Abstract
Background
Ageing is highly associated with cognitive decline and modifiable risk factors such as diet are believed to protect against this process. Specific dietary components and in particular, (poly)phenol-rich fruits such as berries have been increasingly recognised for their protection against age-related neurodegeneration. However, the impact of cranberries on cognitive function and neural functioning in older adults remains unclear.
Design
A 12-week parallel randomised placebo-controlled trial of freeze-dried cranberry powder was conducted in 60 older adults aged between 50 and 80 years. Cognitive assessment, including memory and executive function, neuroimaging and blood sample collection were conducted before and after the intervention to assess the impact of daily cranberry consumption on cognition, brain function and biomarkers of neuronal signalling.
Results
Cranberry supplementation for 12 weeks was associated with improvements in visual episodic memory in aged participants when compared to placebo. Mechanisms of action may include increased regional perfusion in the right entorhinal cortex, the accumbens area and the caudate in the cranberry group. Significant decrease in low-density lipoprotein (LDL) cholesterol during the course of the intervention was also observed. No significant differences were, however, detected for BDNF levels between groups.
Conclusions
The results of this study indicate that daily cranberry supplementation (equivalent to 1 small cup of cranberries) over a 12-week period improves episodic memory performance and neural functioning, providing a basis for future investigations to determine efficacy in the context of neurological disease. This trial was registered at clinicaltrials.gov as NCT03679533 and at ISRCTN as ISRCTN76069316.
Keywords: brain, flavonoids, cognition, BDNF, cerebral blood flow (CBF), arterial spin labelling (ASL), MRI, LDL-cholesterol
Introduction
The WHO estimates that by 2050, 22% of the world’s population will be aged over 60 years (1). This is a product of increasing life expectancy, which should be viewed as an exceptional achievement; yet the improvement remains somewhat overshadowed by the absence of preserved quality-of-life. With age, the predominant risk factor for numerous chronic and degenerative neurological conditions, maintaining quality-of-life represents a significant global challenge. Indeed, dementia incidence is projected to double every 20 years, affecting an estimated 152 million individuals by 2050 (2), placing considerable pressure upon already strained dementia care and disease management strategies. It is therefore imperative that proven effective solutions are developed to curb and manage current projections. The pathophysiological processes leading to the development of cognitive decline and dementias are complex and multifactorial, offering reasonable explanation for the failure of many pharmacological interventions to date (3). Future strategies may therefore benefit from a multifaceted approach. In addition to complex genetic predisposition, dementia risk is further influenced by numerous environmental factors such as diet, exercise and smoking status. Appealingly, these contributing environmental factors are often modifiable, and represent targets to address multiple underlying features of the disease process (4, 5). Therefore, achievable lifestyle interventions may represent an alternative approach to mitigate disease risk.
Epidemiological studies have reported that higher dietary intake of flavonoids is associated with slower rates of cognitive decline (6–8) and dementia (9). Foods rich in anthocyanins (responsible for imparting the red, purple and blue colour to several fruits and vegetables) and proanthocyanidins such as berries are consistently shown to improve cognition (10–13), and are further supported by a wealth of preclinical data (14–19), as well as emerging clinical evidence with fruit juices (20, 21) and freeze-dried fruit powder (22–24). Potential mechanisms include an enhancement of neuronal signalling and synaptic plasticity (17, 25), a modulation of glucose metabolism/insulin resistance (26, 27), a change in microbiota diversity and metabolism (28–30) along with regional increases in cerebral perfusion (31–34), however, further elucidation is required.
Cranberries (Vaccinium macrocarpon) are particularly rich in (poly)phenols such as anthocyanins, proanthocyanidins (both A- and B-type), flavonols and hydroxycinnamic acids (35), and are recognised for their antioxidant and anti-inflammatory effects (36–38) along with their capacity to modulate cardiometabolic endpoints (38–40). However, limited information is currently available regarding the evaluation of cranberries on cognitive performance, with the only previous study exploring the impact of cranberry intake on cognitive performance reporting no cognitive benefit of a cranberry juice consumption for 6 weeks in older adults with normal cognitive functions (41).
Here we report upon a single-centre, 12-week, double-blind, placebo-controlled parallel intervention study in which the impact of a freeze-dried cranberry powder intervention (equivalent of one small bowl) was examined in the context of cognitive health in healthy older adults (50–80 years). Cognitive health was determined using a battery of cognitive tests in combination with comprehensive biochemical and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) assessments.
Materials and Methods
Study Participants
Healthy male and female older adults were recruited in this study through online recruitment databases (Join Dementia Research)1; existing research databases within the Norwich Medical School, University of East Anglia, where participants had previously consented to be contacted about research studies; and community-based advertising (e.g., recruitment posters, leaflets, talks). Participants were first pre-screened over the telephone for eligibility for the study using a screening questionnaire and were invited if they were aged between 50 and 80 years and presented with no subjective memory complaints as assessed by the Cognitive Change Index (CCI) questionnaire (42). Married couples who lived together were particularly targetted to reduce the variability in background diet patterns; however, participants were permitted to take part in the study on their own.
Individuals were excluded if they had any of the following condition: a diagnosis of any form of dementia or significant neurological condition, significant memory complaints, uncontrolled blood pressure, currently smoking or ceased smoking less than 6 months prior to enrolment, clinically diagnosed with psychiatric disorder, or currently on antidepressant or antipsychotic medication, diagnosed with a gastrointestinal disorder, or currently on any medication that alters the function of the gastrointestinal tract, chronic fatigue syndrome, liver disease, diabetes mellitus, or gall bladder abnormalities including gall bladder removal, history or MRI evidence of brain damage, including significant trauma, stroke, learning difficulties or developmental disorders, or a previous loss of consciousness for more than 24 h. In addition, participants were not eligible for the study if they were prescribed anticoagulant medicine such as warfarin, due to potential interactions with the active cranberry powder. Other exclusion criteria were restrictive or unbalanced diet and excessive alcohol consumption (>15 units/week). Participants were also excluded if they were identified as having a high flavonoid intake defined as >15 portions of flavonoid rich foods (fruit, vegetables, tea and coffee, fruit juice, dark chocolate, and cocoa) per day during the telephone screening (43).
For MRI measures, participants were not eligible to undergo the neuroimaging component of the study if they had a cardiac pacemaker, any metal surgical implants that would not be safe within the MRI machine, or experienced claustrophobia in small spaces. If participants were unable to undergo the neuroimaging component of the study, they were still able to take part in the other components of the study.
Study Design
A single-centre, 12-week randomised, double-blind placebo-controlled parallel study design protocol was performed. Participants attended three visits in total: a screening visit (V0), a pre-intervention baseline visit (V1), and a follow-up visit at the end of the intervention (V2).
The screening visit (V0) involved obtaining informed consent, followed by collecting a fasted (>10 h) morning blood and urine sample, physical measurements (height, weight, and blood pressure). Basal blood pressure and heart rate was first collected after participants had been lying supine for 5 min, and then collected again upon standing. Participants were then provided with a standardised breakfast and underwent global cognitive screening using the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination III (ACE-III). Participants were excluded if they scored <88 on the ACE-III, or had abnormal blood biochemistry, blood pressure or urine results indicative of a potential exclusion condition (e.g., diabetes, uncontrolled hypertension).
If participants passed the screening visit, they were invited to a pre-intervention baseline visit (V1). Following a standardised breakfast and other measures, as described above, participants completed a longer cognitive battery (2.5 h) including measures of processing speed, working memory, episodic memory and spatial navigation, and other experimental and perception tests to be published elsewhere, with a half hour break partway through testing to avoid fatigue. A 30-min MRI scan was also conducted either during this visit or within the same week of this visit. At the end of the baseline V1 visit, participants were provided with sachets of study powder, assigned to them using a computer-generated algorithm. The intervention was provided in the form of sachets (4.5 g each) of freeze-dried cranberry powder (Cranberry Institute, United States) designed to be incorporated into food and beverages (see Supplementary Table 1 for the product specifications for the freeze-dried cranberry powder). Participants were instructed to take two sachets per day, one in the morning and one in the evening, to maximise the physiological impact based on current understanding of bioavailability (44). The daily dosage of cranberry powder was roughly equivalent to consuming one cup or 100 g of fresh cranberries. This dosage was calculated to provide 281 mg proanthocyanidins, with increase of 20 mg flavonols and 59 mg anthocyanins per day (see Supplementary Methods and Supplementary Table 2). The placebo powder was designed to match the active cranberry powder for taste, colour, fructose, total sugar and calories and contained a blend of water, maltodextrin (CPC Maltrin M-180), citric acid, artificial cranberry flavour (Lorann oils), fructose, red colour (Lorann oils) and grape shade (Esco Foods) that had been freeze-dried. Participants were asked to return all remaining cranberry sachets at the end of the 12-week treatment period, with the number of leftover sachets being taken as one measure of compliance. Adherence to treatment was also determined by measuring total plasma (poly)phenol metabolites concentration as described previously thereafter.
