Abstract
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) pathology accumulates for decades before the onset of cognitive decline. Cognitively normal individuals with biomarker evidence of AD brain pathology (i.e. biomarker + or preclinical AD) can be differentiated from individuals without AD brain pathology based on naturalistic driving data, such as hard acceleration or braking and speeding, measured using in-vehicle dataloggers. Older adults are at increased risk of injury and death from motor vehicle crashes and driving cessation is also linked to negative health outcomes. Identifying potentially modifiable risk factors that increase driving risk may prolong safe driving in old age. Sleep apnea is associated with adverse driving behaviors across the age span. In this study, we hypothesized that high-risk driving behaviors would be associated with increased sleep apnea severity and AD pathology. We found that higher sleep apnea severity measured by a home sleep apnea test was associated with a higher incidence of adverse driving behaviors even after controlling for multiple confounders (β = 0.24 ± 0.09, p < 0.01). This association was independent of AD biomarker positivity (i.e. increased t-tau/Aβ 42 ratio). Increasing age was associated with a higher likelihood of high-risk driving behaviors in individuals with AD brain pathology (β = 0.12 ± 0.04, p < 0.01), but a lower likelihood in individuals without AD brain pathology (β = −0.06 ± 0.03, p < 0.05). These findings suggest that adverse driving behaviors linked to a higher rate of traffic crashes in older adults are associated with sleep apnea severity and AD pathology even in cognitively unimpaired individuals. Further studies are needed to determine if treatment of sleep apnea decreases high-risk driving behaviors and therefore motor vehicle crashes.
Keywords: obstructive sleep apnea, driving, Alzheimer’s disease, older adults
Statement of Significance.
Older adults are at increased risk of injury and death from motor vehicle crashes. Driving cessation is also linked to negative health outcomes. Identifying potentially modifiable risk factors that increase driving risk may prolong safe driving in old age. Sleep apnea is associated with adverse driving behaviors across the age span. In this study, we found that higher sleep apnea severity measured by a home sleep apnea test and age in individuals with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) brain pathology was associated with a higher incidence of adverse driving behaviors. These findings suggest that adverse driving behaviors linked to a higher rate of traffic crashes in older adults are associated with sleep apnea severity and AD pathology even in individuals without cognitive impairment.
Introduction
The Center for Disease Control and Prevention lists motor vehicle crashes as the second leading cause of deaths by injury in the 65+ age group in 2019 after falls [1]. The age distribution of motor vehicle crashes resembles an inverted bell curve, with rates dropping from ages 16 to 30 before rising again after age 65 [2]. Though this increased risk of crashes does not reach the same level as for younger drivers, older adults are at a much higher risk of death [3], while survivors are more likely to be hospitalized [4] and suffer more severe injuries due to increased frailty [5]. Given the negative health and social outcomes associated with driving cessation [6–9], contrasted with the increased risk of crashes and the associated risks of mortality and/or severe injury, it is important to consider ways to maximize the years of safe driving in older adults while balancing the risks and rewards of access to a personal vehicle.
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a growing public health crisis. An estimated 5.8 million Americans were living with AD in 2019 [10], and this number is expected to increase to around 14 million by 2060 [10, 11]. Older adults with AD are at a higher risk for motor vehicle crashes [12]. Drivers with cognitive impairment due to AD have 2–5 times the risk of motor vehicle crashes compared with healthy older adults [13, 14], and often progressively limit their driving activities as they become aware of new limitations brought on by the disease [15–17]. In addition, 30%–40% of cognitively normal older adults have biomarker evidence of AD brain pathology and are classified as having preclinical AD [18]. Individuals with preclinical AD are cognitively unimpaired but have detectable underlying disease pathologies and an annual conversion rate to symptomatic AD between 20% and 30% [19–21]. Those with preclinical AD make more errors resulting in a failed road test [22, 23] and have a higher frequency of traffic violations and crashes [24]. Machine learning has been able to differentiate between individuals with and without preclinical AD biomarkers based on measures of naturalistic driving [25, 26], suggesting drivers with preclinical AD experience changes in driving well before any clinical diagnosis criteria are met.
Poor sleep quality or sleep deprivation is also associated with a higher likelihood of motor vehicle crashes [27–29]. Obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) is a common sleep disorder affecting an estimated 54 million Americans [30] and often causes daytime sleepiness (DTS) [31, 32]. Individuals with OSA are at risk for impaired driving and increased risks of crashes [33–36]. As with AD, the prevalence of OSA increases with age [37–40]. OSA is also associated with an increased risk of developing cognitive impairment [41]. Previous research has separately investigated the effects of OSA, aging, and preclinical or symptomatic AD on driving. In the current study, we investigated the effects of OSA and preclinical AD status on driving behaviors that have been shown to increase the risk of motor vehicle crashes such as hard braking, hard acceleration, and speeding [42, 43]. While both OSA and preclinical AD have been found to affect driving performance, it is unknown whether these effects are additive or interactive, or how age may moderate these effects. While amyloid-lowering therapeutics are still being developed and refined [44, 45], treatment of OSA through positive airway pressure (PAP) therapy has been shown to counteract sleep-related driving difficulties [33, 46] and improve overall cognitive function [47], including in individuals with MCI and symptomatic AD [48]. We hypothesize that older adults with both OSA and preclinical AD will have an increase in adverse or high-risk driving behaviors, and that these effects will be additive such that those with preclinical AD will show an increased effect of OSA severity on the incidence of high-risk driving behaviors that are linked to increased risk of motor vehicle crashes. If the relationship between aging, preclinical AD, OSA, and driving can be better understood, interventions may be identified that prolong safe driving in older age.
