Skip to main content
PLOS Biology logoLink to PLOS Biology
. 2022 Jun 7;20(6):e3001640. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3001640

The aesthetic value of reef fishes is globally mismatched to their conservation priorities

Juliette Langlois 1,#, François Guilhaumon 1,2, Florian Baletaud 1, Nicolas Casajus 3, Cédric De Almeida Braga 4, Valentine Fleuré 1, Michel Kulbicki 5, Nicolas Loiseau 1, David Mouillot 1,6, Julien P Renoult 7, Aliénor Stahl 8, Rick D Stuart Smith 9, Anne-Sophie Tribot 10,11, Nicolas Mouquet 1,3,*,#
Editor: Andrew J Tanentzap12
PMCID: PMC9173608  PMID: 35671265

Abstract

Reef fishes are closely connected to many human populations, yet their contributions to society are mostly considered through their economic and ecological values. Cultural and intrinsic values of reef fishes to the public can be critical drivers of conservation investment and success, but remain challenging to quantify. Aesthetic value represents one of the most immediate and direct means by which human societies engage with biodiversity, and can be evaluated from species to ecosystems. Here, we provide the aesthetic value of 2,417 ray-finned reef fish species by combining intensive evaluation of photographs of fishes by humans with predicted values from machine learning. We identified important biases in species’ aesthetic value relating to evolutionary history, ecological traits, and International Union for Conservation of Nature (IUCN) threat status. The most beautiful fishes are tightly packed into small parts of both the phylogenetic tree and the ecological trait space. In contrast, the less attractive fishes are the most ecologically and evolutionary distinct species and those recognized as threatened. Our study highlights likely important mismatches between potential public support for conservation and the species most in need of this support. It also provides a pathway for scaling-up our understanding of what are both an important nonmaterial facet of biodiversity and a key component of nature’s contribution to people, which could help better anticipate consequences of species loss and assist in developing appropriate communication strategies.


The most beautiful reef fish are tightly packed into small regions of both the phylogenetic tree and the ecological trait space of the world’s reef fish fauna and are less threatened than unattractive fish. This study highlights likely important mismatches between potential public support for conservation and the species most in need of this support.

Introduction

Numerous nonmaterial facets of biodiversity comprise important components of nature’s contribution to people (NCP) [1,2]. Among these, aesthetic value (or less formally called “beauty”) is one of the most direct emotional links humans can experience with nature [3,4] and can occur through direct (first-hand experience) but also indirect mechanisms (for example, social media, television). It therefore engages a broader cross-section of the human population than most other NCP, but remains relatively poorly studied. The implications of aesthetic value for biodiversity conservation are likely to be substantial [5]. This lack of scientific attention is probably, at least in part, associated with a difficulty in defining aesthetic value [6], and an associated difficulty in consistently and quantitatively measuring the aesthetic value of biodiversity [5,7]. While also applicable to communities and ecosystems, the aesthetic value of individual species is the simplest and most intuitive unit of measurement for understanding this form of connection between humans and nature. Measuring species’ aesthetic value thus remains an important step in better understanding and predicting the willingness and motivation of societies to protect species, and the reasons behind success or failure of conservation efforts [8,9].

Biases in research and conservation efforts have been documented for many taxa. For example, vertebrates are far better represented than invertebrates among articles published in conservation journals [10,11] and in biodiversity datasets [12]. More than half of the billions of occurrences reported in Global Biodiversity Information Facility (GBIF) are for birds, while they represent only 1% of the total number of species in GBIF [12]. These biases are explained by human preferences for particular taxa [1315], with aesthetic value being an important underlying factor in these preferences. For example, fishes and birds displaying bright colors are considered more beautiful by the general public [16,17] and are more likely to be identified and reported in databases of public observations. Such bias is not limited to data collected by the public; Bellwood and colleagues [18] found evidence for potential subconscious bias towards yellow fishes in the published literature on coral reefs.

Previous studies working on aesthetic value have used either expert knowledge or public surveys based on photographic datasets [19]. Such studies are resource and time intensive, and thus they have been limited to a small number of species. Alternative approaches tried to use visual features (that is, pattern analysis, color distribution) in images known to positively influence aesthetic value [20]. These methods are promising but assume prior knowledge on which features, among a myriad, contribute to the aesthetic value and on their relative importance when it comes to compute a single index. Machine learning models, specifically convolutional neural networks (CNNs), have recently become advanced enough to accurately identify patterns (classification tasks) and predict continuous variables on images [21], opening up a new avenue for investigating the visual perception of our environment, without needing to assume or define important features a priori. CNN have already successfully assessed the beauty of outdoor places [22] and coralligenous reefs [23] but have not yet been applied to species level.

Here, we used traditional photographic surveys augmented by a CNN approach to evaluate the aesthetic value of the world’s reef ray-finned fish fauna. Our photographic survey, including 13,000 respondents from the broad public, generated a learning dataset for the CNN that allowed estimation of the aesthetic value for 2,417 reef fish species with high predictive accuracy, based on 4,881 species images, and without a priori knowledge on the features which contribute to fish beauty.

We chose to evaluate reef fishes because this group is emblematic of highly valued reef ecosystems and is known for its exceptional morphological diversity [18], which is presumably associated with a large range of aesthetic values. Reef fishes are also essential to the functioning of some of the most important and endangered ecosystems on Earth [24,25], and are of vital importance for a large part of humanity, including the poorest [26,27], by supporting several economic activities like subsistence fishing, recreational scuba diving, and aquarium trade [24,26,28]. We used data from the widespread and standardized Reef Life Survey (RLS) program [29] to objectively select from all of the world’s reef fish species to a more manageable collection of those most commonly encountered by divers, providing us with a subset of 2,417 species. We then mapped fish aesthetic value across the Tree of Life, and considered ecological traits, IUCN threat categories, and importance for fisheries to better understand the potential implications of human aesthetic bias for reef fish conservation.

Results

Building the CNN training dataset

Our first task was to build a set of fish images for direct evaluation of aesthetic value by humans that could be used for training a CNN (Fig 1A). We combined a set of 157 fish images previously evaluated [16] with a new set of 345 images independently evaluated in an online survey (see Methods and Text A and B in S1 File). The online survey presented images in pairs to the public (hereafter called “respondents”) and asked to choose the image they found the most beautiful. For the analyses, we kept 13,000 respondents without self-reported color vision issues (see Text C in S1 File). We then estimated each fish aesthetic value through Elo scores computation, a rating system based on pairwise comparison (see Methods) [30].

Fig 1. Evaluation and prediction of fish aesthetic values.

Fig 1

(a) Workflow of the online survey and deep learning prediction of aesthetic values. (1) Pairs of images were presented to the public during the online survey and scored using the Elo algorithm (see Methods). Left Parma bicolor and right Abudefduf luridus. (2) Once the 345 new images were evaluated online, the values of the 157 images previously evaluated [16] were corrected using the 21 images shared between the 2 surveys. (3) The resulting 481 images with evaluated aesthetic values were used to train a ResNet50 algorithm (see Text E and Fig L in S1 File). Illustration inspired from the PlotNeuralNet [31]. (b) Left: The r2 of the linear relationship between the predicted values averaged across the 5 validation sets and the evaluated values is 0.79 ± SD 0.04 (the color of points indicates the 5 sets used to perform the cross validation). This algorithm was used to predict the aesthetic values of the 4,400 unevaluated images of our dataset. Right: Distribution of the 481 evaluated values in light blue and of the 4,400 predicted aesthetic values in dark blue. The dots at the bottom of the plot indicate the predicted aesthetic values of the images shown in panel (c). Data and code required to generate this Figure can be found in https://github.com/nmouquet/RLS_AESTHE. (c) Examples of fishes representative of the range of predicted aesthetic values. Decreasing aesthetic value from left to right and top to bottom: Holacanthus ciliaris, Aracana aurita, Amphiprion ephippium, Ctenochaetus marginatus, Scarus spinus, Amphiprion bicinctus, Epinephelides armatus, Fusigobius signipinnis, Diplodus annularis, Odontoscion dentex, Nemadactylus bergi, Mendosoma lineatum. See S1 Data for image copyright.

We tested the potential effect of respondents’ sociocultural background and geographic origins on their selections (see Text C in S1 File) using a backward sequential selection procedure with a generalized linear model (see Methods). We found no significant effect of any of the considered variables on selections; we thus computed the Elo scores by pooling across the 13,000 respondents. Among the 345 images evaluated in the new survey, 21 were deliberate duplications from a previous survey [16], included to test for consistency with previous results and increase our learning dataset. For these 21 images, we found a strong correlation between the 2 evaluations (r2 = 0.89, p-value < 0.001, Text D in S1 File) and used this relationship to correct the scores of the 157 images previously evaluated [16] and pool the 2 datasets. This resulted in a combined dataset of 481 images with empirically evaluated aesthetic scores ranging from 1,085 to 1,910, which was used as the learning dataset for a CNN (Fig 1A).

Predicting the aesthetic values with the CNN

We trained a CNN to estimate the aesthetic value of new fish images and eventually predict values for the remaining 4,400 images in our collection (Fig 1A). We used a ResNet50 model pretrained on ImageNet [32], a popular CNN used for image classification. We replaced the last classification layer by a regression layer, and performed a 5-fold cross-validation to fine-tune this layer and the last convolutional block (see Methods and Text E in S1 File). The r2 of the linear regression between the values predicted by the CNN and the evaluated values from the validation set (averaged over the 5 folds of the cross-validation) was 0.79 ± SD 0.04 (Fig 1B). Applying the trained model to the 4,400 unevaluated fish images, the predicted values ranged from 1,153 to 1,980 (Fig 1B and 1C). The remaining analyses of our study were based on the values of 4,881 images: the 481 evaluated during the online surveys and the 4,400 with predictions from the CNN. We used several images per species to account for intraspecific morphological differences and used the highest predicted value for each species (S2 Data), thereby assuming that humans tend to focus on the most attractive representation of a species (see Discussion).

Determinants of aesthetic value

For each of the 4,881 images in our collection, we extracted 17 image features potentially linked to the aesthetic value, regrouped into 4 classes: (a) the color heterogeneity; (b) the geometry of color patterns; (c) the perceptual lightness and saturation; and (d) the shape of the fish outline (see Methods and Text A in S1 File). After eliminating nonsignificant features using a backward selection procedure, we ended up with 9 features that explained a substantial amount of variation in aesthetic value with a linear model (r2 = 0.64, p-value < 0.001, Fig 2A, Text F in S1 File). The most significant features were color heterogeneity, color saturation, and elongatedness (see Fig 2A; Text A, F, and Fig D in S1 File). The projection of fish aesthetic values on the 2 first axes of the principal component analysis (PCA) performed on the selected features (Fig 2B) confirms that fishes with the highest aesthetic values are those with high color heterogeneity and saturation as well as more circular body shapes (low elongatedness). Fishes with a high standard deviation in perceptual lightness and presence of several repeated patterns also have high aesthetic values. At the opposite of the gradient, drab fishes with elongated body shape and no clearly delineated color patterns have low aesthetic values (Fig 2B).

Fig 2. Image features analysis.

Fig 2

(a) Regression coefficients (with standard errors) from the final model between the aesthetic value and the 9 significant image features (see Text A in S1 File for a complete description of the image features). The variables have been scaled to visualize how the magnitude of the effects differs between them. Most important variables were: color heterogeneity, color saturation, standard deviation in lightness (SD lightness), pattern repetition, and body elongatedness. (b) Principal Component Analysis (PC1 and PC2) performed with the 9 significant images features (see Fig 2A, Text A, F in S1 File for a description of the image features). Points (fishes) are colored by their aesthetic values, and image feature vectors are projected on the 2 axes. Examples of fishes (chosen on the perimeter of the distribution) are provided for illustration. Clockwise order: Calloplesiops altivelis, Epinephelus ongus, Kyphosus vaigiensis, Epinephelus costae, Jenkinsia lamprotaenia, Phyllogobius platycephalops, Belone belone, Ctenogobiops crocineus, Suezichthys devisi, Opistognathus aurifrons, Pseudanthias ignitus, Pomacentrus auriventris, Mecaenichthys immaculatus, Pomacanthus navarchus, Aracana aurita, Pomacanthus sexstriatus. See S1 Data for image copyright. Data and code required to generate this Figure can be found in https://github.com/nmouquet/RLS_AESTHE.

Phylogenetic signal in aesthetic value

We then explored the link between fish aesthetic value and evolutionary history [3335]. The mean age (over 100 trees) of species ranged from 0.41 to 165.53 My. We found that the youngest species have the highest aesthetic value (Fig 3A) and a significant negative relationship between the aesthetic value and the mean Evolutionary Distinctiveness (Fig N in S1 File): species with long, isolated branches in the phylogenetic tree tend to have lower aesthetic value than less phylogenetically isolated species. These results identify members of the most recently diversified families as aesthetic hotspots in the tree (Fig 4, Text G and Fig O in S1 File). For example, the families Pomacanthidae and Acanthuridae, respectively, have a mean aesthetic value of 1,719 ± SD 176 (n = 42 species) and 1,590 ± SD 150 (n = 67), whereas the oldest families Scombridae and Carangidae have respective mean aesthetic values of 1,228 ± SD 54, (n = 18) and 1,278 ± SD 86 (n = 49). Pagel’s λ estimated on the entire tree confirms this strong phylogenetic signal (λ = 0.74 ± SD 0.01, p-value < 0.001). At a finer phylogenetic resolution, we also found significant phylogenetic signals within 9 families: Acanthuridae, Balistidae, Carangidae, Chaetodontidae, Haemulidae, Holocentridae, Pomacanthidae, Sciaenidae, and Scorpaenidae (λ ≥ 0.50, p-values < 0.05, Text G and Table B in S1 File), indicating that the clustering of aesthetic value also operates at the genus level within some families.

Fig 3. Phylogenetic history and ecological originality.

Fig 3

(a) Relationship between the aesthetic value and the age of the species (log transformed) in millions of years (averaged over 100 trees) without (plain line) and after (dashed line) accounting for phylogenetic relatedness. Both models show significant negative slopes (considering phylogenetic relatedness: slope = ‒78.4, p-value < 0.001; not considering phylogenetic relatedness: slope = ‒14.1 ± SD 1.7, p-value < 0.005, over the 100 random trees). (b) Relationship between the aesthetic value and the functional distinctiveness of species without (plain line) and after (dashed line) accounting for phylogenetic relatedness. Both models show significant negative slopes (considering phylogenetic relatedness: slope = ‒1,154.2, p-value < 0.001; not considering phylogenetic relatedness: slope = ‒383.3 ± SD 26.5, p-value < 0.001, over the 100 random trees). On both panels, species’ Evolutionary Distinctiveness (averaged over 100 trees and log transformed) have been used to color the points from low (dark red) to high (dark blue) values. Data and code required to generate this Figure can be found in https://github.com/nmouquet/RLS_AESTHE.

Fig 4. Aesthetic values across the tree of life.

Fig 4

Phylogenetic tree of the 2,417 fishes. Aesthetic values are mapped over the entire phylogeny with a color gradient obtained by estimating states at internal nodes with maximum likelihood [36]. For illustration, we have highlighted 20 families with contrasted aesthetic values using gray arcs and show examples of fishes for each family. Clockwise order: Cantherhines macrocerus, Pseudobalistes naufragium, Anampses femininus, Scarus spinus, Bodianus unimaculatus, Myripristis jacobus, Gymnothorax annasona, Meiacanthus atrodorsalis, Embiotoca jacksoni, Amphiprion bicinctus, Abudefduf bengalensis, Chromis alpha, Carangoides chrysophrys, Istigobius decoratus, Apogon pacificus, Sarda australis, Pterois miles, Acanthistius ocellatus, Amblycirrhitus pinos, Parequula melbournensis, Pomacanthus navarchus, Chaetodon flavirostris, Diplodus puntazzo, Acanthurus tristis. See S1 Data for image copyright. Data and code required to generate the phylogenetic tree can be found in https://github.com/nmouquet/RLS_AESTHE.

Aesthetics value across ecological trait space

To characterize ecological originality of fish species (hereafter called functional distinctiveness), we used 8 ecological traits that describe body size, diet, behavior, and habitat use [37]. We computed Gower’s pairwise distances between species to estimate their functional distinctiveness [38], and found that the most unique species, with respect to their trait combination, have lower aesthetic values than more ecologically redundant species (Fig 3B). A closer look at the distribution of aesthetic values within each trait category (Text H and Fig P in S1 File) shows that most attractive fishes are associated with hard substrates (as opposed to sandy patches within or along the edges of the reef), are demersal (hover above but near the bottom), active during the day, feed on corals or by excavating the reef surface, of intermediate body size, and prefer warmer waters. The least attractive fishes tend to be pelagic, nocturnal, eat other fishes or plankton, have either small or large body size, and prefer cooler waters.

Conservation status and aesthetic value

We categorized fish IUCN status into 3 groups: 190 species in our dataset are Threatened (TH: Critically Endangered, Endangered, or Vulnerable), 1,602 species are Least Concern (LC: Least Concern or Near Threatened), and 556 are Not Evaluated (NE). We found significant differences between the mean aesthetic value of species in these 3 groups (1-way ANOVA, p-value < 0.001). Tukey’s post hoc tests show that Threatened fishes have the lowest aesthetic values on average, and Least Concern fishes the highest (Fig 5A), although variability within these groups was high. Further, we grouped the Least Concern and Threatened fishes into an Evaluated category and found that they had a higher mean aesthetic value than the Not Evaluated category (1-way ANOVA, p-value < 0.001).

Fig 5. Fish aesthetic value and conservation status.

Fig 5

(a) Violin plot of the aesthetic value of reef fishes for three groups of conservation status: TH, NE, and LC. Letters indicate significant differences between the groups (Tukey p-values are respectively p < 0.01 between TH and NE, p < 0.001 between LC and TH, and p < 0.001 between LC and NE). (b) Violin plot of the aesthetic value of reef fishes for the 5 groups of fishery importance: “Data deficient” = no data available; “Non commercial” = no interest for fisheries or potential interest or minor interest; “Subsistence fisheries” = importance for subsistence fisheries; “Commercial” = commercial importance for fisheries; “Highly commercial” = high commercial importance for fisheries. Letters indicate significant differences between the groups (all Tukey p-values are < 0.002). Data and code required to generate this Figure can be found in https://github.com/nmouquet/RLS_AESTHE. LC, Least Concern; NE, Not Evaluated; TH, Threatened.

We also compared species aesthetic value with their importance to fisheries. According to FishBase, 594 of our species are classified as “Non commercial,” 83 as “Subsistence fisheries,” 368 as “Commercial,” and 43 as “Highly commercial.” The remaining 1,329 species are “Data deficient.” One-way ANOVA (p-values < 0.001) and Tukey’s post hoc tests (all p-values < 0.002) showed that species for which no data is available, species of no fishery interest, and species important for subsistence fisheries have similar aesthetic value. The differences between the mean aesthetic value of the other categories are statistically significant, with the Highly commercial species having the lower aesthetic values (Fig 5B).

Discussion

Our study provides a global picture of variation in aesthetic value, an important but understudied facet of biodiversity, of reef fishes. It reveals some predictable differences among species and potential mismatches with conservation priorities. The aesthetic values of reef fishes are highly heterogeneous, with the most beautiful fishes being tightly packed into small regions of both the phylogenetic tree and the ecological trait space of the world’s reef fish fauna. In contrast, the most ecologically and evolutionary distinct species and those recognized as threatened tend to be considered less attractive.

The set of 481 images evaluated through our online survey allowed us to train a deep learning algorithm to predict the aesthetic value of 4,400 fish images without having to arbitrarily predefine any visual features. The predictive power of our algorithm (r2 = 0.79) was high despite the relatively small size of our learning dataset and the cross-validation. This is largely due to the transfer learning procedure that takes advantage of pretrained CNNs and where only the weights of the last few layers needed to be specifically tuned to the new task. The high predictive power can also without doubt be explained by our image preprocessing, including background removal, and position and size standardization. This preprocessing required a considerable amount of work but could be automated in the future by a computerized segmentation and alignment of fishes [39], allowing for a broader use of images and/or videos from larger collections, including from the internet or social media. Our study highlights multiple mechanisms by which recent advances in computer science and artificial intelligence can be used by ecological studies to improve sample sizes. Beyond the scope of our study, our deep learning algorithm could be easily adapted to fishes from other ecosystems such as those from rivers and lakes, and even to other taxa such as birds, mammals, reptiles, or amphibians, for which millions of images are now available online. Our CNN could provide a valuable tool for studying these other taxa in 2 ways: as a pretrained network to fine-tune through transfer learning or without retraining to investigate the generalization of human aesthetic judgments from one animal taxon to others.

As we did not need to predefine any visual features for the automation of image scoring, we could then use post-analysis of image features to characterize the visual components contributing most to the aesthetic value of reef fishes. Our linear model combining 9 features potentially linked to aesthetic value (see Text A and F in S1 File) explained a substantial amount of the variation in aesthetic value (r2 = 0.64, Fig 2A). The features that explained the highest aesthetic values were heterogeneity in color and lightness, saturation of colors, the presence of well delineated and repeated patterns, and the circularity (versus elongatedness) of the body shape. These results can be interpreted through the lens of neuro-aesthetics, which relates aesthetic properties to the activation of dopamine neurotransmitter systems within the human brain [40], and to the processing fluency theory, which hypothesizes that aesthetic value is determined by the dynamics of information processing in the brain, and that stimuli that are easy to process are judged beautiful [41]. For example, components that can easily be separated from the background, or visual features that can obviously be grouped into recognizable objects, both trigger pleasure and tend to be judged beautiful [42]. Some of the features explaining aesthetic value in reef fishes could similarly be explained by fluency. For example, high color heterogeneity and well-delineated patches of contrasted lightness, as observed in angelfishes (Pomacanthidae) and butterflyfishes (Chaetodontidae), increase fluency by facilitating detection and recognizability [43]. The elevated aesthetic value of fishes with circular body shape echoes a general preference of humans for objects with curved outlines [44,45], which can also be explained by fluency given that curved lines are more predictable and thus more easily encoded by the visual system than angular lines [41]. Overall, our analysis suggests that predictable visual features explain human preferences for fishes, creating a strong bottleneck in the diversity of species that are likely to be considered as beautiful by the general public.

This aesthetic bias toward particular color and morphological features leads to strong clustering of aesthetic value across the Tree of Life and ecological trait space. Some families show overall high aesthetic value, such as butterflyfishes (Chaetodontidae), while some others show overall low aesthetic value, such as jacks and pompanos (Carangidae). This strong phylogenetic signal is probably explained by the relatively high ecological niche conservatism within fish families. For example, most Chaetodontidae are found among living corals, and most Carangidae live in open water or above the reef. The ecological niche plays a prominent role in determining color patterns, through its effect on social behavior, feeding activities, and most notably camouflage [46]. We thus suggest that low trophic level species living on rocks and coral have higher aesthetic values than higher trophic level species, such as piscivores, because of their need to blend in with more color-rich habitats (to our trichromatic color vision and under white light) for camouflage purposes [47]. These rock and coral habitats are also visually complex, being highly fractal [48]. By adapting their visual patterns to these structured habitats, coral reef fishes would thus “benefit” from a preference bias that we humans have for fractal patterns [49]. On the other hand, pelagic fishes that have to hide within a poorly contrasted and homogeneous habitat would be considered less attractive [50].

The ecological niche could also influence our perception of fish beauty through its effect on locomotion. Most pelagic fishes are good swimmers with a fusiform body shape that reduces frictional forces, but also increases the angularity of body outline, which appears to be associated with reduced aesthetic value. Conversely, some demersal species that rely on pectoral fins to swim [51] have a circular body shape, associated with higher aesthetic value. We also found that younger species (in evolutionary history) and more diversified clades show higher aesthetic value (Fig 3A), which could be explained by the relatively “recent” diversification of particular families associated with the exploitation of coral reef niches. For example, among the families with high aesthetic values, the angelfishes (Pomacanthidae) appeared during the Eocene, with all genera in place by the mid-Miocene [52]. Likewise, the butterflyfishes (Chaetodontidae) moved to coral reefs in the Miocene, with subsequent cladogenesis [53], the origin of the triggerfishes (Balistidae) is dated to late Miocene [54], and the boxfishes (Aracanidae) appeared during the Pliocene [55].

Together, these results highlight how our aesthetic judgment, which is linked to the ability of the human brain to process visual information, could generate strong bias in our emotional engagement with reef fishes. This bias of perception was suggested by a previous study performed on 116 coral reef fishes, which found that the least attractive fishes have a higher functional richness than the most attractive ones [16]. Our study extends these results to 2,417 reef fish species and shows that the distribution of aesthetic values is strongly skewed along both ecological and evolutionary dimensions. The fact that human visual aesthetic preferences are predictable and narrower than the diversity of reef fish life forms is not surprising in itself, but has important implications as our emotional perception of nature is one of the major drivers of our interest in it. The aesthetic bias could influence decisions in reef fish research and conservation. For example, Bellwood and colleagues [18] have previously shown that scientific published literature is biased towards yellow fishes which, given our results, are likely to be among the most attractive ones. Likewise, in mammals, the most beautiful species are subject to more research effort than less attractive species [13]. Our study extends these results to reef fish conservation as we found a mismatch between the aesthetic value of reef fish species and their conservation status. Less attractive fishes are also less well represented in assessments for the IUCN Red List, and are thus paying what could be considered as a form of “aesthetic-related debt” in a time where the amount of effort devoted to conservation is limited. This debt is a major concern given that, as shown here, the most threatened species (as evaluated for the IUCN Red List) are those with the lowest aesthetic value (Fig 5A). We also found that the less attractive fishes are also more at risk of overexploitation by fisheries (Fig 5B). Beyond species, this debt could have consequences at the whole reef ecosystem level, as we found that the less attractive species have the highest ecological distinctiveness, and thus provide the highest diversity of ecological functions. We thus suggest that the elevated extinction risk of the less attractive fish species might also have disproportionate and overlooked effects on reef ecosystem functioning.

Our approach has some limitations and considerations that may affect the accuracy of individual values, including the use of maximum predicted aesthetic values for each species and the pretreatment of images shown to the human respondents. Some reef fishes show extreme polymorphism in color or body shape (for example, male versus female and adults versus juveniles), but the high number of species included in our study prevented the gathering of an exhaustive collection of all the existing morphs. Because we aimed at providing the most generalizable results to the world’s reef fishes, we deliberately chose to maximize the number of species rather than finely characterizing intraspecific variation. When several images were estimated for a species, we kept only the maximum aesthetic value to characterize this species. This was motivated by our desire to account for a possible “halo effect,” a cognitive tendency of people to generalize the evaluation of the most valued item to other, less valued item of the same category [56]. Nevertheless, reproducing the analysis using averaged, rather than maximum, aesthetic values by species yielded similar results (Text I in S1 File). We further acknowledge that background composition in images could also be important, as it has been shown to influence the perception of beauty in psychological tests [57]. Yet, inclusion of the background would have made it uncertain how much the values related to the fish versus the background. To maximize comparability, we also scaled all fishes to the same size (500 × 500 pixel squares), but if represented proportionally to real size, bigger species may have had an “aesthetic bonus.” People tend to prefer bigger individuals, independently of their intrinsic aesthetic value [58], as it has been shown for birds [59]. This size issue has also motivated our decision to focus only on ray-finned fishes and exclude bigger fishes such as sharks, for which the size aesthetic bonus and charismatic nature make them attractive to people, even though their body shape and coloration would suggest lower aesthetic value. Finally, we removed the Pleuronectiformes (14 species) and Syngnathiformes (31 species) to help standardize the morphologies of fishes within our dataset. Within the Syngnathiformes, seahorses might be considered as highly attractive, yet possess distinct functional traits; a combination which would contradict one of our main findings. They represent, however, only 14 species in the RLS dataset of most common reef fishes encountered by survey divers used for this analysis, which would not likely change the overall trends or tendencies we report. On the other hand, the Pleuronectiformes would be more likely to be scored as less attractive, and hold original ecological traits; a combination which would have reinforced our results. All these limitations must be kept in mind when interpreting our results, but we consider them unlikely to affect the generality of our study’s conclusions for conservation and ecology. We rather see them as future research opportunities and believe that the extent of our study (2,417 species), the large taxonomic coverage (139 families of reef fishes), and the range of predicted aesthetic value (Fig 1B) provide solid generality and robustness to our main findings.

We also anticipated that the results of our predictive model could be biased by respondents individual factors, given that aesthetic preferences can vary with age, gender [60], social group [61], or culture [62]. Nevertheless, we found no such effect when analyzing the robustness of the match outcomes (probability for an image to win a match) to geographic or sociocultural backgrounds. However, the absence of sociocultural effects should be interpreted with caution because of the relatively limited diversity of sociocultural backgrounds among our respondents and geographical biases. For example, 62.4% of the respondents were French, 84.9% had a Bachelor’s degree or a higher diploma, and 52% had some experience scuba diving (Text C in S1 File). Despite these biases, our results highlight some degree of universality in the aesthetic values of fishes that probably outweigh sociocultural differences. This generality may be explained by our protocol for evaluating the aesthetic value. By presenting images by pairs and asking people to simply choose the image they found the most beautiful, we ought to force people to make rapid choices based on a bottom-up, stimulus-driven processing of visual information rather than a cognitive-driven evaluation based on previous knowledge [63]. Although we believe our general conclusions should hold, we would presumably have found differences in individual scores had we compared respondents spanning a much larger breadth of cultural, social, or demographic backgrounds, or asked questions that would have triggered more cognitive processing. Addressing this would require a more balanced pool of respondents and opens interesting perspectives for future research in the framework proposed by “personalized ecology” [64].

Overall, our results show that a nonmaterial facet of NCP, aesthetic value, mismatches with the material and regulating NCP facets provided by fishes. This mismatch strengthens the case for recognizing multiple aspects of NCP, but paradoxically, it also implies the potential for an aesthetic driven debt if the less attractive fishes are more exploited and receive less conservation/research efforts. This should motivate strengthened research efforts in evaluating the aesthetic value of biodiversity more generally, and the extent to which perceptual and emotional biases translate into biases in research, awareness, and conservation. Capitalizing on the capacity of artificial intelligence to work on extensive datasets, future research should also be able to expand our aesthetic valuation at higher levels of biological organization, such as assemblages [65] and whole ecosystems. Investigating the common patterns in aesthetic values across taxa such as birds, mammals, reptiles, or amphibians would also be an exciting research avenue, and could serve to fuel research and conservation programs. Our study further revealed a high consensus across participants in the aesthetic evaluation, although we acknowledge this could change through time, education, and with connectedness to nature [5,66] (even if not captured in our study through an influence of the sociocultural background of participants). We do not believe that identified aesthetic biases will always turn into debts. The first step in minimizing the impacts of these biases for conservation success will be better communication to the public, policy-makers, conservation NGOs, and researchers on the links between the nonmaterial components of nature’s contribution to people [1] (here aesthetic value), the ecology of species and the roles they perform in ecosystems [5].

Methods

Most analyses were carried out using R v.3.6.0 (specific functions within specific packages are indicated in italic). All relevant code and data are available from the associated GitHub repository (see sections Data and Code availability).

List of species

Fishes are the most diverse class of vertebrates, with more than 30,000 species and a huge diversity of shapes, sizes, and colors [67]. We sought to sample this diversity in an ecologically and socially meaningful subset, focusing on reef fishes. We used the RLS database [29] to identify a subset that could be representative of those species likely to be directly encountered and seen by people, but still spanning all major ocean basins and reef areas. The RLS database only includes records from standardized, quantitative visual surveys by scuba divers on rocky and coral reef habitats in shallow waters (mostly 0 to 20 m depth). It is the largest single-method dataset available for reef fishes (134,759 abundance records from 1,879 sites, representing 2,397 species) and has an associated trait database [37]. We decided to focus only on ray-finned fishes (Actinopterygii), and excluded from this clade the orders Pleuronectiformes (14 species) and Syngnathiformes (31 species) to remove the very unusual morphologies that would have been harder to present in the standardized questionnaire and to characterize further in our image features analysis (see below). As one of our foci was on IUCN status, we supplemented the remaining 2,280 RLS fish species list with 137 reef-associated species identified from FishBase that are classified as threatened (critically endangered, endangered, vulnerable), but not observed in the RLS database, so that our final list covered most of the threatened fishes listed on the IUCN red list that are also considered “reef-associated” (according to FishBase). We ended up with a list of 2,417 reef fish species belonging to 139 families (S2 Data). Taxonomic names were checked using the World Register of Marine Species database WoRMS [68,69].

Photographic material

Images were collected on the internet, focusing first on particular sources considered as reliable for species identity. Most of the material (3,198 images) was collected from RLS “Reef Species of the World” web pages (https://reeflifesurvey.com/species/search.php), FishBase (https://www.fishbase.se/), EOL (https://eol.org), and Fishes of Australia (https://fishesofaustralia.net.au/). We also collected material from Google Images (1,683 images; see Text B in S1 File and S4 Data), but only when the species could be unambiguously identified. When available, several images were collected for species with color polymorphism, or with morphological differences between males and females, or between adults and juveniles. The final dataset included 4,881 images. Note that for 8 species we could not find images and we removed them from our analysis (see S2 Data). For each image, we removed the natural background using Clipping Magic (https://fr.clippingmagic.com/) and standardized the position of the fish with photoshop. Correction of color saturation (in the blue and the green essentially) and luminosity was also performed when needed using Photoshop. Final images represent a single individual, horizontally aligned (mouth on the left, tail on the right), and displayed against a white background. All images were resized to 500 × 500 pixels at 96 dpi (Fig 1A).

Direct evaluation of fish aesthetic values

Our first objective was to build the learning dataset for our deep learning algorithm. That is, a dataset of images that are representative of the range in different visual features found among the 4,881 images of our whole collection, and for which individual aesthetic values would be evaluated by the public. To ensure the robustness of the method, the surveyed photographic material must be representative of the taxa considered and evaluated by a sufficiently large panel of humans with contrasted sociocultural background, which can be achieved by using online surveys [14,16].

Our learning dataset was obtained from combining results from a previous study where we empirically evaluated the aesthetic values of 157 images of reef fish species [16] and a new online evaluation of 324 images. The 324 images were subsampled from our 4,881 images collection to maximize variation within visual features. For doing so, we performed a PCA of the visual features measured on all of the 4,881 images (see below and Text A and B in S1 File). The 5 first dimensions of the PCA (representing 77% of the total explained variance) were used to construct a convex 5 dimensional volume. The 162 images were then randomly drawn near the vertices of this volume, and the other 162 images were randomly drawn within the remaining volume. We added 21 further images from the 157 images previously evaluated [16] so that we could quantify any differences between the 2 surveys (see below) and merge the 2 datasets. The robustness of our procedure can be visualized on Fig E in S1 File (see also Text B in S1 File), which shows the high overlap between the 2 ellipses (on the first 2 axes of the PCA) representing the 99% confidence intervals of both the 345 selected images and the whole image set (see Fig E in S1 File).

The public online survey included 2 sections: (a) the aesthetic questionnaire itself during which each participant (hereafter called “respondent”) had to choose the image they found the most beautiful for 30 pairs (hereafter called “matches”) randomly sampled without replacement among the 345 images; and (b) a questionnaire (Text C in S1 File) to gather information on the sociocultural background of respondents (gender, age, education, experience with diving and spearfishing, fishkeeping, place of living, distance from the sea, exposure to natural space, and knowledge about coral reef fishes). The survey was anonymous and available in French and English to the public on a dedicated website (https://www.biodiful.org/) between February and June 2019. The website contained an introduction that stated the objective of the research and an ethical statement to guarantee anonymity to the respondents (see Text C in S1 File). The survey was distributed through massive emailing to authors’ contacts, various mailing lists in France and abroad (scientific societies, universities, aquariums, NGOs, etc.) and social media (Facebook and Twitter) asking the respondents to share the survey to their families and friends. The answers of 13,000 respondents without color perception issues were pooled and we computed the aesthetic values of the 345 images with the Elo algorithm [30] using the EloChoice v0.29.4 R package [70] with 1,000 bootstrapings (Text D in S1 File).

Finally, we computed linear regression between the aesthetic values of the 21 images common to the previous [16] and the new online surveys, and used the intercept and slope of this regression to merge the 2 datasets (Text D in S1 File). The final set used to train the deep learning algorithm thus included 481 images with continuous aesthetic values.

Predictive model of fish aesthetic values

To predict the aesthetic value of the whole set of images of our collection, we used a deep learning algorithm trained with the 481 evaluated images. Deep learning algorithms are most popular for their application to identification or classification tasks, but they are also suited for predicting continuous variables [71]. For computer vision tasks like the regression between the information of an image and a continuous variable, CNNs are considered to perform best [71]. We applied a transfer learning procedure by fine-tuning a ResNet model, one of the most popular architectures in artificial intelligence [72], pretrained on ImageNet [32]. Preliminary tests showed that the commonly used image size of 224 × 224 pixels provided the best results (Text E in S1 File).

We tested 2 different architectures, ResNet18 and ResNet50, which differ by the number of layers and thus the number of learnable parameters. After the hyperparameter configuration was optimized for both architectures, ResNet50 performed best and was thus retained as the predictive model. We performed 5-folds cross-validation such that the learning dataset of 481 images was divided into 5 parts of similar size with all the images of a given species in the same part. ResNet50 was thus trained on 4 parts (training set) and evaluated on the fifth one (evaluation set), repeating the procedure 5 times (each time changing the evaluation set). To limit the risk of overfitting, the training set was artificially augmented at each iteration by applying random rotation (‒5; 5 degrees). We used the r2 of the linear regression between the values predicted by the model and the evaluated values of the learning set to estimate the accuracy of the model. Further details on the parameters of the model and the learning procedure can be found in Text E in S1 File. Once the accuracy of the model on the 5 folds of the cross-validation was satisfying, we trained the model one last time on the 481 images and used the weights of this model to predict the aesthetic values of the remaining 4,400 unevaluated images. Data augmentation, fine-tuning of the models, and prediction of the aesthetic values were carried out using Python 3.7, Pytorch 1.4.0, and torchvision 0.5.0.

As some species with intraspecific morphological differences were represented by several images in our collection (see section “Photographic material”), we decided to characterize species level aesthetic with the highest aesthetic values obtained among images of the same species.

Visual features analysis

The choice of the visual features analyzed in the postprediction analysis was based on literature review of previous works which studied the aesthetic value of biodiversity (Text A in S1 File). We chose 4 different classes of features: (a) the color heterogeneity; (b) the geometry of color patterns; (c) the perceptual lightness and saturation; and (d) the shape of the fish body outline. The heterogeneity of colors and the geometry of color patterns were measured after using a K-means clustering algorithm to separate colors in the CIELAB color space. The lightness, that is, how close to white (high values) or black (low values) a color is, and color saturation were measured using the HSV color space. Features describing the fish shape were computed using morphometric analysis relying on elliptical Fourier transformation. Altogether, we obtained 17 different visual features (see Text A in S1 File for a complete description). We estimated the individual contribution of each visual feature to the aesthetic value obtained for the 4,881 images using a multiple regression approach (see section Statistical analysis). A PCA was performed with the scaled selected features (dudi.pca function in the ape v.1.7–16 R package), and the image coordinates (colored by their aesthetic values) were projected on the first 2 axes of the PCA.

Evolutionary history

We extracted the taxonomy of the 2,417 species from the WoRMS database via the taxize v0.9.91.91 R package [73]. We then used the fishtree v0.3.2 R package [33, 74] to compute the phylogenetic tree. This tree includes grafted species for which the genetic information is not directly available, but which are known from other published phylogenies or inferred from taxonomic positions. Hence, more than one branch descends from a single node in some parts of the tree, whereas a phylogenetic tree should have dichotomous divisions from common ancestors. To bypass these polytomies, 100 realizations of a stochastic polytomy resolver placing missing speciation events were used [75]. We extracted the age of the species as the length of the branches from the first node of the tree to the species’ leaf. We tested for a phylogenetic signal of the aesthetic values with Pagel’s λ coefficient [35] (see section Statistical analysis). Finally, we computed the Evolutionary Distinctiveness (ED; which reflects the phylogenetic isolation of a given species [34] of each species using the function evol.distinct from the picante v1.8.1 R package [76]. ED is high when the species is phylogenetically isolated, that is, it has a long unshared branch in the phylogenetic tree. The 3 indices, species age, Pagel’s λ, and ED were computed on the 100 randomly resolved trees and averaged.

Ecological traits

We used the RLS trait database [37] that covers body size (maximum length), feeding ecology (trophic group, trophic breadth), behavior (water column position, diel activity pattern), and habitat use (preferred temperature, habitat complexity). See Text H in S1 File for details. A total of 129 species had more than 50% missing trait values, while 107 species had less than 50% missing traits, and we used the R package missForest v1.4 [77] to impute them (Text H in S1 File) to complete all ecological traits for 2,288 species (94.7% of the dataset). To characterize species ecological originality, we computed functional distinctiveness (Di), which measures the average functional distance of a focal species to the other species [38]. We first computed a matrix of Gower distances of normalized species ecological traits using the function dist.ktab of the ade4 v1.7–13 R package [78] and then used the funrar v1.4.1 R package [79] to compute functional distinctiveness. Aesthetic values were also compared among categories or along values of ecological continuous traits (Text H in S1 File).

Conservation status and importance for fisheries

We used the rfishbase v3.1.1 R package [80] to obtain the updated IUCN status of the 2,417 fishes [81] and their importance for fisheries. To simplify interpretation, we grouped species into 3 categories according to their IUCN status: “Threatened” (TH) refers to Critically Endangered, Endangered, and Vulnerable species; “Least Concern” (LC) refers to Least Concern and Near Threatened species and “Not Evaluated” (NE) refers to Not Evaluated species. The 69 species had Data Deficient status and were removed from the analysis. We also grouped species into 5 categories according to their importance for fisheries: “Highly commercial,” “Commercial,” “Subsistence fisheries,” “Non commercial,” and “Data deficient.” The “Non commercial” category refers to species indicated as “Minor commercial,” “Of no interest,” or “Of potential interest.”

Statistical analysis

To test for the robustness of the matches’ outcome (probability for an image to win or not a match) to sociocultural background, we computed a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) with a binomial error structure (using the glmer function from the lme4 v 1.1–26 R package) in which the image was considered as a random effect variable to order the sociocultural variables according to their individual effect on the response variable (see also Text C in S1 File).

To estimate the individual contribution of each visual feature to the aesthetic value obtained for the 4,881 images, we used a multiple regression approach (see Text F in S1 File for a complete description). We first computed a correlation matrix between all features (using Pearson correlation coefficients): when 2 or more features were correlated (threshold r < 0.7), we kept only the feature with the highest correlation with the aesthetic value. We then created a linear model (with Gaussian response) explaining aesthetic values where each feature was ordered in the model according to its independent contribution to the total variation in the response variable. We eliminated non-significant terms using a backwards selection procedure, to derive a minimal adequate model and used the coefficients (scaled) of the final model to measure the contribution of each selected feature to the aesthetic value.

To test for a phylogenetic signal of the aesthetic values, we used Pagel’s λ coefficient [35]. Pagel’s λ characterizes the relation between the similarity of a given trait (here the aesthetic value) and the phylogenetic distance between species. It represents the possibility to reconstruct the tree with the studied trait only. A null λ leads to a single polytomy for the basal node while a value of 1 gives the exact tree. A p-value is computed by randomizing the data in order to identify the families for which an inner signal is detected [82]. This analysis was undertaken on the entire tree and only for the families for which we had more than 5 species in our dataset. We used the phylosig function of the phytools v 1.0–1 R package.

To test the relationship between aesthetic values with both the age of the species (in millions of years) and the functional distinctiveness, we used both linear models and linear models accounting for phylogenetic relatedness using the phylolm function of the phylolm v 2.6.2 R package over the distribution of 100 resolved phylogenetic trees (the 100 p-values were combined using the hmp.stat function of the harmonicmeanp v 3.0 R package).

To compare aesthetic values respectively across the 3 IUCN categories and the fisheries importance categories, we performed a 1-way ANOVA and Tukey multiple pairwise comparisons using the aov and TukeyHSD functions of the base-attached stats R package.

Data and materials availability

Most of the data used in this paper are freely available and downloadable from the web. Data on IUCN threat status are available in the IUCN Red List database (https://www.iucnredlist.org/). RLS data for species lists and some trait information are available through an online portal accessible through (http://www.reeflifesurvey.com), with additional trait data available on request by using the contact form. For each species, we provide aesthetic values predicted in the present study (S3 Data) and web links to original photographic material (S4 Data). Images free of copyright can be provided on request. Other datasets used in this study (extraction from the online survey and images features analysis) and all code used for the analysis and figures are available from the GitHub Repository: https://github.com/nmouquet/RLS_AESTHE.

Supporting information

S1 File. Supporting information methods and analysis with associated figures and tables.

Text A. Image features analysis. Text B. Image sampling strategy. Text C. Sociocultural background. Text D. Elo scores. Text E. Deep learning algorithm. Text F. Relationship between features and aesthetic values. Text G. Phylogenetic analysis. Text H. Ecological traits. Text I. Mean aesthetic value. Fig A. Representation of the three-dimensional CIELAB space. Fig B. Cluster analysis performed for Holacanthus ciliaris. Fig C. HSV color space. Fig D. Illustration of the analysis led with the Momocs package. Fig E. Projection of the 4,881 images of our collection on the 2 first axes of the PCA on the images’ features. Fig F. Questionnaire on the sociocultural background of the judges. Fig G. Number of judges per country. The map was obtained from rworldmap v1.3.6 R package, which uses Natural Earth as base layer (https://www.naturalearthdata.com/downloads/10m-physical-vectors/). Fig H. Summary of sociocultural background of the 13,000 judges. Fig I. Variation of the Elo scores of the images as matches accumulate. Fig J. Linear regression between the Elo scores for the 21 images in common between the 2 surveys. Fig K. Effect of the size of the input images on the performances of the model. Fig L. Architecture of ResNet50 modified to predict the aesthetic values. Fig M. Relationship between the Evolutionary Distinctiveness of species (in MY) and their aesthetic value. Fig N. Mean aesthetic values of families with more than 10 species presented in decreasing order. Fig O. Comparison between the aesthetic value of the fish species and their ecological traits. Fig P. Number of images per species. Fig Q. Relationship between the aesthetic values computed using the maximum and the mean values. Fig R. Phylogenetic history and ecological originality with mean aesthetic values. Fig S. Conservation status with mean aesthetic values. Table A. Analysis of deviance in the generalized linear mixed model. Table B. Pagel’s λ. Table C. List of the ecological traits used with their nature and modalities. Table D. P values of the Tukey’s tests.

(DOCX)

S1 Data. Copyright information for the images used in the figures.

(XLSX)

S2 Data. Species included in the study.

(XLSX)

S3 Data. Aesthetic values for the 2,417 species of the study.

(XLSX)

S4 Data. URL links for the 4,881 original images used in the study.

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the thousands of volunteers who participated in the online questionnaire, and Graham J. Edgar, Andrew Green, Natali Lazzari, and Ian Shaw for providing the fishes silhouettes used in our figures.

Abbreviations

CNN

convolutional neural network

ED

Evolutionary Distinctiveness

GBIF

Global Biodiversity Information Facility

GLMM

generalized linear mixed model

IUCN

International Union for Conservation of Nature

LC

Least Concern

NE

Not Evaluated

NCP

nature’s contribution to people

PCA

principal component analysis

RLS

Reef Life Survey

TH

Threatened

Data Availability

Most of the data used in this paper are freely available and downloadable from the web. Data on IUCN threat status are available in the IUCN Red List database (https://www.iucnredlist.org/). RLS data for species lists and some trait information are available through an online portal accessible through (http://www.reeflifesurvey.com), with additional trait data available on request by using the contact form. For each species, we provide aesthetic values predicted in the present study (S3 Data) and web links to original photographic material (S4 Data). Images free of copyright can be provided on request. Other datasets used in this study (extraction from the online survey and images features analysis) and all code used for the analysis and figures are available from the GitHub Repository: https://github.com/nmouquet/RLS_AESTHE.

Funding Statement

This research was partially funded through the 2017–2018 Belmont Forum and BiodivERsA REEF-FUTURES project under the BiodivScen ERA-Net COFUND program with the French National Research Agency (DM and NM). This project received additional funding from the LabEx CeMEB and the program PEPS CNRS (NM). RLS data management is supported by Australia’s Integrated Marine Observing System enabled by the National Collaborative Research Infrastructure Strategy (RSS). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Diaz S, Pascual U, Stenseke M, Martin-Lopez B, Watson RT, Molnar Z, et al. Assessing nature’s contributions to people. Science. 2018;359(6373):270–2. doi: 10.1126/science.aap8826 WOS:000423236700017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hill R, Díaz S, Pascual U, Stenseke M, Molnár Z, Van Velden J. Nature’s contributions to people: Weaving plural perspectives. One Earth. 2021;4(7):910–5. doi: 10.1016/j.oneear.2021.06.009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Novacek MJ. Engaging the public in biodiversity issues. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:11571–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0802599105 WOS:000258561200017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Daniel TC, Muhar A, Arnberger A, Aznar O, Boyd JW, Chan KM, et al. Contributions of cultural services to the ecosystem services agenda. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109(23):8812–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1114773109 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3384142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Tribot A, Deter J, Mouquet N. Integrating the aesthetic value of landscapes and biological diversity. Proc Biol Sci. 2018;285:20180971. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2018.0971 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Shimamura AP, Palmer SE. Aesthetic Science: Connecting Minds, Brains, and Experience. Oxford University Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Soga M, Gaston KJ. The ecology of human-nature interactions. Proc Biol Sci. 2020;287(1918). doi: 10.1098/rspb.2019.1882 WOS:000529202400002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Gobster PH, Nassauer JI, Daniel TC, Fry G. The shared landscape: what does aesthetics have to do with ecology? Landsc Ecol. 2007;22:959–72. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Martín-López B, Montes C, Benayas J. The non-economic motives behind the willingness to pay for biodiversity conservation. Biol Conserv. 2007;139(1):67–82. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2007.06.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Clark JA, May RM. Taxonomic Bias in Conservation Research. Science. 2002;297(5579):191–2. doi: 10.1126/science.297.5579.191b [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Donaldson MR, Burnett NJ, Braun DC, Suski CD, Hinch SG, Cooke SJ, et al. Taxonomic bias and international biodiversity conservation research. Facets. 2016;1:105–13. doi: 10.1139/facets-2016-0011 WOS:000409545000001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Troudet J, Grandcolas P, Blin A, Vignes-Lebbe R, Legendre F. Taxonomic bias in biodiversity data and societal preferences. Sci Rep. 2017;7(1):9132. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-09084-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Fleming PA, Bateman PW. The good, the bad, and the ugly: which Australian terrestrial mammal species attract most research? Mammal Rev. 2016;46(4):241–54. doi: 10.1111/mam.12066 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Miralles A, Raymond M, Lecointre G. Empathy and compassion toward other species decrease with evolutionary divergence time. Sci Rep. 2019;9:8. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-56006-9 WOS:000508872700001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Jaric I, Courchamp F, Correia RA, Crowley SL, Essl F, Fischer A, et al. The role of species charisma in biological invasions. Front Ecol Environ. 2020;18(6):345–52. doi: 10.1002/fee.2195 WOS:000557261100001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Tribot A, Carabeux Q, Deter J, Claverie T, Villeger S, Mouquet N. Confronting species aesthetics with ecological functions of coral reef fishes. Sci Rep. 2018;8:11733. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-29637-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Brambilla M, Gustin M, Celada C. Species appeal predicts conservation status. Biol Conserv. 2013;160:209–13. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bellwood DR, Hemingson CR, Tebbett SB. Subconscious Biases in Coral Reef Fish Studies. Bioscience. 2020;70(7):621–7. doi: 10.1093/biosci/biaa062 WOS:000614724000012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Tribot A-S, Deter J, Mouquet N. Integrating the aesthetic value of landscapes and biological diversity. Proc Biol Sci. 2018;285(1886). doi: 10.1098/rspb.2018.0971 WOS:000444626300003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Haas AF, Guibert M, Foerschner A, Co T, Calhoun S, George E, et al. Can we measure beauty? Computational evaluation of coral reef aesthetics. PeerJ. 2015;3:e1390. doi: 10.7717/peerj.1390 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4647610. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Burke M, Driscoll A, Lobell DB, Ermon S. Using satellite imagery to understand and promote sustainable development. Science. 2021;371(6535):eabe8628. doi: 10.1126/science.abe8628 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Seresinhe CI, Preis T, Moat HS. Using deep learning to quantify the beauty of outdoor places. R Soc Open Sci. 2017;4(7):170170. doi: 10.1098/rsos.170170 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Langlois J, Guilhaumon F, Bockel T, Boissery P, De Almeida Braga C, Deter J, et al. An integrated approach to estimate aesthetic and ecological values of coralligenous reefs. Ecol Indic. 2021;129:107935. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolind.2021.107935 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Brandl SJ, Rasher DB, Côté IM, Casey JM, Darling ES, Lefcheck JS, et al. Coral reef ecosystem functioning: eight core processes and the role of biodiversity. Front Ecol Environ. 2019;17(8):445–54. doi: 10.1002/fee.2088 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Benkwitt CE, Wilson SK, Graham NAJ. Biodiversity increases ecosystem functions despite multiple stressors on coral reefs. Nat Ecol Evol. 2020;4(7):919–26. doi: 10.1038/s41559-020-1203-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Cinner JE, McClanahan TR, Daw TM, Graham NAJ, Maina J, Wilson SK, et al. Linking Social and Ecological Systems to Sustain Coral Reef Fisheries. Curr Biol. 2009;19(3):206–12. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.11.055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Teh LSL, Teh LCL, Sumaila UR. A Global Estimate of the Number of Coral Reef Fishers. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(6):e65397. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0065397 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Robles-Zavala E, Chang Reynoso AG. The recreational value of coral reefs in the Mexican Pacific. Ocean Coast Manag. 2018;157:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ocecoaman.2018.02.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Edgar GJ, Stuart-Smith RD. Systematic global assessment of reef fish communities by the Reef Life Survey program. Sci Data. 2014;1(1):140007. doi: 10.1038/sdata.2014.7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Elo A. The Rating Of Chessplayers, Past and Present. Ishi Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Iqbal H. HarisIqbal88/PlotNeuralNet v1.0.0 (Version v1.0.0). Zenodo. 2018.
  • 32.Deng J, Dong W, Socher R, Li L-J, Li K, Li F-F, et al. ImageNet: A Large-Scale Hierarchical Image Database. Cvpr: 2009 IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition, Vols 1–4. IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition; 2009. p. 248–55.
  • 33.Rabosky DL, Chang J, Title PO, Cowman PF, Sallan L, Friedman M, et al. An inverse latitudinal gradient in speciation rate for marine fishes. Nature. 2018;559(7714):392. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0273-1 WOS:000439059800054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Isaac NJB, Turvey ST, Collen B, Waterman C, Baillie JEM. Mammals on the EDGE: Conservation Priorities Based on Threat and Phylogeny. PLoS ONE. 2007;2(3):e296. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000296 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Pagel M. Inferring the historical patterns of biological evolution. Nature. 1999;401(6756):877–84. doi: 10.1038/44766 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Revell LJ. Two new graphical methods for mapping trait evolution on phylogenies. Methods Ecol Evol. 2013;4:754–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Stuart-Smith RD, Bates AE, Lefcheck JS, Duffy JE, Baker SC, Thomson RJ, et al. Integrating abundance and functional traits reveals new global hotspots of fish diversity. Nature. 2013;501(7468):539–42. doi: 10.1038/nature12529 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Violle C, Thuiller W, Mouquet N, Munoz F, Kraft NJB, Cadotte MW, et al. Functional Rarity: The Ecology of Outliers. Trends Ecol Evol. 2017;32(5):356–67. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2017.02.002 WOS:000399451900011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Villon S, Mouillot D, Chaumont M, Darling ES, Subsol G, Claverie T, et al. A Deep learning method for accurate and fast identification of coral reef fishes in underwater images. Eco Inform. 2018;48:238–44. doi: 10.1016/j.ecoinf.2018.09.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Chatterjee A, Vartanian O. Neuroaesthetics. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18(7):370–5. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.03.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Reber R, Schwarz N, Winkielman P. Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience? Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2004;8:364–82. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Winkielman P, Schwarz N, Fazendeiro TA, Reber R. The hedonic marking of processing fluency: Implications for evaluative judgment. The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion. Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2003. p. 189–217. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Renoult JP, Mendelson TC. Processing bias: extending sensory drive to include efficacy and efficiency in information processing. Proc Biol Sci. 1900;2019(286):20190165. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2019.0165 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Bertamini M, Palumbo L, Gheorghes TN, Galatsidas M. Do observers like curvature or do they dislike angularity? Br J Psychol. 2016;107(1):154–78. Epub 2015/04/13. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12132 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Cotter KN, Silvia PJ, Bertamini M, Palumbo L, Vartanian O. Curve Appeal: Exploring Individual Differences in Preference for Curved Versus Angular Objects. Iperception 2017;8(2):2041669517693023. doi: 10.1177/2041669517693023 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Salis P, Lorin T, Laudet V, Frédérich B. Magic Traits in Magic Fish: Understanding Color Pattern Evolution Using Reef Fish. Trends Genet. 2019;35(4):265–78. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2019.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Marshall NJ, Cortesi F, de Busserolles F, Siebeck UE, Cheney KL. Colours and colour vision in reef fishes: Past, present and future research directions. J Fish Biol. 2019;95(1):5–38. doi: 10.1111/jfb.13849 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Mark DM. Fractal dimension of a coral reef at ecological scales: a discussion. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 1984;14:293–4. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Spehar B, Taylor RP. Fractals in Art and Nature: Why do we like them? In: Rogowitz BE, Pappas TN, DeRidder H, editors. Human Vision and Electronic Imaging Xviii. Proc SPIE. 86512013. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Johnsen S. Hide and Seek in the Open Sea: Pelagic Camouflage and Visual Countermeasures. Annu Rev Mar Sci. 2014;6(1):369–92. doi: 10.1146/annurev-marine-010213-135018 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Fulton CJ. Swimming speed performance in coral reef fishes: field validations reveal distinct functional groups. Coral Reefs. 2007;26(2):217–28. doi: 10.1007/s00338-007-0195-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Bellwood DR, van Herwerden L, Konow N. Evolution and biogeography of marine angelfishes (Pisces: Pomacanthidae). Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2004;33(1):140–55. Epub 2004/08/25. doi: 10.1016/j.ympev.2004.04.015 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Bellwood DR, Klanten S, Cowman PF, Pratchett MS, Konow N, van Herwerden L. Evolutionary history of the butterflyfishes (f: Chaetodontidae) and the rise of coral feeding fishes. J Evol Biol. 2010;23(2):335–49. Epub 2010/05/22. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01904.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Santini F, Sorenson L, Alfaro ME. A new multi-locus timescale reveals the evolutionary basis of diversity patterns in triggerfishes and filefishes (Balistidae, Monacanthidae; Tetraodontiformes). Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2013;69(1):165–76. doi: 10.1016/j.ympev.2013.05.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Santini F, Sorenson L, Marcroft T, Dornburg A, Alfaro ME. A multilocus molecular phylogeny of boxfishes (Aracanidae, Ostraciidae; Tetraodontiformes). Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2013;66(1):153–60. doi: 10.1016/j.ympev.2012.09.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Eagly AH, Ashmore RD, Makhijani MG, Longo LC. What is Beautiful is Good, But. . . A Meta-Analytic Review of Research on the Physical Attractiveness Stereotype. Psychol Bull. 1991;110:109–28. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.109 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Menzel C, Hayn-Leichsenring GU, Langner O, Wiese H, Redies C. Fourier power spectrum characteristics of face photographs: attractiveness perception depends on low-level image properties. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(4):e0122801. Epub 2015/04/04. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0122801 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4383417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Carlson A. Aesthetics and the Environment: The Appreciation of Nature, Art and Architecture. Routledge; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Schuetz JG, Johnston A. Characterizing the cultural niches of North American birds. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2019;116(22):10868–73. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1820670116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Pugach C, Leder H, Graham DJ. How Stable Are Human Aesthetic Preferences Across the Lifespan? Front Hum Neurosci. 2017;11(289). doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00289 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Kalivoda O, Vojar J, Skřivanová Z, Zahradník D. Consensus in landscape preference judgments: the effects of landscape visual aesthetic quality and respondents’ characteristics. J Environ Manag. 2014;(137):36–44. Epub 2014/03/07. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvman.2014.02.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Zhan J, Liu M, Garrod OGB, Daube C, Ince RAA, Jack RE, et al. Modeling individual preferences reveals that face beauty is not universally perceived across cultures. Curr Biol. 2021;31(10):2243–52.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2021.03.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Kahneman D. A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. Am Psychol. 2003;58(9):697–720. Epub 2003/10/31. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Gaston KJ. Personalised ecology and detection functions. People and Nature. 2020;2(4):995–1005. doi: 10.1002/pan3.10129 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Tribot A-S, Deter J, Claverie T, Guillhaumon F, Villeger S, Mouquet N. Species diversity and composition drive the aesthetic value of coral reef fish assemblages. Biol Lett. 2019;15(11). doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2019.0703 WOS:000504840300013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Otto S, Pensini P. Nature-based environmental education of children: Environmental knowledge and connectedness to nature, together, are related to ecological behaviour. Glob Environ Chang. 2017;47:88–94. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2017.09.009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Nelson JS, Grande TC, Wilson MV. Fishes of the World. John Wiley & Sons; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Costello MJ, Bouchet P, Boxshall G, Fauchald K, Gordon D, Hoeksema BW, et al. Global Coordination and Standardisation in Marine Biodiversity through the World Register of Marine Species (WoRMS) and Related Databases. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(1):e51629. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051629 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Horton T, Gofas S, Kroh A, Poore GCB, Read G, Rosenberg G, et al. Improving nomenclatural consistency: a decade of experience in the World Register of Marine Species. Eur J Taxon. 2017;0(389). doi: 10.5852/ejt.2017.389 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Clark AP, Howard KL, Woods AT, Penton-Voak IS, Neumann C. Why rate when you could compare? Using the “EloChoice” package to assess pairwise comparisons of perceived physical strength. PLoS ONE. 2018;13:1–16. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0190393 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Lathuiliere S, Mesejo P, Alameda-Pineda X, Horaud R. A Comprehensive Analysis of Deep Regression. IEEE Trans Pattern Anal Mach Intell. 2020;42(9):2065–81. doi: 10.1109/TPAMI.2019.2910523 WOS:000557354900001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.He K, Zhang X, Ren S, Sun J, Ieee. Delving Deep into Rectifiers: Surpassing Human-Level Performance on ImageNet Classification. IEEE International Conference on Computer Vision; 2015. p. 1026–34.
  • 73.Chamberlain SA, Szocs E. taxize: taxonomic search and retrieval in R. F1000Res. 2013;2:191. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.2-191.v2 MEDLINE:24555091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Chang J, Rabosky DL, Smith SA, Alfaro ME. An r package and online resource for macroevolutionary studies using the ray-finned fish tree of life. Methods Ecol Evol. 2019;10(7):1118–24. doi: 10.1111/2041-210x.13182 WOS:000473662700019. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Chang J, Rabosky DL, Alfaro ME. Estimating Diversification Rates on Incompletely Sampled Phylogenies: Theoretical Concerns and Practical Solutions. Syst Biol. 2019;69(3):602–11. doi: 10.1093/sysbio/syz081 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Kembel SW, Cowan PD, Helmus MR, Cornwell WK, Morlon H, Ackerly DD, et al. Picante: R tools for integrating phylogenies and ecology. Bioinformatics. 2010;26(11):1463–4. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btq166 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Stekhoven DJ, Bühlmann P. MissForest—non-parametric missing value imputation for mixed-type data. Bioinformatics. 2011;28(1):112–8. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btr597 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Thioulouse J, Dray S, Dufour A-B, Siberchicot A, Jombart T, Pavoine S. Multivariate Analysis of Ecological Data with ade4. Springer: New York; 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Grenie M, Denelle P, Tucker CM, Munoz F, Violle C. funrar: An R package to characterize functional rarity. Divers Distrib. 2017;23(12):1365–71. doi: 10.1111/ddi.12629 WOS:000414865500001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Boettiger C, Lang DT, Wainwright PC. rfishbase: exploring, manipulating and visualizing FishBase data from R. J Fish Biol. 2012;81(6):2030–9. Epub 2012/11/08. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8649.2012.03464.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.IUCN. IUCN SSC guiding principles on creating proxies of extinct species for conservation benefit: version 1.0 2016. Available from: https://portals.iucn.org/library/sites/library/files/documents/Rep-2016-009.pdf.
  • 82.Blomberg SP, Garland T Jr., Ives AR. Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data: behavioral traits are more labile. Evolution. 2003;57(4):717–45. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2003.tb00285.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Roland G Roberts

2 Nov 2021

Dear Dr Mouquet,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript entitled "Global mismatch between the aesthetic value of reef fishes and their conservation priorities" for consideration as a Research Article by PLOS Biology.

Your manuscript has now been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editorial staff, as well as by an academic editor with relevant expertise, and I'm writing to let you know that we would like to send your submission out for external peer review.

However, before we can send your manuscript to reviewers, we need you to complete your submission by providing the metadata that is required for full assessment. To this end, please login to Editorial Manager where you will find the paper in the 'Submissions Needing Revisions' folder on your homepage. Please click 'Revise Submission' from the Action Links and complete all additional questions in the submission questionnaire.

Once your full submission is complete, your paper will undergo a series of checks in preparation for peer review. Once your manuscript has passed the checks it will be sent out for review.

If your manuscript has been previously reviewed at another journal, PLOS Biology is willing to work with those reviews in order to avoid re-starting the process. Submission of the previous reviews is entirely optional and our ability to use them effectively will depend on the willingness of the previous journal to confirm the content of the reports and share the reviewer identities. Please note that we reserve the right to invite additional reviewers if we consider that additional/independent reviewers are needed, although we aim to avoid this as far as possible. In our experience, working with previous reviews does save time.

If you would like to send your previous reviewer reports to us, please specify this in the cover letter, mentioning the name of the previous journal and the manuscript ID the study was given, and include a point-by-point response to reviewers that details how you have or plan to address the reviewers' concerns. Please contact me at the email that can be found below my signature if you have questions.

Please re-submit your manuscript within two working days, i.e. by Nov 04 2021 11:59PM.

Login to Editorial Manager here: https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology

During resubmission, you will be invited to opt-in to posting your pre-review manuscript as a bioRxiv preprint. Visit http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/preprints for full details. If you consent to posting your current manuscript as a preprint, please upload a single Preprint PDF when you re-submit.

Given the disruptions resulting from the ongoing COVID-19 pandemic, please expect delays in the editorial process. We apologise in advance for any inconvenience caused and will do our best to minimize impact as far as possible.

Feel free to email us at plosbiology@plos.org if you have any queries relating to your submission.

Kind regards,

Roli Roberts

Roland Roberts

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

rroberts@plos.org

Decision Letter 1

Roland G Roberts

23 Dec 2021

Dear Dr Mouquet,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript "Global mismatch between the aesthetic value of reef fishes and their conservation priorities" for consideration as a Research Article at PLOS Biology. Your manuscript has been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editors, an Academic Editor with relevant expertise, and by four independent reviewers.

You’ll see that all four reviewers are quite positive about the study; however, they all raise a number of concerns, the most serious of which seems to be that raised by both reviewers #1 and #2 about the representativeness of the questionnaire respondents. The remaining issues are largely textual and/or presentational. These must all be addressed for further consideration.

IMPORTANT: The Academic Editor also wants you to address in your Discussion a point that s/he mentioned to me during the initial assessment, namely: "What can we do with that information? How does that address the challenge that a panda will always get more conservation attention than some rare ugly rat?"

In light of the reviews (below), we are pleased to offer you the opportunity to address the [comments/remaining points] from the reviewers in a revised version that we anticipate should not take you very long. We will then assess your revised manuscript and your response to the reviewers' comments and we may consult the reviewers again.

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within 2 months.

Please email us (plosbiology@plos.org) if you have any questions or concerns, or would like to request an extension. At this stage, your manuscript remains formally under active consideration at our journal; please notify us by email if you do not intend to submit a revision so that we may end consideration of the manuscript at PLOS Biology.

**IMPORTANT - SUBMITTING YOUR REVISION**

Your revisions should address the specific points made by each reviewer. Please submit the following files along with your revised manuscript:

1. A 'Response to Reviewers' file - this should detail your responses to the editorial requests, present a point-by-point response to all of the reviewers' comments, and indicate the changes made to the manuscript.

*NOTE: In your point by point response to the reviewers, please provide the full context of each review. Do not selectively quote paragraphs or sentences to reply to. The entire set of reviewer comments should be present in full and each specific point should be responded to individually.

You should also cite any additional relevant literature that has been published since the original submission and mention any additional citations in your response.

2. In addition to a clean copy of the manuscript, please also upload a 'track-changes' version of your manuscript that specifies the edits made. This should be uploaded as a "Related" file type.

*Resubmission Checklist*

When you are ready to resubmit your revised manuscript, please refer to this resubmission checklist: https://plos.io/Biology_Checklist

To submit a revised version of your manuscript, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' where you will find your submission record.

Please make sure to read the following important policies and guidelines while preparing your revision:

*Published Peer Review*

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*PLOS Data Policy*

Please note that as a condition of publication PLOS' data policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/data-availability) requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions arrived at in your manuscript. If you have not already done so, you must include any data used in your manuscript either in appropriate repositories, within the body of the manuscript, or as supporting information (N.B. this includes any numerical values that were used to generate graphs, histograms etc.). For an example see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5

*Blot and Gel Data Policy*

We require the original, uncropped and minimally adjusted images supporting all blot and gel results reported in an article's figures or Supporting Information files. We will require these files before a manuscript can be accepted so please prepare them now, if you have not already uploaded them. Please carefully read our guidelines for how to prepare and upload this data: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/figures#loc-blot-and-gel-reporting-requirements

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols

Thank you again for your submission to our journal. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive thus far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Roli Roberts

Roland Roberts

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

rroberts@plos.org

*****************************************************

REVIEWERS' COMMENTS:

Reviewer #1:

In their paper titled "Global mismatch between the aesthetic value of reef fishes and their conservation priorities" (MS PBIOLOGY-D-21-02764R1), authors have presented an approach for analysis of aesthetic values and its application to ray-finned reef fishes.

I found it an excellent and important study, considering both the implications of their results and the great potential value of the presented approach, with wide applicability in the fields of ecology, conservation, and social sciences. The manuscript is generally well developed, and the study should be relevant and of interest for a wider audience.

There are however several issues that need to be addressed before I would encourage publication of the study.

Main comments

1. Probably the major problem in the manuscript is the fact that one part of the species were excluded from the analysis, if I understood excluded species were mainly flatfishes and seahorses. As I understood, the reason for their exclusion was that their specific morphology made them behave like outliers in the dataset that complicated the analysis? While this issue has been mostly glossed over, it is one of the main problems in the study, because it may be a source of a bias. It is important to explain and address this issue properly: 1) to provide more detailed arguments for this decision; 2) to provide exact number of species excluded from each species group; 3) to address this issue in Discussion, both in the section with caveats where it is currently not mentioned at all, as well as when discussing results. While being closer with some traits to species classified as having low aesthetic value, seahorses are considered highly attractive. I would therefore expect that they would rank highly with their aesthetic value, and they are at the same time endangered, which is an important caveat with respect to the conclusions drawn.

2. Study included only ray-finned fishes, which is important to acknowledge better, including in the title and abstract (i.e. instead of "reef fishes" to use "ray-finned reef fishes"). For example, reef sharks are considered as highly charismatic and likely to get high aesthetic scores, while their morphology (body shape and coloration) match traits identified here as non-aesthetic.

3. Aesthetic preferences and perceptions can considerably vary regionally, culturally, among different social and demographic groups, as well as temporally. This needs to be better addressed in the manuscript, including relevant literature, especially considering that the pool of respondents is strongly biased towards France, as well as Europe and the Global North.

4. There is insufficient information about how the questionnaire was distributed, whether there were financial or other incentives to distribute it, and especially the languages in which it was provided, to understand the reasons for such strongly biased pool of respondents.

5. I suspect based on information in Lines 491-492 that the questionnaire dissemination was improperly planned, aiming mainly to respondents in France, and using academic or similar mailing lists, which resulted in such skewed pool of respondents, both spatially and with respect to education level, mostly failing to reach other countries, continents and regions, as well as general population, beyond the highly educated respondents. This is one of the major weaknesses of the study, and needs to be better acknowledged and discussed in the paper.

6. Authors have pooled species that are categorized by the IUCN Red List as Not Evaluated (NE) and Data Deficient (DD) into a single group for analyses, "Not Evaluated". However, there is a considerable literature showing that Data Deficient species tend to be overall highly threatened (i.e., their traits, low population size, small ranges, and other characteristics that are making them highly threatened are also making them poorly studied and consequently data deficient). For this reason, DD species do not really belong together with NE, especially considering that comparisons of NE and TH groups were used to conclude that threatened species have lower aesthetic value. I do not think or suggest that it is necessary to redo complete analysis with DD species considered separately, but authors should at least acknowledge this issue and refer to the literature that addressed the issue of the threat status of data deficient species.

7. It would be good to include a table, at least in the supplementary material, which would present number of species and photos from different sources, used in each step (e.g. initial number of species, species excluded, etc.), to make it easier to understand the dataset used.

Other comments

8. Referring to respondents of the questionnaire as "judges" makes those sections confusing. It would be clearer and more appropriate to refer to them as "respondents".

9. There is some repetition in Results section of the information that is already presented in Methods section. Most of the subchapters in Results begin with description of methods. Such sections should be deleted, or at least condensed to reduce repetitiveness of the text.

10. Line 132, "Figure 1A" - "A" shouldn't be capitalized

11. It is unnecessary to refer to names of variables that were used during the analysis (e.g. "CL_cie_d_mean"), these names are not relevant and make it more difficult to follow the text (e.g. in Lines 225-228 and in Figure 2). Instead of those names it would be better to simply refer to the actual variable (e.g. "color heterogeneity")

12. Figure 3 caption (Line 269) mentions "ecological distinctiveness", while the X axis in the figure is named "Functional distinctiveness". Terms should be consistently used in the study.

13. Some species names in Figure 4 caption appear twice (Pterois miles and Cephalopholis sexmaculata in Lines 283-284). Just in case, please check this also in other parts of the manuscript, figures and supplementary material, as well as spelling of all species names, as it is one of common source of errors.

14. Line 324 - "Least Concerned" should be corrected to "Least Concern"

15. Line 400 - "though" should be corrected to "through"

16. Line 545 - it is left unclear why were these 137 species from FishBase not already covered by the original dataset of 2280 species, why were they missing from RLS?

17. Line 593, "Fig S5" - Which figure is this? Figures in the supplementary material are all named with capital letters.

18. Line 609 - "images" should be in singular

19. Line 763 - Sentence should be corrected to "Data on IUCN threat status are available in the IUCN Red List database".

20. S1 Fig G - legend of the inset figure needs more values on the color scale, not just the maximum and minimum. It should also not be log transformed, as it does not present clearly bias in spatial coverage.

21. S1 Fig. H - Y axis categories need to be made clear (e.g. education, place, distance, etc.)

Reviewer #2:

Langlois et al. present a compelling and important study connecting aesthetic and ecological values across almost 2500 coral reef fish species. The researchers conduct a large-scale online image analysis survey ('competing' paired images), as well as previous data, to score >300 species and then use these images/scores to train an AI algorithm, allowing rapid predictions of aesthetic score for the remaining ~2000 species. The paper first confirms that certain traits such as color intensity and diversity and disc-like shapes are favored by observers. It then goes on to provide novel evidence that aesthetic score declines with species age, as well as evolutionary and functional distinctiveness. The paper further provides a complementary ancestral state analysis to show the evolution and pattern of aesthetic value across coral reef fish in the Tree of Life. Finally, the team show aesthetic score is slightly lower in threatened and commercially important species. As conservation, public/political, and research attention is greatly affected by aesthetic qualities of biota, this research is highly important and stands to make a large impact.

I have a concern about the representativeness of the population of people surveyed. The paper addresses the aesthetic value of global coral reef fishes, and does so by distributing online surveys through existing networks. Does this create a cultural bias in the sampled population with respect to the global population, perhaps especially neglecting those people interacting with coral reef fish through subsistence fishing or other livelihoods? Furthermore, it is indicated that the selection of fish species ensured that they were those encountered by divers, but the diving theme is not further developed or linked to the sampled population. Generally I would like to see more consideration and discussion of the rationale and global representativeness of the sampled population, and a more explicit explanation of whether the focus is on fish encountered by SCUBA divers (as hinted at), and therefore perhaps a population of potential tourist SCUBA divers, or whether the study and results are meant to be more broadly applicable across populations around the world and with respect to different ways in which people worldwide interact with coral reef fish, including in subsistence settings where some of the highest rated fishes are fished.

Generally the AI algorithm does well at predicting aesthetic scores (Fig. 1B). However, in perhaps the top 5 or 10% of the range of evaluated aesthetic scores the algorithm does much more poorly. After X = 1750 all of the points are above the line, meaning AI was over-estimating the aesthetic values. I don't expect this to influence the results (looking at the other figures) but checking these points don't change the later models might be useful.

The statistical models relating aesthetic value with species age, ED and functional distinctiveness do not appear to account for phylogenetic relatedness, even though aesthetic value shows a strong phylogenetic signal. I realize that it is not always relevant to account for phylogeny in functional analyses, but it would be useful if the authors could comment on the rationale for leaving out phylogeny from the functional analysis, and what the implications are regarding inference? Does this limit our approach to conclude whether the differences are being driven by a couple of key contrasting families (e.g., beautiful butterflyfishes vs. brutish barracudas?), as opposed to a more general effect across the Tree?

Red squiggly spell-check lines in 1A should be removed

Reviewer #3:

[identifies himself as Uri Roll]

I found this contribution to be interesting, novel, and important. I have but few minor comments to it.

I thought that the abstract could benefit from specifically mentioning that you used fishes' images (perhaps also mention from readily available online sources)

Perhaps mention in the methods the socio-cultural variables you explored even broadly (and not just in the supp.)

In figure 2 try to give the actual names of features and not their abbreviations.

Generally, try to reduce a bit the repetition between the methods and results section.

Reviewer #4:

[see attached file for fully formatted version]

Thank you for this opportunity to review this manuscript. The review below is also attached as a PDF file through the reviewing platform.

Review PBIOLOGY-D-21-02764_R1

Global mismatch between the aesthetic value of reef fishes and their conservation priorities

Thank you for this opportunity to review this manuscript entitled "Global mismatch between the aesthetic value of reef fishes and their conservation priorities" for Plos Biology. This manuscript presents an elegant way to evaluate the aesthetic value of reef fishes and compares this value to several attributes of the fish species, like their conservation status, their ecological traits and ecological distinctiveness. It relies on unparalleled datasets on reef fish images on one side and corresponding evaluation from the broad public (>13,000 respondents) on the other side. It thus makes for an impressive social-ecological assessment of >4,000 species of fish underpinning complementary regulating, material, and most importantly (and innovatively in the present study) non-material Nature's Contributions to People. The paper is very well written and easy to read, and the figures nicely convey the main messages of the paper. I would like to simply congratulate the whole team of authors for their very innovative social-ecological work dedicated to find ways to quantify how human societies value their environment. I have a list of comments and suggestions that pertain to the different sections of the manuscript, that I hope will help the team improve some arguments in the introduction and discussion, and clarify a few aspects of the methods and results.

COMMENT ON THE TITLE

The title advertises an evaluation of the "global mismatches" between aesthetics and conservation threat. In the valuation literature, the term mismatch is widely used, in particular to describe the mismatches between the supply and demand for an object of valuation. In this manuscript, the term is not used in the text, neither in the introduction nor in the discussion, and having it in the title may be misleading for the readership. Besides, you also use the term "match" to describe a pair of images being compared to one another, and the term "mismatch" may also be confusing. I would suggest to slightly change the title to avoid this potential double confusion. In addition, the mismatch between the aesthetic value and the conservation status is postulated to be a concern, but this may not be the case (see my comments on the discussion).

COMMENTS ON THE INTRODUCTION

L68: what do you mean by "means of NCP" ? consider rephrasing?

L95-96: can you be a bit more specific here and explain what you mean by "visual feature"?

L.101: the sentence "to accurately identify patterns and predict values on images" is confusing. What "value" is being predicted and to what is this value assigned? As this sentence is key to describing the advantages of CNN, I would recommend to be crystal clear on what CNN does that other approaches don't, and how it operates in more concrete terms.

L.108: "public members" feels odd (but note that I'm not a native speaker). I would suggest "13,000 respondents from the broad public".

L.112: typo "evaluated"

L.112-119: In this paragraph, you justify the use of coral reef fishes as a study object by demonstrating their importance first for material NCP (economic activity and industry), regulating NCP (functioning of coral reef), and then only for non-material NCP (aesthetic values). I believe it would make more sense to reverse the argument, as you nicely explain in the paragraphs above that assessments of aesthetic values and, more broadly, non-material NCP are lagging behind material and regulating ones. I would therefore recommend to start with their importance in non-material terms with greater emphasis (i.e. what makes them the perfect object of study to investigate non-material NCP and aesthetic value in particular; diversity of morphologies, aesthetic values, opportunities for recreation, and probably more?), then the regulating (their contribution to the functioning of ecosystem) and then their contribution to material NCP. Besides, there is a slight contradiction between the introduction and the interpretation of the results, because reef fish species are presented as key components for economic activity (L.113-115) to justify using them as a study object, when "only" a fifth of them according to Fig.5b are used for the fishing industry. Rearranging the paragraph would restore the balance in the argumentation a bit.

COMMENTS ON THE RESULTS

Fi.1a panel (ii): the screen capture of the box capture the red underlining of Word/Powerpoint, which should be removed.

L.131-142 & L167-169: these sentences rather belong to material and methods, but I let the authors and/or the editor appreciate whether they are essential to the presentation of the results.

L.138: the term "judge" is rather unusual. I would recommend using "respondent" and stick to it throughout the text and in the supplement

L.193: specific "predicted aesthetic values"; the values have not been described so far in the text, and I believe this is an important aspect to clarify. The valuation literature is full of examples of different valuation methods, and giving a few words on the Elo method would be critical, in my humble opinion.

Fig.2a: I would suggest to have clearer labels for each of the 9 image features in the figure. The labels are rather cryptic in their current form and do not help the reading.

L212-219: why don't you provide formal testing of the relationship between aesthetic value and both PCA axes, instead of only a visual interpretation of the patterns in Fig. 2b?

Fig.4: the color gradient should be displayed as a legend on the figure itself.

COMMENTS ON THE DISCUSSION

The paper highlights that the aesthetic value of fish is inversely proportional to their threat status and to their ecological originality/distinctiveness/usefulness (e.g.L.293). Playing the advocates' devil, isn't that a good thing that the aesthetic, intangibles values associated non-material NCP are complementary to the material and regulating NCP provided by those fish? This complementarity is an exciting finding in my opinion, as a broad spectrum of fish species are valued for complementary reasons, which makes the case for paying attention to multiple aspects to preserve them: ecological, aesthetic, and economic. To frame my question differently: what would happen if the fish species that were appreciated for their aesthetics were also highly valued by the industry, and were the most original ones and essential to the functioning of ecosystems? Worse, if they were also threatened, there would be a greater risk of an anthropogenic allee effect, whereby rare + beautiful species would suffer from an even greater demand from the broad public.

L.443-446: how is this statement backed-up by the results, and what do you mean by "less well-represented in the assessments"? I would recommend greater clarity in this statement, as it is not straightforward that data deficient species are less attractive than species present in the IUCN assessments. Besides, isn't this contradictory to the earlier statements in this paragraph about charismatic and aesthetically attractive mammal species receiving greater conservation attention (L.441-442)? Here, you demonstrate that the most aesthetically pleasing species are not particularly threatened, and will therefore not necessarily need greater conservation attention.

COMMENTS ON THE METHODS

L. 566-567: given the fact that the evaluation of the 9 features in an image is based on saturation and lightness (L.665), how did you make sure that your photoshop correction did not bias the results in any way? How many pictures did you correct that way? I do acknowledge that some corrections may have been necessary, but given the later evaluation of the image characteristics, why not discarding the images that were deemed in need of a photoshop correction?

I could not find the information on the number of "matches" undertaken by each species in the dataset. I don't doubt that with over 13,000 respondents evaluating 30 matches, the distribution of matches over all species is hypothetically even, but I was curious to know if this was the case.

It is a bit surprising that no ethical statement is provided concerning the use of a social survey. Usually, an informed consent is provided by the respondent, acknowledging that the participant undertakes the survey on a voluntary basis, that anonymity is safeguarded (this is the case here), that the respondent can opt out at no cost, and that the objective of the research has been clearly stated. Was such a consent from the participants clearly formulated?

Attachment

Submitted filename: Review_PBIOLOGY-D-21-02764_R1.pdf

Decision Letter 2

Roland G Roberts

6 Apr 2022

Dear Dr Mouquet,

Thank you for submitting your revised Research Article entitled "Global mismatch between the aesthetic value of reef fishes and their conservation priorities" for publication in PLOS Biology. The Academic Editor and I have now assessed your responses and revisions.

Based on this assessment, we will probably accept this manuscript for publication, provided you satisfactorily address the remaining points raised by the Academic Editor, and the following data and other policy-related requests.

IMPORTANT: Please attend to the following:

a) Please re-arrange your title to incorporate an active verb. We suggest the following: "The aesthetic value of reef fishes is globally mismatched to their conservation priorities."

b) Please address the minor requests from the Academic Editor, pasted into the bottom of this email.

c) The Academic Editor thinks (and we agree) that it would be extremely useful for future readers to make the peer-review history of your manuscript accessible, so they can read and assess your thoughtful answers to the reviewers. You will be offered this option later on during the production process, and we ask that you take it.

d) When I was reading about your questionnaire in the Methods section, I wondered how the participants were advised regarding the nature of the project and their consent. In the Supplementary Information I found the following paragraph: "Note that the internet questionnaire we used contained an introduction that clearly stated the objective of the research and an ethical statement to guarantee anonymity to the respondents; we provided each respondent with an anonymity identification number which could be used to opt out at no cost following the French Data Protection Authority (CNIL) recommendation." Please include this helpful information in the main paper.

e) Please address my Data Policy requests below; specifically, we need you to supply the numerical values underlying Figs 1B, 2AB, 3, 4, 5, S1E, S1G, S1H, S1I, S1J, S1M, S1N, S1O, S1P, S1Q, S1R, S1S, S1T. Please also cite the location of the data clearly in each relevant main and supplementary Fig legend, e.g. if, for example, the Figs can all be generated using the data and code in your Gthub deposition, you should write “Data and code required to generate this Figure can be found in https://github.com/nmouquet/RLS_AESTHE”.

As you address these items, please take this last chance to review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the cover letter that accompanies your revised manuscript.

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within two weeks.

To submit your revision, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' to find your submission record. Your revised submission must include the following:

-  a cover letter that should detail your responses to any editorial requests, if applicable, and whether changes have been made to the reference list

-  a Response to Reviewers file that provides a detailed response to the reviewers' comments (if applicable)

-  a track-changes file indicating any changes that you have made to the manuscript. 

NOTE: If Supporting Information files are included with your article, note that these are not copyedited and will be published as they are submitted. Please ensure that these files are legible and of high quality (at least 300 dpi) in an easily accessible file format. For this reason, please be aware that any references listed in an SI file will not be indexed. For more information, see our Supporting Information guidelines:

https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/supporting-information  

*Published Peer Review History*

Please note that you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*Press*

Should you, your institution's press office or the journal office choose to press release your paper, please ensure you have opted out of Early Article Posting on the submission form. We ask that you notify us as soon as possible if you or your institution is planning to press release the article.

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols

Please do not hesitate to contact me should you have any questions.

Sincerely,

Roli Roberts

Roland G Roberts, PhD,

Senior Editor,

rroberts@plos.org,

PLOS Biology

------------------------------------------------------------------------

DATA POLICY:

You may be aware of the PLOS Data Policy, which requires that all data be made available without restriction: http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/data-availability. For more information, please also see this editorial: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001797 

Note that we do not require all raw data. Rather, we ask that all individual quantitative observations that underlie the data summarized in the figures and results of your paper be made available in one of the following forms:

1) Supplementary files (e.g., excel). Please ensure that all data files are uploaded as 'Supporting Information' and are invariably referred to (in the manuscript, figure legends, and the Description field when uploading your files) using the following format verbatim: S1 Data, S2 Data, etc. Multiple panels of a single or even several figures can be included as multiple sheets in one excel file that is saved using exactly the following convention: S1_Data.xlsx (using an underscore).

2) Deposition in a publicly available repository. Please also provide the accession code or a reviewer link so that we may view your data before publication. 

Regardless of the method selected, please ensure that you provide the individual numerical values that underlie the summary data displayed in the following figure panels as they are essential for readers to assess your analysis and to reproduce it: Figs 1B, 2AB, 3, 4, 5, S1E, S1G, S1H, S1I, S1J, S1M, S1N, S1O, S1P, S1Q, S1R, S1S, S1T. NOTE: the numerical data provided should include all replicates AND the way in which the plotted mean and errors were derived (it should not present only the mean/average values).

IMPORTANT: Please also ensure that figure legends in your manuscript include information on where the underlying data can be found, and ensure your supplemental data file/s has a legend.

Please ensure that your Data Statement in the submission system accurately describes where your data can be found.

------------------------------------------------------------------------

DATA NOT SHOWN?

- Please note that per journal policy, we do not allow the mention of "data not shown", "personal communication", "manuscript in preparation" or other references to data that is not publicly available or contained within this manuscript. Please either remove mention of these data or provide figures presenting the results and the data underlying the figure(s).

------------------------------------------------------------------------

COMMENTS FROM THE ACADEMIC EDITOR:

I caught a few very minor points on my reread:

* "judges" still appears in Fig. 1 despite the authors saying that they have changed all instances to "respondents” in response to one of the reviewer’s comments

* Line 229 of the SI: It would be good to clarify explicitly that the +/- refers to a standard deviation on first usage (I presume it does?). The same issue appears in the Main Text, e.g. line 156, 192, 248-250 (where by the way there is no space after the +/- but there is on line 252).

* There is an extra full stop on line 104 after "variables" that should be removed.

* I agree with R4 on line 143, which I don’t think the authors have addressed satisfactorily. I think that instead of "through Elo scores computation" the authors should say something like "by computing a metric that considers ... few words go here to explain what a Elo score is to a general reader".

Decision Letter 3

Roland G Roberts

21 Apr 2022

Dear Dr Mouquet,

On behalf of my colleagues and the Academic Editor, Andrew Tanentzap, I'm pleased to say that we can in principle accept your Research Article "The aesthetic value of reef fishes is globally mismatched to their conservation priorities" for publication in PLOS Biology, provided you address any remaining formatting and reporting issues. These will be detailed in an email that will follow this letter and that you will usually receive within 2-3 business days, during which time no action is required from you. Please note that we will not be able to formally accept your manuscript and schedule it for publication until you have completed any requested changes.

Please take a minute to log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/, click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information to ensure an efficient production process.

As mentioned previously, we think that it would be extremely useful for future readers to make the peer-review history of your manuscript accessible, so they can read and assess your thoughtful answers to the reviewers. You will be offered this option later on during the production process, and we ask that you take it (we note that you are happy to do this).

PRESS: We frequently collaborate with press offices. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximise its impact. If the press office is planning to promote your findings, we would be grateful if they could coordinate with biologypress@plos.org. If you have previously opted in to the early version process, we ask that you notify us immediately of any press plans so that we may opt out on your behalf.

We also ask that you take this opportunity to read our Embargo Policy regarding the discussion, promotion and media coverage of work that is yet to be published by PLOS. As your manuscript is not yet published, it is bound by the conditions of our Embargo Policy. Please be aware that this policy is in place both to ensure that any press coverage of your article is fully substantiated and to provide a direct link between such coverage and the published work. For full details of our Embargo Policy, please visit http://www.plos.org/about/media-inquiries/embargo-policy/.

Thank you again for choosing PLOS Biology for publication and supporting Open Access publishing. We look forward to publishing your study. 

Sincerely, 

Roli Roberts

Roland G Roberts, PhD 

Senior Editor 

PLOS Biology

rroberts@plos.org

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 File. Supporting information methods and analysis with associated figures and tables.

    Text A. Image features analysis. Text B. Image sampling strategy. Text C. Sociocultural background. Text D. Elo scores. Text E. Deep learning algorithm. Text F. Relationship between features and aesthetic values. Text G. Phylogenetic analysis. Text H. Ecological traits. Text I. Mean aesthetic value. Fig A. Representation of the three-dimensional CIELAB space. Fig B. Cluster analysis performed for Holacanthus ciliaris. Fig C. HSV color space. Fig D. Illustration of the analysis led with the Momocs package. Fig E. Projection of the 4,881 images of our collection on the 2 first axes of the PCA on the images’ features. Fig F. Questionnaire on the sociocultural background of the judges. Fig G. Number of judges per country. The map was obtained from rworldmap v1.3.6 R package, which uses Natural Earth as base layer (https://www.naturalearthdata.com/downloads/10m-physical-vectors/). Fig H. Summary of sociocultural background of the 13,000 judges. Fig I. Variation of the Elo scores of the images as matches accumulate. Fig J. Linear regression between the Elo scores for the 21 images in common between the 2 surveys. Fig K. Effect of the size of the input images on the performances of the model. Fig L. Architecture of ResNet50 modified to predict the aesthetic values. Fig M. Relationship between the Evolutionary Distinctiveness of species (in MY) and their aesthetic value. Fig N. Mean aesthetic values of families with more than 10 species presented in decreasing order. Fig O. Comparison between the aesthetic value of the fish species and their ecological traits. Fig P. Number of images per species. Fig Q. Relationship between the aesthetic values computed using the maximum and the mean values. Fig R. Phylogenetic history and ecological originality with mean aesthetic values. Fig S. Conservation status with mean aesthetic values. Table A. Analysis of deviance in the generalized linear mixed model. Table B. Pagel’s λ. Table C. List of the ecological traits used with their nature and modalities. Table D. P values of the Tukey’s tests.

    (DOCX)

    S1 Data. Copyright information for the images used in the figures.

    (XLSX)

    S2 Data. Species included in the study.

    (XLSX)

    S3 Data. Aesthetic values for the 2,417 species of the study.

    (XLSX)

    S4 Data. URL links for the 4,881 original images used in the study.

    (XLSX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Review_PBIOLOGY-D-21-02764_R1.pdf

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: RLSAESTHE_responses.pdf

    Data Availability Statement

    Most of the data used in this paper are freely available and downloadable from the web. Data on IUCN threat status are available in the IUCN Red List database (https://www.iucnredlist.org/). RLS data for species lists and some trait information are available through an online portal accessible through (http://www.reeflifesurvey.com), with additional trait data available on request by using the contact form. For each species, we provide aesthetic values predicted in the present study (S3 Data) and web links to original photographic material (S4 Data). Images free of copyright can be provided on request. Other datasets used in this study (extraction from the online survey and images features analysis) and all code used for the analysis and figures are available from the GitHub Repository: https://github.com/nmouquet/RLS_AESTHE.


    Articles from PLoS Biology are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES