Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Jun 5.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2018;38:249–265. doi: 10.1007/7854_2018_51

Does Anhedonia Presage Increased Risk of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder?

Adolescent Anhedonia and Posttraumatic Disorders

Victoria B Risbrough 1, Laura M Glynn 2, Elysia P Davis 3, Curt A Sandman 4, Andre Obenaus 5, Hal S Stern 6, David B Keator 7, Michael A Yassa 8, Tallie Z Baram 9, Dewleen G Baker 10
PMCID: PMC9167566  NIHMSID: NIHMS1784406  PMID: 29796839

Abstract

Anhedonia, the reduced ability to experience pleasure, is a dimensional entity linked to multiple neuropsychiatric disorders, where it is associated with diminished treatment response, reduced global function, and increased suicidality. It has been suggested that anhedonia and the related disruption in reward processing may be critical precursors to development of psychiatric symptoms later in life. Here, we examine cross-species evidence supporting the hypothesis that early life experiences modulate development of reward processing, which if disrupted, result in anhedonia. Importantly, we find that anhedonia may confer risk for later neuropsychiatric disorders, especially posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Whereas childhood trauma has long been associated with increased anhedonia and increased subsequent risk for trauma-related disorders in adulthood, here we focus on an additional novel, emerging direct contributor to anhedonia in rodents and humans: fragmented, chaotic environmental signals (“FRAG”) during critical periods of development. In rodents, recent data suggest that adolescent anhedonia may derive from aberrant pleasure/reward circuit maturation. In humans, recent longitudinal studies support that FRAG is associated with increased anhedonia in adolescence. Both human and rodent FRAG exposure also leads to aberrant hippocampal function. Prospective studies are underway to examine if anhedonia is also a marker of PTSD risk. These preliminary cross-species studies provide a critical construct for future examination of the etiology of trauma-related symptoms in adults and for and development of prophylactic and therapeutic interventions. In addition, longitudinal studies of reward circuit development with and without FRAG will be critical to test the mechanistic hypothesis that early life FRAG modifies reward circuitry with subsequent consequences for adolescent-emergent anhedonia and contributes to risk and resilience to trauma and stress in adulthood.

Keywords: Anhedonia, Brain circuits, Corticotropin releasing factor (CRF), Depression, Early life adversity, Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), Reward circuits, Rodent, Unpredictability and fragmented sensory signals (FRAG)

1. Anhedonia, A Dimensional Construct with Transdiagnostic Relevance

Mental illness, including PTSD, depression, and suicide, afflict >20% of adolescents and young adults, with significant social and fiscal costs (Insel 2011; Merikangas et al. 2010). The origins of mental illness are complex, involving genetic and environmental contributions, specifically during sensitive developmental periods (Bale et al. 2010; Lupien et al. 2009; Nelson et al. 2007; Osborne and Monk 2013). Anhedonia, defined as a reduction in pleasure and appetitive/reward seeking behaviors, is a dimensional construct that is transdiagnostic, cutting across mood, anxiety, and substance use disorders, with clearly delineated operational measures and underlying neurocircuitry [for review, see (Pizzagalli 2014; Rizvi et al. 2016)]. Across mood and anxiety disorders, anhedonia correlates with poor treatment response, suicidality, and diminished global function (Pizzagalli 2014; Rizvi et al. 2016). Because of its association with poor treatment outcomes, it is clear that understanding mechanisms underlying anhedonia and its role in both development and maintenance of neuropsychiatric disorders may be critical for effective treatment strategies. Here, we will discuss a neurodevelopmental hypothesis of anhedonia and its contribution to risk and resilience to stress and trauma in adulthood. Specifically, we will review the role of anhedonia and its underlying reward circuitry in trauma-related disorders, discuss its known association with early life adversity, and its link to a newly described form of early life adversity, fragmented and chaotic maternal signals (FRAG) during critical phases of development. We will describe recent cross-species evidence for this form of fragmented and chaotic maternal care to mediate anhedonia in adolescence/early adulthood (Molet et al. 2016a; Bolton et al. 2018) and discuss how this newly identified form of developmental stress might shape reward circuit development and neuropsychiatric risk. Throughout, we will identify knowledge gaps and research needed to understand how FRAG may affect reward processes and reward circuitry, and how it subsequently affects susceptibility to trauma disorders in later life.

2. Anhedonia and Disrupted Reward Circuits as a Marker of Heterogeneity in Trauma Disorders

Posttraumatic stress (PTS) symptoms manifest after severe trauma exposure, affecting 7–8% of the US population (Seedat and Stein 2001) and up to 20% of armed service members (Thomas et al. 2010). Besides the extensive mental health service utilization required for treatment, trauma-related disorders, such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), are associated with greater overall medical service utilization due to higher rates of chronic physical illness experienced by these patients (Baker et al. 2009; O’Donnell et al. 2013). PTSD is phenotypically and etiologically heterogeneous, posing a significant challenge to identifying its biological mechanisms, creation of objective, non-symptom-based nosological categories that cut across current diagnostic boundaries, and development of novel therapeutics. Hence, the current greater focus in psychiatric research is towards identifying fundamental underlying dimensional constructs that contribute significantly to mortality, global function, and treatment response, such as anhedonia.

Anhedonia and Reward Abnormalities in Trauma-Related Disorders

Self-reported anhedonia contributes to symptom heterogeneity in PTSD; it is associated with increased risk for suicide (Spitzer et al. 2018) and social withdrawal (Cao et al. 2016) as well as reduced reward responsiveness (Nawijn et al. 2016). PTS-related anhedonic symptoms in combat veterans increase the risk for comorbidity with substance use, depression, and anxiety disorders (>50% of veterans with PTSD have at least one of these comorbidities) (Kashdan et al. 2006). Theoretically, anhedonia is derived from both reward motivation and consumption (“wanting” and “liking,” respectively). A comprehensive recent review by Nawijn et al. suggests that reward abnormalities most robustly observed in trauma disorders are in reward motivation or “wanting” components of the hedonic process such a reward response and effortful-approach behavior, while arousal and valence ratings of rewarding stimuli are not different from those of controls (Nawijn et al. 2015). PTSD patients report significant reductions in the experience of pleasure, i.e., anhedonia (Vujanovic et al. 2017). PTSD patients exhibit anhedonia with or without co-occurring major depressive disorder (MDD), suggesting that anhedonia in trauma-exposed patients is not purely a function of comorbid major depression (Franklin and Zimmerman 2001). Importantly, trauma exposure alone is not related to reduced reward functioning (Nawijn et al. 2015).

Reward Circuit Abnormalities Associated with Anhedonia and Trauma-Related Disorders

Anhedonia has been examined across multiple neuropsychiatric disorders including mood, trauma-related, and psychotic disorders. Evidence clearly indicates overlap in the circuitry mediating anhedonia and reward abnormalities across disorders, supporting that anhedonia spans classical diagnostic categories (Sharma et al. 2017; Zhang et al. 2016). Symptoms of anhedonia are robustly associated with the brain’s reward processing pathways and reduced responsiveness to reward information (Berghorst et al. 2013; Bogdan and Pizzagalli 2006; Pizzagalli et al. 2008).

The brain’s reward circuits consist of the ventral tegmental area (VTA) which sends dopaminergic projections to the nucleus accumbens (NAc) in the ventral striatum. VTA neurons also innervate the prefrontal cortex (PFC), the amygdala, and the hippocampus. Most of the extant literature on the reward pathway in humans comes from fMRI studies that implicate decreased activity levels in the VTA and NAc in anhedonia (Drevets et al. 1992; Lee et al. 2012; Mayberg et al. 2000; Russo and Nestler 2013). Consistent with these imaging findings, deep brain stimulation within the NAc reduces anhedonia in treatment-resistant depression (Bewernick et al. 2010, 2012; Schlaepfer et al. 2008). There is also some evidence that amygdala activation is associated with anhedonia (Kumar et al. 2014; Stuhrmann et al. 2013). Amygdala hyperactivity is one of the most robust phenotypes in PTSD (Koch et al. 2016; Etkin and Wager 2007) and may be a pre-trauma risk factor for PTSD (Stevens et al. 2017). However, there is no reliable evidence as to the level of amygdala excitability in the context of anhedonia. It is possible that amygdala hyperactivation is a distinct feature of emotional processing aberrations found in mood and trauma disorders and not reward processing per se.

In general, fewer studies have examined anhedonia in individuals with PTSD compared to depression and schizophrenia. The circuit activation patterns are similar across these disorders in relation to anhedonic symptoms, with altered BOLD fMRI activity levels in reward circuitry including the ventral striatum/NAc, the amygdala, and the PFC (Nawijn et al. 2016; Frewen et al. 2012). Reductions in ventral striatum/NAc activity in response to rewards is reliably associated with increased PTS symptoms or PTSD diagnosis (Admon et al. 2013a, b; Elman et al. 2009; Sailer et al. 2008). Interestingly, CT-based lesion analysis also revealed a link between anhedonia and injury to the ventrolateral PFC in Vietnam combat veterans (Lewis et al. 2015). This finding poses a potential complication in studying populations with comorbid head injury and PTSD as regional vulnerabilities in the PFC may manifest in both and may be linked to anhedonic symptoms.

3. Anhedonia, Potential Risk Factor or Marker of Symptom State in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder?

Evidence for Anhedonia as a Preexisting Risk Factor for Depression and PTSD

Substantial evidence supports anhedonia as a robust phenotype associated with reward circuit disruption in PTSD patients. However, whether anhedonia and reward dysfunction are precursors to clinical dysfunction precipitated by environmental challenges such as stress and trauma or only develop after trauma/chronic stress exposure and symptom development is unknown. Evidence for anhedonia as a risk factor is quite preliminary and still circumstantial at this stage, consisting primarily of correlational and cross-sectional findings. First, depressed and PTSD subjects continue to show deficient reward learning even after symptom remission (Whitton et al. 2016; Kalebasi et al. 2015) (note that this PTSD study is very small, N = 12/group). PTSD subjects also show reward processing and response abnormalities when controlling for psychoactive medication (Elman et al. 2005; Hopper et al. 2008). Potential interpretations of these findings are that anhedonia may be a more “fixed” trait that increases risk for development of PTSD, or that anhedonia is less responsive than other symptoms to current treatments. To determine if anhedonia and reward processing abnormalities are pre-trauma risk factors, twin studies and prospective longitudinal studies will be required. No studies have yet examined this question in trauma-related disorders; however, one small prospective study in adolescent girls reported that low reward sensitivity predicts later adult depression (Bress et al. 2013). To answer this question, we have used the Marine Resiliency Study (MRS) database to test a prospective role of multiple risk factors for PTSD. MRS is a prospective, longitudinal study of psychological, physiological, and biological risk factors for development of combat-related PTSD, in which infantry Marine participants (average age range 18–22) were comprehensively assessed both before leaving for a combat deployment to Iraq or Afghanistan, and 3 and 6 months after their return from deployment (Baker et al. 2012). We previously reported that childhood trauma is associated with PTSD in this population (Agorastos et al. 2014). We are currently examining if in healthy participants at pre-deployment, self-reported levels of anhedonia (as measured by the anhedonia subscale of the Beck Depression Inventory) predict increased risk for their developing PTSD after deployment. Our findings are in preparation for publication and were reported at the 2017 American College of Neuropharmacology Annual Meeting (Risbrough et al. 2017). The findings suggest that anhedonia is a significant predictor of risk, and this association is independent of self-reported depression symptoms and deployment trauma exposure. Studies such as this ongoing analysis in MRS will be required to support the hypothesis that anhedonia in late-adolescence/early adulthood may contribute to pre-trauma risk for PTS.

Might Anhedonia Mediate the Relationship Between PTSD Vulnerability and Early Life Adversity?