Methods
Participants
Study participants consisted of 96 older adults aged 65 and older (47 female and 49 male) enrolled in studies of aging, AD, and driving behavior at the Washington University Knight Alzheimer Disease Research Center (ADRC). Participants are community-dwelling adults who are followed at the Knight ADRC for research studies only. All participants in Knight ADRC studies undergo annual clinical and cognitive assessments by a clinician. For inclusion in the current study, participants needed to meet the following inclusion criteria: (1) be cognitively normal based on a Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) [49] of “0”; (2) have undergone lumbar puncture for the collection of cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarker data within 1 year of the sleep study date; (3) have a valid driver’s license and drive at least once a week; (4) have both sleep apnea data and sleep quality data collected; and (5) have one full year of naturalistic data collected with overlapping dates within 1 year of the sleep apnea and sleep quality data collection. Studies were approved by the Washington University Institutional Review Board and each participant signed informed consent.
CSF biomarkers
CSF was collected as previously described [50]. CSF amyloid-beta42 (Aβ 42) and total tau (t-Tau) were measured using an automated electrochemiluminescence immunoassay (Lumipulse G1200, Fujirebio). The diagnostic biomarker ratio of total Tau to amyloid-beta42 (t-Tau/Aβ 42) was used to detect preclinical AD. Participants were classified based on cutoffs that have high concordance with amyloid PET [51]. Participants were classed as “positive” and coded as “1” for the analysis, if their t-Tau/Aβ 42 ratio was over 0.488 they were coded as “0” otherwise.
Driving
Naturalistic driving data were captured using a commercial GPS data logger (“G2 Tracking Device,” Azuga Inc, San Jose, CA) plugged into the onboard diagnostics-II (OBDII) port of the participants’ personal vehicles. The OBDII port is a standardized port present in all vehicles built and sold in the United States after January 1, 1996 [52]. The ports are primarily located on the driver’s side of the vehicle under the dashboard. The chips use the vehicle’s battery for power, are a minimally invasive means of collecting naturalistic driving data, take approximately 20 s to install, and elicit no behavioral change in the participants. The Driving Real-world In-Vehicle Evaluation System (DRIVES) samples data every 30 s anytime a vehicle is driven, and provide a wide range of data including date, time, speed, latitude, and longitude, as well as a number of driving behaviors including hard braking, sudden acceleration, and speeding. The use of this system has been described in detail in previous publications [53–56]. Speed data are collected every 30 s unless a hard braking or sudden acceleration event occurs, which are logged immediately. Sudden acceleration and hard braking are defined as acceleration/deceleration in excess of 8 mph/s. Speeding is defined as a speed at least 6 mph over the posted speed limit. Driving data were captured over a 12-month period for each participant.
In order to examine the effects of both OSA and preclinical AD on driving behavior, we created a composite of adverse or high-risk driving behaviors that included hard braking, sudden acceleration, and speeding. These three variables have been combined into a single measure as in previous research [57], with observed differences in baseline measures of incidences of high-risk driving behaviors between negative and positive biomarker groups. The variable was labeled “adverse driving” and was coded as “1” if there were any incidences of high-risk driving behaviors during a trip and “0” if there were no instances.
Sleep
Participants underwent a one-night home sleep apnea test (HSAT; Alice PDx, Philips Respironics Inc, Murrysville, PA). The Alice PDx is a type III HSAT device that monitors oxygen saturation (SpO2) and pulse rate from an oximeter finger probe, nasal pressure-based airflow monitor and thermistor, thoracic and abdominal effort via inductance plethysmography, and body position. The Alice PDx has been found to have 96.4% agreement with simultaneously recorded in-laboratory polysomnography [58]. Participants pressed the event monitor at lights off and lights on. Bed and rise times were also confirmed with sleep logs and actigraphy as previously described [59]. In the morning, participants checked the “good study” indicator on the device to confirm a minimum of 4 h of recording. A minimum of 4 h of artifact-free recording was obtained for all participants and participants not meeting this criterion were asked to repeat monitoring. Respiratory events were scored by registered polysomnographic technologists using the American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) criteria [60] and were reviewed by a board-certified sleep medicine physician (B.P.L.). Hypopneas were scored using 4% oxygen desaturation criteria. The apnea–hypopnea index (AHI) was calculated per hour of monitoring time for each participant. Participants using PAP therapy or dental devices were asked to use them as usual during the HSAT (PAP: 12 participants in the preclinical AD group, 1 in the non-preclinical AD group; dental device: 1 in non-preclinical AD group, 0 in the preclinical AD group). The same respiratory event scoring criteria were used for all participants (untreated, treated with PAP, and treated with dental device).
Sleep was also assessed over six nights at home using a single-channel electroencephalography (scEEG) device (Sleep Profiler, Advanced Brain Monitoring, Carlsbad, CA). As with the Alice PDx, bed and rise times were confirmed with sleep logs and actigraphy. The scEEG device was worn on the forehead and recorded at 256 samples per second from three frontal sensors placed at approximately AF7, AF8, and Fpz. Only the AF7–AF8 channel was used for scoring by registered polysomnographic technologists using modified AASM criteria [61]. Average total sleep time (TST) and sleep efficiency (SE) were used in the analyses. Participants’ self-reported DTS was also assessed via the Epworth Sleepiness Scale [62]. This is an eight-question scale that asks participants to rate how likely they are to fall asleep during a number of different daytime activities, such as watching television, sitting and talking to someone, or while driving a car that has stopped in traffic.
Cognition
Preclinical AD is defined by the presence of biomarkers for AD brain pathology in the absence of cognitive symptoms. In order to ensure that the preclinical AD positive and negative groups did not differ in terms of cognition, their performance on a number of cognitive tasks was compared. Cognition was compared using a composite score [63] that included sub-tests measuring episodic memory (Free and cued selective reminding task: free recall score) [64], semantic category naming (Animal fluency) [65], processing speed (Trail making A) [66], and executive function (Trail making B) [66]. The composite score was calculated by standardizing scores with a z-score using the sample mean and standard deviation of each of the sub-tests and then calculating a mean of the standardized scores.
Statistical analysis
The data used in the analysis included every trip taken by the sample of 96 participants during the 12-month study period—a total of 119 754 trips. Differences in demographics, driving outcomes, sleep metrics, and cognition were examined between biomarker negative and biomarker positive groups to ensure that no confounding group differences were present. The data were then analyzed using binomial generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs). Binomial GLMMs allow the analysis of binary outcomes; in this case, whether a trip had an aggressive driving event or not. This analysis approach was selected since adverse driving events are both rare and highly variable between participants, which means that Poisson regression analyses are inappropriate due to both zero inflation and overdispersion. Also, due to the variability between participants, and because observations (trips) were nested by the participant, the models include a random intercept for participant ID in order to improve model fit. All models controlled for age, sex, and years of education. Age was centered by subtracting the mean from each age value, which is a common method used in aging research to generate coefficient estimates that are based on the mean age of the sample rather than an age of “0” [67]. To avoid convergence issues due to different scales, and to aid interpretation of main effects and interactions [68–70], the AHI, TST, SE, DTS, education, and biomarker variables were scaled by subtracting the mean from each value and dividing by the standard deviation.
Results
Participant characteristics
Participant characteristics are summarized in Table 1. Variables used in the regression modeling are shown in bold. There were no statistically significant differences in age or years of education, or differences in the distributions of sex or race between the biomarker negative and biomarker positive groups. There were also no statistical differences in the mean driving behaviors or any of the sleep measures. While the groups differed on the episodic memory sub-score, there was no statistically significant difference for the composite score. This result was expected since only participants rated as cognitively normal (CDR = 0) were included in the study.
Table 1.
Participant characteristics
Total sample | Biomarker negative (n = 61)* | Biomarker positive (n = 35)* | Test† | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Demographics | ||||
Age, years | 74.13 ± 5.05 | 73.46 ± 4.94 | 75.31 ± 5.11 | p = 0.09 |
Sex, N = female (%) | 47 (49.00) | 30 (49.18) | 17 (48.57) | p = 1.00 |
Education, years | 16.65 ± 2.00 | 16.72 ± 2.16 | 16.51 ± 2.32 | p = 0.67 |
Race‡, N = White (%) | 84 (87.50) | 51 (83.61) | 33 (94.29) | p = 0.20 |
APOE ε4 + carrier, N (%) | 41 (42.71) | 18 (29.51) | 23 (65.71) | p < 0.01** |
CSF biomarkers | ||||
t-Tau/Aβ 42 > 0.488 | 0.50 ± 0.39 | 0.284 ± 0.075 | 0.869 ± 0.444 | p < 0.001*** |
p-Tau/Aβ 42 > 0.0649 | 0.07 ± 0.06 | 0.038 ± 0.011 | 0.125 ± 0.073 | p < 0.001*** |
Aβ 42/Aβ 40 < 0.0673 | 0.07 ± 0.02 | 0.086 ± 0.013 | 0.048 ± 0.012 | p < 0.001*** |
Driving | ||||
Trips with speeding (%) | 9.34 ± 10.44 | 10.22 ± 9.73 | 7.82 ± 11.57 | p = 0.34 |
Trips with hard braking (%) | 9.38 ± 7.14 | 9.79 ± 6.36 | 8.66 ± 8.38 | p = 0.49 |
Trips with hard acceleration (%) | 4.70 ± 9.04 | 5.28 ± 10.49 | 3.70 ± 5.69 | p = 0.31 |
Trips with any adverse driving events(%) | 19.50 ± 13.74 | 21.13 ± 13.34 | 16.64 ± 14.16 | p = 0.13 |
Cognition | ||||
Free & cued serial reminding task | 29.39 ± 6.11 | 30.47 ± 5.75 | 27.50 ± 6.36 | p < 0.05* |
Animal fluency | 20.73 ± 5.62 | 20.85 ± 5.86 | 20.51 ± 5.25 | p = 0.77 |
Trail making A | 31.31 ± 10.00 | 32.04 ± 10.74 | 30.09 ± 8.63 | p = 0.35 |
Trail making B | 75.32 ± 26.11 | 76.86 ± 26.81 | 72.74 ± 25.08 | p = 0.46 |
Composite score | 0.01 ± 0.63 | 0.028 ± 0.680 | −0.012 ± 0.550 | p = 0.76 |
Sleep | ||||
Central apnea index | 0.35 ± 0.78 | 0.28 ± 0.45 | 0.48 ± 1.14 | p = 0.32 |
Obstructive apnea index | 0.62 ± 1.89 | 0.62 ± 2.00 | 0.61 ± 1.73 | p = 0.97 |
Mixed apnea index | 0.07 ± 0.31 | 0.05 ± 0.25 | 0.11 ± 0.41 | p = 0.42 |
Hypopnea index | 7.36 ± 7.49 | 7.45 ± 8.19 | 7.19 ± 6.2 | p = 0.86 |
Apnea–hypopnea index | 8.40 ± 8.46 | 8.40 ± 9.19 | 8.39 ± 7.15 | p = 1.00 |
Sleep efficiency (%) | 79.39 ± 8.91 | 80.27 ± 7.66 | 77.87 ± 10.71 | p = 0.25 |
Total sleep time, min | 374.52 ± 52.14 | 376.98 ± 50.78 | 370.34 ± 54.92 | p = 0.56 |
Daytime sleepiness | 6.54 ± 3.90 | 6.59 ± 4.25 | 6.46 ± 3.23 | p = 0.86 |
APOE, apolipoprotein E composite; CSF, cerebrospinal fluid. Variables in bold were included in the models.
*Preclinical AD status was based on t-Tau/Aβ 42 ratio.
†Means were compared using t-tests. Frequencies were compared using Fisher’s exact test.
‡The sample contains only Black and White races.
Increased AHI associated with more adverse driving events
The regression analyses for driving and AHI are summarized in Table 2. The table summarizes four models: (1) only main effects; (2) main effects plus an AHI:t-Tau/Aβ 42 interaction; (3) main effects plus a t-Tau/Aβ 42:age interaction; and (4) main effects plus the two interaction terms. An Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) value is included with the models to allow comparisons of model fit. Out of a set of models, the model with the smallest AIC value is considered the best-fitting in terms of under- and over-fitting. For the regressions summarized in Table 2, the best-fitting model is that which includes the t-Tau/Aβ 42:age interaction (Model 3).
Table 2.
Hierarchical logistic regressions of aggressive driving behavior using CSF t-Tau/Aβ 42 to define biomarker status
Dependent variable | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Adverse driving | ||||
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | |
(Intercept) | −1.600*** (0.155) | −1.596*** (0.163) | −1.654*** (0.154) | −1.652*** (0.154) |
AHI | 0.218* (0.097) | 0.250* (0.111) | 0.242** (0.092) | 0.256* (0.107) |
t-Tau/Aβ 42+ | −0.420* (0.213) | −0.421* (0.208) | −0.475* (0.202) | −0.475* (0.192) |
Age | −0.019 (0.021) | −0.020 (0.021) | −0.062* (0.026) | −0.062* (0.026) |
Sex (F vs. M) | 0.108 (0.208) | 0.102 (0.220) | 0.169 (0.198) | 0.166 (0.205) |
Education | −0.099 (0.103) | −0.111 (0.109) | −0.143 (0.102) | −0.148 (0.103) |
AHI:tTau/Aβ 42+ | −0.125 (0.219) | −0.057 (0.214) | ||
t-Tau/Aβ 42+:Age | 0.115** (0.043) | 0.114** (0.044) | ||
Observations | 119 754 | 119 754 | 119 754 | 119 754 |
Log likelihood | −51 488.090 | −51 487.950 | −51 484.690 | −51 484.660 |
Akaike inf. crit. | 102 990.200 | 102 991.900 | 102 985.400 | 102 987.300 |
Coefficient estimates for changes in log odds are displayed, with standard errors. The best fitting model according to Akaike Information Criterion is Model 3.
*p < 0.05.
** p < 0.01.
*** p < 0.001.
The coefficients represent changes in the log odds of a trip having an incident of adverse driving given a point increase in the value for each independent variable. It is possible to calculate odds ratios from these coefficients. For example, in Table 2, Model 3, the AHI coefficient is 0.24, meaning that participants were e0.24 = 1.27 times more likely to have an adverse driving incident during a trip for every 1 point increase in the scaled AHI variable, which translates to an approximate eight-point increase in the AHI. Conversely, the t-Tau/Aβ 42+ group were 1/e−0.47 = 1.61 times “less” likely to have an adverse driving event during a trip compared with the t-Tau/Aβ 42− group. The effects of AHI and biomarkers are shown in Figure 1, which plots AHI against the percentage of trips with an adverse driving event across the three biomarkers.
Figure 1.
Comparison trips with adverse driving plotted against the apnea–hypopnea index (AHI) and grouped by biomarker status. Greater AHI results in increased mean number of trips with adverse driving behavior in both groups, although the mean number of adverse driving events was lower for the biomarker negative group. Note: To increase legibility of the plots two outliers have been excluded from the data. These participants were included in the data used to construct the statistical models as they did not meet any a priori exclusion criteria.
To confirm that these differences could be attributed to AHI rather than general sleep quality, three additional regression models were run that matched the structure of Table 2, Model 3, except that the AHI factor was replaced with SE, TST, and DTS as measured by the Epworth Sleepiness Scale [75]. None of these sleep variables were statistically significant factors in explaining adverse driving behavior (Table 3).
Table 3.
Supplementary regression models investigating measures of sleep quality
Dependent variable | |||
---|---|---|---|
Adverse driving | |||
(1) | (2) | (3) | |
(Intercept) | −1.601*** (0.153) | −1.599*** (0.166) | −1.819*** (0.233) |
Sleep efficiency | 0.008 (0.105) | ||
Total sleep time, min | 0.047 (0.105) | ||
Daytime sleepiness | 0.032 (0.026) | ||
t-Tau/Aβ 42+ | −0.462* (0.208) | −0.457* (0.211) | −0.460* (0.210) |
Age | −0.062* (0.027) | −0.062* (0.027) | −0.065* (0.027) |
Sex (F vs. M) | 0.051 (0.198) | 0.038 (0.216) | 0.057 (0.203) |
Education | −0.137 (0.106) | −0.133 (0.110) | −0.135 (0.109) |
t-Tau/Aβ 42+:Age | 0.105* (0.045) | 0.104* (0.045) | 0.112* (0.044) |
Observations | 119 754 | 119 754 | 119 754 |
Log likelihood | −51 487.670 | −51 487.570 | −51 486.960 |
*p < 0.05.
** p < 0.01.
*** p < 0.001.
Relationship between OSA and AD biomarker status
We hypothesized that AHI and biomarker status would have a compounding effect on adverse driving. However, this pattern was not observed in the analysis: the best fitting model (Table 2, Model 3) did not contain an interaction between AHI and biomarker status, and when the interaction term was included (Table 2, Model 4) it was not statistically significant. It appears, therefore, that AHI and biomarker status have independent effects on adverse driving behavior.
Relationship between age and adverse driving differs by AD biomarker status
The decreased overall risk of adverse driving in the biomarker positive group also did not match our predictions. However, there is a statistically significant interaction between age and t-Tau/Aβ 42 in the best-fitting model (Table 2, Model 3). To interpret this effect, we first examined the main effect of age. Focusing on Table 2, Model 3, for every 1-year increase in age, adverse driving events are 1/e−0.47 = 1.61 times “less” likely. Since this coefficient estimate is based on all other factors being set to zero, this 1.61 figure represents the decrease in the odds of an adverse driving event in the negative biomarker group (which was coded as “0”). Conversely, the t-Tau/Aβ 42+:age interaction represents a 1.17 “increase” in the probability of an adverse driving event with every 1-year increase in age in the t-Tau/Aβ 42+ group. To help visualize this interaction, the percentage of trips with adverse driving is plotted against age and biomarker group in Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Mean number of trips with adverse driving plotted by age and biomarker status. The biomarker negative group demonstrates a decrease in adverse driving with age, while the biomarker positive group demonstrates an increase in adverse driving with age.
Discussion
The effect of OSA on driving has been explored largely by comparing mean performance between different groups or performance in the same group pre-/post-treatment [46, 71] rather than how the severity of sleep apnea is related to driving behaviors, and/or have relied on driving simulators to measure driving performance rather than real-world driving [72, 73]. Additionally, previous research has investigated the effects of dementia and preclinical AD [22–24, 53, 54, 74] on driving [12, 15–17], the contributions of comorbidities have not been thoroughly investigated. Links between sleep quality and AD risk have also been the focus of recent research [75, 76], but little work has investigated their combined effects on real-world behaviors such as driving. This study combines these areas of research and provides novel findings in the sleep, AD, and driving fields of research.
The effects of sleep apnea and preclinical AD biomarker status on driving
In this study, we found that OSA, as measured by AHI, increased the likelihood of a participant having an adverse driving event during a trip, with around a 1.25 times increase in the odds of an event for each approximate eight-point increase in the AHI. This increase in AHI is meaningful given the typical clinical cutoffs of <5: no sleep apnea; 5–15: mild sleep apnea; 15–30: moderate sleep apnea; and >30: severe sleep apnea [77]. This finding adds to the existing sleep apnea and driving literature as it supports previous findings comparing participants with sleep apnea with healthy controls, while also providing evidence that more severe sleep apnea as measured by the AHI is associated with a higher likelihood of adverse driving events.
We did not find a significant relationship between adverse driving and either TST, SE, or DTS. This may be due to a limitation of data collection in our study. Adverse driving scores were measured over 1 year of driving monitoring, while TST and SE were the averages of six nights of monitoring. Further, TST, SE, and an Epworth Sleepiness Score may not be optimal markers for daytime impairment in individuals with sleep apnea. The effect of night-to-night sleep variability on driving behavior cannot be determined in this study. However, these results are promising in that they support the hypothesis that AHI has a specific effect on driving behavior.
Our hypotheses regarding a compounding effect of OSA and preclinical AD on adverse driving behaviors were not supported by the analysis, suggesting that (at least for participants without cognitive symptoms) these two factors have independent effects on adverse driving.
While not an initial hypothesis, there was an interaction between age and biomarker status which revealed that older participants in the biomarker positive group have an increased risk of adverse driving events compared with both younger positive biomarker participants and older negative biomarker participants. This finding is also important when interpreted alongside the lack of interaction between OSA and biomarker status. It appears that for a sample who are not showing cognitive symptoms (as measured by CDR and the combined cognitive index score), sleep apnea, and biomarker status have independent effects on driving behavior. It is unclear at this stage what causes this different pattern in the preclinical AD group, but one potential hypothesis is that younger individuals with preclinical AD have some awareness of change in their driving abilities and so are self-regulating their behavior (as has been observed in previous research [70]). However, older individuals with preclinical AD appear to be less effectively self-regulating their driving—particularly when contrasted with the negative group.
One possible confound may have been trips with aggression were longer on average (mean duration for trips without aggression was 11.33 min, while the mean duration for trips with aggression was 27.71 min). However, biomarker positive participants’ average trip lengths were not statistically different from biomarker negative participants’ (13.53 min vs. 15.24 min, t(58.34) = −1.70, p = 0.09). Additionally, age did not correlate with trip duration for either the positive (r = −0.15, p = 0.36) or negative (r = −0.12, p = 0.36) groups, despite the latter having higher incidences of adverse driving with age. These additional findings support the initial interpretation that age had a different effect on the risk of adverse driving behaviors in individuals with preclinical AD biomarkers compared with those without.
Limitations and future research
The main limitation of our study was the relatively low number of participants in our sample. While we were able to include over 100 000 trips in our analysis, the fact that adverse driving events remain relatively uncommon means a large amount of data are required to detect effects—trips without adverse driving incidents were approximately five times more likely than trips with incidents. One result of this limitation was that all of our analyses dichotomized the biomarker data into positive and negative groups. However, the DRIVES project has ongoing recruitment and so it may be possible to investigate how the full range of biomarker ratios relate to driving behavior, for example, does adverse driving increase as ratios tend toward the preclinical AD cutoffs, and do more extreme values show larger effects.
A related limitation, again due to the sample size but also the long timescale of the ongoing project, is that all comparisons were cross-sectional. The interaction between age and biomarker status suggests that age plays a different role in driving behavior in the positive and negative groups. Future research aims to track participants’ changes in sleep, driving behavior, and biomarker status over time to investigate how these factors interact across the span of old age.
Another caveat was that the sleep assessments were performed at home and not via gold standard attended polysomnography. However, the HSAT and scEEG have a high agreement with polysomnography, and using these methods, we were able to measure sleep in the more naturalistic home environment. A further limitation of the participant sample is that only a subset of study participants had moderate-to-severe OSA. We found significant associations between adverse driving behaviors despite this limitation suggesting that even mild OSA has adverse effects on driving behaviors. However, OSA severity may be underestimated since HSATs do not monitor sleep and rely on monitoring time [58].
By design, our sample was limited to participants with no cognitive impairment (i.e. CDR = 0) and this may be why we did not observe any interaction between OSA and preclinical AD factors. There is also value in understanding how OSA affects individuals further along with the AD pathology. This would also allow for a wider range of biomarker values for the investigation described earlier. An ongoing recruitment process is recruiting participants with CDR > 0.
Conclusion
In this study, we show that OSA increases the likelihood of adverse driving behaviors that have been linked with a higher rate of road traffic crashes, and this risk increases with OSA severity. Older individuals with biomarker evidence of AD are especially likely to have these adverse behaviors. When discussing self-regulated driving and/or driving cessation with participants who are at an increased risk of developing dementia, clinicians may wish to consider potential comorbidities that could have independent effects on driving behavior and whose treatment could prolong safe driving in old age. Although future studies are needed, these findings suggest that treatment of OSA could decrease adverse driving and therefore motor vehicle crashes in cognitively normal older adults.
Contributor Information
Jason M Doherty, Department of Neurology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA.
Catherine M Roe, Department of Neurology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA.
Samantha A Murphy, Department of Neurology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA.
Ann M Johnson, Center for Clinical Studies, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA.
Ella Fleischer, Byram Hills High School, New York, NY, USA.
Cristina D Toedebusch, Department of Neurology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA.
Tiara Redrick, Department of Neurology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA.
David Freund, Department of Neurology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA.
John C Morris, Department of Neurology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA; Hope Center for Neurological Disorders, Washington University in St Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA; Knight Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, Washington University in St Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA.
Suzanne E Schindler, Department of Neurology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA; Knight Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, Washington University in St Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA.
Anne M Fagan, Department of Neurology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA; Hope Center for Neurological Disorders, Washington University in St Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA; Knight Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, Washington University in St Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA.
David M Holtzman, Department of Neurology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA; Hope Center for Neurological Disorders, Washington University in St Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA; Knight Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, Washington University in St Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA; Center on Biological Rhythms and Sleep, Washington University in St Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA.
Brendan P Lucey, Department of Neurology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA; Hope Center for Neurological Disorders, Washington University in St Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA; Center on Biological Rhythms and Sleep, Washington University in St Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA.
Ganesh M Babulal, Department of Neurology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA; Institute of Public Health, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA; Department of Psychology, Faculty of Humanities, University of Johannesburg, Johannesburg , South Africa; Department of Clinical Research and Leadership, The George Washington University School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Washington, DC, USA.
Funding
This work was funded by the National Institute of Health (NIH) and National Institute on Aging (NIH/NIA) grants R01AG068183 (GMB/CMR), R01AG056466 (GMB), R01AG067428 (GMB), R01AG074302 (GMB), P01 AG03991 (JCM), P01 AG026276 (JCM), P30 AG066444 (JCM), and K76 AG054863 (BPL). This work was also supported by the BrightFocus Foundation A2021142S (GMB).
Disclosure Statements
Financial disclosure: JMD, CMR, SAM, AMJ, EF, CDT, TR, DF, JCM, SES, and GMB report no financial disclosures. DMH reports that he is an inventor on a patent licensed by Washington University to C2N Diagnostics on the therapeutic use of anti-tau antibodies. DMH cofounded and is on the scientific advisory board of C2N Diagnostics. C2N Diagnostics has licensed certain anti-tau antibodies to AbbVie for therapeutic development. DMH is on the scientific advisory board of Denali, Genentech, Cajal Neuroscience, and consults for Eli Lilly and Alector. AMF reports that she is a member of the Scientific Advisory Boards for Roche Diagnostics, Genentech and Diadem, and also consults for DiamiR and Siemens Healthcare Diagnostics Inc. BPL reports that he has consulting relationships with Merck and Eli Lilly.
Non-financial disclosure: JMD, CMR, SAM, AMJ, EF, CDT, TR, DF, JCM, SES, AMF, DMH, BPL, and GMB report no non-financial disclosures related to this article.
References
- 1. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Injury Prevention and Control. Web-based Injury Statistics Query and Reporting System (WISQARS). Atlanta, GA: CDC; 2018. http://cdc.gov/injury/wisqars. Accessed October 21, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Tefft B. Rates of motor vehicle crashes, injuries and deaths in relation to driver age, United States, 2014–2015. Washington, DC: AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety; 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 3. Li G, et al. Fragility versus excessive crash involvement as determinants of high death rates per vehicle-mile of travel among older drivers. Accid Anal Prev. 2003;35:227–235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Cook LJ, et al. Motor vehicle crash characteristics and medical outcomes among older drivers in Utah, 1992–1995. Ann Emerg Med. 2000;35:585–591. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Oskutis MQ, et al. Are frailty markers associated with serious thoracic and spinal injuries among motor vehicle crash occupants? J Trauma Acute Care Surg. 2016;81:156–161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Fonda SJ, et al. Changes in driving patterns and worsening depressive symptoms among older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2001;56:S343–S351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Edwards JD, et al. Driving status and three-year mortality among community-dwelling older adults. J Gerontol A Biomed Sci Med Sci. 2009;64:300–305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Adler G, et al. Older adults’ perspectives on driving cessation. J Aging Stud. 2006;20:227–235. [Google Scholar]
- 9. Freeman EE, et al. Driving status and risk of entry into long-term care in older adults. Am J Public Health. 2006;96:1254–1259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Alzheimer’s Association. 2021 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement. 2021;17:327–406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Matthews KA, et al. Racial and ethnic estimates of Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias in the United States (2015–2060) in adults aged > 65 years. Alzheimers Dement. 2019;15:17–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Davis RL, et al. Driving in early-stage Alzheimer’s disease: an integrative review of the literature. Res Gerontol Nurs. 2017;10:86–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Marshall NS, et al. Sleep apnea as an independent risk factor for all-cause mortality: the Busselton Health Study. Sleep. 2008;31:1079–1085. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Charlton KL, et al. Influence of chronic illness on crash involvement of motor vehicle drivers. Monash University Accident Research Centre Reports; 2004:482. [Google Scholar]