Apart from the addition of the study powder, participants were asked not to modify their dietary intake in any further way, including any changes to their caloric intake. However, participants were asked to refrain from consuming any other non-essential supplements that could have a significant impact on the outcome measures for the duration of the study. Participants were also asked to fill in a validated, semi-quantitative SCG FFQ (version 6.6) (45) to account for their background diet.
The follow-up visit V2 was scheduled exactly 12 weeks following the baseline visit at the end of the intervention and was identical in procedures to the baseline visit. Fasted blood and urine samples were collected in addition to physical measurements, followed by the cognitive battery and MRI.
Cognitive Assessment
Participants completed all of the following cognitive tests at the baseline and follow-up visits. The only exception to this was the ACE-III, which was conducted at the screening visit in the first instance to assess eligibility for the study (i.e., total score >88) and then repeated at the follow-up visit.
Global cognition was assessed using the ACE-III questionnaire, which covers domains including attention and orientation, memory, fluency, language and visuospatial functions (46). Executive functions and working memory were measured by using the Trail Making Test (TMT) (47), a short test of processing speed, attention, and set-shifting, and the Digit Span (DS) test, a subtest from the Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale–third edition (WAIS III) that assesses attention and short-term memory. DS is composed of two tasks administered independently of each other: “digits forward” and digits backward. For each “digits forward” item, participants are presented with a series of digits in increasing length and must immediately repeat them to the examiner in the same order as presented. For digits backward, the participant is required to repeat the number sequence in the reverse order. A composite executive function score was also calculated out of the ACE-III Category Fluency score (/7), the DS Backwards Raw Score (/14), and the Scaled Trails B from the TMT based on previously published normative data (48).
Memory was evaluated by using the Rey Complex Figure Test (RCF), a short measure of visual memory and visuospatial constructional ability (49). This study included the copy and 3-min recall trials of the test. A measure of verbal episodic memory was also measured using the delayed recall of the name and address on the ACE-III (score out of 7).
The Supermarket Test is a computer- and tablet-based assessment of spatial orientation that uses an ecological shopping environment (50). It includes a path integration test and measures (1) egocentric orientation, (2) short-term spatial memory, (3) heading direction, and (4) central (vs. boundary) based navigation preferences.
All tests were conducted using pen and paper, with the exception of the Supermarket Test which was administered using an Apple iPad.
Magnetic Resonance Imaging
Data Acquisition
Magnetic resonance imaging scans were conducted in all eligible and willing participants at baseline and end of intervention and took approximately 30 min. In order to monitor structural brain information across the study, a T1-weighted 3D gradient-echo MR sequence was conducted at each testing visit. A T2-weighted fluid attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) scan was also conducted during the study visits. Arterial spin labelling (ASL) has previously been used to monitor changes in cerebral blood flow (CBF) in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment patients (51, 52).
All data were acquired on a 3 Tesla Discovery 750 w wide bore MR system (GE Healthcare, Milwaukee, WI, United States) with a 12-channel phased-array head coil for signal reception. After localisers, T1-weighted structural data were acquired using a 3D inversion-recovery fast spoiled gradient recalled echo (IR-FSPGR) sequence with repetition time (TR) = 7.7 ms; echo time (TE) = 3.1 ms; inversion time = 400 ms; field-of-view = 256 mm × 256 mm; acquired matrix = 256 × 256; 200 sagittal sections of 1 mm thickness; flip angle = 11°; and ASSET acceleration factor = 2 in the phase-encoding direction. Furthermore, a 3D T2-weighted FLAIR (T2w FLAIR) sequence was prescribed as follows: TR = 4800 ms; TE = 129 ms; inversion time = 1462 ms; field-of-view = 256 mm × 256 mm; acquired matrix = 256 × 256; 182 sagittal sections of 1 mm thickness; flip angle = 90°; an ARC acceleration factor of 2 in the phase-encoding direction; and a “HyperSense” compressed sensing subsampling factor of 2. The ASL scan consisted of a 3D spiral pseudo-continuous ASL (pCASL) acquisition with the following parameters: TE = 10.7 ms, TR = 4854 ms, 8 spiral interleaves with 512 sample points, field-of-view = 240 mm × 240 mm × 128 mm with a reconstructed resolution of 1.9 mm × 1.9 mm × 4 mm; post-label delay = 1500 ms, number of excitations = 3. Before analyses, all participant scans were visually inspected for significant head movements and artefacts.
Image Analysis
Voxel-Based Morphometry (VBM) was used on whole-brain T1-weighted scans using the VBM package in FSL (FMRIB Software Library, Oxford, United Kingdom) to confirm that there were no grey matter structural differences between the cranberry and placebo groups (53).
White matter hyperintensities (WMH) were rated using Multi-image Analysis GUI (Mango version 4.1, Research Imaging Institute, UTHSCSA, San Antonio, TX, United States) by one rater (EF). A well-established rating scale developed by Fazekas et al. (54) was used to qualitatively rate WMH in periventricular (PWMH) and deep (DWMH) regions using FLAIR images. WMH in the periventricular areas was rated as 0 = absent, 1 = “caps” or pencil-thin lining, 2 = smooth “halo,” or 3 = irregular, whereas DWMH were rated as 0 = absent, 1 = punctate foci, 2 = beginning confluence of foci, or 3 = large confluent areas.
For regional perfusion (ASL), equilibrium magnetisation (M0) and perfusion-weighted images were calculated in-line on the scanner workstation. All further analyses were performed using a processing pipeline written in bash and Python (v3.6, Python Software Foundation),2 which was run on the ADA high-performance computing cluster at the University of East Anglia. The pipeline closely resembled that used for the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI)3 ASL sub-study, substituting FastSurfer for brain segmentation instead of FreeSurfer (55). In brief, M0 and perfusion-weighted images were scaled and used to calculate CBF maps in physical units of arterial water density (mL/min/100 g). T1-weighted data were then segmented using FSL’s FAST algorithm and the derived grey matter probability maps were used to register the ASL perfusion-weighted images to T1 space—via FSL’s FLIRT algorithm. ROIs from the FastSurfer segmentation were then used to determine ROI-wise CBF statistics: minimum maximum, mean, median, and standard deviation.
To visualise voxel-wise differences between groups, we performed higher-level general linear model analysis using FSL’s “randomise” permutation-testing tool. To facilitate this, structural T1-weighted images for all participants were first non-linearly registered to the Montreal Neurological Institute standard brain using FNIRT and the resulting transformation was applied to the ASL data. Difference images were then generated for each individual through subtraction of the baseline ASL scan from the post-intervention scan. An unpaired t-test was then performed on these data using “randomise” with threshold-free cluster enhancement to compare changes in perfusion between the cranberry and placebo groups.
Biological Samples and (poly)Phenol Metabolites Analyses
A fasted blood sample was taken at the screening assessment visit and sent to the accredited pathology laboratories at the Norfolk & Norwich University Hospital (NNUH) for determination of markers of general health. Further blood samples were collected at baseline and follow-up in EDTA, SST and heparin vacutainer tubes (Becton-Dickinson, United Kingdom) for assessment of circulating metabolites, APOE genotype and blood biochemistry. Samples were immediately processed for serum/plasma, aliquoted and stored at −80°C until analysis. BDNF levels in plasma were assessed by ELISA (R&D Systems, United Kingdom) following the manufacturers’ instructions.
Plasma extraction of polyphenol metabolites was performed using microelution solid phase extraction (μSPE) according to validated protocols, with some modifications (56, 57). Briefly, plasma samples (350 μl) were diluted (1:1) with phosphoric acid 4% to reduce phenolic-protein interactions. Each sample (600 μl) was loaded on a 96 well μSPE plate, washed with water (200 μl) and 0.2% acetic acid (200 μl) and finally eluted with methanol (60 μl). The 96 well collection plates were directly put in the UHPLC autosampler for immediate analysis. Plasma samples were analysed through UHPLC DIONEX Ultimate 3000 fitted with a TSQ Vantage Triple Quadrupole Mass Spectrometer (Thermo Fisher Scientific Inc., San Jose, CA, United States) equipped with a heated-electrospray ionisation source (H-ESI-II; Thermo Fisher Scientific Inc.). Separations were performed with a Kinetex EVO C18 (100 mm × 2.1 mm), 2.6 μm particle size (Phenomenex). For UHPLC, mobile phase A was water containing 0.01% formic acid and mobile phase B was acetonitrile containing 0.01% formic acid. The gradient started with 5% B, keeping isocratic conditions for 0.5 min, reaching 95% B at 7 min, followed by 1 min at 95% B and then 4 min at the start conditions to re-equilibrate the column. The flow rate was set at 0.4 ml/min, the injection volume was 5 μl, and the column was thermostated at 40°C. The MS worked in negative ionisation mode with capillary temperature at 270°C, while the source was at 300°C. The sheath gas flow was 60 units, while auxiliary gas pressure was set to 10 units. The source voltage was 3 kV. Ultra-high-purity argon gas was used for collision-induced dissociation (CID). Compounds were monitored in selective reaction monitoring (SRM) mode, and characteristic MS conditions (S-lens RF amplitude voltage and collision energy) were optimised for each compound. Chromatograms, mass spectral data and data processing were performed using Xcalibur software 2.1 (Thermo Fisher Scientific Inc.). Quantification was performed with calibration curves of standards, when available; when not available, metabolites were quantified with the most structurally similar compound. Due to failure in collecting follow-up plasma, 14 volunteers were not considered for the calculations of plasma metabolite content.
Statistical Analyses
Data were analysed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS; v28.0), applying standard statistical thresholds (p < 0.05) and were tested for normality using the Shapiro–Wilk test. Mann–Whitney U Independent-Samples tests were employed to detect differences between demographic, anthropometric and biochemical data at baseline. One-way ANCOVA’s were used to detect baseline differences in cognition between groups controlling for age, education and gender. The impact of treatment on the cognitive outcomes of interest was established using mixed linear model with time and treatment as independent variables and with age, education and gender entered as covariates. Whole-brain differences in grey matter intensities were analysed between cranberry and placebo groups at baseline and follow-up with age added as a covariate. Periventricular and deep WMH were compared between cranberry and placebo groups at baseline and follow-up using ANCOVAs with age added as a covariate. Mean regional perfusion derived from ASL scans were analysed using mixed linear modelling with age entered as a covariate to determine and group × time interactions. Pearson correlations between significant cognition and regional perfusion at follow-up were also conducted.
Plasma metabolites, anthropometric and biochemical measures were compared between groups at baseline using non-parametric Mann-Whitney U-test analysis. Mixed linear modelling was used to detect within group differences between baseline and follow-up, as well as group × time interactions. Correlations analyses between plasma (poly)phenol metabolites and RCF delayed score along with regional blood perfusion from MRI ASL in regions found to be impacted by the cranberry intervention were conducted using non-parametric Spearman rank order. Unless otherwise stated, all results are presented as means (SD).
Results
Study Participants
Figure 1 shows a Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) flow diagram. Seventy participants were consented into the study. Of these participants, seven of them did not pass screening and three declined further participation between the screening and baseline visits. The final study population consisted of 60 participants who attended the baseline visit and commenced the intervention. All participants reported being in good health, not consuming any food supplements or medications that would interfere with the tested product. No participants discontinued the intervention or were withdrawn after they had attended the baseline visit, resulting in 60 participants completing the follow-up visit. There were no serious adverse events or protocol deviations reported during the study; however, there were two cases of participants experiencing dental changes, which were documented. Of the participants who were eligible for the intervention, 29 participants were randomised into the active cranberry treatment group and 31 into the control groups. All baseline values were within the physiological range and groups did not differ in age, education, distribution of gender or global cognitive performance at screening (Table 1).
TABLE 1.
Characteristics | Cranberry | Placebo | Sig. (p) | ||
N | 29 | 31 | |||
Gender (M/F) | 12/17 | 13/18 | 0.965 | ||
| |||||
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||
| |||||
Age (years) | 65.86 | 5.51 | 65.32 | 4.91 | 0.929 |
Education | 14.38 | 2.60 | 14.61 | 3.01 | 0.610 |
ACE-III | 96.58 | 2.39 | 96.10 | 2.80 | 0.644 |
(Chi-Squared and Mann–Whitney U Independent-Samples Test).
Overall, compliance was excellent based on returned empty sachets or unused study products and based on increased plasma total concentration of (poly)phenol metabolites. Specifically, plasma total concentration of (poly)phenol metabolites increased by 1.82 ± 0.57 μM in the cranberry group. No increase in plasma total (poly)phenol concentration was observed in the placebo group (Supplementary Figure 1).
Food-frequency data indicated there was no difference between the placebo and the cranberry groups on the macronutrient content of their background diets. However, a significant difference was observed (p = 0.02) in vitamin D concentration between the two groups, with the placebo group having a lower concentration (Supplementary Table 3). Although not reaching significance, the cranberry group presented with increased concentration of caffeine intake (p = 0.09). Flavonoid content was similar between the two groups with an average intake of 228 mg/day in the placebo group and 217 mg/day in the cranberry group (Supplementary Table 3).
Cognitive Performance
There were no differences between cranberry and placebo groups at baseline on the ACE-III total score or on the sub-scores (Attention and Orientation, Memory, Fluency, Language, Address Delayed Recall and Category Fluency). No difference at baseline was also observed for RCF, Digits spans backward, Trail-making test A–B Scaled Score, or for the composite executive function score but there was a significant difference at baseline on visuospatial performance (p = 0.008, Table 2).
TABLE 2.
Baseline |
Follow-up |
Group × Time interaction |
||||||
Measures | Treatment | M | SD | Sig. (p) | M | SD | Sig. (p) | |
ACE-III | Attention | Cranberry | 17.65 | 0.72 | 0.219 | 17.66 | 0.55 | 0.467 |
Placebo | 17.35 | 1.11 | 17.60 | 0.62 | ||||
Memory | Cranberry | 24.97 | 1.18 | 0.726 | 25.10 | 1.05 | 0.498 | |
Placebo | 24.84 | 1.70 | 25.67 | 1.95 | ||||
Fluency | Cranberry | 12.45 | 1.53 | 0.512 | 13.24 | 0.91 | 0.164 | |
Placebo | 12.26 | 1.44 | 12.43 | 1.48 | ||||
Language | Cranberry | 25.79 | 0.49 | 0.305 | 25.76 | 0.51 | 0.401 | |
Placebo | 25.58 | 0.54 | 25.83 | 0.46 | ||||
Visuospatial | Cranberry | 15.62 | 0.73 | 0.008 | 15.76 | 0.51 | 0.120 | |
Placebo | 15.97 | 0.18 | 15.83 | 0.38 | ||||
Address delayed recall | Cranberry | 6.10 | 1.05 | 0.466 | 6.34 | 0.81 | 0.332 | |
Placebo | 6.19 | 1.28 | 6.07 | 1.46 | ||||
Category fluency | Cranberry | 6.34 | 1.17 | 0.759 | 6.66 | 0.61 | 0.431 | |
Placebo | 6.32 | 0.91 | 6.43 | 0.68 | ||||
RCF | Copy score | Cranberry | 34.52 | 2.61 | 0.994 | 35.34 | 1.05 | 0.092 |
Placebo | 35.00 | 1.29 | 34.97 | 1.28 | ||||
Delayed recall score | Cranberry | 18.59 | 7.67 | 0.416 | 23.41 | 5.96 | 0.028 | |
Placebo | 20.53 | 5.92 | 22.25 | 6.06 | ||||
Digit span | Forward raw score | Cranberry | 11.28 | 2.28 | 0.437 | 11.41 | 2.01 | 0.309 |
Placebo | 10.84 | 2.28 | 11.39 | 2.50 | ||||
Backward raw score | Cranberry | 7.76 | 2.18 | 0.781 | 7.86 | 2.5 | 0.165 | |
Placebo | 7.58 | 2.03 | 8.30 | 2.55 | ||||
TMT | A–B | Cranberry | 37.41 | 21.84 | 0.416 | 35.93 | 16.22 | 0.639 |
Placebo | 33.29 | 16.89 | 34.14 | 13.73 | ||||
B Scaled | Cranberry | 14.83 | 1.79 | 0.756 | 15.03 | 1.66 | 0.127 | |
Placebo | 15.13 | 1.73 | 15.37 | 1.97 | ||||
Executive composite score | Cranberry | 28.93 | 3.50 | 0.964 | 29.34 | 3.73 | 0.430 | |
Placebo | 29.03 | 3.41 | 29.93 | 3.45 |
ACE III, Addenbrooke’s cognitive examination III; RCF, Rey complex figure test; TMT, trail making test. Significant values p < 0.05 are in bold.
At follow-up, a significant group × time interaction [F(1, 55) = 5.060; p = 0.028] was observed in performance of the RCF test delayed recall such that the cranberry group showed a significant improvement in performance between baseline and follow-up compared to the placebo group. Linear mixed modelling to detect group × time interactions between the groups between baseline and follow-up did not reveal any differential impact of the intervention on groups pre- to post-treatment for the copy score nor the other cognitive tests (p > 0.05) (Table 2).
No significant differences were detected between groups on the egocentric, allocentric error or allocentric heading subtotals and totals of the Supermarket Test (Table 3). When the linear mixed modelling was run on these subtotals and totals, no significant group × time interactions were found (p > 0.05 in all cases).
TABLE 3.
Measures | Baseline |
Follow-up |
Group × Time interaction |
|||||
Treatment | Mean | SD | Sig. (p) | Mean | SD | Sig. (p) | ||
Supermarket test | Egocentric score section 1 | Cranberry | 3.32 | 1.84 | 0.383 | 3.76 | 1.96 | 0.906 |
Placebo | 3.67 | 1.76 | 3.92 | 1.91 | ||||
Egocentric section 2 | Cranberry | 5.28 | 1.95 | 0.917 | 5.68 | 1.65 | 0.936 | |
Placebo | 5.04 | 2.01 | 5.58 | 1.79 | ||||
Egocentric total | Cranberry | 8.60 | 3.29 | 0.656 | 9.44 | 3.29 | 0.885 | |
Placebo | 8.71 | 3.36 | 9.50 | 3.27 | ||||
Allocentric error section 1 | Cranberry | 11.72 | 4.35 | 0.324 | 12.54 | 8.31 | 0.267 | |
Placebo | 13.00 | 4.94 | 11.96 | 5.81 | ||||
Allocentric error section 2 | Cranberry | 14.28 | 3.00 | 0.164 | 15.47 | 10.54 | 0.481 | |
Placebo | 14.11 | 4.55 | 13.27 | 4.76 | ||||
Allocentric error total | Cranberry | 13.00 | 3.14 | 0.964 | 13.98 | 8.88 | 0.325 | |
Placebo | 13.55 | 4.47 | 12.70 | 4.70 | ||||
Allocentric heading section 1 | Cranberry | 5.64 | 1.50 | 0.562 | 5.92 | 1.80 | 0.417 | |
Placebo | 5.33 | 1.66 | 6.00 | 1.25 | ||||
Allocentric heading section 2 | Cranberry | 5.60 | 1.50 | 0.489 | 5.84 | 1.34 | 0.697 | |
Placebo | 5.38 | 1.41 | 5.79 | 1.29 | ||||
Allocentric heading total | Cranberry | 11.24 | 2.67 | 0.386 | 11.76 | 8.44 | 0.413 | |
Placebo | 10.71 | 2.46 | 11.79 | 2.23 |
Magnetic Resonance Imaging
Among our study population, 47 participants were eligible and underwent the neuroimaging component of the study (26 in the cranberry group and 21 in the placebo group). Due to COVID-19 restrictions and reduced capacity of hospital facilities, 10 follow-up scans could not be conducted. An additional five follow-up scans could not be scheduled due to participants (n = 1) or scanning facilities (n = 4) being unavailable during the critical follow-up time window. Additionally, ASL data for 2 baseline scans and 1 follow-up scan were not usable due to severe motion artefacts.
There were no differences in whole brain grey matter intensity between cranberry and placebo groups found at either baseline or follow-up scans as assessed by the VBM package in FSL with a p threshold set at 0.05 with age added as a covariate. No statistically significant differences were observed between groups in periventricular WMH between the cranberry and placebo groups at baseline (p = 0.688) or follow-up (p = 0.833), or for deep WMH at baseline (p = 0.693) or follow-up (p = 0.723) in ANCOVA’s with age added as a covariate. Two participants were rated as “3” for periventricular WMH (i.e., “irregular”), with the remaining participants being rated between 0–2.
Figures 2A–D depict representative magnetic resonance imaging data. Mean regional CBF from ASL for the cranberry and placebo groups at baseline and follow-up are summarised in Supplementary Table 4. No significant differences in regional perfusion between cranberry and placebo groups were detected at baseline (p > 0.05). ASL group analysis with FSL’s “randomise” tool indicated voxels covering similar volumes to the ROI’s indicated in the ROI-wise analyses (Figure 2E). Mixed linear modelling controlling for age and education detected significant group × time interactions for the right caudate [F(1, 29.275) = 4.207, p = 0.049], right accumbens area [F(1, 31.744) = 4.916, p = 0.034], and right entorhinal cortex [F(1, 30.558) = 5.202, p = 0.030]. All models involved an increase in perfusion between baseline and follow-up in the cranberry group compared to a relative decrease in perfusion over time in the placebo group (Figure 2F).
As a significant group × time interaction was found for the delayed recall of the RCF, a correlation analysis was performed between the follow-up RCF delay scores and follow-up regional perfusion data, however, no significant correlation was found for either group (p > 0.05).
Biological Samples and Polyphenol Metabolites Analyses
There was a group × time effect on cholesterol, such that total, low-density lipoprotein (LDL) but not high-density lipoprotein (HDL) cholesterol decreased over the 12 weeks in cranberry but not in the placebo group [LDL; F(1, 40) = 4.100; p = 0.048]. Although not reaching significance, a trend toward a decrease in total cholesterol was also observed in the cranberry group [F(1, 42) = 4.073; p = 0.050] when compared to the placebo group (Table 4).
TABLE 4.
Baseline |
Follow-up |
Group × Time interaction |
||||||
Measures | Treatment | Mean | SD | Sig. (p) | Mean | SD | Sig. (p) | |
Biochemistry | Cholesterol (mmol/L) | Cranberry | 5.6 | 1.1 | 0.98 | 5.4 | 1.2 | 0.050 |
Placebo | 5.5 | 1.0 | 5.4 | 1.1 | ||||
HDL cholesterol (mmol/L) | Cranberry | 1.7 | 0.4 | 0.98 | 1.7 | 0.3 | 0.428 | |
Placebo | 1.6 | 0.4 | 1.6 | 0.4 | ||||
LDL cholesterol (mmol/L) | Cranberry | 3.5 | 1.0 | 0.81 | 3.2 | 1.0 | 0.048 | |
Placebo | 3.4 | 0.9 | 3.3 | 1.2 | ||||
Triglyceride (mmol/L) | Cranberry | 1.0 | 0.4 | 0.96 | 1.1 | 0.4 | 0.518 | |
Placebo | 1.2 | 0.6 | 1.1 | 0.5 | ||||
Glucose fasting (mmol/L) | Cranberry | 4.7 | 0.4 | 0.98 | 4.6 | 0.4 | 0.963 | |
Placebo | 4.8 | 0.5 | 4.7 | 0.5 | ||||
ALT (U/L) | Cranberry | 17.8 | 5.2 | 0.90 | 18.6 | 7.6 | 0.812 | |
Placebo | 17.3 | 8.5 | 17.6 | 6.5 | ||||
AST (U/L) | Cranberry | 22.4 | 4.6 | 0.83 | 22.8 | 6.8 | 0.459 | |
Placebo | 24.6 | 3.6 | 22.4 | 5.1 | ||||
Alkaline phosphatase (U/L) | Cranberry | 69.3 | 17.7 | 0.24 | 69.2 | 16.1 | 0.860 | |
Placebo | 73.8 | 22.0 | 73.0 | 18.0 | ||||
Creatinine (μmol/L) | Cranberry | 72.9 | 12.4 | 0.96 | 73.4 | 10.5 | 0.609 | |
Placebo | 73.1 | 14.0 | 71.3 | 11.6 | ||||
Total bilirubin (μmol/L) | Cranberry | 11.2 | 4.1 | 0.45 | 10.3 | 3.8 | 0.110 | |
Placebo | 14.1 | 4.9 | 15.1 | 6.1 | ||||
Total protein (g/L) | Cranberry | 71.0 | 3.8 | 0.81 | 70.7 | 4.0 | 0.669 | |
Placebo | 71.9 | 3.5 | 72.0 | 3.3 | ||||
Albumin (g/L) | Cranberry | 40.6 | 2.3 | 0.86 | 40.4 | 3.1 | 0.133 | |
Placebo | 40.0 | 2.3 | 40.8 | 2.2 | ||||
Globulin (g/L) | Cranberry | 30.4 | 3.8 | 0.71 | 30.2 | 3.7 | 0.803 | |
Placebo | 31.8 | 3.1 | 31.3 | 3.2 | ||||
Urea (mmol/L) | Cranberry | 5.1 | 1.0 | 0.96 | 5.0 | 1.1 | 0.862 | |
Placebo | 4.9 | 1.0 | 4.7 | 1.2 | ||||
Calcium (mmol/L) | Cranberry | 2.3 | 0.1 | 0.98 | 2.3 | 0.1 | 0.198 | |
Placebo | 2.4 | 0.1 | 2.4 | 0.1 | ||||
Adjusted calcium (mmol/L) | Cranberry | 2.4 | 0.1 | >0.99 | 2.4 | 0.1 | 0.757 | |
Placebo | 2.4 | 0.1 | 2.4 | 0.1 | ||||
Phosphate (mmol/L) | Cranberry | 1.0 | 0.2 | >0.99 | 1.0 | 0.2 | 0.947 | |
Placebo | 1.0 | 0.2 | 1.0 | 0.2 | ||||
Bicarbonate (mmol/L) | Cranberry | 27.0 | 3.2 | 0.77 | 26.7 | 2.2 | 0.341 | |
Placebo | 25.9 | 2.2 | 26.1 | 1.9 | ||||
Na (mmol/L) | Cranberry | 139.5 | 2.3 | 0.19 | 139.7 | 2.4 | 0.496 | |
Placebo | 134.5 | 2.9 | 138.0 | 2.2 | ||||
K (mmol/L) | Cranberry | 4.5 | 0.3 | >0.99 | 4.4 | 0.2 | 0.996 | |
Placebo | 4.5 | 0.3 | 4.5 | 0.4 | ||||
Blood pressure and heart rate | Diastolic (mm Hg) | Cranberry | 79.3 | 9.9 | 0.60 | 81.2 | 10.7 | 0.786 |
Placebo | 81.3 | 11.2 | 82.9 | 10.5 | ||||
Systolic (mm Hg) | Cranberry | 139.0 | 17.4 | 0.85 | 134.9 | 18.5 | 0.521 | |
Placebo | 139.7 | 18.3 | 132 | 13.6 | ||||
Heart rate (bpm) | Cranberry | 62.7 | 8.6 | 0.61 | 61.5 | 8.1 | 0.209 | |
Placebo | 61.6 | 8.2 | 62.9 | 10.2 | ||||
Anthropometry | BMI (Kg/m2) | Cranberry | 24.9 | 4.0 | 0.93 | 24.1 | 3.0 | 0.305 |
Placebo | 25.0 | 5.9 | 25.7 | 3.9 | ||||
Weight (Kg) | Cranberry | 71.5 | 15.2 | 0.88 | 68.5 | 11.8 | 0.271 | |
Placebo | 72.1 | 18.5 | 73.7 | 13.8 |
ALT, alanine transaminase; AST, aspartate transaminase; BMI, body mass index; HDL, high-density lipoprotein; K, potassium; LDL, low-density lipoprotein; Na, sodium. Significant values p < 0.05 are in bold.
Investigating our data further, we observed a strong interindividual variability with male participants possibly experiencing a greater benefit from the cranberry intake. Indeed, a steep but not significant decrease in BMI (from 27 ± 4.2 to 24.5 ± 2.5 kg/m2) in male participants was observed following 12 weeks of cranberry intake. This decrease also applied to fasting blood glucose and systolic blood pressure along with an increase in HDL cholesterol (Supplementary Figure 2).
There was no significant difference in BDNF concentration following cranberry intake for 12 weeks when compared to placebo (p = 0.7119) although concentrations were higher than in the placebo group at follow up (Supplementary Figure 3). High concentrations of BDNF in the cranberry group at baseline may be explained by higher concentration of caffeine intake in this group (Supplementary Table 2). BDNF at the follow-up visit did not correlate significantly with CBF in any treatment group (p > 0.05).
There were no significant differences in plasma total (poly)phenol metabolites at baseline, apart from the flavonol kaempferol-3-glucuronide which presented higher concentration in the placebo group [Mann Whitney-U (1, 43) = 351.00, p = 0.026; Table 5]. At follow-up, there were significant increases in 4-methylcatechol-sulphate, hippuric acid, caffeic acid and total (poly)phenol metabolites in the cranberry group when compared to the placebo (Table 5). No significant correlation between circulating (poly)phenol metabolites, RCF scores or regional blood perfusion at follow-up were observed (p > 0.05), indicating an indirect effect of the cranberry treatment on cognitive performance and brain perfusion.
TABLE 5.
Metabolite | Group | Baseline (μ mol/L) |
Baseline difference |
Follow-up (μ mol/L) |
Group × Time |
||
M | SD | p | M | SD | p | ||
Kaempferol-3-glucuronide (Flavonols) | Cranberry | 0.017 | 0.018 | 0.026 | 0.019 | 0.020 | 0.274 |
Placebo | 0.033 | 0.034 | 0.028 | 0.022 | |||
4-Methylcatechol-sulphate (Catechols) | Cranberry | 0.033 | 0.016 | 0.617 | 0.042 | 0.015 | 0.003 |
Placebo | 0.032 | 0.012 | 0.024 | 0.010 | |||
4-Hydroxybenzaldehyde (Benzaldehydes) | Cranberry | 0.013 | 0.006 | 0.609 | 0.019 | 0.013 | 0.307 |
Placebo | 0.013 | 0.006 | 0.018 | 0.009 | |||
Hippuric acid | Cranberry | 2.82 | 1.58 | 0.892 | 4.29 | 1.94 | 0.002 |
Placebo | 2.76 | 1.12 | 2.53 | 1.78 | |||
4-Hydroxyhippuric acid | Cranberry | 0.039 | 0.020 | 0.540 | 0.043 | 0.025 | 0.508 |
Placebo | 0.043 | 0.023 | 0.041 | 0.025 | |||
4-Hydroxybenzoic acid | Cranberry | 0.024 | 0.022 | 0.056 | 0.027 | 0.012 | 0.358 |
Placebo | 0.032 | 0.079 | 0.038 | 0.054 | |||
Benzoic acid-4-sulphate | Cranberry | 0.023 | 0.023 | 0.107 | 0.028 | 0.024 | 0.192 |
Placebo | 0.015 | 0.009 | 0.014 | 0.009 | |||
Benzoic acid-3-sulphate | Cranberry | 0.026 | 0.038 | 0.609 | 0.020 | 0.021 | 0.588 |
Placebo | 0.015 | 0.014 | 0.012 | 0.011 | |||
Caffeic acid (3′,4′-Dihydroxycinnamic acid) | Cranberry | 0.021 | 0.012 | 0.856 | 0.032 | 0.014 | 0.005 |
Placebo | 0.020 | 0.008 | 0.019 | 0.013 | |||
Ferulic acid-4-glucuronide | Cranberry | 0.018 | 0.020 | 0.927 | 0.025 | 0.015 | 0.061 |
Placebo | 0.016 | 0.015 | 0.012 | 0.010 | |||
3-Hydroxyphenylacetic acid (Phenylacetic acids) | Cranberry | 0.078 | 0.041 | 0.496 | 0.087 | 0.042 | 0.811 |
Placebo | 0.068 | 0.033 | 0.074 | 0.028 | |||
3-(3′-Hydroxyphenyl)propanoic acid (Phenylpropanoic acids) | Cranberry | 0.051 | 0.043 | 0.115 | 0.039 | 0.036 | 0.166 |
Placebo | 0.077 | 0.054 | 0.041 | 0.049 | |||
5-(Phenyl)-γ-valerolactone-methoxy-glucuronide (3′,4′) (Phenyl-γ-valerolactones) | Cranberry | 0.018 | 0.011 | 0.751 | 0.017 | 0.010 | 0.190 |
Placebo | 0.017 | 0.009 | 0.013 | 0.008 | |||
Total metabolites | Cranberry | 3.18 | 1.68 | 0.820 | 4.69 | 2.01 | 0.002 |
Placebo | 3.14 | 1.20 | 2.87 | 0.321 |
Significance of baseline group differences was determined by non-parametric Mann-Whitney U independent samples tests, and main effects of time within group between baseline and follow-up along with interactions between group and time was determined using linear mixed modelling. Significant values p < 0.05 are in bold.
No participants were found to carry two copies of the APOE-4 mutation. Furthermore, there were no differences between the cranberry and placebo groups for distribution of APOE genetic types, χ2 (3) = 2.60, p = 0.457 (Table 6).
TABLE 6.
APOE genotype | Cranberry | Placebo | Total |
E2/E3 | 3 | 4 | 7 (11.67%) |
E2/E4 | 0 | 2 | 2 (3.33%) |
E3/E3 | 21 | 22 | 43 (71.67%) |
E3/E4 | 5 | 3 | 8 (13.33%) |
APOE, Apolipoprotein E.
Discussion
This investigation reports the effect of long-term cranberry supplementation (12-week placebo-controlled intervention) upon cognitive performance and brain health. Daily supplementation with freeze-dried cranberry extract (equivalent to one cup of fresh cranberries) led to significant improvements in episodic memory performance, which coincided with increased perfusion of key neural areas which support cognition in older adults. Our results are in direct contrast to a previously conducted clinical investigation in which no significant change in memory performance was established following cranberry intake (41). The discordance between these results likely relates to experimental inconsistencies such as duration of the intake (6 vs. 12 weeks) or product formulation (cranberry juice vs. freeze-dried whole cranberry powder).
Surprisingly, the cranberry intervention had no further impact upon additional neurocognitive domains. Working memory and executive functioning (including the executive functioning composite score) remained unaltered despite the contrary being reported by others investigating flavonoid-rich juices e.g., blueberry, Concord grape juice and orange juice (21). This could in part relate to the distinct (poly)phenolic composition of each distinct intervention. Conversely, this may be a product of cognitive test choice, with tests such as the DS backward and trail making A and B believed to be less sensitive in detecting changes in executive functioning in non-cognitively impaired adults (58). In agreement with our results, a longer-term 12-week intervention with wild blueberry juice (20) led to similar improvements upon episodic memory performance in older adults. These two studies highlight that longer duration of supplementation is required to establish episodic memory enhancement associated with high anthocyanin and proanthocyanidin containing berries.
As anticipated, the cranberry intervention had no impact upon differences in structural grey matter between groups, nor did it influence differences in WMH over the 12-week period of investigation. However, in line with results suggesting that the cranberry intervention led to improved episodic memory performance, differences in perfusion in response to the intervention were detected between cranberry and placebo groups in key cerebral regions supporting memory consolidation and retrieval (59). A relative increase in perfusion was detected in the cranberry group between baseline and follow-up compared to the placebo group in medial temporal (entorhinal) and prefrontal (orbitofrontal) regions, as well as in the nucleus accumbens, which would provide optimal distribution of the essential nutrients for neuronal activity, such as oxygen and glucose (60).
The pathophysiological processes leading to neurodegeneration, like many other diseases, are proposed to involve the dysfunction of multiple systems within the body. Neurodegeneration is hypothesised to be characterised by progressive changes in several interlinked cellular and molecular mechanisms, including chronic neuroinflammation, oxidative stress and metabolic imbalances, as well as loss of vascular integrity and function, deposition of aggregated proteins, mechanisms that underlie not only pathological but also normal brain ageing, resulting in loss of neural plasticity and neuronal death (61). However, the exact nature and results of these processes are still being elucidated. Treatments that can target and slow down these processes would be valuable in counteracting brain ageing and cognitive decline. Results from intervention studies involving humans remain less prevalent, and as such findings that lend support to the causal effects of berry (poly)phenols on the prevention of age-related cognitive decline and dementia from interventional studies remain sparse, and further attention needs to be dedicated to determining the causality of flavonoid consumption on improving cognition and preventing dementia.
Although it was anticipated that BDNF may increase in the cranberry group (in line with improved cognition) and as reported for other flavonoids (62), such results were not significant. In particular, a high concentration of BDNF was measured at baseline which masked the overall impact of cranberries at follow-up. Such increased concentration may be related to participants’ higher consumption of caffeine in this group at baseline. During our analysis, we also observed a significant decrease in LDL cholesterol following cranberry intake, along with a trend in total cholesterol decreases. Such results are in agreement with a previous study demonstrating that cranberry supplements were effective in reducing atherosclerotic cholesterol profiles, including LDL cholesterol and total cholesterol levels (63). Interestingly, in our study, cranberry intake seemed to better benefit older male participants where a steep decrease in body weight and associated parameters [fasting glucose, high-density lipoprotein (HDL) cholesterol, blood pressure] were better controlled. Such results are in agreement with data demonstrating the anti-obesogenic impact of cranberries, although most of the information is derived from animal studies (64). Further studies would be necessary to confirm such findings in well controlled clinical studies.
In addition to biochemical measures, the circulating plasma (poly)phenol metabolites indicated that both groups were well matched at baseline, apart from flavanols which were higher in the placebo group before the intervention. The results of the background diet questionnaires also indicated that the placebo group had a higher average intake of flavonols, although this difference between groups was not significant. The high molecular weight fraction in our study represented 70.4%, so high concentrations of phenolic metabolites/catabolites were not expected after overnight fasting. There were, however, increases in total metabolites in the cranberry group as a result of the intervention, which appeared to be driven largely by significant increases in catechols and hippuric acid. Further and contrary to expectations, plasma (poly)phenol metabolite concentrations did not relate to either RCF delay score or regional blood perfusion within the regions found to be differentially changed between groups as a result of the intervention. It is therefore possible that the mechanisms underpinning the changes in cognition and regional blood perfusion in the brain were not the direct interaction between these metabolites and neural targets.
The relatively small sample size of this intervention may have been a limiting factor particularly with regards to having sufficient power to detect significant differences in both cognition and brain perfusion. Indeed, regarding the neuroimaging data, several regions showed a trend for perfusion differences such as the insula and the medial orbitofrontal cortex, which may have reached significance if more participant scans were available. Only a subsection of the study sample was able to have complete baseline and follow-up MRI scans due to practical constraints and the impact of COVID-19 lockdowns on hospital imaging facilities during the critical follow-up window for several (n = 14) patients. Furthermore, although it was not reported by any participants, other health conditions which may have influenced cognitive results such as sleep apnoea were not systematically excluded in this study. It is also important to note that the cranberries also contain other health-promoting compounds and nutrients, including fibre and other nutrients, making it sometimes difficult to determine whether it is in fact these specific polyphenols producing the health effects. For example, nutrients such as fermentable fibres can influence gut microbial metabolism of polyphenols (65). Furthermore, other mechanisms such as chronic neuroinflammation, mitochondrial function and compromised vascular integrity and function are increasingly becoming understood to be key mechanisms which also contribute to age-related cognitive decline and neurodegenerative conditions and provide targets for interventions to curtail the disease processes contributing to age-related neurodegeneration [for review, see Flanagan et al. (66)]. Indeed, these mechanisms are also among the targets of nutritional interventions including those involving high-polyphenol foods, particularly in light of their suggested bidirectional relationships with the function of gut microbiota. Similarly, markers of other factors that could be impacted by cranberry intake and could also relate to neurodegeneration such as chronic infection (67) were not measured. The focus of the intervention discussed in this study was the impact of a long-term cranberry intervention on cognition and brain function, and as such the investigation of the impact on these additional mechanisms, although important, fell outside the scope of this study. Finally, as the physicochemical properties and dietary intake forms could impact the absorption and bioactivity of nutrients (68) contained in the cranberry powder, and as such could be controlled in future investigations to ensure that the effectiveness of the cranberry is not impacted by different methods of incorporating it into the diet.
These findings are, however, certainly encouraging that sustained intake of cranberry over a 12-week period produced significant improvements in memory and neural function in older adults who were cognitively healthy. Future studies investigating whether these changes translate to a clinical population of cognitively impaired adults in the context of neurodegenerative conditions such as mild cognitive impairment or dementia is warranted based on these results. Determining whether these changes are sustained following the cessation of intake, for how long and to what degree would also be of interest. Replication of this study in a larger sample size might also produce more robust results.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the University of East Anglia’s Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences Ethical Review Committee (Reference: 201819–039) and the Health Research Authority (IRAS number: 237251). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
AN, MM, MH, and DV contributed to the conception and design of the study, funding acquisition, and wrote the manuscript. EF contributed to the day-to-day management of the study, data and sample acquisition, formal analysis and wrote the manuscript, and had primary responsibility for final content. MP analysed BDNF concentrations in biological samples. DC, CW, RS, and SS contributed to the MRI data analysis and statistical analyses. NT, PM, and DD analysed the (poly)phenol content in biological samples and extracts. All authors contributed to the manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.
Conflict of Interest
DV, MH, MM, and AN received funding from the Cranberry Institute. The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Acknowledgments
We thank our nursing and research support staff at the Clinical Research Facilities at the University of East Anglia for administrative assistance. We would also like to thank the participants for their time and contribution to the study.
Footnotes
Funding
This research was supported by a Cranberry Institute grant (R204719). The Cranberry Institute was not involved in the design, implementation, analysis, and interpretation of the data.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnut.2022.849902/full#supplementary-material
References
- 1.Bloom DE, Boersch-Supan A, McGee P, Seike A. Population aging: facts, challenges, and responses. Benefits Compens Int. (2011) 41:22. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Alzheimer’s Disease International. World Alzheimer Report 2019: Attitudes to Dementia. London: Alzheimer’s Disease International; (2019). [Google Scholar]
- 3.Long JM, Holtzman DM. Alzheimer disease: an update on pathobiology and treatment strategies. Cell. (2019) 179:312–39. 10.1016/j.cell.2019.09.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Miquel S, Champ C, Day J, Aarts E, Bahr BA, Bakker M, et al. Poor cognitive ageing: vulnerabilities, mechanisms and the impact of nutritional interventions. Ageing Res Rev. (2018) 42:40–55. 10.1016/j.arr.2017.12.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Vauzour D, Camprubi-Robles M, Miquel-Kergoat S, Andres-Lacueva C, Bánáti D, Barberger-Gateau P, et al. Nutrition for the ageing brain: towards evidence for an optimal diet. Ageing Res Rev. (2017) 35:222–40. 10.1016/j.arr.2016.09.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Devore EE, Kang JH, Breteler MM, Grodstein F. Dietary intakes of berries and flavonoids in relation to cognitive decline. Ann Neurol. (2012) 72:135–43. 10.1002/ana.23594 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Letenneur L, Proust-Lima C, Le Gouge A, Dartigues J, Barberger-Gateau P. Flavonoid Intake and Cognitive Decline over a 10-Year Period. Am J Epidemiol. (2007) 165:1364–71. 10.1093/aje/kwm036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Shishtar E, Rogers GT, Blumberg JB, Au R, Jacques PF. Long-term dietary flavonoid intake and change in cognitive function in the Framingham Offspring cohort. Public Health Nutrit. (2020) 23:1576–88. 10.1017/s136898001900394x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Lefèvre-Arbogast S, Gaudout D, Bensalem J, Letenneur L, Dartigues J-F, Hejblum BP, et al. Pattern of polyphenol intake and the long-term risk of dementia in older persons. Neurology. (2018) 90:e1979–88. 10.1212/wnl.0000000000005607 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Bell L, Williams CM. Blueberry benefits to cognitive function across the lifespan. Int J Food Sci Nutr. (2021) 72:650–2. 10.1080/09637486.2020.1852192 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Jennings A, Steves CJ, Macgregor A, Spector T, Cassidy A. Increased habitual flavonoid intake predicts attenuation of cognitive ageing in twins. BMC Med. (2021) 19:185. 10.1186/s12916-021-02057-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ahles S, Joris PJ, Plat J. Effects of berry anthocyanins on cognitive performance, vascular function and cardiometabolic risk markers: a systematic review of randomized placebo-controlled intervention studies in humans. Int J Mol Sci. (2021) 22:6482. 10.3390/ijms22126482 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Travica N, D’Cunha NM, Naumovski N, Kent K, Mellor DD, Firth J, et al. The effect of blueberry interventions on cognitive performance and mood: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Brain Behav Immun. (2020) 85:96–105. 10.1016/j.bbi.2019.04.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Gutierres JM, Carvalho FB, Schetinger MR, Marisco P, Agostinho P, Rodrigues M, et al. Anthocyanins restore behavioral and biochemical changes caused by streptozotocin-induced sporadic dementia of Alzheimer’s type. Life Sci. (2014) 96:7–17. 10.1016/j.lfs.2013.11.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Meireles M, Marques C, Norberto S, Fernandes I, Mateus N, Rendeiro C, et al. The impact of chronic blackberry intake on the neuroinflammatory status of rats fed a standard or high-fat diet. J Nutr Biochem. (2015) 26:1166–73. 10.1016/j.jnutbio.2015.05.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Zhang Y, Meng Q, Yin J, Zhang Z, Bao H, Wang X. Anthocyanins attenuate neuroinflammation through the suppression of MLK3 activation in a mouse model of perioperative neurocognitive disorders. Brain Res. (2020) 1726:146504. 10.1016/j.brainres.2019.146504 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Rendeiro C, Vauzour D, Kean RJ, Butler LT, Rattray M, Spencer JP, et al. Blueberry supplementation induces spatial memory improvements and region-specific regulation of hippocampal BDNF mRNA expression in young rats. Psychopharmacology. (2012) 223:319–30. 10.1007/s00213-012-2719-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Williams CM, El Mohsen MA, Vauzour D, Rendeiro C, Butler LT, Ellis JA, et al. Blueberry-induced changes in spatial working memory correlate with changes in hippocampal CREB phosphorylation and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) levels. Free Radic Biol Med. (2008) 45:295–305. 10.1016/j.freeradbiomed.2008.04.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Vauzour D, Rendeiro C, D’Amato A, Waffo-Téguo P, Richard T, Mérillon JM, et al. Anthocyanins promote learning through modulation of synaptic plasticity related proteins in an animal model of ageing. Antioxidants. (2021) 10:10081235. 10.3390/antiox10081235 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Krikorian R, Shidler MD, Nash TA, Kalt W, Vinqvist-Tymchuk MR, Shukitt-Hale B, et al. Blueberry supplementation improves memory in older adults. J Agric Food Chem. (2010) 58:3996–4000. 10.1021/jf9029332 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Lamport DJ, Lawton CL, Merat N, Jamson H, Myrissa K, Hofman D, et al. Concord grape juice, cognitive function, and driving performance: a 12-wk, placebo-controlled, randomized crossover trial in mothers of preteen children. Am J Clin Nutr. (2016) 103:775–83. 10.3945/ajcn.115.114553 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Boespflug EL, Eliassen JC, Dudley JA, Shidler MD, Kalt W, Summer SS, et al. Enhanced neural activation with blueberry supplementation in mild cognitive impairment. Nutr Neurosci. (2018) 21:297–305. 10.1080/1028415x.2017.1287833 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Whyte AR, Rahman S, Bell L, Edirisinghe I, Krikorian R, Williams CM, et al. Improved metabolic function and cognitive performance in middle-aged adults following a single dose of wild blueberry. Eur J Nutr. (2021) 60:1521–36. 10.1007/s00394-020-02336-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lee J, Torosyan N, Silverman DH. Examining the impact of grape consumption on brain metabolism and cognitive function in patients with mild decline in cognition: a double-blinded placebo controlled pilot study. Exp Gerontol. (2017) 87:121–8. 10.1016/j.exger.2016.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Vauzour D. Dietary polyphenols as modulators of brain functions: biological actions and molecular mechanisms underpinning their beneficial effects. Oxidat Med Cell Longev. (2012) 2012:1–16. 10.1155/2012/914273 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Seymour EM, Tanone II, Urcuyo-Llanes DE, Lewis SK, Kirakosyan A, Kondoleon MG, et al. Blueberry intake alters skeletal muscle and adipose tissue peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor activity and reduces insulin resistance in Obese Rats. J Med Food. (2011) 14:1511–8. 10.1089/jmf.2010.0292 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Stull AJ, Cash KC, Johnson WD, Champagne CM, Cefalu WT. Bioactives in blueberries improve insulin sensitivity in obese, insulin-resistant men and women. J Nutrit. (2010) 140:1764–8. 10.3945/jn.110.125336 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kumar Singh A, Cabral C, Kumar R, Ganguly R, Kumar Rana H, Gupta A, et al. Beneficial effects of dietary polyphenols on gut microbiota and strategies to improve delivery efficiency. Nutrients. (2019) 11:11092216. 10.3390/nu11092216 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Rodríguez-Daza MC, Pulido-Mateos EC, Lupien-Meilleur J, Guyonnet D, Desjardins Y, Roy D. Polyphenol-mediated gut microbiota modulation: toward prebiotics and further. Front Nutrit. (2021) 8:689456. 10.3389/fnut.2021.689456 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Crispi S, Filosa S, Di Meo F. Polyphenols-gut microbiota interplay and brain neuromodulation. Neural Regenerat Res. (2018) 13:241429. 10.4103/1673-5374.241429 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Bowtell JL, Aboo-Bakkar Z, Conway ME, Adlam AR, Fulford J. Enhanced task-related brain activation and resting perfusion in healthy older adults after chronic blueberry supplementation. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. (2017) 42:773–9. 10.1139/apnm-2016-0550 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Lamport DJ, Pal D, Moutsiana C, Field DT, Williams CM, Spencer JP, et al. The effect of flavanol-rich cocoa on cerebral perfusion in healthy older adults during conscious resting state: a placebo controlled, crossover, acute trial. Psychopharmacology. (2015) 232:3227–34. 10.1007/s00213-015-3972-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Haskell-Ramsay C, Schmitt J, Actis-Goretta L. The impact of epicatechin on human cognition: the role of cerebral blood Flow. Nutrients. (2018) 10:10080986. 10.3390/nu10080986 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Brickman AM, Khan UA, Provenzano FA, Yeung L-K, Suzuki W, Schroeter H, et al. Enhancing dentate gyrus function with dietary flavanols improves cognition in older adults. Nat Neurosci. (2014) 17:1798–803. 10.1038/nn.3850 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Zhao S, Liu H, Gu L. American cranberries and health benefits – an evolving story of 25 years. J Sci Food Agricult. (2018) 100:5111–6. 10.1002/jsfa.8882 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Bariexca T, Ezdebski J, Redan BW, Vinson J. Pure polyphenols and cranberry juice high in anthocyanins increase antioxidant capacity in animal organs. Foods. (2019) 8:8080340. 10.3390/foods8080340 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Shukitt-Hale B, Galli RL, Meterko V, Carey A, Bielinski DF, McGhie T, et al. Dietary supplementation with fruit polyphenolics ameliorates age-related deficits in behavior and neuronal markers of inflammation and oxidative stress. Age. (2005) 27:49–57. 10.1007/s11357-005-4004-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Basu A, Betts NM, Ortiz J, Simmons B, Wu M, Lyons TJ. Low-energy cranberry juice decreases lipid oxidation and increases plasma antioxidant capacity in women with metabolic syndrome. Nutrit Res. (2011) 31:190–6. 10.1016/j.nutres.2011.02.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Pourmasoumi M, Hadi A, Najafgholizadeh A, Joukar F, Mansour-Ghanaei F. The effects of cranberry on cardiovascular metabolic risk factors: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Nutrit. (2020) 39:774–88. 10.1016/j.clnu.2019.04.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Rodriguez-Mateos A, Feliciano RP, Boeres A, Weber T, dos Santos CN, Ventura MR, et al. Cranberry (poly)phenol metabolites correlate with improvements in vascular function: a double-blind, randomized, controlled, dose-response, crossover study. Mol Nutrit Food Res. (2016) 60:2130–40. 10.1002/mnfr.201600250 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Crews WD, Harrison DW, Griffin ML, Addison K, Yount AM, Giovenco MA, et al. A double-blinded, placebo-controlled, randomized trial of the neuropsychologic efficacy of cranberry juice in a sample of cognitively intact older adults: pilot study findings. J Alternat Complement Med. (2005) 11:305–9. 10.1089/acm.2005.11.305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Rattanabannakit C, Risacher SL, Gao S, Lane KA, Brown SA, McDonald BC, et al. The cognitive change index as a measure of self and informant perception of cognitive decline: relation to neuropsychological tests. J Alzheimer’s Dis. (2016) 51:1145–55. 10.3233/jad-150729 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Irvine MA, Scholey A, King R, Gillings R, Vauzour D, Demichele SJ, et al. The cognitive ageing, nutrition and neurogenesis (CANN) trial: design and progress. Alzheimers Dement. (2018) 4:591–601. 10.1016/j.trci.2018.08.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Rodriguez-Mateos A, Vauzour D, Krueger CG, Shanmuganayagam D, Reed J, Calani L, et al. Bioavailability, bioactivity and impact on health of dietary flavonoids and related compounds: an update. Arch Toxicol. (2014) 88:1803–53. 10.1007/s00204-014-1330-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Hollis JL, Craig LC, Whybrow S, Clark H, Kyle JA, McNeill G. Assessing the relative validity of the Scottish Collaborative Group FFQ for measuring dietary intake in adults. Public Health Nutr. (2017) 20:449–55. 10.1017/S1368980016002421 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Hsieh S, Schubert S, Hoon C, Mioshi E, Hodges JR. Validation of the addenbrooke’s cognitive examination III in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2013) 36:242–50. 10.1159/000351671 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Reitan RM. Trail Making Test: Manual for Administration and Scoring. Reitan: Neuropsychology Laboratory; (1992). [Google Scholar]
- 48.Tombaugh T. Trail Making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2004) 19:203–14. 10.1016/s0887-6177(03)00039-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Meyers JE, Meyers KR. Rey Complex Figure Test and Recognition trial Professional Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; (1995). [Google Scholar]
- 50.Tu S, Wong S, Hodges JR, Irish M, Piguet O, Hornberger M. Lost in spatial translation - A novel tool to objectively assess spatial disorientation in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Cortex. (2015) 67:83–94. 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.03.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Zhang N, Gordon ML, Goldberg TE. Cerebral blood flow measured by arterial spin labeling MRI at resting state in normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2017) 72:168–75. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.11.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Sierra-Marcos A. Regional cerebral blood flow in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease measured with arterial spin labeling magnetic resonance imaging. Int J Alzheimer’s Dis. (2017) 2017:1–10. 10.1155/2017/5479597 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RNA, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RSJ. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. NeuroImage. (2001) 14:21–36. 10.1006/nimg.2001.0786 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Fazekas F, Chawluk JB, Alavi A, Hurtig HI, Zimmerman RA. MR signal abnormalities at 1.5 T in Alzheimer’s dementia and normal aging. Am J Roentgenol. (1987) 149:351–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Henschel L, Conjeti S, Estrada S, Diers K, Fischl B, Reuter M. FastSurfer - A fast and accurate deep learning based neuroimaging pipeline. NeuroImage. (2020) 219:117012. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Feliciano R, Mills C, Istas G, Heiss C, Rodriguez-Mateos A. Absorption, metabolism and excretion of cranberry (Poly)phenols in humans: a dose response study and assessment of inter-individual variability. Nutrients. (2017) 9:9030268. 10.3390/nu9030268 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Feliciano RP, Mecha E, Bronze MR, Rodriguez-Mateos A. Development and validation of a high-throughput micro solid-phase extraction method coupled with ultra-high-performance liquid chromatography-quadrupole time-of-flight mass spectrometry for rapid identification and quantification of phenolic metabolites in human plasma and urine. J Chromatogr A. (2016) 1464:21–31. 10.1016/j.chroma.2016.08.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.de Jager CA, Dye L, de Bruin EA, Butler L, Fletcher J, Lamport DJ, et al. Criteria for validation and selection of cognitive tests for investigating the effects of foods and nutrients. Nutrit Rev. (2014) 72:162–79. 10.1111/nure.12094 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Spencer JPE. The impact of flavonoids on memory: physiological and molecular considerations. Chem Soc Rev. (2009) 38:b800422f. 10.1039/b800422f [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Naiberg MR, Newton DF, Goldstein BI. Flow-mediated dilation and neurocognition: systematic review and future directions. Psychosom Med. (2016) 78:192–207. 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000266 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Foster TC. Biological markers of age-related memory deficits: treatment of senescent physiology. CNS Drugs. (2006) 20:153–66. 10.2165/00023210-200620020-00006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Neshatdoust S, Saunders C, Castle SM, Vauzour D, Williams C, Butler L, et al. High-flavonoid intake induces cognitive improvements linked to changes in serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor: two randomised, controlled trials. Nutrit Healthy Aging. (2016) 4:81–93. 10.3233/nha-1615 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Lee IT, Chan YC, Lin CW, Lee WJ, Sheu WH. Effect of cranberry extracts on lipid profiles in subjects with Type 2 diabetes. Diabet Med. (2008) 25:1473–7. 10.1111/j.1464-5491.2008.02588.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Jiang H, Zhang W, Li X, Xu Y, Cao J, Jiang W. The anti-obesogenic effects of dietary berry fruits: a review. Food Res Int. (2021) 147:110539. 10.1016/j.foodres.2021.110539 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Mansoorian B, Combet E, Alkhaldy A, Garcia AL, Edwards CA. Impact of fermentable fibres on the colonic microbiota metabolism of dietary polyphenols rutin and quercetin. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:292. 10.3390/ijerph16020292 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Flanagan E, Muller M, Hornberger M, Vauzour D. Impact of flavonoids on cellular and molecular mechanisms underlying age-related cognitive decline and neurodegeneration. Curr Nutr Rep. (2018) 7:49–57. 10.1007/s13668-018-0226-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Lim SL, Rodriguez-Ortiz CJ, Kitazawa M. Infection, systemic inflammation, and Alzheimer’s disease. Microbes Infect. (2015) 17:549–56. 10.1016/j.micinf.2015.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Selby-Pham SNB, Miller RB, Howell K, Dunshea F, Bennett LE. Physicochemical properties of dietary phytochemicals can predict their passive absorption in the human small intestine. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:1888–w. 10.1038/s41598-017-01888-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.