Abstract
Background:
Oncologists often refer to forest plots to determine which patient subgroups may be more likely to benefit from a therapy tested in a randomized clinical trial (RCT). We sought to empirically determine the information content of subgroup comparisons from forest plots of RCTs.
Methods:
We assessed all forest plots from RCTs of therapeutic interventions presented orally at the American Society of Clinical Oncology Annual Meetings in 2020 and 2021. Subgroups were considered as showing evidence of treatment effect heterogeneity in forest plots when their confidence intervals (CIs) did not overlap with the vertical line corresponding to the main effect observed in the overall RCT cohort. Subgroups were considered as showing evidence of treatment effect homogeneity in forest plots when their CIs did not meaningfully differ, within 80–125% equivalence range, with the values compatible with the main effect. All other subgroups were considered as inconclusive.
Results:
A total of 99 forest plots were presented, and only 24.2% contained one or more subgroups suggestive of treatment effect heterogeneity. A total of 81 forest plots provided enough information to evaluate treatment effect heterogeneity and homogeneity. These 81 forest plots represented a total of 1344 individual subgroups, of which 57.2% were inconclusive, 41.1% showed evidence of treatment effect homogeneity, and 1.6% yielded evidence suggestive of treatment effect heterogeneity.
Conclusion:
The majority of subgroup comparisons were inconclusive in this empirical analysis of forest plots used in oncology RCTs. Different strategies should be considered to improve the estimation and representation of subgroup-specific effects.
Keywords: forest plots, precision medicine, predictive biomarkers, subgroup analyses
Introduction
Oncologists are regularly tasked to make individualized recommendations for their patients using evidence derived from clinical trials. 1 A key step toward this goal is to determine whether the estimated treatment effect in the overall trial cohort, also known as the ‘main effect’, varied in the subset of trial participants most relevant to the patient seen in the clinic. 2 Forest plots are a commonly used tool to visualize and facilitate such subgroup comparisons in oncology clinical trials.1,3 A common mistake when interpreting forest plots is to conclude that the treatment effect is not significant for subgroups with confidence intervals (CIs) that cross the vertical line corresponding to the null point of no effect, that is, 1.0 when the hazard ratio (HR) relative scale is used.1,4,5 However, crossing the null point only means that the data for these subgroups were statistically compatible with no effect; the data may be equally or more compatible with many other values (Figure 1).4,6
To facilitate interpretation of forest plots, Cuzick 5 proposed to de-emphasize the no-effect point and instead focus on the vertical line corresponding to the point estimate for the main effect. This is because the question clinicians are interested in when looking at forest plots is whether the treatment effect in a particular subgroup differs from the reported main effect. If the CIs of a subgroup do not cross the vertical line corresponding to the main effect, then this can serve as a signal of treatment effect heterogeneity that may merit further exploration.1,3,5 More comprehensive subgroup analyses may include tests for treatment-by-subgroup interactions to formally determine whether the relative treatment effect, commonly expressed by HRs in oncology, varies across subgroups.3,7–9 However, some guidelines recommend against the presentation of p values of treatment-by-subgroup tests for interaction in forest plots due to the risk of misinterpretation and type I error inflation from multiple comparisons. 10 However, the same concerns apply for the crude visualization of subgroup point estimates and their CIs in forest plots, with the added limitation that forest plots are less informative and less sensitive to potential signals of exploratory subgroup differences than full modeling for treatment-by-subgroup interaction effects. 3 Motivated by these considerations, we empirically evaluated the information content of forest plots in studies presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Society of Clinical Oncology (ASCO).
Methods
Study design and outcome definitions
We focused on the most recent ASCO Annual Meetings, 2020 and 2021, and evaluated all oral presentations (Plenary Sessions, n = 30, or Oral Abstract Sessions, n = 646). Presentations were included if they reported the results of a randomized clinical trial (RCT) that evaluated any therapeutic intervention (n = 147). We initially screened each presentation that met inclusion criteria for the presence of a forest plot, and if present, we descriptively characterized the forest plot using consistent terminology (Figure 1 and Supplemental File 1).
Although forest plots are based on refutational metrics naturally suited toward demonstrating treatment effect heterogeneity,5,6,11 readers often seek to determine evidence of treatment effect homogeneity (i.e. lack of treatment effect heterogeneity) from forest plots. To facilitate this task, we developed a simple approach (see section on ‘Assessing treatment effect homogeneity in forest plots’ below for additional details) and calculator (Supplemental File 2) to estimate an ‘indifference zone’ of no clinically meaningful difference between the main effect and each subgroup visualized by a forest plot (Figure 1). We used indifference limits of 80–125% corresponding to the commonly used thresholds for bioequivalence and clinical equivalence. 12 A subgroup comparison represented by a forest plot was accordingly deemed informative if it either (1) provided a signal of subgroup treatment effect heterogeneity compared with the main effect as evidenced by the subgroup CIs not overlapping with the main effect 5 (e.g. subgroups 2, 3, and 4 in Figure 1), or (2) indicated subgroup treatment effect homogeneity compared with the main effect as evidenced by the subgroup CIs being only compatible with values within the indifference zone (e.g. subgroups 6 and 8 in Figure 1). Forest plots providing no evidence of treatment effect heterogeneity or homogeneity were deemed inconclusive (e.g. subgroups 1, 5, 7, and 9 in Figure 1).
Assessing treatment effect homogeneity in forest plots
Theoretical considerations
In the frequentist approach most commonly used in forest plots, the point estimate is the value that the data are most compatible with the background statistical modeling assumptions. 11 The subgroup CIs include all other values compatible with the data under the background assumptions at the specified confidence level, which is usually set at 95% confidence, corresponding to hypothesis tests at the 0.05 type I error alpha level.11,13 The higher statistical uncertainty inherent in subgroup analyses can yield wide CIs that may include values compatible with favoring the treatment or control group and may include the null point. No meaningful conclusions can be drawn when the wide CIs of a subgroup cover divergent treatment effects. Conversely, a common error known as ‘second-order nullism’ is to conclude consistency of treatment effect between a subgroup and the overall cohort when the CIs of the subgroup do cross the vertical line at the main effect level. 14 This ‘absence of evidence is not evidence of absence’ fallacy is a more intricate, but equally incorrect version of first-order nullism whereby a large p value is thought to provide evidence in support of no treatment effect.6,15 The wide CIs of patient subgroups may include treatment effect values that are not compatible with those included in the typically more narrow CIs of the overall trial cohort. Thus, accepting the null hypothesis of no difference between a subgroup and the overall cohort based on the lack of a statistical signal in noisy data obscured by random error may mislead researchers into failing to capture subtle, but real signals of treatment effect heterogeneity.6,15,16 It is therefore more prudent to deem the results of such subgroups with wide CIs represented by forest plots as ‘inconclusive’.1,4,14,17,18
First- and second-order nullism arise because frequentist metrics are naturally refutational and are thus well-suited to detecting signals of incompatibility with the tested hypothesis (usually the null hypothesis), such as the absence of treatment effect homogeneity.6,11,19,20 Refutation of the null hypothesis of treatment effect homogeneity suggests the presence of treatment effect heterogeneity. On the other hand, determining the presence of treatment effect homogeneity is a more indirect task that typically requires first the specification of equivalence or noninferiority margins and then to show that the CIs of the subgroup of interest lie within these margins.21,22 However, no such approach has been developed to date for forest plots of subgroup differences in RCTs, despite the clinical interest in this setting to detect signals of treatment effect homogeneity between subgroups and the main effect. To address this need, we developed a practical approach and calculator (Supplemental File 2) based on the estimation of indifference margins defining an ‘indifference zone’ of no clinically meaningful difference between the main effect and each subgroup visualized by a forest plot.
Indifference zone estimation
All indifference zone estimations are performed in the log-scale (additive scale) because modeling of relative treatment effect estimates, such as HRs, in RCTs is always done in this scale. The indifference zone can be specified based on clinically plausible indifference limits. For example, treatment effects that differ within 80–125% of each other are commonly considered to be clinically equivalent. At the HR scale, this corresponds to a HR for the main treatment effect being clinically meaningful when it is less than 0.8 or greater than 1.25. Note that the inverse of 0.8 (1/0.8) = 1.25 and thus specifying the lower indifference limit (e.g. 80%) will automatically yield the corresponding upper indifference limit (e.g. 125%). The 80–125% criterion is also typically used as the bioequivalence limit by the World Health Organization and the United States Food and Drug Administration. 12 We accordingly used this limit to define the indifference zone used in the present study.
The point estimate and CIs of the overall group define the main treatment effect values that are most compatible with the observed data at the specified confidence level, which is usually set at 95% confidence. This defines the confidence margin that includes all values compatible with the data for the main treatment effect at the specified confidence level. The indifference zone is used to determine the treatment effect values that differ within 80–125% (or any other specified indifference limit) from the confidence margin of the main treatment effect. The specified indifference level i (e.g. 0.8, corresponding to 80%) and its inverse (e.g. 1/0.8 = 1.25, corresponding to 125%) is logarithmically transformed to the additive scale (loge(i) and loge(1/i), respectively). Loge(i) is then added to the log-transformed lower bound of the main treatment effect CI to obtain the lower bound of the indifference zone, whereas loge(1/i) is added to the log-transformed upper bound of the main treatment effect CI to obtain the upper bound of the indifference zone. The estimated lower and upper bounds of the indifference zone can then be exponentiated from the additive scale to the relative treatment effect scale commonly presented in forest plots.
Indifference zone calculator
We provide a simple calculator (Supplemental File 2) for readers to estimate the lower and upper bounds of the indifference zone for the main treatment effect presented in forest plots. The calculator allows the specification of the indifference level i (e.g. 80%) for relative treatment effect reduction and then estimates the corresponding indifference level 1/i (e.g. 125%) for relative treatment effect increase. The lower and upper bounds of the CI for the main treatment effect shown in the forest plot can then be inputted, and the calculator estimates the corresponding lower and upper bounds of the indifference zone, highlighted in red.
Results
The results are summarized in Table 1, and the detailed extracted features of each abstract are provided in Supplemental File 1. Almost half of the studies used forest plots for subgroup analyses, with some studies displaying more than one forest plot (31.4%). Most forest plots (85.9%) did not include a vertical line at the overall effect point estimate. Out of the 99 forest plots presented, only 24.2% showed one or more subgroups with evidence of treatment effect heterogeneity. Treatment effect homogeneity was not evaluable in 18/99 (18.2%) of forest plots presented, primarily because CI numerical values were not provided. Out of the 1344 individual subgroups presented in the 81 forest plots where both treatment effect heterogeneity and homogeneity were evaluable, 769 were inconclusive (57.2%), 553 indicated treatment effect homogeneity (41.1%), and only 22 yielded a signal suggestive of treatment effect heterogeneity (1.6%).
Table 1.
Forest plot in presentation | |
Yes | 70 |
No | 77 |
Number of forest plots per presentation | |
1 | 48 |
2 | 17 |
3 | 3 |
4 | 2 |
Total number of forest plots analyzed | 99 |
Treatment effect heterogeneity in any subgroup shown in each of the 99 forest plots (%) | |
Yes | 24 (24.2) |
No | 75 (75.8) |
Treatment effect heterogeneity in each individual subgroup shown in the 99 forest plots (%) | |
Yes | 36 (2.2) |
No | 1576 (97.8) |
Total number of forest plots evaluable for treatment effect homogeneity | 81 |
Interpretation of each individual subgroup shown the 81 forest plots evaluable for homogeneity (%) | |
Homogeneity present | 553 (41.1) |
Heterogeneity present | 22 (1.6) |
Inconclusive | 769 (57.2) |
p-Values for interaction shown (%) | |
Yes | 29 (29.3) |
No | 70 (70.7) |
Vertical line at overall effect point estimate (%) | |
Yes | 14 (14.1) |
No | 85 (85.9) |
Statistical approach used (%) | |
Frequentist | 99 (100) |
Bayesian | 0 (0) |
Specified confidence level (%) | |
95% | 95 (96.0) |
Other | 4 (4.0) |
95% CI numerical value shown (%) | |
Yes | 85 (85.9) |
No | 14 (14.1) |
Forest plot endpoint (%) | |
OS | 39 (39.4) |
PFS | 35 (35.4) |
DFS | 17 (17.2) |
Other | 8 (8.1) |
Relative outcome scale (%) | |
HR | 98 (99.0) |
OR | 1 (1.0) |
Disease setting (%) | |
Metastatic | 72 (72.7) |
Adjuvant | 24 (24.2) |
Neoadjuvant | 3 (3.0) |
Type of intervention (%) | |
Immune checkpoint therapy | 37 (37.3) |
Targeted therapy | 30 (30.3) |
Chemotherapy | 25 (25.3) |
Hormone | 3 (3.0) |
Other | 3 (3.0) |
Procedural intervention | 1 (1.0) |
Cancer type (%) | |
Breast | 18 (18.2) |
NSCLC | 15 (15.2) |
Colorectal cancer | 12 (12.1) |
Other GI | 12 (12.1) |
Genitourinary | 8 (8.1) |
Malignant heme | 8 (8.1) |
Melanoma | 6 (6.1) |
SCLC | 5 (5.1) |
Gynecologic | 5 (5.1) |
HNSCC | 4 (4.0) |
Sarcoma | 4 (4.0) |
CNS | 1 (1.0) |
Other | 1 (1.0) |
CI, confidence interval; CNS, central nervous system; DFS, disease-free survival; GI, gastrointestinal; HNSCC, head and neck squamous cell carcinoma; HR, hazard ratio; NSCLC, non-small cell lung cancer; OR, odds ratio; OS, overall survival; PFS, progression-free survival; SCLC, small cell lung cancer; heme, hematology.
Discussion
Our results suggest that forest plots are commonly inconclusive when used to determine subgroup differences or similarities in treatment effect in oncology RCTs. This is despite the fact that our study used very lenient definitions for treatment effect heterogeneity and homogeneity. More specifically, we considered as positive any signal of treatment effect heterogeneity evidenced by the subgroup CIs not overlapping with the main effect 5 and defined our indifference zone for treatment effect homogeneity using the bioequivalence limits of 80–125% commonly used by the World Health Organization and the United States Food and Drug Administration and corresponding to the clinically meaningful HR limits of 0.8–1.25 typically used in RCTs. 12 More strict definitions of treatment effect heterogeneity or more narrow bioequivalence limits would have yielded even higher numbers of inconclusive forest plots.
The statistical power of forest plots is reduced by the smaller sample sizes of each subgroup compared with the overall trial population. Given that the subgroup comparisons presented by forest plots are typically underpowered and often inconclusive, cautious interpretation in oral or written presentations should be promoted by journals, professional organizations, and regulatory bodies. In addition to the increased type II error probability due to low power, scanning through multiple subgroups in forest plots also increases type I error.8,9 To reduce type I error, analyses for treatment effect heterogeneity should instead focus on prespecified biologically and clinically plausible subgroups. Statistical power can be improved by full treatment effect modeling that accounts for mediator-outcome confounding and preserves all information from continuous variables by flexibly incorporating them into the analysis model using approaches such as cubic splines.1,17,23,24 Interpretation of forest plots can be facilitated by consistently including the vertical line corresponding to the point estimate for the main effect, 5 and by showing the indifference zone for treatment effect homogeneity using prespecified commonly accepted indifference limits such as 80–125%.
Limitations
We focused our analysis on studies presented at the last two annual ASCO meetings. The ASCO annual meeting is the largest multidisciplinary cancer conference where practice-changing findings from large RCTs are often first presented. Although physical attendance was limited by the COVID-19 pandemic, the 2020 and 2021 ASCO Annual Meetings were highly attended oncology gatherings, and the studies presented are reflective of contemporary oncology practice. Nevertheless, it is possible that the information content was different in forest plots used in previous years, other oncology meetings, journal publications, or different medical fields.
Conclusion
We have performed the first empirical analysis of the information content of forest plots used for subgroup comparisons of treatment effect heterogeneity or homogeneity and have found the majority of forest plots to be inconclusive. Different strategies may therefore be preferable to investigate treatment effect heterogeneity across trial participants.
Supplemental Material
Supplemental material, sj-xlsx-1-tam-10.1177_17588359221103199 for Missing the trees for the forest: most subgroup analyses using forest plots at the ASCO annual meeting are inconclusive by Andrew W. Hahn, Nazli Dizman and Pavlos Msaouel in Therapeutic Advances in Medical Oncology
Supplemental material, sj-xlsx-2-tam-10.1177_17588359221103199 for Missing the trees for the forest: most subgroup analyses using forest plots at the ASCO annual meeting are inconclusive by Andrew W. Hahn, Nazli Dizman and Pavlos Msaouel in Therapeutic Advances in Medical Oncology
Footnotes
Author contribution(s): Andrew W. Hahn: Data curation; Formal analysis; Investigation; Writing – original draft; Writing – review and editing.
Nazli Dizman: Data curation; Investigation; Writing – review and editing.
Pavlos Msaouel: Conceptualization; Investigation; Methodology; Supervision; Writing – original draft; Writing – review and editing.
ORCID iD: Andrew W. Hahn https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4153-205X
Funding: The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: Pavlos Msaouel was supported by a Career Development Award by the American Society of Clinical Oncology Conquer Cancer Foundation, a Research Award by KCCure, the MD Anderson Khalifa Scholar Award, the Andrew Sabin Family Foundation Fellowship, a Translational Research Partnership Award (KC200096P1) by the United States Department of Defense, an Advanced Discovery Award by the Kidney Cancer Association, the MD Anderson Physician-Scientist Award, and philanthropic donations by Mike and Mary Allen. Andrew Hahn was supported by a Young Investigator Award by the American Society of Clinical Oncology Conquer Cancer Foundation and the Rob Heyvaert and Paul Heynen Young Investigator Award by the Prostate Cancer Foundation.
Conflict of interest statement: Pavlos Msaouel has received honoraria for service on a Scientific Advisory Board for Mirati Therapeutics, Bristol Myers Squibb, and Exelixis; consulting for Axiom Healthcare Strategies; non-branded educational programs supported by Exelixis and Pfizer; and research funding for clinical trials from Takeda, Bristol Myers Squibb, Mirati Therapeutics, Gateway for Cancer Research, and UT MD Anderson Cancer Center. Nazli Dizman has received consulting fees from Vivreon Gastrosciences Inc. Andrew W Hahn has nothing to disclose.
Supplemental material: Supplemental material for this article is available online.
Contributor Information
Andrew W. Hahn, Division of Cancer Medicine, The University of Texas MD Anderson Cancer Center, Houston, TX, USA Department of Genitourinary Medical Oncology, The University of Texas MD Anderson Cancer Center, Houston, TX, USA.
Nazli Dizman, Department of Internal Medicine, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA.
Pavlos Msaouel, Division of Cancer Medicine, Department of Genitourinary Medical Oncology, The University of Texas MD Anderson Cancer Center, Unit 1374, 1155 Pressler Street, Houston, TX 77030-3721, USA; Division of Pathology and Laboratory Medicine, Department of Translational Molecular Pathology, The University of Texas MD Anderson Cancer Center, Houston, TX, USA; David H. Koch Center for Applied Research of Genitourinary Cancers, The University of Texas, MD Anderson Cancer Center, Houston, TX, USA.
References
- 1. Msaouel P, Lee J, Thall PF. Making patient-specific treatment decisions using prognostic variables and utilities of clinical outcomes. Cancers (Basel) 2021; 13: 2741. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Kent DM, van Klaveren D, Paulus JK, et al. The Predictive Approaches to Treatment effect Heterogeneity (PATH) statement: explanation and elaboration. Ann Intern Med 2020; 172: W1–W25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Kent DM, Steyerberg E, Van Klaveren D. Personalized evidence based medicine: predictive approaches to heterogeneous treatment effects. BMJ 2018; 363: k4245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Amrhein V, Greenland S, McShane B. Scientists rise up against statistical significance. Nature 2019; 567: 305–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Cuzick J. Forest plots and the interpretation of subgroups. Lancet 2005; 365: 1308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Greenland S, Senn SJ, Rothman KJ, et al. Statistical tests, P values, confidence intervals, and power: a guide to misinterpretations. Eur J Epidemiol 2016; 31: 337–350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Spears MR, James ND, Sydes MR. ‘Thursday’s child has far to go’—interpreting subgroups and the STAMPEDE trial. Ann Oncol 2017; 28: 2327–2330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Sun X, Briel M, Walter SD, et al. Is a subgroup effect believable? Updating criteria to evaluate the credibility of subgroup analyses. BMJ 2010; 340: c117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Sun X, Ioannidis JP, Agoritsas T, et al. How to use a subgroup analysis: users’ guide to the medical literature. JAMA 2014; 311: 405–411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Harrington D, D’Agostino RB, Sr, Gatsonis C, et al. New guidelines for statistical reporting in the journal. N Engl J Med 2019; 381: 285–286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Rafi Z, Greenland S. Semantic and cognitive tools to aid statistical science: replace confidence and significance by compatibility and surprise. BMC Med Res Methodol 2020; 20: 244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Schuirmann DJ. A comparison of the two one-sided tests procedure and the power approach for assessing the equivalence of average bioavailability. J Pharmacokinet Biopharm 1987; 15: 657–680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Kennedy-Shaffer L. When the alpha is the omega: p-values, “substantial evidence,” and the 0.05 standard at FDA. Food Drug Law J 2017; 72: 595–635. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Greenland S. Invited commentary: the need for cognitive science in methodology. Am J Epidemiol 2017; 186: 639–645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Altman DG, Bland JM. Absence of evidence is not evidence of absence. BMJ 1995; 311: 485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Carmona-Bayonas A, Jimenez-Fonseca P, Gallego J, et al. Causal considerations can inform the interpretation of surprising associations in medical registries. Cancer Invest 2022; 40: 1–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Harrell JFE. Regression modeling strategies: with applications to linear models, logistic and ordinal regression, and survival analysis. In: Springer Series in Statistics. 2nd ed. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2015, pp. 13–44. [Google Scholar]
- 18. Greenland S, Hofman A. Multiple comparisons controversies are about context and costs, not frequentism versus Bayesianism. Eur J Epidemiol 2019; 34: 801–808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Senn SJ. Falsificationism and clinical trials. Stat Med 1991; 10: 1679–1692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Greenland S. Valid P-values behave exactly as they should: some misleading criticisms of P-values and their resolution with S-values. The American Statistician 2019; 73: 106–114. [Google Scholar]
- 21. Mauri L, D’Agostino RB, Sr. Challenges in the design and interpretation of noninferiority trials. N Engl J Med 2017; 377: 1357–1367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Senn S. Controversies concerning randomization and additivity in clinical trials. Stat Med 2004; 23: 3729–3753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Msaouel P. Impervious to randomness: confounding and selection biases in randomized clinical trials. Cancer Invest 2021; 39: 783–788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Gauthier J, Wu QV, Gooley TA. Cubic splines to model relationships between continuous variables and outcomes: a guide for clinicians. Bone Marrow Transplant 2020; 55: 675–680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Supplemental material, sj-xlsx-1-tam-10.1177_17588359221103199 for Missing the trees for the forest: most subgroup analyses using forest plots at the ASCO annual meeting are inconclusive by Andrew W. Hahn, Nazli Dizman and Pavlos Msaouel in Therapeutic Advances in Medical Oncology
Supplemental material, sj-xlsx-2-tam-10.1177_17588359221103199 for Missing the trees for the forest: most subgroup analyses using forest plots at the ASCO annual meeting are inconclusive by Andrew W. Hahn, Nazli Dizman and Pavlos Msaouel in Therapeutic Advances in Medical Oncology