During early life, the fetus and infant are vulnerable to the consequences of adversity with lasting consequences for infant, toddler, child, and adolescent outcomes (Kim et al. 2014, 2016, 2017; Sandman et al. 2012, 2015; Davis et al. 2011a, b; Davis and Sandman 2006, 2010, 2012; Davis et al. 2007; Glynn et al. 2007; Howland et al. 2016; Stout et al. 2015). The evidence that early experiences including maternal mental health (Goodman and Gotlib 1999; Oberlander et al. 2008; Monk 2001; Monk et al. 2011; Plant et al. 2015), quality of maternal care (Belsky and Fearon 2002; NICHD Early Child Care Research Network 1999, 2006; Masur et al. 2005; Hane et al. 2010; Feldman 2007, 2010, 2015), trauma (Treadway et al. 2009; Rao et al. 2010; Copeland et al. 2007; Mulvihill 2005; Pynoos et al. 1999; Young et al. 2017; Nelson 2013; McLaughlin et al. 2010), and poverty (Kim et al. 2013; Evans and Kim 2007, 2012; Barch et al. 2016; Javanbakht et al. 2016; Noble et al. 2015; Johnson et al. 2016; Does amount of time spent in child care predict socioemotional adjustment during the transition to kindergarten? 2003) profoundly influence later mental health is undisputed. There is also emerging research implicating exposure to early life adversity in dysfunction of reward-related circuitry (Pechtel and Pizzagalli 2011), particularly in response to adversity during early childhood (Hanson et al. 2016). Developmental trajectories of reward-related ventral striatum (VS) activity mediate the relationship between early life stress and mood disorders in adolescence (Hanson et al. 2015). Childhood trauma, for example, is a well-known risk factor for increased depression, PTSD, and physical health problems (Agorastos et al. 2014), yet it can also increase resilience in some populations (Liu et al. 2017). Others have shown that the strongest correlation of aspects of early life trauma to subsequent PTSD (Lowe et al. 2016) was indication of safety or lack thereof. Childhood trauma is also linked to anhedonia and aberrant reward processing in later life [e.g., (Frewen et al. 2012; Pechtel and Pizzagalli 2013)]. But, trauma exposure is not the only factor that can alter reward processing in development; both children and adults exposed to early social deprivation also exhibit altered reward processing and reduced activity in corticostriatal circuits (Dillon et al. 2009; Mehta et al. 2010; Goff et al. 2013; Guyer et al. 2006; Hanson et al. 2017). Crucially, the scope and trajectory of differential developmental effects of early life experiences and mechanisms associated with resilience vs. risk are still being elucidated. We and others have shown that maternal signals alone can have profound developmental effects on neural circuit development, including small-world network architecture, rich-club organization, and distinct modular structure (Kim et al. 2014, 2016, 2017; Sandman et al. 2012, 2015; Davis et al. 2007, 2011a, b; Davis and Sandman 2006, 2010, 2012; Glynn et al. 2007; Howland et al. 2016; Stout et al. 2015). We have recently identified a novel source of early life adversity across rodent models and humans: unpredictable and fragmented sensory signals (FRAG), particularly those derived from maternal care and the home environment. We have proposed that this relatively unexamined component of early developmental experience may be critical in shaping neural circuits underlying risk for neuropsychiatric disorders in adulthood (Baram et al. 2012). In humans, FRAG is assessed in two broad manners. Prenatally, inconsistency and fragmentation of maternal mood is examined via questionnaires and analyzed by applying Shannon’s entropy to the distribution of self-reported mood over multiple time points both in pregnancy and after birth (Glynn et al. 2017). In addition, unpredictability of maternal care sequences is examined using behavioral observations of the mother’s interactions with her infant (Molet et al. 2016a; Davis et al. 2017). In animals, fragmentation of maternal care behaviors is measured via assessment of duration of stereotyped maternal behavior bouts (e.g., nursing and grooming). In addition, unpredictability of the sequences of maternal care behaviors is examined via observations, and analyzed using Shannon’s entropy, as done for humans (Guyer et al. 2006; Hanson et al. 2017). This new construct, predictability of maternal and environmental signals during early development, may be a vital factor that influences synapse strengthening and circuit maturation of reward processing and related circuitry.

4. Anhedonia Develops in Adolescent/Young Adult Rodents as a Result of FRAG

Because of the links between poverty, neglect, abuse, and other forms of childhood trauma with neuropathology and psychiatric symptoms in adulthood, numerous animal models of early life adversity have been developed. However, effects of early life stress on anhedonia in animal models have been conflicting, potentially because of their variable effects on the prime determinant of adversity: maternal care. Maternal care has been well recognized as influencing offspring outcome: Gene expression within the brain and the long-lasting emotional and cognitive consequences of early life experience are governed primarily by sensory signals derived from active maternal nurturing behaviors (Baram et al. 2012; Champagne et al. 2003; Raineki et al. 2012; Weaver et al. 2004; Pena et al. 2014; Eghbal-Ahmadi et al. 1999; Suchecki et al. 1993). Most studies have manipulated the quantity of maternal care using well-established models of maternal separation (Stanton and Levine 1990; Aisa et al. 2007; Colorado et al. 2006; Hill et al. 2014; Kundakovic et al. 2013; Matthews et al. 1996; Michaels and Holtzman 2006, 2007). Reduced quantity of maternal care has often led in the offspring to measures of depressive-like behavior in the forced swim test (Stanton and Levine 1990; Aisa et al. 2007; Shalev and Kafkafi 2002; Dimatelis et al. 2012; Lee et al. 2007) as well as to anxiety-like behaviors (Shalev and Kafkafi 2002; Lee et al. 2007). However, the development of anhedonia does not seem to depend on quantity of care: Reduced quantity of maternal care through intermittent deprivation has been reported to increase (Hill et al. 2014; Kundakovic et al. 2013; Matthews et al. 1996; Michaels and Holtzman 2006), reduce (Stanton and Levine 1990), or not change sucrose preference, a measure of anhedonia.

To develop a robust and reproducible model of early life adversity, the Baram Laboratory developed a model of simulated poverty, which involves normal quantity of maternal care but changes the patterns of care. Specifically, limiting bedding and nesting material in the cages induces fragmented and unpredictable patterns of maternal care. This paradigm, adopted widely around the world (Walker et al. 2017), provokes profound anhedonia in adolescent male rats (Molet et al. 2016a; Bolton et al. 2017, 2018; Der-Avakian and Markou 2012). Anhedonia is manifest as both reduced preference for sucrose (a highly rewarding stimulus in rodents) and reduced social play without effecting other social behaviors. More recently, rats exposed to early life fragmentation also exhibit reduced pleasure from sweet rich foods and reduced response to cocaine have been established (Molet et al. 2016a). These data suggest that some types of early life adversity, specifically those associated with disruption of maternal care patterns (e.g., chaotic household, evictions, and change of caretakers) might predispose to pathology specifically in reward response.

5. Anhedonia Reflects Aberrant Brain Circuits Subsequent to Early Life Adversity in Rodents

Cognitive and emotional brain functions involve coordinated activities of brain circuits that integrate molecular, cellular, synaptic, and network signaling (Khazipov et al. 2004; Caspi et al. 2003). And as previously noted, mental disorders may arise from dysfunction (failure) of crucial brain circuits originating from genetic risk and environmental influences, particularly during sensitive developmental periods (Khazipov et al. 2004; Caspi et al. 2003; Heim and Binder 2012). Environment-derived sensory signals clearly influence development of brain circuits (Khazipov et al. 2004; Espinosa and Stryker 2012; Dulac et al. 2014) and, in some cases, may drive aberrant circuit maturation that can promote mental and cognitive problems.

Specifically, we found that a type of early life adversity characterized by unpredictable sensory input early in life reduced sucrose preference and social play during adolescence (Molet et al. 2016a). Both of these behaviors depend on an intact dopaminergic pleasure/reward circuitry (Dym et al. 2009; Kraft et al. 2015; Muscat and Willner 1989; Siviy et al. 2011; Siviy and Panksepp 2011; Trezza et al. 2010; Vanderschuren et al. 1997). The dopaminergic pleasure/reward system is not fully mature until the third postnatal week in rodents (Voorn et al. 1988) and is sensitive to the influence of early life experiences (Pena et al. 2014; Ventura et al. 2013). It has been reported that predictable sequences of events engage and shape the reward system (Berns et al. 2001; Rutledge et al. 2014). These observations lead us to speculate that predictable patterns of maternal care provide crucial cues for maturation of the pleasure/reward system (Pena et al. 2014; Khazipov et al. 2004; Berns et al. 2001). In the absence of such input, the ability to experience reward from pleasurable sensations including the sweetness of sucrose or the joy of play with peers might be (Pena et al. 2014; Ventura et al. 2013) generating anhedonia (Romer Thomsen et al. 2015). Indeed, recent data demonstrate aberrant interactions of reward and fear/anxiety circuits after early life FRAG (Der-Avakian and Markou 2012; Glenn et al. 2017).

Sensory input early in life governs neuronal activity, which influences circuit maturation (Khazipov et al. 2004; Espinosa and Stryker 2012; Evans et al. 2005) as demonstrated in visual, tactile, and olfactory circuits. We propose that patterns of maternal-derived sensory input influence the maturation of both fear and pleasure–reward emotional systems within the developing brain. The pleasure/reward circuit in rodents is remarkably similar to that found in the human (Russo and Nestler 2013; Der-Avakian and Markou 2012; Nestler 2015), comprised of connectivity between the VTA, NAc, PFC, amygdala, and the hippocampus. The FRAG model appears to disrupt this circuit at a number of nodes, including the amygdala, hippocampus, frontal cortex, and NAc. First, we have recently reported that FRAG produces altered connectivity in adolescent rats between the medial PFC and amygdala as measured by diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) (Bolton et al. 2018). The anhedonia phenotype in the FRAG model is reversed by transcriptional suppression of corticotropin releasing hormone (CRH) signaling in the amygdala, suggesting that augmented CRH release from amygdala-centered cell bodies is required for anhedonia associated with FRAG exposure. Another group recently examined FRAG effects on resting state fMRI of the reward circuit in adults, reporting reduced connectivity between the PFC and caudate putamen in FRAG exposed rats compared to controls (Yan et al. 2017). Finally, in the FRAG model of early life adversity, there is a significant loss of hippocampal dendritic arborization and volumes as well as increased fractional anisotropy (FA – a measure of structural integrity derived from DTI) in the hippocampus (Molet et al. 2016b). These hippocampal effects of FRAG also have real functional consequences. We have recently reported that FRAG across both humans and rodents, as measured by entropy of maternal interactions with offspring, is associated with poor performance in hippocampus-dependent memory tasks in later life (Davis et al. 2017; Molet et al. 2016b). Importantly, low hippocampal volume is a robust phenotype in PTSD, and both twin and prospective studies indicate that reductions in hippocampal function and structure are risk factors for development of PTSD [for review, see (Acheson et al. 2012; Logue et al. 2018; van Rooij et al. 2017)]. In rodents, high hippocampal FA correlates with susceptibility to chronic social defeat stress (Anacker et al. 2016), suggesting that across species, developmental effects on hippocampal circuits and functions may play a role in trauma susceptibility.

6. Conclusions and Future Research Directions

There is clear evidence for anhedonia and reward disruption in PTSD, but if these symptoms develop after trauma or contribute to pre-trauma risk is still very unclear. There are hints from the depression field and our own preliminary data in the MRS that adolescent anhedonia may be predictive of later risk for depression and PTSD. Childhood adversity is robustly linked to PTSD risk and anhedonia, but the specific early life experiences that contribute to this risk are not well understood. The FRAG model of early life adversity in both animals and humans suggests that anhedonia in adolescence/early adulthood can be a consequence of chaotic sensory and maternal signals during development. First, entropy of maternal mood during pregnancy influences mood and anxiety of her children at ages 10–13 (Glynn et al. 2017). In rodents, FRAG is robustly linked to increased anhedonia-like behaviors in adolescence with concurrent alterations in reward circuits (Bolton et al. 2018; Glenn et al. 2017). As discussed above, mood in adolescence is important an predictor of neuropsychiatric disorders in adulthood (Carlson and Pataki 2016; Hofstra et al. 2002; Copeland et al. 2014). Second, FRAG, as assessed by observing entropy of maternal interactions with infants in both rodents and humans, is associated with reductions in hippocampal function in later life (Davis et al. 2017). Hippocampal function has emerged as an important risk factor for the development of trauma disorders (Acheson et al. 2012; Glenn et al. 2017) and may provide an additional mechanism through which FRAG could contribute to PTSD risk. Clearly, research is required to understand the developmental trajectory of FRAG effects on circuit development, and most importantly, its contribution as an independent risk factor for trauma disorders. To begin to answer these questions, our group has developed novel measures in humans to operationalize FRAG: (1) in utero via entropy of maternal mood (Glynn et al. 2017), (2) in early life via observational studies of predictability of maternal care (Davis et al. 2017), and (3) in later life via a retrospective self-report instrument to document past FRAG exposure for use in adult populations such as in the MRS. These complementary measures of FRAG will support efforts to integrate studies across age groups and high-risk populations to identify the developmental trajectory of FRAG effects on reward circuits and subsequent anhedonia, and their contribution to adult trauma disorders.

Acknowledgements

Support for this work includes NIMH P50MH096889 (Drs. Baram, Glynn, Davis, Sandman, Stern, Keator, and Baram) and MH73136 (Baram), a VA Merit Award and NIH R01AA026560 (Risbrough), project No. SDR 09-0128 (Drs. Baker and Risbrough) from the Veterans Administration Health Service Research and Development, the US Marine Corps and Navy Bureau of Medicine and Surgery (Drs. Baker, and Risbrough).

Contributor Information

Victoria B. Risbrough, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA; Center of Excellence for Stress and Mental Health, San Diego Veterans Administration, La Jolla, CA, USA

Laura M. Glynn, Department of Psychology, Chapman University, Orange, CA, USA; Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA

Elysia P. Davis, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA; Department of Psychology, University of Denver, Denver, CO, USA

Curt A. Sandman, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA

Andre Obenaus, Department of Pediatrics, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA.

Hal S. Stern, Department of Statistics, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA

David B. Keator, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA; Department of Computer Science, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA; Department of Neurology, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA

Michael A. Yassa, Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA; Center for the Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA

Tallie Z. Baram, Department of Pediatrics, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA; Department of Neurology, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA; Department of Anatomy/Neurobiology, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA

Dewleen G. Baker, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA; Center of Excellence for Stress and Mental Health, San Diego Veterans Administration, La Jolla, CA, USA

References

  1. Acheson DT, Gresack JE, Risbrough VB (2012) Hippocampal dysfunction effects on context memory: possible etiology for posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropharmacology 62(2):674–685 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Admon R et al. (2013a) Imbalanced neural responsivity to risk and reward indicates stress vulnerability in humans. Cereb Cortex 23(1):28–35 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Admon R, Milad MR, Hendler T (2013b) A causal model of post-traumatic stress disorder: disentangling predisposed from acquired neural abnormalities. Trends Cogn Sci 17(7):337–347 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Agorastos A et al. (2014) The cumulative effect of different childhood trauma types on self-reported symptoms of adult male depression and PTSD, substance abuse and health-related quality of life in a large active-duty military cohort. J Psychiatr Res 58:46–54 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Aisa B et al. (2007) Cognitive impairment associated to HPA axis hyperactivity after maternal separation in rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32(3):256–266 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Anacker C et al. (2016) Neuroanatomic differences associated with stress susceptibility and resilience. Biol Psychiatry 79(10):840–849 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baker DG et al. (2009) Trauma exposure, branch of service, and physical injury in relation to mental health among U.S. veterans returning from Iraq and Afghanistan. Mil Med 174(8):773–778 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Baker DG et al. (2012) Predictors of risk and resilience for posttraumatic stress disorder among ground combat Marines: methods of the Marine Resiliency Study. Prev Chronic Dis 9:E97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bale TL et al. (2010) Early life programming and neurodevelopmental disorders. Biol Psychiatry 68(4):314–319 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Baram TZ et al. (2012) Fragmentation and unpredictability of early-life experience in mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry 169(9):907–915 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Barch D et al. (2016) Effect of hippocampal and amygdala connectivity on the relationship between preschool poverty and school-age depression. Am J Psychiatry 173(6):625–634 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Belsky J, Fearon RM (2002) Early attachment security, subsequent maternal sensitivity, and later child development: does continuity in development depend upon continuity of caregiving? Attach Hum Dev 4(3):361–387 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Berghorst LH et al. (2013) Acute stress selectively reduces reward sensitivity. Front Hum Neurosci 7:133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Berns GS et al. (2001) Predictability modulates human brain response to reward. J Neurosci 21(8):2793–2798 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bewernick BH et al. (2010) Nucleus accumbens deep brain stimulation decreases ratings of depression and anxiety in treatment-resistant depression. Biol Psychiatry 67(2):110–116 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bewernick BH et al. (2012) Long-term effects of nucleus accumbens deep brain stimulation in treatment-resistant depression: evidence for sustained efficacy. Neuropsychopharmacology 37(9):1975–1985 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bogdan R, Pizzagalli DA (2006) Acute stress reduces reward responsiveness: implications for depression. Biol Psychiatry 60(10):1147–1154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bolton JL et al. (2017) New insights into early-life stress and behavioral outcomes. Curr Opin Behav Sci 14:133–139 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bolton JL et al. (2018) Anhedonia following early-life adversity involves aberrant interaction of reward and anxiety circuits and is reversed by partial silencing of amygdala corticotropin-releasing hormone gene. Biol Psychiatry 83(2):137–147 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Bress JN et al. (2013) Blunted neural response to rewards prospectively predicts depression in adolescent girls. Psychophysiology 50(1):74–81 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cao X et al. (2016) DSM-5 posttraumatic stress disorder symptom structure in disaster-exposed adolescents: stability across gender and relation to behavioral problems. J Abnorm Child Psychol [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Carlson GA, Pataki C (2016) Understanding early age of onset: a review of the last 5 years. Curr Psychiatry Rep 18(12):114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Caspi A et al. (2003) Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science 301(5631):386–389 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Champagne FA et al. (2003) Variations in maternal care in the rat as a mediating influence for the effects of environment on development. Physiol Behav 79(3):359–371 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Colorado RA et al. (2006) Effects of maternal separation, early handling, and standard facility rearing on orienting and impulsive behavior of adolescent rats. Behav Processes 71(1):51–58 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Copeland WE et al. (2007) Traumatic events and posttraumatic stress in childhood. Arch Gen Psychiatry 64(5):577–584 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Copeland WE et al. (2014) Longitudinal patterns of anxiety from childhood to adulthood: the Great Smoky Mountains Study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 53(1):21–33 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Davis EP, Sandman CA (2006) Prenatal exposure to maternal stress and stress hormones influences child development. Infants Young Child 19(3):246–259 [Google Scholar]
  29. Davis EP, Sandman CA (2010) The timing of prenatal exposure to maternal cortisol and psychosocial stress is associated with human infant cognitive development. Child Dev 81(1):131–148 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Davis EP, Sandman CA (2012) Prenatal psychobiological predictors of anxiety risk in preadolescent children. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37(8):1224–1233 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Davis EP et al. (2007) Prenatal exposure to maternal depression and cortisol influences infant temperament. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 46(6):737–746 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Davis EP et al. (2011a) Children’s brain development benefits from longer gestation. Front Psychol 2:1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Davis EP et al. (2011b) Prenatal maternal stress programs infant stress regulation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 52(2):119–129 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Davis EP et al. (2017) Exposure to unpredictable maternal sensory signals influences cognitive development across species. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 114(39):10390–10395 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Der-Avakian A, Markou A (2012) The neurobiology of anhedonia and other reward-related deficits. Trends Neurosci 35(1):68–77 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Dillon DG et al. (2009) Childhood adversity is associated with left basal ganglia dysfunction during reward anticipation in adulthood. Biol Psychiatry 66(3):206–213 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Dimatelis JJ, Stein DJ, Russell VA (2012) Behavioral changes after maternal separation are reversed by chronic constant light treatment. Brain Res 1480:61–71 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Does amount of time spent in child care predict socioemotional adjustment during the transition to kindergarten? (2003) Child Dev 74(4):976–1005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Drevets WC et al. (1992) A functional anatomical study of unipolar depression. J Neurosci 12(9):3628–3641 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Dulac C, O’Connell LA, Wu Z (2014) Neural control of maternal and paternal behaviors. Science 345(6198):765–770 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Dym CT et al. (2009) Genetic variance contributes to dopamine receptor antagonist-induced inhibition of sucrose intake in inbred and outbred mouse strains. Brain Res 1257:40–52 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Eghbal-Ahmadi M et al. (1999) Differential regulation of the expression of corticotropin-releasing factor receptor type 2 (CRF2) in hypothalamus and amygdala of the immature rat by sensory input and food intake. J Neurosci 19(10):3982–3991 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Elman I et al. (2005) Probing reward function in post-traumatic stress disorder with beautiful facial images. Psychiatry Res 135(3):179–183 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Elman I et al. (2009) Functional neuroimaging of reward circuitry responsivity to monetary gains and losses in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 66(12):1083–1090 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Espinosa JS, Stryker MP (2012) Development and plasticity of the primary visual cortex. Neuron 75(2):230–249 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Etkin A, Wager TD (2007) Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. Am J Psychiatry 164(10):1476–1488 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Evans GW, Kim P (2007) Childhood poverty and health: cumulative risk exposure and stress dysregulation. Psychol Sci 18(11):953–957 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Evans GW, Kim P (2012) Childhood poverty and young adults’ allostatic load: the mediating role of childhood cumulative risk exposure. Psychol Sci 23(9):979–983 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Evans GW et al. (2005) The role of chaos in poverty and children’s socioemotional adjustment. Psychol Sci 16(7):560–565 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Feldman R (2007) Parent-infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 48(3–4):329–354 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Feldman R (2010) The relational basis of adolescent adjustment: trajectories of mother-child interactive behaviors from infancy to adolescence shape adolescents’ adaptation. Attach Hum Dev 12(1–2):173–192 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Feldman R (2015) Mutual influences between child emotion regulation and parent-child reciprocity support development across the first 10 years of life: implications for developmental psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol 27(4 Pt 1):1007–1023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Franklin CL, Zimmerman M (2001) Posttraumatic stress disorder and major depressive disorder: investigating the role of overlapping symptoms in diagnostic comorbidity. J Nerv Ment Dis 189(8):548–551 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Frewen PA, Dozois DJ, Lanius RA (2012) Assessment of anhedonia in psychological trauma: psychometric and neuroimaging perspectives. Eur J Psychotraumatol 3. 10.3402/ejpt.v3i0.8587 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Glenn DE et al. (2017) The future of contextual fear learning for PTSD research: a methodological review of neuroimaging studies. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 10.1007/7854_2017_30 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Glynn LM et al. (2007) Postnatal maternal cortisol levels predict temperament in healthy breastfed infants. Early Hum Dev 83(10):675–681 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Glynn LM et al. (2017) Prenatal maternal mood patterns predict child temperament and adolescent mental health. J Affect Disord 228:83–90 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Goff B et al. (2013) Reduced nucleus accumbens reactivity and adolescent depression following early-life stress. Neuroscience 249:129–138 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Goodman SH, Gotlib IH (1999) Risk for psychopathology in the children of depressed mothers: a developmental model for understanding mechanisms of transmission. Psychol Rev 106(3):458–490 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Guyer AE et al. (2006) Behavioral alterations in reward system function: the role of childhood maltreatment and psychopathology. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 45(9):1059–1067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Hane AA et al. (2010) Ordinary variations in human maternal caregiving in infancy and biobehavioral development in early childhood: a follow-up study. Dev Psychobiol 52(6):558–567 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Hanson JL, Hariri AR, Williamson DE (2015) Blunted ventral striatum development in adolescence reflects emotional neglect and predicts depressive symptoms. Biol Psychiatry 78(9):598–605 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Hanson JL et al. (2016) Cumulative stress in childhood is associated with blunted reward-related brain activity in adulthood. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 11(3):405–412 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Hanson JL et al. (2017) Early adversity and learning: implications for typical and atypical behavioral development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 58(7):770–778 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Heim C, Binder EB (2012) Current research trends in early life stress and depression: review of human studies on sensitive periods, gene-environment interactions, and epigenetics. Exp Neurol 233(1):102–111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Hill RA et al. (2014) Sex-specific disruptions in spatial memory and anhedonia in a “two hit” rat model correspond with alterations in hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor expression and signaling. Hippocampus 24(10):1197–1211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Hofstra MB, Van Der Ende JAN, Verhulst FC (2002) Child and adolescent problems predict DSM-IV disorders in adulthood: a 14-year follow-up of a Dutch epidemiological sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 41(2):182–189 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Hopper JW et al. (2008) Probing reward function in posttraumatic stress disorder: expectancy and satisfaction with monetary gains and losses. J Psychiatr Res 42(10):802–807 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Howland MA et al. (2016) Fetal exposure to placental corticotropin-releasing hormone is associated with child self-reported internalizing symptoms. Psychoneuroendocrinology 67:10–17 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Insel TR (2011) The global cost of mental illness. [Cited 28 September 2011]. https://www.nimh.nih.gov/about/directors/thomas-insel/blog/2011/the-global-cost-of-mental-illness.shtml
  71. Javanbakht A et al. (2016) Sex-specific effects of childhood poverty on neurocircuitry of processing of emotional cues: a neuroimaging study. Behav Sci (Basel) 6(4) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Johnson SB, Riis JL, Noble KG (2016) State of the art review: poverty and the developing brain. Pediatrics 137(4). pii: e20153075. 10.1542/peds.2015-3075 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Kalebasi N et al. (2015) Blunted responses to reward in remitted post-traumatic stress disorder. Brain Behav 5(8):e00357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Kashdan TB, Elhai JD, Frueh BC (2006) Anhedonia and emotional numbing in combat veterans with PTSD. Behav Res Ther 44(3):457–467 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Khazipov R et al. (2004) Early motor activity drives spindle bursts in the developing somatosensory cortex. Nature 432(7018):758–761 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Kim P et al. (2013) Effects of childhood poverty and chronic stress on emotion regulatory brain function in adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110(46):18442–18447 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Kim DJ et al. (2014) Longer gestation is associated with more efficient brain networks in preadolescent children. Neuroimage 100:619–627 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Kim DJ et al. (2016) Children’s intellectual ability is associated with structural network integrity. Neuroimage 124(Pt A):550–556 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Kim DJ et al. (2017) Prenatal maternal cortisol has sex-specific associations with child brain network properties. Cereb Cortex 27(11):5230–5241 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Koch SB et al. (2016) Aberrant resting-state brain activity in posttraumatic stress disorder: a meta-analysis and systematic review. Depress Anxiety 33(7):592–605 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Kraft TT et al. (2015) Dopamine D1 and opioid receptor antagonist-induced reductions of fructose and saccharin intake in BALB/c and SWR inbred mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 131:13–18 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Kumar P et al. (2014) Differential effects of acute stress on anticipatory and consummatory phases of reward processing. Neuroscience 266:1–12 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Kundakovic M et al. (2013) Sex-specific and strain-dependent effects of early life adversity on behavioral and epigenetic outcomes. Front Psych 4:78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Lee JH et al. (2007) Depressive behaviors and decreased expression of serotonin reuptake transporter in rats that experienced neonatal maternal separation. Neurosci Res 58(1):32–39 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Lee JS et al. (2012) Hippocampus and nucleus accumbens activity during neutral word recognition related to trait physical anhedonia in patients with schizophrenia: an fMRI study. Psychiatry Res 203(1):46–53 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Lewis JD et al. (2015) Anhedonia in combat veterans with penetrating head injury. Brain Imaging Behav 9(3):456–460 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Liu H et al. (2017) Association of DSM-IV posttraumatic stress disorder with traumatic experience type and history in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. JAMA Psychiat 74(3):270–281 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Logue MW et al. (2018) Smaller hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress disorder: a multisite ENIGMA-PGC study: subcortical volumetry results from posttraumatic stress disorder consortia. Biol Psychiatry 83(3):244–253 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Lowe SR et al. (2016) Childhood trauma and neighborhood-level crime interact in predicting adult posttraumatic stress and major depression symptoms. Child Abuse Negl 51:212–222 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Lupien SJ et al. (2009) Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 10(6):434–445 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Masur EF, Flynn V, Eichorst DL (2005) Maternal responsive and directive behaviours and utterances as predictors of children’s lexical development. J Child Lang 32(1):63–91 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Matthews K, Wilkinson LS, Robbins TW (1996) Repeated maternal separation of preweanling rats attenuates behavioral responses to primary and conditioned incentives in adulthood. Physiol Behav 59(1):99–107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Mayberg HS et al. (2000) Regional metabolic effects of fluoxetine in major depression: serial changes and relationship to clinical response. Biol Psychiatry 48(8):830–843 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. McLaughlin KA et al. (2010) Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication II: associations with persistence of DSM-IV disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 67(2):124–132 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Mehta MA et al. (2010) Hyporesponsive reward anticipation in the basal ganglia following severe institutional deprivation early in life. J Cogn Neurosci 22(10):2316–2325 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Merikangas KR et al. (2010) Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents: results from the national comorbidity survey replication–adolescent supplement (NCS-A). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 49(10):980–989 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Michaels CC, Holtzman SG (2006) Neonatal stress and litter composition alter sucrose intake in both rat dam and offspring. Physiol Behav 89(5):735–741 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Michaels CC, Holtzman SG (2007) Enhanced sensitivity to naltrexone-induced drinking suppression of fluid intake and sucrose consumption in maternally separated rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 86(4):784–796 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Molet J et al. (2016a) Fragmentation and high entropy of neonatal experience predict adolescent emotional outcome. Transl Psychiatry 6:e702. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Molet J et al. (2016b) MRI uncovers disrupted hippocampal microstructure that underlies memory impairments after early-life adversity. Hippocampus 26(12):1618–1632 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Monk C (2001) Stress and mood disorders during pregnancy: implications for child development. Psychiatry Q 72(4):347–357 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Monk C, Fitelson EM, Werner E (2011) Mood disorders and their pharmacological treatment during pregnancy: is the future child affected? Pediatr Res 69(5 Pt 2):3R–10R [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Mulvihill D (2005) The health impact of childhood trauma: an interdisciplinary review, 1997–2003. Issues Compr Pediatr Nurs 28(2):115–136 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Muscat R, Willner P (1989) Effects of dopamine receptor antagonists on sucrose consumption and preference. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 99(1):98–102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Nawijn L et al. (2015) Reward functioning in PTSD: a systematic review exploring the mechanisms underlying anhedonia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 51:189–204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Nawijn L et al. (2016) Intranasal oxytocin enhances neural processing of monetary reward and loss in post-traumatic stress disorder and traumatized controls. Psychoneuroendocrinology 66:228–237 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Nelson CA (2013) Biological embedding of early life adversity. JAMA Pediatr 167(12):1098–1100 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Nelson CA 3rd et al. (2007) Cognitive recovery in socially deprived young children: the Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Science 318(5858):1937–1940 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Nestler EJ (2015) Role of the brain’s reward circuitry in depression: transcriptional mechanisms. Int Rev Neurobiol 124:151–170 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (1999) Chronicity of maternal depressive symptoms, maternal sensitivity, and child functioning at 36 months. Dev Psychol 35(5):1297–1310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2006) Infant-mother attachment classification: risk and protection in relation to changing maternal caregiving quality. Dev Psychol 42:38–58 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Noble KG et al. (2015) Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nat Neurosci 18(5):773–778 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. O’Donnell ML et al. (2013) Disability after injury: the cumulative burden of physical and mental health. J Clin Psychiatry 74(2):e137–e143 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Oberlander TF et al. (2008) Prenatal exposure to maternal depression, neonatal methylation of human glucocorticoid receptor gene (NR3C1) and infant cortisol stress responses. Epigenetics 3(2):97–106 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Osborne LM, Monk C (2013) Perinatal depression – the fourth inflammatory morbidity of pregnancy?: Theory and literature review. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38(10):1929–1952 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Pechtel P, Pizzagalli DA (2011) Effects of early life stress on cognitive and affective function: an integrated review of human literature. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 214(1):55–70 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Pechtel P, Pizzagalli DA (2013) Disrupted reinforcement learning and maladaptive behavior in women with a history of childhood sexual abuse: a high-density event-related potential study. JAMA Psychiat 70(5):499–507 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Pena CJ et al. (2014) Effects of maternal care on the development of midbrain dopamine pathways and reward-directed behavior in female offspring. Eur J Neurosci 39(6):946–956 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Pizzagalli DA (2014) Depression, stress, and anhedonia: toward a synthesis and integrated model. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 10:393–423 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Pizzagalli DA et al. (2008) Reduced hedonic capacity in major depressive disorder: evidence from a probabilistic reward task. J Psychiatr Res 43(1):76–87 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Plant DT et al. (2015) Maternal depression during pregnancy and offspring depression in adulthood: role of child maltreatment. Br J Psychiatry 207(3):213–220 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Pynoos RS, Steinberg AM, Piacentini JC (1999) A developmental psychopathology model of childhood traumatic stress and intersection with anxiety disorders. Biol Psychiatry 46(11):1542–1554 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Raineki C et al. (2012) Effects of early-life abuse differ across development: infant social behavior deficits are followed by adolescent depressive-like behaviors mediated by the amygdala. J Neurosci 32(22):7758–7765 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Rao U et al. (2010) Hippocampal changes associated with early-life adversity and vulnerability to depression. Biol Psychiatry 67(4):357–364 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Risbrough V et al. (2017) Pre-deployment anhedonia is a risk factor for posttraumatic psychopathology after combat trauma. In: ACNP 56th annual meeting. Neuropsychopharmacology. Palm Springs, CA, pp S111–S293 [Google Scholar]
  126. Rizvi SJ et al. (2016) Assessing anhedonia in depression: potentials and pitfalls. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 65:21–35 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Romer Thomsen K, Whybrow PC, Kringelbach ML (2015) Reconceptualizing anhedonia: novel perspectives on balancing the pleasure networks in the human brain. Front Behav Neurosci 9:49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Russo SJ, Nestler EJ (2013) The brain reward circuitry in mood disorders. Nat Rev Neurosci 14(9):609–625 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Rutledge RB et al. (2014) A computational and neural model of momentary subjective well-being. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111(33):12252–12257 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Sailer U et al. (2008) Altered reward processing in the nucleus accumbens and mesial prefrontal cortex of patients with posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychologia 46(11):2836–2844 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Sandman CA, Davis EP, Glynn LM (2012) Prescient human fetuses thrive. Psychol Sci 23(1):93–100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Sandman CA et al. (2015) Fetal exposure to maternal depressive symptoms is associated with cortical thickness in late childhood. Biol Psychiatry 77(4):324–334 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Schlaepfer TE et al. (2008) Deep brain stimulation to reward circuitry alleviates anhedonia in refractory major depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 33(2):368–377 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Seedat S, Stein MB (2001) Post-traumatic stress disorder: a review of recent findings. Curr Psychiatry Rep 3(4):288–294 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Shalev U, Kafkafi N (2002) Repeated maternal separation does not alter sucrose-reinforced and open-field behaviors. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 73(1):115–122 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Sharma A et al. (2017) Common dimensional reward deficits across mood and psychotic disorders: a connectome-wide association study. Am J Psychiatry 174(7):657–666 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Siviy SM, Panksepp J (2011) In search of the neurobiological substrates for social playfulness in mammalian brains. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 35(9):1821–1830 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Siviy SM et al. (2011) Dysfunctional play and dopamine physiology in the Fischer 344 rat. Behav Brain Res 220(2):294–304 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Spitzer EG et al. (2018) Posttraumatic stress disorder symptom clusters and acquired capability for suicide: a reexamination using DSM-5 criteria. Suicide Life Threat Behav 48(1):105–115 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Stanton ME, Levine S (1990) Inhibition of infant glucocorticoid stress response: specific role of maternal cues. Dev Psychobiol 23(5):411–426 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Stevens JS et al. (2017) Amygdala reactivity and anterior cingulate habituation predict post-traumatic stress disorder symptom maintenance after acute civilian trauma. Biol Psychiatry 81(12):1023–1029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Stout SA et al. (2015) Fetal programming of children’s obesity risk. Psychoneuroendocrinology 53:29–39 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Stuhrmann A et al. (2013) Mood-congruent amygdala responses to subliminally presented facial expressions in major depression: associations with anhedonia. J Psychiatry Neurosci 38(4):249–258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Suchecki D, Rosenfeld P, Levine S (1993) Maternal regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in the infant rat: the roles of feeding and stroking. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 75(2):185–192 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Thomas JL et al. (2010) Prevalence of mental health problems and functional impairment among active component and National Guard soldiers 3 and 12 months following combat in Iraq. Arch Gen Psychiatry 67(6):614–623 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Treadway MT et al. (2009) Early adverse events, HPA activity and rostral anterior cingulate volume in MDD. PLoS One 4(3):e4887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Trezza V, Baarendse PJ, Vanderschuren LJ (2010) The pleasures of play: pharmacological insights into social reward mechanisms. Trends Pharmacol Sci 31(10):463–469 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. van Rooij SJH et al. (2017) The role of the hippocampus in predicting future posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in recently traumatized civilians. Biol Psychiatry. pii: S0006-3223(17) 31989–3. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.09.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Vanderschuren LJ, Niesink RJ, Van Ree JM (1997) The neurobiology of social play behavior in rats. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 21(3):309–326 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Ventura R et al. (2013) Postnatal aversive experience impairs sensitivity to natural rewards and increases susceptibility to negative events in adult life. Cereb Cortex 23(7):1606–1617 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Voorn P et al. (1988) The pre- and postnatal development of the dopaminergic cell groups in the ventral mesencephalon and the dopaminergic innervation of the striatum of the rat. Neuroscience 25(3):857–887 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Vujanovic AA et al. (2017) Reward functioning in posttraumatic stress and substance use disorders. Curr Opin Psychol 14:49–55 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Walker CD et al. (2017) Chronic early life stress induced by limited bedding and nesting (LBN) material in rodents: critical considerations of methodology, outcomes and translational potential. Stress 20(5):421–448 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Weaver IC et al. (2004) Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nat Neurosci 7(8):847–854 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Whitton AE et al. (2016) Blunted neural responses to reward in remitted major depression: a high-density event-related potential study. Biol Psychiatry 1(1):87–95 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Yan CG et al. (2017) Aberrant development of intrinsic brain activity in a rat model of caregiver maltreatment of offspring. Transl Psychiatry 7(1):e1005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Young A et al. (2017) The effects of early institutionalization on emotional face processing: evidence for sparing via an experience-dependent mechanism. Br J Dev Psychol 35(3):439–453 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Zhang B et al. (2016) Mapping anhedonia-specific dysfunction in a transdiagnostic approach: an ALE meta-analysis. Brain Imaging Behav 10(3):920–939 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES