whether the skill level of performers moderates the attentional focus’s effects on sprint performance. In the research field of attentional focus, there is some dispute about whether skill level interacts with the attentional focus effect. Some studies indicate that the benefits of EF instructions have generalizability and show no dependency on skill [20], but other studies have shown that novices in the early phases of skill acquisition benefit less than experts because it is difficult for novices to shift their attentional focus from focusing internally on the correct execution of skills to focusing externally on the coordination of movements [27,28]. This issue set us thinking about whether skill level mediates the attentional focus effect on sprint performance, which is probably the most fundamental movement skill. Therefore, the current study included the skill level as a moderating variable to identify whether the attentional focus effect would vary according to the performer’s skill level.
Due to the above-mentioned views, the principal objective of the present work was to systematically assess the efficacy of EF versus IF in immediate sprint performance, including start reaction time and total sprint time. The secondary objective was to compare the immediate attentional focus effect between high-skill and low-skill sprinters.
2. Materials and Methods
This meta-analysis conformed with the PRISMA guidelines and adhered to the pre-registration protocol on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/cyzu5, accessed on 29 December 2021).
2.1. Literature Search Strategy
Five electronic databases (APA PsycINFO, PubMed, Scopus, SPORTDiscus, and Web of Science) were searched for predetermined key terms ((“attentional focus” OR “external focus” OR “focus of attention”) AND (“sprint”)) until December 2021. Furthermore, a supplementary manual search was conducted to identify relevant articles from the reference lists of each included article. The search was restricted to peer-reviewed, English-language research articles.
2.2. Eligibility Criteria
Studies were considered eligible according to the following criteria:
The participants were in a healthy state and over the average age of 18;
An EF intervention was employed;
A comparison between EF and IF was drawn;
The immediate effect of focus instruction on sprint performance was examined, including start reaction time or total sprint time;
A within-subjects study design was employed.
2.3. Literature Selection
Two reviewers (D.L. and X.Y.) independently selected the studies that met the eligibility criteria. At the end of the selection, when disagreements existed between the reviewers, the reviewers discussed the reasoning for their selections, and if one reviewer realized she or he had made a mistake, then the process was complete [29]. All cases of disagreement were solved in this way.
2.4. Data Extraction
One reviewer (D.L.) extracted and entered the following data from all included studies into Microsoft Excel. Another reviewer (X.Y.) checked the data for accuracy: author name (s), publication year, title, sample size, participant characteristics (age and expertise level), intervention instruction, study design, and sprint performance outcome (s).
Based on the included studies’ descriptions and the reviewers’ judgments, the participants’ skill levels were categorized into two types: high-skill and low-skill sprinters. The participants classified as highly skilled at sprinting were skilled sprinters participating in high-level competitions, collegiate track sprinters with more than five years’ experience in sprinting, or elite athletes in other sports events actively engaged in an organized strength and conditioning program including professional running form and technique training. (How long does it take to be a high-level sprinter? There is currently no unified definition. However, according to some studies’ recruiting criteria for high-level sprinters [30,31] and some studies’ reporting [32,33], it was seen as appropriate to set more than 5 years’ training experience as the cut-off for high-level sprinters. For example, Maćkała et al. [30] defined sprinters with a minimum of 5 years of training experience as high-performance sprinters, and Čoh et al. [31] regarded sprinters with at least 6 years of training experience as high-level sprinters. In addition, Ericsson et al. [32] and McAuley et al. [33] revealed that in most sporting domains, the development of high performance took 5–30 years). The participants categorized as low-skilled were novices, performers with no formal sprint mechanics training, or performers involved in other sports with no experience in sprinting training.
2.5. Data Synthesis
Standardized mean differences (effect sizes) were estimated using Hedge’s g, as it takes small sample bias into account [34]. The sample size, mean, and standard deviation under EF and IF conditions were acquired from the included studies. For the study [19] providing standard errors instead of standard deviations, standard errors were converted to standard deviations. For the studies using a within-subjects design, the actual value of the pre-post correlation coefficient was estimated from the raw data and imputed to calculate the effect size. When the pre–post correlation was unavailable, the analysis used an imputed value of 0.5 [23].
2.6. Data Analysis
For the primary analysis, the effect of EF versus IF on sprint performance was investigated using a random-effects model meta-analysis, taking into account the heterogeneity within attentional focus instructions, sample characteristics, and outcome measures from the included studies. The effect size was calculated using the Hedges’ g with 95% CIs, the magnitude of which was assessed as small (0.2~0.5), moderate (0.5~0.8), or large (>0.8) [35].
I2 was used to measure study heterogeneity, which was classified as low (25%), moderate (50%), or high (75%) [36]. The τ2 statistics are additionally reported to facilitate the interpretation of between-study heterogeneity beyond the current list of included studies [37]. In order to determine the possible source of heterogeneity, a subgroup analysis was conducted using Cochran’s Q test and I2 [38,39,40]. In the present study, the subgroup analysis was performed according to the participants’ skill levels to see any difference in the attentional focus effect between low-skill and high-skill sprinters. In addition to reporting whether there is a statistically significant subgroup effect (the results of the Q test) and the extent of heterogeneity within each subgroup (the results of I2), it is helpful to point out whether the subgroup effect is “quantitative” or “qualitative” [38,41,42] (In “quantitative interaction,” the magnitude of the treatment effect fluctuates between subgroups, but the treatment effects in different subgroups are in the same direction, whereas in “qualitative interaction,” the treatment effect is favorable in one subgroup but is unfavorable or neutral for the other subgroup [41,43,44]) in order to provide useful and practical insights for practitioners into how the focus instructions should be implemented in practice [41,43], although caution is needed when interpreting these results [45].
Funnel plot inspection [46] and Egger’s regression test [47] were carried out to examine potential publication bias. In addition, sensitivity analysis was performed by sequentially removing each study to evaluate whether the pooled estimate was influenced excessively by a single study [48].
All the statistical analyses were performed using the Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (CMA) 3.0 software (Biostat Inc., Englewood, NJ, USA).
2.7. Risk of Bias
Through the independent assessments of two reviewers (D.L. and X.Y.), the risk of bias of each eligible study was evaluated by the updated Cochrane Risk of Bias Tool (RoB 2.0) [49]. The evaluation considered the “effect of assignment to an intervention.” Any disagreement during the assessments was resolved by consensus. The RoB 2.0 tool designed for randomized crossover trials was applied to the studies using a within-subjects design. The RoB 2.0 tool was organized around five domains: randomization and allocation process, deviations from the intended interventions, missing outcome data, measurement of the outcome, and selection of the reported result. Each domain featured signaling questions used to assess study bias and applicability. Finally, these domain-level assessments were used to construct an overall risk of bias rating for each study.
3. Results
3.1. Included Studies
Figure 1 depicts the PRISMA flow diagram, which shows the study selection process at various stages. The search yielded 78 records from five databases. Once duplicates were deleted (n = 27), the abstracts (n = 51) and full-text articles (n = 11) were screened. Following the final screening procedure, the final meta-analysis comprised six studies (10 comparisons).
3.2. Study Characteristics
Table 1 lists the main characteristics of the included studies. Publications in this collection were published between 2013 and 2018. The sample size range varied from 8 to 84, totaling 166 participants. The participants had various skill levels, from novice to elite. The tasks included a 6 m sprint, 10 m sprint, 20 yard (18.28 m) sprint, and 20 m sprint.
Table 1.
Study | Year | Skill Level | Sample Size | Age (Year) | Task | Verbal Instruction | Outcome | Overall Bias |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bezodis et al. [50] | 2017 | high-skill | 15 | 22 ± 4 | 10 m sprint | EF: “focus on clawing backwards at the ground with your shoe in every step you take” IF: “focus on pulling your leg backwards just before each contact with the ground” | total sprint time | some concern |
Ille et al. [18] | 2013 | high-skill | 8 | 20~30 | 10 m sprint | EF: “get off the starting blocks as quickly as possible, head towards the finish line rapidly, and cross it as soon as possible” IF: “push quickly on your legs and keep going as fast as possible while swinging both arms back and forth and raising rapidly your knees” | total sprint time; start reaction time | some concern |
low-skill | 8 | some concern | ||||||
Kovacs et al. [19] | 2018 | high-skill | 12 | 20.8 ± 1.7 | 6 m sprint | EF: “focus on pushing the blocks away” IF: “focus on extending your knees” | start reaction time | some concern |
Porter and Sims [51] | 2013 | high-skill | 9 | 21.11 ± 1.22 | 20-yard sprint | EF: “while you are running the 20-yard dash with maximum effort, focus on gradually raising up. Also, focus on powerfully driving forward while clawing the floor as quickly as possible” IF: “while you are running the 20-yard dash with maximum effort, focus on gradually raising your body level. Also, focus on powerfully driving one leg forward while moving your other leg and foot down and back as quickly as possible” | total sprint time | some concern |
Porter et al. [52] | 2015 | low-skill | 84 | 20.32 ± 1.73 | 20 m sprint | EF: “while you are running the 20-m dash, focus on driving forward as powerfully as possible while clawing the floor with your shoe as quickly as possible as you accelerate” IF: “while you are running the 20-m dash, focus on driving one leg forward as powerfully as possible while moving your other leg and foot down and back as quickly as possible as you accelerate” | total sprint time | some concern |
Winkelman et al. [53] | 2017 Exp 1 | low-skill | 17 | 19.41 ± 1.06 | 10 m sprint | EF: “focus on driving the ground back as explosively as you can” IF: “focus on driving your legs back as explosively as you can” | total sprint time | some concern |
2017 Exp 2 | high-skill | 13 | 28 ± 4.32 | 10 m sprint | total sprint time | some concern |
3.3. Meta-Analytic Results
Figure 2 displays the individual and aggregate effect sizes for the effect of attentional focus on sprint performance. Overall, compared to IF, EF had a small positive impact on sprint performance (g = 0.279, 95% CI [0.088, 0.470], p = 0.004, τ2 = 0.025), demonstrating that on average, individuals who followed EF verbal instructions fared better than those who used IF. The magnitude of heterogeneity was demonstrated by an I2 value of 28.478%, which explained a moderate proportion of the between-study variance.
3.4. Moderator Analysis Results
Subgroup analyses were conducted based on participants’ skill levels, whether low or high (see Figure 3). The test for subgroup differences indicated that there was no statistically significant subgroup effect (Q = 0.181, df = 1, p = 0.670), suggesting that skill level did not moderate the effect of EF in comparison to IF.
Although no significant difference was observed between these groups, the subgroup effect was found to be “qualitative interaction” [41,44,54]. That is, the benefits of the EF strategy were significant in the subgroup of low-skill participants (g = 0.337, 95% CI [0.032, 0.642], p = 0.030, I2 = 18.527), indicating that EF instruction significantly improved their sprint performance, but the effect was not significant among high-skill participants (g = 0.246, 95% CI [−0.042, 0.533], p = 0.094, I2 = 43.223), indicating that EF did not significantly improve their sprint performance. However, caution should be exercised when considering these results (see Discussion).
3.5. Publication Bias and Sensitivity Analysis
A visual inspection of the funnel plot (see Figure 4) showed that the distribution of the included studies was slightly asymmetrical. However, Egger’s regression test (p = 0.222) showed no statistically significant publication bias. In addition, the results of the sensitivity analysis revealed that the pooled estimate of the attentional focus effect on sprint performance ranged from g = 0.227 (95% CI [0.052, 0.402]) to g = 0.317 (95% CI [0.118, 0.516]) after one study was removed at a time, indicating that no single study considerably changed the overall estimate.
3.6. Risk of Bias
Table 1 shows the results of the risk of bias assessments. All eligible studies were scored as causing some concerns. The most dominant problem was the randomization process, which caused some concern in all studies, partly because of the inadequate reporting of allocation concealment.
4. Discussion
The present study aimed to use a meta-analytic statistical technique to assess the effect of an EF on sprint performance. The findings from the main analysis demonstrated that employing EF had a small positive effect on sprint performance compared to IF (g = 0.279, p = 0.004) but also revealed moderate heterogeneity in the data (I2 = 28.478%). Additionally, the subgroup analysis suggested that the benefits of the EF strategy were significant among low-skill sprinters (g = 0.337, p = 0.030) but not significant among high-skill sprinters (g = 0.246, p = 0.094), although no significant difference was seen between these subgroups (p = 0.670). Together, these findings show that EF outperforms IF in sprint performance.
4.1. Why Is EF Better for Sprint Performance Than IF (i.e., Attentional Focus Effect)?
The constrained action hypothesis [55,56] can explain this phenomenon. That is, when attention is focused externally, it permits the motor control system to work under nonconscious automatic processes, resulting in reflexive movement and improved performance. Conversely, as attention is focused internally, the motor control system is constrained by consciously regulated processes, resulting in less reflexive and reduced fluent movement patterns. Consequently, IF generated worse outcomes than EF. Over the past two decades or more, myriad studies have lent support to this hypothesis and have revealed the generalizability of EF’s benefits, as shown over a broad range of motor tasks (e.g., balance, dart throwing, soccer, ballet, golf, running, swimming, and jumping), populations (at various skill levels, ages, and disabilities), and performance measures (movement accuracy, movement form and movement efficiency); for reviews, see [20,21,22].
In addition, Wulf and Lewthwaite [57,58] recently proposed the goal-action coupling mechanism in the OPTIMAL theory, illustrating the neuromuscular change under EF circumstances, which could provide another explanation for the benefits of EF. That is, like the other two factors (enhanced expectancies and autonomy support), EF improves neuromuscular efficiency [59,60,61]. The enhanced neuromuscular efficiency, to a certain extent, indicates the establishment of smooth, sizable, and opportune functional connectivity between task-relevant motor networks with less effort, and the disconnection of unnecessary linkages relatively easily [57]. Furthermore, an EF, which is helpful in suppressing needless neural activity [62] and muscular co-contractions [59], contributes to clarifying neuromuscular coordination. Sprinting, as a complex motor skill, requires the contraction of several muscle groups at accurate times and intensities during the stride cycle for the purpose of achieving optimal sprint performance [16]. Therefore, through more efficient muscle activation at the neuromuscular level, an EF has the possibility to boost sprint performance.
4.2. How Should We Interpret the Subgroup Analysis of the Attentional Focus Effect for High-Skill Sprinters and Low-Skill Sprinters?
First, the subgroup analysis shows that skill level does not play a moderating role in the attentional focus effect. However, the uneven covariate distribution (six effect sizes in the high-skill group vs. four effect sizes in the low-skill group) suggests that the subgroup analysis may be incapable of yielding accurate results [42], and a smaller number of participants contributed data to the high-skill subgroup (sample sizes of 57) than to the low-skill subgroup (sample sizes of 109) means that the analysis may be unable to identify subgroup differences [42]. It would be advisable for more research to be undertaken in the research field to examine the subgroup effect.
Second, although no significant difference was seen between the low- and high-skill subgroups, a qualitative interaction was found between the attentional focus effect and the skill level. Although some researchers suggested that qualitative interaction of that kind should be considered “with skepticism” [45], other researchers deemed that the qualitative interaction may be more frequent [41] and more important [43] in terms of public health and clinical implications because the pathways between intervention and outcome are frequently intricate, and the treatments may have varying effects on various populations through different pathways, and consequently, these results could help policymakers or practitioners develop effective and refined intervention programs. In order to provide practical implications for sports practitioners, the current study has performed further analyses of the qualitative interaction, although caution is needed when interpreting these results.
That is, on the one hand, the benefits in favor of the EF strategy were significant in the subgroup of low-skill participants. However, due to the number of effect sizes included in the analysis being small (i.e., four effect sizes in the low-skill group), fewer than the five recommended by Jackson and Turner [63], the analysis does not have enough statistical power to conclude that EF intervention has a powerful effect on low-skill performers. Careful investigation via more randomized controlled trials may provide stronger evidence, which would be useful to suggest the direction of future research.
On the other hand, the benefits of EF instruction were non-significant in the subgroup of high-skill participants. There are three possible explanations for this observation. First, the compensation account (“who has will less be given”), which is used to explain the improvements in cognition from cognitive training, primarily in working memory and attention control processes (e.g., [64]), could shed light on the aforementioned result. That is, the highly skilled individuals already perform close to the ceiling and cannot improve as much. Therefore, baseline performance might be negatively related to intervention gains. Another possible explanation for this is, as Wulf [65] put forward, that for attentional focus effects to occur, a certain level of task challenge appears to be needed. If the task has already been automated, there will be a lower possibility for the attentional focus effect to emerge. Obviously, it appears that the sprint task is not difficult enough for the highly skilled participants because the sprint task is already handled with a high degree of automaticity due to previous practice and experience; thus, directing attention externally has no further benefit. Moreover, an alternative explanation, mentioned in the study of Winkelman et al. [53], is also plausible—namely, the way that abstracting information or meaning from attentional focus instructions is influenced by the level of experience. Abstracting information or meaning involves the ability to overlook unnecessary elements while focusing on the critical components and constructing generalized thoughts based on previous experience and knowledge [66]. According to one study [67], individuals with a lot of experience usually extract and summarize the general subjective implication from given instructions. As a consequence, highly skilled performers may frequently return to their natural focus, which has become consolidated and automated after years of practice, and self-select the appropriate goal-relevant aspect to focus on irrespective of the instructions presented. Therefore, the difference between EF and IF instructions may be canceled out among the high-skill performers. However, more randomized controlled trials are needed to investigate the true effect. In addition, the distance effect of EF in the facilitation of motor performance and learning has been found in several studies [68,69]. That is, increasing the distance of an EF from the body movement improves sports performance and motor learning, and the distance effect seems to be more effective in high-skill performers [70]. Future studies may take the distance of EF into consideration to improve high-skill sprinters’ performance.
Finally, as Higgins et al. [38] emphasized, it must be remembered that subgroup analyses are essentially observational and are not performed on the basis of randomized comparisons. Hence, subgroup analyses have the limitations inherent in every observational study, such as potential bias due to confounding with other study-level factors. Therefore, caution is needed when interpreting findings from subgroup analyses [40].
4.3. Applied Practice and Future Directions
This study highlights that sprinting coaches and athletes should be aware of the advantage of EF and induce or adopt EF during practice and competitions to boost sprint performance in competitive venues. As shown in this study, making minimal changes in verbal instructions can result in considerable performance improvement. Yet, it is worth noting that what seems to be missing from focus instruction literature is the examination of the effect of attentional focus instructions in competitive settings. Although Martin et al. [71] pointed out the complexity of competitions and the difficulty of controlling limited sport psychological interventions in competitions, well-designed research investigating the attentional focus effect on sprint competitions will further advance the application of attentional focus. Furthermore, based on the study of Winkelman [17], there are three characteristics of attentional focus cues (i.e., distance, direction, and description) that moderate the positive influence of an EF. Therefore, coaches employing attentional focus intervention are encouraged to take the distance, direction, and description of an EF into consideration and provide athletes with individually tailored EF instructions. In addition, it would be useful to examine any potential changes in the intervention’s efficacy based on the type of sports event (e.g., individual sport versus team sport), and the type of motor action. Most importantly, according to Porter et al. [24], there is some inconsistency between science and application in giving verbal instructions to athletes in track and field sports. Sport scientists need to bridge this gap by reaching out to coaches and athletes, giving an ear to their practical concerns, and conveying scientific methods to optimize sports performance.
4.4. Limitations
When evaluating the study’s findings, the following limitations should be kept in mind. First, the limited number of included studies imposed restrictions on assessing the effect of attentional focus in sprinting. Furthermore, most of the studies included had a small sample size, which could result in overestimating the attentional focus effect in relation to studies with larger sample sizes. Therefore, when examining and reproducing the efficacy of attentional focus interventions in sprint performance, larger sample sizes should be used to increase statistical power. In addition, the inadequate descriptions of participants’ skill levels in the included studies made it difficult to rate the actual skill levels of participants; therefore, future studies should provide clarified descriptions of participants’ expertise.
5. Conclusions
In summary, the present meta-analysis corroborates that EF could improve sprint performance compared to IF. Furthermore, a qualitative interaction has been found between the attentional focus effect and skill level, although the subgroup analysis showed that skill level does not play a moderating role. Specifically, the attentional focus effects are significant in low-skill sprint performers but non-significant in high-skill sprint performers. However, caution is needed when interpreting the subgroup analysis. Finally, coaches and athletes themselves should be aware of the strength of EF and design EF strategies in accordance with their skill development to improve sprinting, as a 1% improvement can increase the chances of success and boost medal positions in international competitions, just like it did for Lyles, who beat Coleman by a scant 0.006 s (9.852 to 9.858) at the finish line in the 2019 Diamond League Shanghai 100 m.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, D.L.; methodology, D.L. and X.Y.; software, D.L. and X.Y.; formal analysis, D.L. and X.Y.; writing—Original draft preparation, D.L.; writing—Review and editing, L.Z., D.M. and Y.Z.; visualization, D.L.; supervision, L.Z. and D.M.; funding acquisition, L.Z. and D.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Institutional Review Board Statement
Not applicable.
Informed Consent Statement
Not applicable.
Data Availability Statement
The data used for the meta-analysis are available from the corresponding author upon request.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Funding Statement
This research was funded by the National Key Technology R&D Program of China, grant number 2018YFF0300902; the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities, grant number 20211023; and the China Scholarship Council, grant number 202106520011.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Haug W.B., Drinkwater E.J., Cicero N.J., Barthell J.A., Chapman D.W. The impact of dry-land sprint start training on the short track speed skating start. J. Strength Cond. Res. 2019;33:544–548. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0000000000001892. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Maciejczyk M., Blyszczuk R., Drwal A., Nowak B., Strzala M. Effects of short-term plyometric training on agility, jump and repeated sprint performance in female soccer players. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2021;18:2274. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18052274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Feser E.H., Korfist C., Lindley K., Bezodis N.E., Clark K., Cronin J. The effects of lower-limb wearable resistance on sprint performance in high school American football athletes: A nine-week training study. Int. J. Sport Sci. Coach. 2021;16:1187–1195. doi: 10.1177/17479541211003403. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Rønnestad B.R., Slettaløkken G., Ellefsen S. Adding whole body vibration to preconditioning exercise increases subsequent on-ice sprint performance in ice-hockey players. J. Strength Cond. Res. 2016;30:1021–1026. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0b013e3182a9535a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hoffman J. NSCA’s Guide to Program Design. Human Kinetics; Champaign, IL, USA: 2011. pp. 168–170. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Harland M.J., Steele J.R. Biomechanics of the sprint start. Sports Med. 1997;23:11–20. doi: 10.2165/00007256-199723010-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Majumdar A.S., Robergs R.A. The science of speed: Determinants of performance in the 100 m sprint. Int. J. Sport Sci. Coach. 2011;6:479–493. doi: 10.1260/1747-9541.6.3.479. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Gürses V.V., Kamiş O. The relationship between reaction time and 60 m performance in elite athletes. J. Educ. Train. Stud. 2019;6:64–69. doi: 10.11114/jets.v6i12a.3931. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Collet C. Strategic aspects of reaction time in world-class sprinters. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1999;88:65–75. doi: 10.2466/pms.1999.88.1.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Henson P., Cooper J., Perry T. A wider look at the sprint start. Track Field Coach. Rev. 2002;75:19–21. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Michel S., Jarver J. The start is (almost) everything in sprint performance. Track Coach. 2002;160:5121. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Rimmer E., Sleivert G. Effects of a plyometrics intervention program on sprint performance. J. Strength Cond. Res. 2000;14:295–301. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Cahill M.J., Oliver J.L., Cronin J.B., Clark K.P., Cross M.R., Lloyd R.S. Influence of resisted sled-push training on the sprint force-velocity profile of male high school athletes. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports. 2020;30:442–449. doi: 10.1111/sms.13600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Cottle C.A., Carlson L.A., Lawrence M.A. Effects of sled towing on sprint starts. J. Strength Cond. Res. 2014;28:1241–1245. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0000000000000396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Feser E.H., Bayne H., Loubser I., Bezodis N.E., Cronin J.B. Wearable resistance sprint running is superior to training with no load for retaining performance in pre-season training for rugby athletes. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 2021;21:967–975. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2020.1802516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Benz A., Winkelman N., Porter J., Nimphius S. Coaching instructions and cues for enhancing sprint performance. Strength Cond. J. 2016;38:1–11. doi: 10.1519/SSC.0000000000000185. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Winkelman N.C. Attentional focus and cueing for speed development. Strength Cond. J. 2018;40:13–25. doi: 10.1519/SSC.0000000000000266. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ille A., Selin I., Do M.-C., Thon B. Attentional focus effects on sprint start performance as a function of skill level. J. Sports Sci. 2013;31:1705–1712. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2013.797097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Kovacs A.J., Miles G.F., Baweja H.S. Thinking outside the block: External focus of attention improves reaction times and movement preparation times in collegiate track sprinters. Sports. 2018;6:120. doi: 10.3390/sports6040120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Wulf G. Attentional focus and motor learning: A review of 15 years. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2013;6:77–104. doi: 10.1080/1750984X.2012.723728. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Wulf G., Lewthwaite R. Optimizing attentional focus. In: Tenenbaum G., Eklund R.C., editors. Handbook of Sport Psychology. 4th ed. Wiley; Hoboken, NJ, USA: 2020. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Wulf G., Prinz W. Directing attention to movement effects enhances learning: A review. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2001;8:648–660. doi: 10.3758/BF03196201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Chua L.K., Jimenez-Diaz J., Lewthwaite R., Kim T., Wulf G. Superiority of external attentional focus for motor performance and learning: Systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Psychol. Bull. 2021;147:618–645. doi: 10.1037/bul0000335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Porter J.M., Wu W.F.W., Partridge J.A. Focus of attention and verbal instructions: Strategies of elite track and field coaches and athletes. Sport Sci. Rev. 2010;19:77–89. doi: 10.2478/v10237-011-0018-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Elliott J.H., Turner T., Clavisi O., Thomas J., Higgins J.P., Mavergames C., Gruen R.L. Living systematic reviews: An emerging opportunity to narrow the evidence-practice gap. PLoS Med. 2014;11:e1001603. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Gurevitch J., Koricheva J., Nakagawa S., Stewart G. Meta-analysis and the science of research synthesis. Nature. 2018;555:175–182. doi: 10.1038/nature25753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Anderson J.R. Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychol. Rev. 1982;89:369–406. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.89.4.369. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Schneider W., Shiffrin R.M. Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychol. Rev. 1977;84:1–66. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Devereaux P.J., Schünemann H.J., Ravindran N., Bhandari M., Garg A.X., Choi P.T.L., Grant B.J.B., Haines T., Lacchetti C., Weaver B., et al. Comparison of mortality between private for-profit and private not-for-profit hemodialysis centers: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA. 2022;288:2449–2457. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.19.2449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Maćkała K., Fostiak M., Kowalski K. Selected determinants of acceleration in the 100m sprint. J. Hum. Kinet. 2015;45:135–148. doi: 10.1515/hukin-2015-0014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Čoh M., Peharec S., Bacic P., Mackala K. Biomechanical differences in the sprint start between faster and slower high-level sprinters. J. Hum. Kinet. 2017;56:29–38. doi: 10.1515/hukin-2017-0020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ericsson K.A., Harwell K.W. Deliberate practice and proposed limits on the effects of practice on the acquisition of expert performance: Why the original definition matters and recommendations for future research. Front. Psychol. 2019;10:2396. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.McAuley A.B.T., Baker J., Kelly A.L. Defining “elite” status in sport: From chaos to clarity. Ger. J. Exerc. Sport Res. 2021;52:193–197. doi: 10.1007/s12662-021-00737-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ferguson C.J. An effect size primer: A guide for clinicians and researchers. Prof. Psychol. Res. Pract. 2009;40:532–538. doi: 10.1037/a0015808. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd ed. Lawrence Earlbaum Associates; Hillsdale, NJ, USA: 1988. pp. 24–27. Chapter 2. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Higgins J.P., Thompson S.G., Deeks J.J., Altman D.G. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. 2003;327:557–560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Rücker G., Schwarzer G., Carpenter J.R., Schumacher M. Undue reliance on I2 in assessing heterogeneity may mislead. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2008;8:79–87. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-8-79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Higgins J.P.T., Thomas J., Chandler J., Cumpston M., Li T.J., Page M.J., Welch V.A. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 6.3. Cochrane; London, UK: 2022. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Sedgwick P. Meta-analyses: Heterogeneity and subgroup analysis. BMJ. 2013;346:f4040. doi: 10.1136/bmj.f4040. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Sedgwick P. Meta-analyses: What is heterogeneity? BMJ. 2015;350:h1435. doi: 10.1136/bmj.h1435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Petticrew M., Tugwell P., Kristjansson E., Oliver S., Ueffing E., Welch V. Damned if you do, damned if you don’t: Subgroup analysis and equity. J. Epidemiol. Community Health. 2012;66:95–98. doi: 10.1136/jech.2010.121095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Richardson M., Garner P., Donegan S. Interpretation of subgroup analyses in systematic reviews: A tutorial. Clin. Epidemiol. Glob. Health. 2019;7:192–198. doi: 10.1016/j.cegh.2018.05.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Lagakos S.W. The challenge of subgroup analyses—Reporting without distorting. N. Engl. J. Med. 2006;354:1667–1669. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp068070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Wang X., Piantadosi S., Le-Rademacher J., Mandrekar S.J. Statistical Considerations for Subgroup Analyses. J. Thorac. Oncol. 2021;16:375–380. doi: 10.1016/j.jtho.2020.12.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Yusuf S., Wittes J., Probstfield J., Tyroler H.A. Analysis and interpretation of treatment effects in subgroups of patients in randomized clinical trials. JAMA. 1991;266:93–98. doi: 10.1001/jama.1991.03470010097038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Sedgwick P., Marston L. How to read a funnel plot in a meta-analysis. BMJ. 2015;351:h4718. doi: 10.1136/bmj.h4718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Egger M., Smith G.D., Schneider M., Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. 1997;315:629–634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Tobias A. Assessing the influence of a single study in the meta-analysis estimate. Stata Tech. Bull. 1999;47:15–17. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Higgins J.P., Savović J., Page M.J., Elbers R.G., Sterne J.A.C. Chapter 8: Assessing risk of bias in a randomized trial. In: Higgins J.P.T., Thomas J., Chandler J., Cumpston M., Li T.J., Page M.J., Welch V.A., editors. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. 2nd ed. Wiley-Blackwell; Hoboken, NJ, USA: 2019. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Bezodis N.E., North J.S., Razavet J.L. Alterations to the orientation of the ground reaction force vector affect sprint acceleration performance in team sports athletes. J. Sports Sci. 2017;35:1817–1824. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2016.1239024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Porter J.M., Sims B. Altering focus of attention influences elite athletes sprinting performance. Int. J. Coach. Sci. 2013;7:41–51. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Porter J.M., Wu W.F.W., Crossley R.M., Knopp S.W., Campbell O.C. Adopting an external focus of attention improves sprinting performance in low-skilled sprinters. J. Strength Cond. Res. 2015;29:947–953. doi: 10.1097/JSC.0000000000000229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Winkelman N.C., Clark K.P., Ryan L.J. Experience level influences the effect of attentional focus on sprint performance. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2017;52:84–95. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2017.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Brookes S.T., Whitely E., Egger M., Smith G.D., Mulheran P.A., Peters T.J. Subgroup analyses in randomized trials: Risks of subgroup-specific analyses; power and sample size for the interaction test. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2004;57:229–236. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2003.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Wulf G., McNevin N., Shea C.H. The automaticity of complex motor skill learning as a function of attentional focus. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2001;54:1143–1154. doi: 10.1080/713756012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Wulf G., Shea C., Park J.H. Attention and motor performance: Preferences for and advantages of an external focus. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport. 2001;72:335–344. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2001.10608970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Wulf G., Lewthwaite R. Translating thoughts into action: Optimizing motor performance and learning through brief motivational and attentional influences. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2021;30:535–541. doi: 10.1177/09637214211046199. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Wulf G., Lewthwaite R. Optimizing performance through intrinsic motivation and attention for learning: The Optimal theory of motor learning. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2016;23:1382–1414. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0999-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Lohse K.R., Sherwood D.E., Healy A.F. Neuromuscular effects of shifting the focus of attention in a simple force production task. J. Mot. Behav. 2011;43:173–184. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2011.555436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Zachry T., Wulf G., Mercer J., Bezodis N. Increased movement accuracy and reduced EMG activity as the result of adopting an external focus of attention. Brain Res. Bull. 2005;67:304–309. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.06.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Wulf G., Dufek J.S., Lozano L., Pettigrew C. Increased jump height and reduced EMG activity with an external focus. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2010;29:440–448. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2009.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Kuhn Y.A., Keller M., Lauber B., Taube W. Surround inhibition can instantly be modulated by changing the attentional focus. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:1085. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-19077-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Jackson D., Turner R. Power analysis for random-effects meta-analysis. Res. Synth. Methods. 2017;8:290–302. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.1240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Karbach J., Unger K. Executive control training from middle childhood to adolescence. Front. Psychol. 2014;5:390. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Wulf G. Attention and Motor Skill Learning. Human Kinetics; Champaign, IL, USA: 2007. pp. 137–154. [Google Scholar]
- 66.Vas A.K., Spence J., Chapman S.B. Abstracting meaning from complex information (gist reasoning) in adult traumatic brain injury. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2015;37:152–161. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2014.994478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Corbin J.C., Reyna V.F., Weldon R.B., Brainerd C.J. How reasoning, judgment, and decision making are colored by gist-based intuition: A fuzzy-trace theory approach. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 2015;4:344–355. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2015.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Bell J.J., Hardy J. Effects of Attentional Focus on Skilled Performance in Golf. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 2009;21:163–177. doi: 10.1080/10413200902795323. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.McNevin N.H., Shea C.H., Wulf G. Increasing the distance of an external focus of attention enhances learning. Psychol. Res. 2003;67:22–29. doi: 10.1007/s00426-002-0093-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Singh H., Wulf G. The distance effect and level of expertise: Is the optimal external focus different for low-skilled and high-skilled performers? Hum. Mov. Sci. 2020;73:102663. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2020.102663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Martin G.L., Vause T., Schwartzman L. Experimental studies of psychological interventions with athletes in competitions: Why so few? Behav. Modif. 2005;29:616–641. doi: 10.1177/0145445503259394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The data used for the meta-analysis are available from the corresponding author upon request.