Abstract
The current study investigated how parents’ and teachers’ educational expectations both directly and indirectly shaped young people’s academic outcomes in a nationally-representative sample of high school students (Education Longitudinal Study; N = 9,654 adolescents). Higher parent and math teacher expectations in 10th grade were associated with better 12th grade math scores and higher GPAs, math course-taking sequence, and educational attainment two years post-high school. High parent expectations generally magnified the particularly strong positive effects of high math teacher expectations, and there was some evidence of variation in links between adult expectations and outcomes by both student race/ethnicity and SES. Parents’ educational involvement at school, teacher-student relationships, and school-parent communication mediated the links between adult educational expectations and academic outcomes.
Keywords: teacher educational expectations, parent educational expectations, parental involvement, teacher-student relationships, academic achievement
The U.S. has long struggled with racial/ethnic and socioeconomic achievement gaps that are observed as early as preschool and are stubbornly persistent across elementary and secondary school and into postsecondary education (National Center for Education Statistics, 2020; Reardon & Portilla, 2016). Lower educational attainment, in turn, is related to a host of negative outcomes across the life course, including higher rates of poverty and incarceration (Institute of Medicine 2014), and these persistent educational achievement gaps contribute to the intergenerational transmission of socioeconomic disadvantage (Bloomeet al., 2018). Understanding the processes that chip away at these disparities in achievement are of critical importance to educational practice. The current study investigates how a set of interpersonal processes among students, their parents, and their math teachers influence young people’s educational success in high school and beyond (see conceptual model in Figure 1) and whether certain interpersonal processes tied to education are particularly important in promoting the academic success of low-income youth and youth of color.
How well young people do academically is strongly influenced by the adults with whom and the environments in which they spend most of their time (Rumberger and Lim 2008). Most parents have educational goals for their children, and teachers expect certain academic futures based on students’ performance and interactions. Those expectations are strongly linked to later educational success (Benneret al., 2016; Zhan & Sherraden, 2011), yet parent and teacher expectations do not always align, resulting in dissonant or incongruent expectations (Benner & Mistry, 2007). Furthermore, although adults may directly communicate their educational expectations for young people, it is also quite likely that these expectations influence the interactions they have that can subsequently either promote or inhibit achievement.
Informed by the life course perspective and bioecological theory, we argue that adults in two key contexts of youth’s lives―families and schools―have independent and conjoint influences on educational competencies and that these influences are both direct and indirect via proximal processes that unfold within these contexts. This study thus highlights expectations held by parents and math teachers (see Rubie-Davies et al., 2015) and interactions within these contexts as points of entree for policies aimed at improving academic progress and achievement.
Adult Educational Expectations and Youth’s Educational Outcomes
The expectations held by adults for youth’s educational futures have strong implications for short- and long-term academic progress and achievement. Low teacher expectations are linked to poorer grades, lower achievement test scores, and a decreased likelihood of high school completion (Friedrich et al., 2015; Hinnant,et al., 2009; Schoonet al., 2004). These negative effects of teacher expectations and bias—that is, teachers holding lower expectations than would be expected given a student’s prior achievement levels—are persistent, having impacts both within and across school years (De Boeret al., 2010; Jamil et al., 2018; Sorhagen, 2013). Conversely, parents’ high expectations are positively associated with youth’s academic achievement, college attendance and completion, grades, and educational attainment both within and across time (Benner et al. 2016; Pinquart & Ebeling, 2020; Zhan & Sherraden, 2011).
While evidence suggests that both teacher and parent educational expectations are influential for students’ educational outcomes, less is known about how adult expectations come together to influence young people’s educational success—that is, the interactional effects. Two studies have attempted to unpack the effects of congruent versus dissonant adult expectations, revealing that mothers’ high expectations seem to buffer some of the negative effects of low teachers’ expectations (Benner & Mistry, 2007; Hauser-Cram et al., 2003). Each, however, had limited samples (kindergarten students in Hauser-Cram et al., 2003; low-income urban students in Benner & Mistry, 2007), limiting generalizability. The current study aims to expand on this prior work by examining the interactional effects of teacher and parent expectations on children’s educational outcomes using a large, nationally representative sample of U.S. high school students. This focal population is understudied in the existing scholarship on congruence of adult expectations, yet it is critically important as SES and race/ethnic differences in who persists in high school and beyond become readily apparent at this juncture.
The life course perspective orients our examination of parents’ and teachers’ expectations, with the linked lives principle positing that individuals’ experiences of life events are shaped by the important others in their social networks (Elder, 1998), consistent with bioecological theory’s attention to microsystems, or the everyday contexts of individuals’ lives (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006). More broadly, life course theory identifies the importance of interactions between micro- and macro-level contexts that set youth on developmental, social, and institutional trajectories (Crosnoe & Johnson, 2011). This connection suggests the micro-level contexts of families and schools are interwoven, such that the expectations of adults in those contexts come together in ways that could promote or deter educational success. In the current study, we examined the extent to which parent and math teacher expectations independently and conjointly influenced high school students’ educational outcomes. Our focus on mathematics teachers and mathematics-related outcomes was purposeful, as mathematics is a key marker and gatekeeper for higher academic tracks in the short-term and college readiness and success in the longer term (Frank et al., 2008), yet evidence consistently documents that U.S. students lag behind those in many OECD countries on international mathematics assessments such at the PISA (Schleicher, 2019).
Mechanisms Linking Adult Expectations and Youth’s Educational Outcomes
Although there is mounting evidence that parent and teacher educational expectations matter for young people’s academic success, the mechanisms by which this occurs are less clear. Certainly, important adults in students’ lives may directly communicate their expectations, but it is likely that these expectations actually inform the interactions between young people and their environments, and it is the quality of these interactions that may be at least, in part, driving expectancy effects (Seginer, 1983; Jussim & Harber, 2005). Bioecological theory highlights the centrality of interactions within proximal contexts (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006), suggesting that the interpersonal processes that unfold between young people and the important others in their lives have great repercussions for subsequent growth and development. During adolescence, two central proximal contexts are families and schools. In the current study, we were interested in how interactions in these contexts might be both the product of adults’ educational expectations and a potential driver of expectancy effects.
In their review of the parent expectations literature, Yamamoto and Holloway (2010) contend that interactional processes between children and their parents—particularly parents’ educational involvement—mediate the relation between parental expectations and youth’s academic success. Parents’ involvement in school includes activities such as volunteering in the classroom or school and participating in formal parent-teacher conferences and parent-teacher organizations (Hill & Tyson, 2009). Research with elementary school populations has found that parents’ school-based involvement partially mediates the link between parents’ educational expectations and children’s academic achievement (Briley et al., 2014; Englund et al., 2004). Similarly, educational involvement at home—including provision of educational materials or homework assistance—may also be a mechanism by which educational expectations exert their influence. For example, higher parental educational expectations have been associated with greater literacy support at home, and this home-based involvement, in turn, is linked to greater achievement for middle school students (Davis-Kean, 2005). In contrast, assistance with homework and home-school communication appear to be strategies parents employ when their children are struggling (Hoglund et al., 2015). A focus of the current study is whether parents’ educational involvement at home and at school exerts a similar influence when adolescents are in high school and is exacted in response to academic concerns or instead high expectations.
It is also likely that the expectations that adults hold may influence the interactions and supports that they provide to young people. Much of the teacher expectancy literature suggests that high teacher expectations are likely manifested through various processes, such as the classroom climate they create and the feedback they provide to students (see Murdock-Perriera and Sedlacek 2018 for a literature review). In turn, research shows that teacher-student relationships influence the academic success of young people (Roorda et al. 2017). As such, these positive interactions represent another potential mechanism through which expectations may be linked to academic achievement.
Finally, both the life course perspective and bioecological theory suggests that human agency and self-system processes also matter for the effects that linked lives can exert on individuals’ growth and development (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006; Elder, 1998). Yamamoto and Holloway’s (2010) theoretical model linking parental expectations to students’ academic outcomes suggests that students’ academic efficacy is an important mediator of this relation. Indeed, extant research has documented that the relation between adult expectations and youth achievement is mediated, at least in part, by young people’s academic self-concept (Friedrich et al., 2015; Loughlin-Presnal & Bierman, 2017; Pinquart & Ebeling, 2020). As such, in addition to the potential mediating proximal processes under study, we also investigated whether students’ academic self-concept was an intermediary mechanism linking adult educational expectations, interactions within linked lives, and young people’s academic outcomes.
Educational Expectations, Linked Lives, Academic Outcomes, and Inequality
The life course perspective posits that individuals create their lives within broader structural opportunities and constraints (Elder, 1998). Thus, although youth exert agency over their educational trajectories, their experiences of adult expectations and interactions with adults at the micro-level of school and family are further shaped by macro-level structures and the social inequalities therein. Low-income and racial/ethnic minority students are often faced with macro-level social inequalities, such as stigma and discrimination, that may limit their cultural capital, and thus they may be differentially impacted by adult expectations. Additionally, cultural mobility theory (DiMaggio, 1982) suggests cultural capital is especially important for youth from disadvantaged backgrounds; because marginalized youth are more likely to lack financial resources and social connections, cultural capital represents a central means of achieving upward mobility through education. Yet access to such capital is limited for marginalized youth who are often educated in schools dominated by White middle class educational norms that are silent to culturally relevant pedagogy (Chambers & Huggins, 2014). Taken together, these theoretical models suggest that adults’ educational expectations and youth’s academic outcomes—and the direct and indirect mechanisms linking them—should be conditioned by markers of structural and social inequalities, including socioeconomic status and race/ethnicity.
Initial research documents this theoretically-informed variation. On average, teachers have poorer perceptions and expectations of low-income students as well as students who are racial and ethnic minorities (Cherng, 2017; Glock, 2016; Krolak-Schwerdt et al., 2013; Ready & Wright, 2011), which teachers communicate to students through direct converstaions and more subtle instructional choices (Chambers & Huggins, 2014; Ford & Moore, 2013). Furthermore, research suggests that students who are most at risk educationally (racial/ethnic minority students, low-income students) can be particularly susceptible to the negative effects of low teacher expectations and teacher expectancy biases (Cherng, 2017; Jamil et al., 2018; Sorhagen 2013), and the positive association between teacher-student relationships and student academic achievement tends to be stronger for both racial/ethnic minorities and low-income students (Roorda et al., 2011). Evidence of demographic variation in parents’ educational expectations is more mixed. African American and Hispanic parents are shown to hold higher educational expectations for their children compared White parents, while Asian parents’ expectations did not significantly differ from White parents (Lawrence, 2015); however, the effect of parent expectations on youth’s academic outcomes tends to be weaker among racial/ethnic minority families (Pinquart & Ebeling, 2020). In contrast, various indicators of family socioeconomic status are positively associated with youth’s academic success and parents’ educational expectations (Carolan & Wasserman, 2015; Davis-Kean, 2005; Zhan & Sherraden, 2011), and evidence suggest that the links between parents’ expectations and both achievement test scores and educational attainment may be stronger among children from higher SES families (Benner et al. 2016; Lee and Bowen 2006). In the current study, we examine whether the relations under study differed by student SES and race/ethnicity.
Current Study
The current study used data from a large, nationally-representative dataset—the Educational Longitudinal Study of 2002—to examine the processes by which adult expectations influenced young people’s educational success (see conceptual model in Figure 1). The first research question examined the extent to which the congruence or dissonance of parents’ and teachers’ educational expectations was associated with students’ academic outcomes (i.e., math course-taking sequence, math achievement test scores, grade point average, educational attainment). Based on previous empirical research on parent and teacher expectations and youth’s academic performance, we hypothesized congruently high adult expectations would be most beneficial for youth’s academic outcomes, whereas congruently low adult expectations would be particularly harmful. Furthermore, given the unique role that within-context interactions play for development (Bronfenbrenner, 1979), we hypothesized youth with dissonant high teacher but low parent expectations would have better academic outcomes than youth with dissonant high parent but low teacher expectations because teachers drive both how education plays out in the classroom and how students are evaluated.
For research question two, we examined the mechanisms by which parent and math teacher educational expectations influenced later educational success. Here, we focused on linked lives within the family and school contexts that best reflected the pathways by which expectations are communicated and acted upon. Consistent with the theoretical model proposed by Yamamoto and Holloway (2010), we hypothesized that parent and teacher expectations would be related to processes within families (i.e., parents’ home-based educational involvement, parent-youth relationships) and within schools (i.e., teacher-student relationships), as well as the connections between these contexts (i.e., parents’ school-based involvement, parent-teacher communication). In turn, we expected these interactional aspects of linked lives to influence young people’s subsequent academic outcomes. Furthermore, taking into account the role of youth’s personal agency as suggested by the life course perspective and consistent with prior research (Benner & Mistry, 2007), we hypothesized that adult expectations and proximal processes within the home and school would be related to young people’s academic self-concept, which in turn was expected to be associated with their subsequent academic outcomes.
Our third and final research question examined potential variation in the relations between adult expectations, linked lives, and academic outcomes by markers of social disadvantage. Based on the empirical research presented above and following a mobility model of cultural capital (DiMaggio, 1982), we expected adults’ educational expectations and the interactions between young people and the important adults in their lives would matter more for the academic outcomes of youth who were socially marginalized and disadvantaged (i.e., low-income youth, racial and ethnic minorities), with positive expectations being particularly promotive and negative expectations being particularly deleterious for these populations.
Method
Data and Sample
The Education Longitudinal Study (ELS), conducted by the National Center for Education Statistics (NCES), is a nationally representative, longitudinal sample of adolescents who were in 10th grade in 2002. NCES used a two-stage sampling design applying a stratified probability proportional to size criterion, resulting in 17,591 10th grade students in 752 schools (Ingels et al. 2004). Student survey data were collected during 10th grade (Wave 1) and two years later in 2004 (Wave 2). Wave 3 (W3) data were collected in 2006 and included approximately 14,200 participants. In addition to student survey data from W1 to W3, we also included data from math achievement tests (W1, W2), parent surveys (W1), teacher surveys (W1), and school records (secondary school academic transcripts). Given our central focus on the intersection of adult expectations for young people’s educational attainment, 6,365 adolescents without data on parent and math teacher expectations were excluded. Our final analytic sample included 9,654 adolescents from 730 schools in the ELS public use dataset. Table 1 provides descriptive information on adolescents and their families and schools.
Table 1.
Variable | n | Frequency (%) | M | SD |
---|---|---|---|---|
Student gender | ||||
Male | 4,769 | 49.4 | ||
Female | 4,885 | 50.6 | ||
Student race/ethnicity | ||||
White | 6,171 | 64.2 | ||
African American | 999 | 10.4 | ||
Latinx | 1,248 | 13.0 | ||
Asian American | 695 | 7.2 | ||
Other race/ethnicity | 504 | 5.2 | ||
Student immigration status | ||||
1st generation | 843 | 8.8 | ||
2nd generation | 977 | 10.2 | ||
3rd generation | 7,745 | 81.0 | ||
Family SES | .14 | .74 | ||
Family structure intact | ||||
No | 3,597 | 37.3 | ||
Yes | 6,057 | 62.7 | ||
Ever retained in grade | ||||
No | 8,476 | 88.8 | ||
Yes | 1,070 | 11.2 | ||
Learning disability | ||||
No | 8,525 | 89.0 | ||
Yes | 1,054 | 11.0 | ||
School urbanicity | ||||
Urban | 2,975 | 30.8 | ||
Suburban | 4,760 | 49.3 | ||
Rural | 1,919 | 19.9 | ||
School geographic region | ||||
Northeast | 1,686 | 17.5 | ||
Midwest | 2,655 | 27.5 | ||
South | 3,592 | 37.2 | ||
West | 1,721 | 17.8 |
Measures
This multi-informant study included student-, parent-, and teacher-level data, school records, and direct assessments. Adult expectations and parent, teacher, home-school, and student mediators were measured at W1, and educational outcomes were drawn from W2 and W3. Table 2 provides descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations for the focal constructs.
Table 2.
Variable | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. P expectations | -- | ||||||||||
2. T expectations | .47*** | -- | |||||||||
3. P involvement at home | .00 | −.10*** | -- | ||||||||
4. P involvement at school | .18*** | .19*** | .19*** | -- | |||||||
5. P school communication | −.08*** | −.11*** | .22*** | .28*** | -- | ||||||
6. T-Y connections | .06*** | .15*** | .03** | .05*** | .04*** | -- | |||||
7. Y academic self-concept | .55*** | .70*** | −.02 | .21*** | −.10*** | .11*** | -- | ||||
8. Y math test score | .39*** | .63*** | −.18*** | .13*** | −.08*** | .06*** | .83*** | -- | |||
9. Y math course sequence | .41*** | .57*** | −.12*** | .16*** | −.09*** | .05*** | .71*** | .58*** | -- | ||
10. Y cumulative GPA | .40*** | .63*** | −.12*** | .17*** | −.17*** | .13*** | .72*** | .57*** | .50*** | -- | |
11. Y educational attainment | .39*** | .51*** | −.05*** | .18*** | −.08*** | .07*** | .56*** | .47*** | .43*** | .54*** | -- |
Mean | 5.01 | 4.26 | −0.00 | 1.27 | 2.53 | 0.37 | 0.03 | 51.99 | 5.16 | 4.15 | 4.67 |
SD | 1.39 | 1.44 | 0.36 | 1.37 | 2.23 | 0.48 | 0.69 | 9.95 | 1.11 | 1.46 | 1.95 |
N | 9,654 | 9,654 | 9,654 | 9,181 | 8,780 | 9,542 | 9,654 | 8,544 | 9,085 | 9,018 | 8,216 |
Note. P = parent, T = teacher, Y = youth;
p < .01,
p < .001. For variables that were modeled as a latent variable, factor scores were used in the correlational analyses. Academic outcomes were assessed at Wave 2 & 3. All other variables were assessed at Wave 1.
Adult expectations.
At W1, parents and math teachers reported the highest level of education they expected the student to complete on a seven-point continuum ranging from 1 (less than a high school degree) to 7 (PhD, MD, or other advanced degree).
Family processes.
Parents’ educational involvement at home was modeled as a latent variable using parent reports of checking homework and discussing report cards (1 = never, 4 = always) and adolescent reports of their parents checking and helping with homework (1 = never, 4 = often). Higher scores indicated greater educational involvement at home.
Home-school connections.
We included two measures of home-school connections at W1. First, parents’ involvement at school was a sum of four dichotomous indicators assessing whether parents were members of school organizations, attended school organization meetings, participated in school activities, and volunteered at the school (1 = yes, 0 = no). Second, for parents’ communication with school, parents reported the frequency with which themselves or their spouse/partner had initiated contact with school personnel since the beginning of the school year (1 = none, 4 = more than four times) about the student (e.g., student’s poor performance, post-high school plans). We created dichotomous indicators capturing whether parents ever engaged in each of the ten communication topics (coded as 1) or not (coded as 0) and then summed these indicators to determine the total number of communication topics discussed.
School processes.
Students’ math teachers answered the following question about their interactions with the target student: “Does this student talk with you outside class about school work, plans after high school, or personal matters?” Responses were yes (1) or no (0). This item was utilized as a proxy of the extent to which the students made connections with their math teachers outside of class.
Student academic self-concept.
Student academic self-concept was modeled as a latent variable indicated by three measures assessed at W1. Students reported their educational expectations (1 = less than high school degree, 7 = PhD, MD, or other advanced degree). Two scales tapped into different aspects of students’ self-competency beliefs; all items were rated using a 4-point scale (1 = applies never, 4 = applies almost always). First, the 5-item math self-efficacy subscale assessed students’ perceptions of their math abilities (e.g., “I am confident I can do an excellent job on math tests”). Higher mean scores reflected more positive perceptions of ability in mathematics (α = .94). Second, the 5-item action control subscale assessed students’ effort and persistence when studying (e.g., “When studying, I put forth my best effort”). Higher mean scores indicated greater effort and task persistence (α = .89).
Academic outcomes.
We included four measures of academic outcomes. First, ELS designed, conducted, and collected data on a standardized math achievement test for high school students using questions from previous standardized tests such as NELS. We used the standardized t-scores of these test scores in W2 to measure math achievement relative to the standardized 12th grade population. Second, an ELS-constructed composite measured math course-taking using student reports of years in math courses (1 = no or other math course, 6 = trigonometry, pre-calculus, or calculus) to determine highest level of math course taken by the end of 12th grade. Third, a categorical variable created from high school transcripts captured cumulative grade point average (GPA) for 9th through 12th grades (0 = F (0.00 – 1.00), 6 = A (3.51 – 4.00)). Finally, at W3, individuals reported on their highest level of educational attainment from 1 (less than a high school credential) to 7 (doctoral degree).
Sociodemographic moderators and study covariates.
All analyses controlled for sociodemographic covariates (assessed at W1). Students reported their race/ethnicity as White, African American, Hispanic, Asian American, or some other race/ethnicity. Students also self-reported their gender. Parents reported their own and their child’s nativity, which resulted in the ELS-constructed immigrant generational status (first, second, or third-plus generations). Parents reported family structure (recoded into a dichotomous variable: 1 = student lives with both biological parents, 0 = other family structure) as well as whether the adolescent had ever been retained in grade (1 = yes, 0 = no) or had a learning disability (1 = yes, 0 = no). An ELS constructed socioeconomic status (SES) composite was created based on family income, parents’ highest education level, and parents’ occupations. A standardized continuous SES score was used as a covariate in the main model, but when SES was tested as a moderator, we recoded it into a binary variable, such that the low SES group had a composite score smaller than 0, and the high SES group had a composite score equal to or greater than 0. Additional individual-level control variables included students’ prior academic achievement, as measured by the ELS math achievement test in W1 (10th grade) as well as the survey respondent’s relationship to the focal student (1 = biological mother, 0 = any other relation). We also included two school-level covariates: geographic region (Northeast, Midwest, South, West) and urbanicity (urban, suburban, rural).
Plan of Analyses
We conducted path analyses in a structural equation modeling (SEM) framework to examine the magnitude and significance of associations between the predictors (expectations), mediators (family and school processes, home-school connections, and student academic self-concept), and academic outcomes (see conceptual model, Figure 1). The analyses included five steps. As an initial step, we conducted a measurement model to determine fit of the latent variables to be used in the path analyses (i.e., parents’ educational involvement at home, student academic self-concept). Overall, the measurement model fit the data well (χ2 (12) = 239.65, p < .001; CFI = .95; RMSEA = .044 [CI: .040 - .049]. The standardized coefficients and standardized errors for all loadings are presented in Table 3. All loadings were significant at p < .001.
Table 3.
B (SE) | p-value | |
---|---|---|
Parents’ educational involvement at home | ||
Homework help (parent report) | .59 (.05) | .000 |
Discuss grades and report cards (parent report) | .42 (.03) | .000 |
Homework help (student report) | .29 (.03) | .000 |
Discuss grades and report cards (student report) | .23 (.03) | .000 |
Student self-concept | ||
Educational expectations | .41 (.02) | .000 |
Math self-efficacy | .64 (.02) | .000 |
Effort and persistence | .83 (.02) | .000 |
Note. N = 9,653. χ2 (12) = 239.65, p < .001; CFI = .95; RMSEA = .044 [CI: .040 - .049].
We then analyzed a structural equation model examining the direct associations between adult expectations and adolescent outcomes (Model 1). Next, we added the parent expectations X math teacher expectations interaction term to the model (Model 2). This was followed by a model (Model 3) that examined whether the direct or interaction effects varied by race/ethnicity or SES using multiple group analyses; chi-square difference tests with Satorra-Bentler corrections were conducted to identify where exactly paths differed across groups. Model 4 introduced the mediators; here, we examined the indirect effects from predictors to outcomes via the family, school, home-school connections, and adolescent self-concept mediators, with the standard error of indirect effects caluculated through the Delta method in Mplus. All models included a host of covariates outlined in the measures section.
All analyses were conducted using Mplus 8.2 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2019). The CLUSTER function was used to account for the nesting of ELS participants within schools to produce correctly adjusted standard errors in model estimations; a standard weight was used to correct for oversampling and differential attrition for the longitudinal data. Missing data for our analytic sample were handled through full-information maximum likelihood (FIML), which enabled inclusion of all available data in the analyses (Enders 2011). In the path analyses, both direct and indirect effects were estimated simultaneously, and inferences for the indirect effects were estimated using the delta method (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2015).
Results
Parent-Teacher Expectations Congruence/Dissonance and Academic Outcomes
For the main effects model investigating the association between adult expectations and adolescents’ academic outcomes (Model 1), as shown in the upper portion of Table 4, parent and math teacher expectations were significantly and positively linked to all adolescent academic outcomes under consideration at the p < .001 level. The effect size for math teacher expectations was approximately two times the size of that of parent expectations for both math course-taking sequence and educational attainment, more than three times as strong for cumulative high school grade point average, and five times as strong for math achievement test scores.
Table 4.
Model 1 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Math Test Scores B (SE) | Math Course Sequence B (SE) | High School Cumulative GPA B (SE) | Educational Attainment B (SE) | |
Parent expectations | .03 (.01)*** | .11 (.02)*** | .12 (.01)*** | .14 (.01)*** |
Math Teacher expectations | .15 (.01)*** | .28 (.02)*** | .44 (.01)*** | .28 (.02)*** |
Model 2 | ||||
Math Test Scores B (SE) | Math Course Sequence B (SE) | High School Cumulative GPA B (SE) | Educational Attainment B (SE) | |
Parent expectations | .03 (.01)*** | .10 (.01)*** | .14 (.01)*** | .16 (.01)*** |
Teacher expectations | .15 (.01)*** | .28 (.02)*** | .44 (.01)*** | .28 (.02)*** |
Parent-by-teacher expectations | .03 (.01)** | −.033 (.01)* | .04 (.01)*** | .06 (.01)*** |
Female | −.05 (.01)*** | .02 (.01) | .15 (.01)*** | .09 (.01)*** |
African American | −.02 (.01)** | .05 (.01)*** | −.10 (.01)*** | .00 (.01) |
Latinx | −.01 (.01) | .01 (.02) | −.05 (.02)*** | −.03 (.02)* |
Asian American | .01 (.01) | .02 (.01)* | .00 (.01) | .01 (.01) |
Other race/ethnicity | .01 (.01) | −.01 (.01) | −.04 (.01)*** | −.02 (.01) |
First generation | −.00 (.01) | .01 (.01) | −.03 (.01)* | .00 (.01) |
Second generation | .00 (.01) | .02 (.01) | −.03 (.01)** | .03 (.01)* |
Suburban locale | −.01 (.01) | −.03 (.02)* | .06 (.02)** | −.00 (.01) |
Rural locale | −.00 (.01) | −.04 (.02)* | .09 (.02)*** | .00 (.02) |
Northeast geographic region | .00 (.01) | .06 (.02)** | −.13 (.02)*** | .05 (.02)** |
Midwest geographic region | −.00 (.01) | .06 (.02)** | −.03 (.02) | .06 (.02)*** |
South geographic region | −.01 (.01) | .10 (.02)*** | −.07 (.02)** | .02 (.02) |
Prior retention in grade | −.04 (.01)*** | −.06 (.02)*** | −.03 (.01)** | −.02 (.01)* |
Disability diagnosis | −.04 (.01)*** | −.08 (.02)*** | −0.00 (.01) | −.04 (.01)*** |
SES | .04 (.01)*** | .05 (.01)** | −.00 (.01) | .15 (.01)*** |
Intact family status | .00 (.01) | .02 (.01) | .08 (.01)*** | .06 (.01)*** |
Mother is interview respondent | .00 (.01) | .00 (.01) | −.00 (.01) | .01 (.01) |
10th grade math achievement test score | .74 (.01)*** | .28 (.01)*** | .17 (.01)*** | .12 (.02)*** |
Note. N = 9,654. *** p < .001. No fit statistics presented for Model 1 or Model 2 as they are just identified.
p < .05
p < ,01
p < .001. Covariate effects included but not tabled for Model
Model 2, which introduced the interaction term between parent and math teacher expectations documented significant interactions for all four outcomes under study (see lower portion of Table 4). A similar pattern of effects was observed for three of the four academic outcomes under study—math achievement test scores, cumulative GPA, and educational attainment (see Figure 2). Specifically, the positive effects of higher math teacher expectations on these academic outcomes were maximized when parents also held higher expectations for youth, particularly for cumulative GPA and educational attainment. In contrast, for math course-taking sequence, higher parent expectations buffered the negative effects of lower math teacher expectations. Overall, adolescents performed best academically across all four domains when both math teacher and parent expectations were high, and they had the worst academic outcomes when both math teacher and parent expectations were low. When adult expectations were dissonant, adolescents with high teacher but low parent expectations had slightly better GPAs and educational attainment than adolescents with low teacher but high parent expectations. In contrast, for math achievement test scores and math course-taking sequence, there appeared to be little difference in the two discrepancy groups.
We then examined whether the links between adult expectations and their interaction with the academic outcomes varied by students’ race/ethnicity or SES. Here, we first compared a model where all parameters were free to vary across groups to a model where the central paths of interest were fixed across groups. For both race/ethnicity and SES, we found that the fully constrained model fit the data significantly worse that the unconstrained model (see Table 5). We then tested individual pathways to identify exactly where group differences existed. As shown in the upper panel of Table 5, we observed variations across race/ethnicity for the links between parent expectations and both GPA and math course-taking sequence and between parent-by-teacher expectations and math course-taking sequence. Specifically, parent expectations were significantly related to GPA for White (B = .17, p < .001), African American (B = .11, p < .01), and Asian American students (B =.11, p < .01), but not for Latinx students (B = .04, p = .39). In contrast, the link between parent expectations and math course-taking sequence was significant for White students (B = .12, p < .001) but not for Black (B = .05, p = .18), Latinx (B = .02, p = .69), or Asian American students (B = .03, p = .42). Additionally, the interaction between parent and math teacher expectations was significantly related to math course-taking sequence for White (B = −.06, p < .01) and Asian American students (B = −.16, p < .01) but not for African American (B = .04, p =.23) or Latinx students (B = −.03, p = .44). As shown in Figure 3, for White students, higher parent expectations seemed to buffer the negative effects of low math teacher expectations on math course-taking sequence, consistent with the overall pattern observed for the full sample. For Asian American students, in contrast, high math teacher expectations seemed to buffer the negative effects of low parent expectations on math course-taking sequence, yet math teacher expectations were unrelated to math course-taking sequence for students whose parent held high expectations for them.
Table 5.
S-B Scaled χ2 Difference | df | p-value | |
---|---|---|---|
Multiple Group: Race/ethnicity | |||
Fully constrained | 70.82 | 36 | .000 |
Parent expectations → math test scores | 7.63 | 3 | .054 |
Parent expectations → math course sequence | 12.30 | 3 | .006 |
Parent expectations → grade point average | 10.59 | 3 | .014 |
Parent expectations → educational attainment | 3.28 | 3 | .350 |
Teacher expectations → math test scores | 6.89 | 3 | .076 |
Teacher expectations → math course sequence | 8.05 | 3 | .045 |
Teacher expectations → grade point average | 3.40 | 3 | .334 |
Teacher expectations → educational attainment | 4.00 | 3 | .262 |
Interaction → math test scores | 1.78 | 3 | .620 |
Interaction → math course sequence | 11.69 | 3 | .009 |
Interaction → grade point average | 2.50 | 3 | .476 |
Interaction → educational attainment | 3.59 | 3 | .310 |
Multiple Group: SES | |||
Fully constrained | 38.32 | 12 | .000 |
Parent expectations → math test scores | 8.80 | 1 | .003 |
Parent expectations → math course sequence | 0.47 | 1 | .493 |
Parent expectations → grade point average | 0.27 | 1 | .601 |
Parent expectations → educational attainment | 6.55 | 1 | .010 |
Teacher expectations → math test scores | 0.30 | 1 | .585 |
Teacher expectations → math course sequence | 3.73 | 1 | .054 |
Teacher expectations → grade point average | 0.59 | 1 | .444 |
Teacher expectations → educational attainment | 0.92 | 1 | .336 |
Interaction → math test scores | 0.02 | 1 | .888 |
Interaction → math course sequence | 6.93 | 1 | .008 |
Interaction → grade point average | 4.97 | 1 | .026 |
Interaction → educational attainment | 0.05 | 1 | .820 |
Note. N = 9,654. Significant Satorra-Bentler scaled chi-square difference test results (p < .05) are bolded. Comparison model is the fully free model for all difference tests. Interaction = parent expectations-by-teacher expectations interaction.
Moving to potential variation by SES (lower panel of Table 5), group differences were observed for the link between parent expectations and both educational attainment and math achievement test scores and between parent-by-teacher expectations and students’ math course-taking sequence and GPA. Specifically, higher parent expectations were associated with higher educational attainment for both low-SES (B = .16, p < .001) and high-SES students (B = .17, p < .001), but this link was slightly stronger for high-SES students. Parallel to these findings, higher parent expectations were related to higher math achievement scores for high-SES (B = .05, p < .001) but not low-SES students (B = .01, p = .24).
The interaction between parent and math teacher expectations was significantly related to math course-taking sequence for high-SES (B = −.08, p < .01) but not low-SES students (B = −.00, p = .96), whereas this interaction was related to GPA for low-SES (B = .06, p < .01) but not high-SES students (B = .01, p = .62). As shown in Figure 4, high parent expectations seem to buffer the negative impact of low math teacher expectations on math course-taking sequence for high-SES but not low-SES students. For GPA, the positive effects of high math teacher expectations on GPA were maximized when parents also held higher expectations for students, but only for low-SES students.
Mechanisms Linking Adult Expectations and Adolescents’ Academic Outcomes
The mediation model fit the data well, χ2 (253) = 3,114.27, p < .001; CFI = .89; RMSEA = .034 [CI: .033 - .035]. The standardized path coefficients for the model are presented in Figure 5. Parents expressing higher educational expectations were more involved in their adolescents’ education at home and at school and had children with higher academic self-concepts; however, higher parental educational expectations were associated with less parent-school communication. Math teacher expectations were linked to all central constructs, such that higher math teacher expectations were associated with more teacher-student connections, greater parental educational involvement at school, and higher student academic self-concept but lower levels of parent educational involvement at home and parent-school communication.
Of the home, school, and home-school connection mediators, only parental educational involvement at school was related to students’ academic self-concept, with greater involvement linked to higher self-concept. For youth academic outcomes, we observed some variation. Greater parent-school communication was linked to lower student GPAs, whereas greater teacher-student connections were linked to higher GPAs. Further, greater parent educational involvement at school was related to lower math achievement scores. Parent educational involvement at home was not significantly related to any of the outcomes under study. Students’ academic self-concept was also positively related to all youth academic outcomes. Of the three intermediate academic outcomes, only GPA was related to educational attainment, with higher GPA’s associated with greater educational attainment two years after high school.
Formal tests of the indirect effects of adult expectations on youth academic outcomes were observed. Parameters for all indirect pathways are presented in the Appendix. There were two common indirect pathways from parents’ and math teachers’ educational expectations to youth academic outcomes: a) via students’ academic self-concept b) through parents’ communication with the school, and c) through parents’ involvement at school and students’ academic self-concept. For parent educational expectations, we observed an additional mediated pathway for educational attainment via students’ GPA’s, and for math teacher educational expectations, we observed additional mediated pathways via both teacher-student connections and parent communication with the school.
Discussion
Academic success is shaped by youth themselves as well as parents and teachers, who represent two of the most influential groups of adults in adolescents’ linked lives. Understanding how parent and teacher expectations come together thus provides a more comprehensive picture of the academic socialization that young people receive. The main goal of this study was to investigate how adult educational expectations both directly and indirectly shape young people’s academic outcomes in a nationally representative sample of high school students. Because prior research suggests that adult expectations and academic outcomes differ across sociodemographic groups (Yamamoto & Holloway, 2010), the second goal of this study was to determine whether the links between those expectations and academic outcomes varied by important markers of social disadvantage including race/ethnicity and social class.
Prior research focused on the independent effects of parent and teacher educational expectations has consistently identified the particularly strong role that teacher expectations play in the achievement of youth across their educational careers (Gregory & Huang, 2013; Lee et al., 2015). The current study replicated these findings, highlighting the critical nature of parents and teachers in the academic lives of youth. In particular, high parent expectations seemed to magnify high teacher expectations, although for math course-taking sequence, we did observe a buffering effect wherein high parent expectations seemed to be protective in the face of low teacher expectations, more consistent with prior research (Benner & Mistry, 2007).
Results of the current study, however, revealed high teacher expectations were even more protective against low parent expectations, with the effect size for teacher expectations varying between two and five times that of parental expectations. These findings support a particularly strong “linked lives” connection between teachers and students. Although parents hold influence over how their children do in school, there is wide variation in exactly what this looks like across families (Lareau, 2003). Teachers, however, interact daily with students’ academic progress and achievement and, based on our findings, have more influence over how they perform and progress through school. From a cultural capital perspective (Bourdieu, 1977), youth may “inherit” cultural capital from their parents, but how their teachers reward them for it matters for their academic progress and achievement. This study thus suggests that strong teacher expectations are crucial for improving youth academic achievement.
Turning to mechanisms of influence, we relied on both life course theory and Yamamoto and Holloway’s model of adult expectation effects (2010) to inform our study. To this end, we proposed that adult expectations would influence interactions within linked lives spanning family and school as well as home-school connections. These interactions, in turn, were expected to drive students’ self-concept and their academic success. In general, these hypotheses were meted out in our findings. When parents and teachers held high educational expectations for youth, these were related to high levels of parental school involvement, which in turn were linked to higher academic self-concept, that were then related to better student academic outcomes. However, contrary to hypotheses, higher levels of parental school involvement were associated with poorer math achievement scores and unrelated to all other academic outcomes of interest.
Parent-school communication occurred more frequently when both parents and teachers held low expectations for youth, and higher communication levels were associated with poorer subsequent GPA. We suspect that these negative links reflect underlying mechanisms that are informing these processes—specifically, parents are likely engaging in educational activities at home and communicating with their children’s schools in response to academic challenges their children are displaying, and these academic challenges are likely reflected in the educational expectations that both parents and teachers are expressing for these students. Educational activities such as these, however, have been found to exact an effect opposite of what is intended, leading to poorer academic achievement (Pomerantz & Eaton, 2001; Robinson & Harris, 2014). Future research should examine the mechanisms underlying the negative connections observed and investigate ways that home-based activities and parent-school communications might be structured to better promote student engagement and achievement.
As for teacher-student connections, we observed that higher teacher educational expectations were related to more teacher and student connections, which was, in turn, associated with higher student GPAs. Meta-analytic research has identified a consistent association between positive relationships with teachers and students’ engagement and achievement, particularly for secondary school students (Roorda et al., 2011). Given that prior research has highlighted the compensatory role of positive teacher-student relationships for the academic achievement of young children who struggled with academic-related tasks (Liew et al., 2010), identifying ways to promote positive teacher-students relationships for educationally at-risk students may help promote academic success for this vulnerable population.
We observed some variation across students’ SES and race/ethnicity; however, our results generally run counter to DiMaggio’s (1982) theory of cultural mobility with some exceptions. Overall, we did not find that cultural capital was especially important to youth from disadvantaged backgrounds, which goes against the theory of cultural mobility. Instead, moderation analyses revealed that the positive link between parent expectations and math course-taking sequence was only significant for White students, and parent expectations appeared to be more beneficial for high-SES students’ educational attainment and math achievement scores than low-SES students. These findings are more consistent with Bourdieu’s social capital theory (1977), which suggests that more advantaged families are able to transmit to their children the types of cultural capital that are expected and valued by institutions such as schools. It is also likely that parents are able to maximize their cultural capital to advocate and provide the resources necessary to connect their adolescents to educators who can inform and support course selection and academic tracking that is advantageous for high school and postsecondary pursuits.
In contrast, the links between teacher expectations and educational outcomes were consistent across student race/ethnicity and SES. That is not to say, however, that educational opportunities and support from teachers to students are uniform across diverse groups of students. Indeed, ancillary analyses showed that, on average, teachers had lower educational expectations of students of color and of low-income students, in line with previous research (Auwarter & Aruguete, 2008; Ready & Wright, 2011). In other words, teachers may have lower expectations and their expectations may matter most for educational outcomes, but the link between those expectations and outcomes is neither stronger nor weaker across sociodemographic groups above and beyond the cultural capital with which students’ parents equip them (Bourdieu, 1977; Lareau, 2003).
Strengths, Caveats, and Limitations
The current study used a large, nationally representative sample to conduct robust tests of a comprehensive set of mechanisms linking adult educational expectation and adolescent academic achievement. Moreover, we included multiple indicators of academic achievement, ranging from short-term to long-term achievement outcomes. The current study thus extends the literature on adults’ educational expectations and youth self-concept and outcomes by considering multiple mechanisms that could be at play. Whereas much of the literature has focused on direct relationships for students, we studied direct and indirect associations as well as mediational processes and potential sociodemographic moderators to provide a more holistic picture of how adults’ expectations matter for the academic outcomes of youth toward the end of their secondary educations.
Although the current study contributes to our understanding of adult educational expectations and young people’s academic achievement, the study is not without limitations. Because data are drawn from a national study, some measures include only one or two items; although we benefit from breadth of the measures included, which allowed us to investigate several potential mechanisms, the limited depth of measurement must be acknowledged. Future research with more multi-item measures could provide further validation for our study findings. Likewise, inclusion of more diverse aspects of parents’ involvement at home and at school that are less culturally and socioeconomically biased (Lareau, 2003) would provide greater illumination into the ways in which adult expectations are shaped by and manifested in parents’ academic interactions with their children and with the larger educational system. Additionally, given that peers are increasingly important socializing agents during adolescence (Brown, 2004) and that peer groups often exhibit homophily in terms of educational expectations (Kiuru et al., 2007), future research should also integrate attention to peers when considering mechanisms of influence for young people’s educational success.
Additionally, although nationally representative, the majority of the analytical sample was White (62%), with racial/ethnic minority groups making up much smaller shares of the sample. Future research should further investigate the links between adult expectations and academic outcomes across students from comparably-sized racial/ethnic groups, particularly given the shifting demographic landscape of American schools wherein White students are now the numeric minority (Hussar et al., 2020). Given the continued diversification of U.S. society coupled with the increasing sociodemographic segregation of U.S. schools, cross-school comparisons could be one avenue for comparing the adult expectations and academic outcomes of students in majority white schools compared to those of students in majority African American or majority Hispanic schools, for example.
Finally, in the current study, we proposed directional pathways linking adult educational expectations to interpersonal processes to students’ academic self-concept and academic success. Such work was based on prior theoretical models; however, the life course perspective suggests that linked lives are bidirectional, wherein key social agents influence young people’s growth and development, but the characteristics, strengths, and challenges that young people face also influence their subsequent interactions with these important others in their lives. Indeed, prior research has shown that teacher expectations both influence and are influenced by children’s academic achievement (Mistry et al., 2009). Future research should unpack longitudinally the complex bidirectional influences that may be at play, particularly given that academic tracking begins early in students educational careers, thus potentially setting the stage for adults’ educational expectations early in students’ lives (Oakes, 2005). Finally, the current study focused on math teachers’ expectations and the linked lives students have with those social agents. In U.S. secondary schools, students interact with a variety of teachers across the school day, and thus math teachers represent only one of many educators adolescents are encountering in their school lives. In the curriculum, however, mathematics is often a marker and gatekeeper for higher academic tracks in high school, and success in mathematics is linked to both college readiness and postsecondary success (Crosnoe & Benner, 2015; Frank et al., 2008). This motivated our focus on the expectations of math teachers’ expectations in particular, although it should also be noted that the educational expectations espoused by students’ math and English/language arts teachers were highly correlated in the ELS (r = .66, p < .001).
The findings presented here clearly indicate the direct and indirect ways that adult expectations influence students’ academic success. Our study has practical implications for assisting disadvantaged youth through improving educational expectations that teachers hold for all students. Randomized-group trials have been effective in providing teachers tools to emulate classrooms where high expectations are the norm for all (Rubie-Davies et al., 2015). Given our findings of the direct importance of teacher expectations and the benefits of strong student-teacher relationships for students’ academic self-concept, teacher-focused interventions are likely a crucial point of entrée for improving student achievement. This intervention work could also integrate methods for encouraging parents to enact more effective home-based involvement strategies. Such work is crucial for ensuring the educational well-being of all students.
Appendix. Significant indirect effects from adult expectations to youth academic outcomes
From parent expectations to academic outcomes | From teacher expectations to academic outcomes | |
---|---|---|
Indirect pathways through… | B (SE) | B (SE) |
Math Achievement Test Scores | ||
Parents’ educational involvement at home | −.001 (.001) | .004 (.002) |
Teacher-student connections | .000 (.000) | −.003 (.002) |
Parents’ involvement at school | −.005 (.001)*** | −.004 (.001)** |
Parent-school communication | .000 (.000) | −.001 (.001) |
Student academic self-concept | .440 (.074)*** | .347 (.061)*** |
Parents’ educational involvement at home → Student academic self-concept | .000 (.001) | .001 (.002) |
Teacher-student connections → Student academic self-concept | .000 (.000) | .001 (.002) |
Parents’ involvement at school → Student academic self-concept | .004 (.001)*** | .003 (.001)*** |
Parent-school communication → Student academic self-concept | .000 (.000) | .001 (.001) |
Math Course-taking Sequence | ||
Parents’ educational involvement at home | .000 (.001) | .001 (.003) |
Teacher-student connections | .000 (.000) | −.002 (.003) |
Parents’ involvement at school | −.003 (.002) | −.002 (.002) |
Parent-school communication | .000 (.001) | .000 (.002) |
Student academic self-concept | .771 (.128)*** | .608 (.104)*** |
Parents’ educational involvement at home → Student academic self-concept | −.001 (.001) | .001 (.003) |
Teacher-student connections → Student academic self-concept | .000 (.000) | .002 (.003) |
Parents’ involvement at school → Student academic self-concept | .007 (.002)** | .006 (.002)** |
Parent-school communication → Student academic self-concept | .001 (.001) | .002 (.002) |
Cumulative High School Grade Point Average | ||
Parents’ educational involvement at home | .000 (.001) | .001 (.002) |
Teacher-student connections | .000 (.001) | .007 (.003)* |
Parents’ involvement at school | .002 (.002) | .002 (.002) |
Parent-school communication | .003 (.001)* | .010 (.002)*** |
Student academic self-concept | .548 (.082)*** | .433 (.069)*** |
Parents’ educational involvement at home → Student academic self-concept | .000 (.001) | .001 (.002) |
Teacher-student connections → Student academic self-concept | .000 (.000) | .002 (.002) |
Parents’ involvement at school → Student academic self-concept | .005 (.001)** | .004 (.001)** |
Parent-school communication → Student academic self-concept | .000 (.000) | .002 (.001) |
From parent expectations to academic outcomes | From math teacher expectations to academic outcomes | |
Indirect pathways through… | B (SE) | B (SE) |
Educational Attainment | ||
Math achievement test scores | .033 (.043) | .016 (.022) |
Math course-taking sequence | .037 (.039) | .018 (.020) |
Cumulative high school grade point average | −.077 (.017)*** | −.003 (.012) |
Student academic self-concept | .588 (.277)* | .464 (.220)* |
Parents’ educational involvement at home → math scores | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Teacher-student connections → math scores | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Parents’ involvement at school → math scores | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Parent-school communication → math scores | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Student academic self-concept → math scores | −.036 (.047) | −.028 (.037) |
Parents’ educational involvement at home → math sequence | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Teacher-student connections → math sequence | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Parents’ involvement at school → math sequence | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Parent-school communication → math sequence | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Student academic self-concept → math sequence | −.043 (.044) | −.034 (.035) |
Parents’ educational involvement at home → GPA | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Teacher-student connections → GPA | .000 (.000) | .001 (.000)** |
Parents’ involvement at school → GPA | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Parent-school communication → GPA | .001 (.000) | .002 (.001)** |
Student academic self-concept → GPA | .101 (.020)*** | .080 (.016)*** |
Parents’ educational involvement at home → Student academic self-concept | .000 (.001) | .001 (.003) |
Teacher-student connections → Student academic self-concept | .000 (.000) | .002 (.002) |
Parents’ involvement at school → Student academic self-concept | .005 (.002)** | .004 (.002)* |
Parent-school communication → Student academic self-concept | .000 (.001) | .002 (.001) |
Parents’ educational involvement at home → Student academic self-concept → math scores | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Teacher-student connections → Student academic self-concept → math scores | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Parents’ involvement at school → Student academic self-concept → math scores | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Parent-school communication → Student academic self-concept → math scores | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Parents’ educational involvement at home → Student academic self-concept → math sequence | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Teacher-student connections → Student academic self-concept → math sequence | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Parents’ involvement at school → Student academic self-concept → math sequence | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Parent-school communication → Student academic self-concept → math sequence | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Parents’ educational involvement at home → Student academic self-concept → GPA | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Teacher-student connections → Student academic self-concept → GPA | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
Parents’ involvement at school → Student academic self-concept → GPA | .001 (.000)* | .001 (.000)* |
Parent-school communication → Student academic self-concept → GPA | .000 (.000) | .000 (.000) |
References
- Auwarter Amy E., and Aruguete Mara S.. 2008. “Effects of Student Gender and Socioeconomic Status on Teacher Perceptions.” Journal of Educational Research 101(4):243–46. doi: 10.3200/JOER.101.4.243-246. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Benner Aprile D., Boyle Alaina E., and Sadler Sydney. 2016. “Parental Involvement and Adolescents’ Educational Success: The Roles of Prior Achievement and Socioeconomic Status.” Journal of Youth and Adolescence 45(6):1053–64. doi: 10.1007/s10964-016-0431-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benner Aprile D., and Mistry Rashmita S.. 2007. “Congruence of Mother and Teacher Educational Expectations and Low-Income Youth’s Academic Competence.” Journal of Educational Psychology 99(1):140–53. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.99.1.140. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bloome Deirdre, Dyer Shauna, and Zhou Xiang. 2018. “Educational Inequality, Educational Expansion, and Intergenerational Income Persistence in the United States.” American Sociological Review 83(6):1215–53. doi: 10.1177/0003122418809374. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bourdieu Pierre.. (1977). Outline of a Theory of Practice. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge university press [Google Scholar]
- Bronfenbrenner Urie. 1979. The Ecology of Human Development. Harvard university press. [Google Scholar]
- Briley Daniel A., Harden K. Paige, and Tucker-Drob Elliot M.. 2014. “Child Characteristics and Parental Educational Expectations: Evidence for Transmission with Transaction.” Developmental Psychology 50(12):2614. doi: 10.1037/a0038094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carolan Brian V., and Wasserman Sara J.. 2015. “Does Parenting Style Matter? Concerted Cultivation, Educational Expectations, and the Transmission of Educational Advantage.” Sociological Perspectives 58(2):168–86. doi: 10.1177/0731121414562967. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chambers TTV, & Huggins KS (2014). The influence of school factors on racial opportunity cost for high-achieving students of color. Journal of School Leadership, 24, 189–225. [Google Scholar]
- Cherng Hua-Yu Sebastian. 2017. “If They Think I Can: Teacher Bias and Youth of Color Expectations and Achievement.” Social Science Research 66:170–86. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2017.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crosnoe Robert, and Benner Aprile D.. 2015. “Children at school.” Handbook of child psychology and developmental science, 1–37. [Google Scholar]
- Crosnoe Robert, and Johnson Monica Kirkpatrick. 2011. “Research on Adolescence in the Twenty-First Century.” Annual Review of Sociology 37(1):439–60. doi: 10.1146/annurev-soc-081309-150008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis-Kean Pamela E. 2005. “The Influence of Parent Education and Family Income on Child Achievement: The Indirect Role of Parental Expectations and the Home Environment.” Journal of Family Psychology 19(2):294–304. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.19.2.294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Boer Hester, Bosker Roel J., and van der Werf Margaretha P. C.. 2010. “Sustainability of Teacher Expectation Bias Effects on Long-Term Student Performance.” Journal of Educational Psychology 102(1):168–79. doi: 10.1037/a0017289. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- DiMaggio Paul. 1982. “Cultural Capital and School Success: The Impact of Status Culture Participation on the Grades of U.S. High School Students.” American Sociological Review 47(2):189–201. doi: 10.2307/2094962. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Elder Glen H. 1998. “The Life Course as Developmental Theory.” Child Development 69(1):1–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06128.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enders Craig K. 2011. “Analyzing Longitudinal Data with Missing Values.” Rehabilitation Psychology 56(4):267–88. doi: 10.1037/a0025579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Englund Michelle M., Luckner Amy E., Whaley Gloria J. L., and Egeland Byron. 2004. “Children’s Achievement in Early Elementary School: Longitudinal Effects of Parental Involvement, Expectations, and Quality of Assistance.” Journal of Educational Psychology 96(4):723–30. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.96.4.723. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ford DY, & Moore JL (2013). Understanding and reversing underachievement, low achievement, and achievement gaps among high-ability African American males in urban school contexts. The Urban Review, 45, 399–415. [Google Scholar]
- Frank Kenneth A., Muller Chandra, Schiller Kathryn S., Riegle‐Crumb Catherine, Mueller Anna Strassmann, Crosnoe Robert, and Pearson Jennifer. 2008. “The Social Dynamics of Mathematics Coursetaking in High School.” American Journal of Sociology 113(6):1645–96. doi: 10.1086/587153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedrich Alena, Flunger Barbara, Nagengast Benjamin, Jonkmann Kathrin, and Trautwein Ulrich. 2015. “Pygmalion Effects in the Classroom: Teacher Expectancy Effects on Students’ Math Achievement.” Contemporary Educational Psychology 41:1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.10.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gregory A, and Huang Francis. (2013). “It Takes a Village: The Effects of 10th Grade College-going Expectations of Students, Parents, and Teachers Four Years Later.” American Journal of Community Psychology, 52, 41–55. doi: 10.1007/s10464-013-9575-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glock Sabine. 2016. “Does Ethnicity Matter? The Impact of Stereotypical Expectations on in-Service Teachers’ Judgments of Students.” Social Psychology of Education 19(3):493–509. doi: 10.1007/s11218-016-9349-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hauser-Cram Penny, Sirin Selcuk R., and Stipek Deborah. 2003. “When Teachers’ and Parents’ Values Differ: Teachers’ Ratings of Academic Competence in Children From Low-Income Families.” Journal of Educational Psychology 95(4):813–20. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.813. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hill Nancy E., and Tyson Diana F.. 2009. “Parental Involvement in Middle School: A Meta-Analytic Assessment of the Strategies That Promote Achievement.” Developmental Psychology 45(3):740. doi: 10.1037/a0015362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinnant J. Benjamin, O’Brien Marion, and Ghazarian Sharon R.. 2009. “The Longitudinal Relations of Teacher Expectations to Achievement in the Early School Years.” Journal of Educational Psychology 101(3):662–70. doi: 10.1037/a0014306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoglund Wendy L. G., Jones Stephanie M., Brown Joshua L., and Aber J. Lawrence. 2015. “The Evocative Influence of Child Academic and Social-Emotional Adjustment on Parent Involvement in Inner-City Schools.” Journal of Educational Psychology 107(2):517–32. doi: 10.1037/a0037266. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hussar Bill, Zhang Jijun, Hein Sarah, Wang Ke, Roberts Ashley, Cui Jiashan, Smith Mary, Mann Farrah Bullock, Barmer Amy, Dilig Rita, Nachazel Thomas, Barnett Megan, and Purcell Stephen. n.d. “The Condition of Education 2020.” 348. [Google Scholar]
- Institute of Medicine (2014). “Investing in the Health and Well-being of Young Adults.
- Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. [Google Scholar]
- Jussim Lee, and Harber Kent D.. 2005. “Teacher Expectations and Self-Fulfilling Prophecies: Knowns and Unknowns, Resolved and Unresolved Controversies.” Personality and Social Psychology Review 9(2):131–55. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0902_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krolak-Schwerdt Sabine, Böhmer Matthias, and Gräsel Cornelia 2013. “The Impact of Accountability on Teachers’ Assessments of Student Performance: A Social Cognitive Analysis.” Social Psychology of Education 16(2):215–39. doi: 10.1007/s11218-013-9215-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lareau Annette. (2011). Unequal childhoods: Class, race, and family life. Univ of California Press. [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence Elizabeth. 2015. “The Family-School Interaction: School Composition and Parental Educational Expectations in the United States.” British Educational Research Journal 41(2):183–209. [Google Scholar]
- Lee Jung-Sook, and Bowen Natasha K.. 2006. “Parent Involvement, Cultural Capital, and the Achievement Gap Among Elementary School Children.” American Educational Research Journal 43(2):193–218. doi: 10.3102/00028312043002193. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lee Se Woong., Min Sookweon, and Mamerow Geoffrey P.. (2015). “Pygmalion in the Classroom and the Home: Expectation’s Role in the Pipeline to STEMM.” Teachers College Record, 117, 1–40. [Google Scholar]
- Liew Jeffrey, Chen Qi, and Hughes Jan N.. 2010. “Child Effortful Control, Teacher–Student Relationships, and Achievement in Academically at-Risk Children: Additive and Interactive Effects.” Early Childhood Research Quarterly 25(1):51–64. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loughlin-Presnal John, and Bierman Karen L.. 2017. “How Do Parent Expectations Promote Child Academic Achievement in Early Elementary School? A Test of Three Mediators.” Developmental Psychology 53(9):1694–1708. doi: 10.1037/dev0000369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mistry Rashmita S., White Elizabeth S., Benner Aprile D., and Huynh Virginia W.. 2009. “A Longitudinal Study of the Simultaneous Influence of Mothers’ and Teachers’ Educational Expectations on Low-Income Youth’s Academic Achievement.” Journal of Youth & Adolescence 38(6):826–38. doi: 10.1007/s10964-008-9300-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murdock-Perriera Lisel Alice, and Quentin Charles Sedlacek. 2018. “Questioning Pygmalion in the Twenty-First Century: The Formation, Transmission, and Attributional Influence of Teacher Expectancies.” Social Psychology of Education: An International Journal 21(3):691–707. doi: 10.1007/s11218-018-9439-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén Linda K., & Muthén Bengt O.. (1998 – 2015). Mplus User’s Guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. [Google Scholar]
- Pinquart Martin, and Ebeling Markus. 2020. “Parental Educational Expectations and Academic Achievement in Children and Adolescents—a Meta-Analysis.” Educational Psychology Review 32(2):463–80. doi: 10.1007/s10648-019-09506-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pomerantz Eva M., and Eaton Missa Murry. 2001. “Maternal Intrusive Support in the Academic Context: Transactional Socialization Processes.” Developmental Psychology 37(2):174–86. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.37.2.174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ready Douglas D., and Wright David L.. 2011. “Accuracy and Inaccuracy in Teachers’ Perceptions of Young Children’s Cognitive Abilities: The Role of Child Background and Classroom Context.” American Educational Research Journal 48(2):335–60. doi: 10.3102/0002831210374874. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Reardon Sean F., and Portilla Ximena A.. 2016. “Recent Trends in Income, Racial, and Ethnic School Readiness Gaps at Kindergarten Entry.” AERA Open 2(3):2332858416657343. doi: 10.1177/2332858416657343. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson Keith, and Harris Angel L.. (2014). The Broken Compass: Parental Involvement With Children’s Education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Roorda Debora L., Jak Suzanne, Zee Marjolein, Oort Frans J., and Koomen Helma M. Y.. 2017. “Affective Teacher–Student Relationships and Students’ Engagement and Achievement: A Meta-Analytic Update and Test of the Mediating Role of Engagement.” School Psychology Review 46(3):239–61. doi: 10.17105/SPR-2017-0035.V46-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Roorda Debora L., Koomen Helma M. Y., Spilt Jantine L., and Oort Frans J.. 2011. “The Influence of Affective Teacher–Student Relationships on Students’ School Engagement and Achievement: A Meta-Analytic Approach.” Review of Educational Research 81(4):493–529. doi: 10.3102/0034654311421793. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rubie-Davies Christine M., Peterson Elizabeth R., Sibley Chris G., and Rosenthal Robert. 2015. “A Teacher Expectation Intervention: Modelling the Practices of High Expectation Teachers.” Contemporary Educational Psychology 40:72–85. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.03.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rumberger Russell W. and Lim Sun Ah. 2008. “Why Students Drop Out of School: A Review of 25 Years of Research.” Santa Barbara, CA: California Dropout Research Project. [Google Scholar]
- Schleicher Andreas. “2019. PISA 2018: Insights and Interpretations.” OECD Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Schoon Ingrid, Parsons Samantha, and Sacker Amanda. 2004. “Socioeconomic Adversity, Educational Resilience, and Subsequent Levels of Adult Adaptation.” Journal of Adolescent Research 19(4):383–404. doi: 10.1177/0743558403258856. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Seginer Rachel. 1983. “Parents’ Educational Expectations and Children’s Academic Achievements: A Literature Review.” Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 29(1):1–23. [Google Scholar]
- Sorhagen Nicole S. 2013. “Early Teacher Expectations Disproportionately Affect Poor Children’s High School Performance.” Journal of Educational Psychology 105(2):465–77. doi: 10.1037/a0031754. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Yamamoto Yoko, and Holloway Susan D.. 2010. “Parental Expectations and Children’s Academic Performance in Sociocultural Context.” Educational Psychology Review 22(3):189–214. doi: 10.1007/s10648-010-9121-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zhan Min, and Sherraden Michael. 2011. “Assets and Liabilities, Educational Expectations, and Children’s College Degree Attainment.” Children and Youth Services Review 33(6):846–54. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2010.12.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]