- 15. Molnar LJ, et al. Reprint of “driving avoidance by older adults: is it always self-regulation?” Accid Anal Prev. 2013;61:272–280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Molnar LJ, et al. Tactical, strategic, and life-goal self-regulation of driving by older adults: Development and testing of a questionnaire. J Safety Res. 2013;46:107–117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Anstey KJ, et al. Predicting driving cessation over 5 years in older adults: psychological well-being and cognitive competence are stronger predictors than physical health. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2006;54:121–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Jansen WJ, et al. Prevalence of cerebral amyloid pathology in persons without dementia: a meta-analysis. JAMA. 2015;313:1924–1938. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Morris JC, et al. Pittsburgh compound B imaging and prediction of progression from cognitive normality to symptomatic Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2009;66:1469–1475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. JackKnopman CRDS, Jr , et al. Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer’s pathological cascade. Lancet Neurol. 2010;9:119–128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Schönknecht P, et al. Increased tau protein differentiates mild cognitive impairment from geriatric depression and predicts conversion to dementia. Neurosci Lett. 2007;416:39–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Roe CM, et al. Preclinical Alzheimer’s disease and longitudinal driving decline. Alzheimers Dement Transl Res Clin Interv. 2017;3:74–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Roe CM, et al. Amyloid imaging, cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers predict driving performance among cognitively normal individuals. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2017;31:69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Ott BR, et al. Brain amyloid in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease is associated with increased driving risk. Alzheimers Dement Diagn Assess Dis Monit. 2017;6:136–142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Bayat S, et al. GPS driving: a digital biomarker for preclinical Alzheimer disease. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2021;13:1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Bayat S, et al. Identifying preclinical Alzheimer disease from driving patterns: a machine learning approach. In: 2021 Alzheimer’s Association International Conference ALZ; 2021. [Google Scholar]
- 27. Horne JA, et al. Sleep related vehicle accidents. Br Med J. 1995;310:565–567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Philip P, et al. Fatigue, sleep restriction and driving performance. Accid Anal Prev. 2005;37:473–478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Wörle J, et al. Differential effects of driver sleepiness and sleep inertia on driving behavior. Transp Res F: Traffic Psychol Behav. 2021;82:111–120. [Google Scholar]
- 30. Benjafield AV, et al. Estimation of the global prevalence and burden of obstructive sleep apnoea: a literature-based analysis. Lancet Resp Med. 2019;7:687–698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Young T, et al. The occurrence of sleep-disordered breathing among middle-aged adults. N Engl J Med. 1993;328:1230–1235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Lévy P, et al. Obstructive sleep apnoea syndrome. Nat Rev Dis Primers. 2015;1:1–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Bonsignore MR, et al. Sleep apnea, sleepiness, and driving risk. Sleep Med Clin. 2019;14:431–439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Arita A, et al. Risk factors for automobile accidents caused by falling asleep while driving in obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Sleep Breath. 2015;19:1229–1234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Partinen M, et al. Daytime sleepiness and vascular morbidity at seven-year follow-up in obstructive sleep apnea patients. Chest. 1990;97:27–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Vakulin A, et al. Quantitative sleep EEG and polysomnographic predictors of driving simulator performance in obstructive sleep apnea. Clin Neurophysiol. 2016;127:1428–1435. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Bixler EO, et al. Prevalence of sleep-disordered breathing in women: effects of gender. Am J Resp Crit Care Med. 2001;163:608–613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Bixler EO, et al. Effects of age on sleep apnea in men: I. Prevalence and severity. Am J Resp Crit Care Med. 1998;157:144–148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Durán J, et al. Obstructive sleep apnea–hypopnea and related clinical features in a population-based sample of subjects aged 30 to 70 yr. Am J Resp Crit Care Med. 2001;163:685–689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Sleep-disordered breathing in community-dwelling elderly. Sleep. 1991;14:486–495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Yaffe K, et al. Sleep-disordered breathing, hypoxia, and risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in older women. JAMA. 2011;306:613–619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Klauer SG, et al. Comparing real-world behaviors of drivers with high versus low rates of crashes and near crashes tech. rep. 2009.
- 43. Guo F, et al. Near crashes as crash surrogate for naturalistic driving studies. Transp Res Rec. 2010;2147:66–74. [Google Scholar]
- 44. Mukhopadhyay S, et al. A primer on the evolution of aducanumab: the first antibody approved for treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2021; 83(4):1–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Musiek ES, et al. Possible consequences of the approval of a disease-modifying therapy for Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol. 2021;78:141–142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Walia HK, et al. Effect of positive airway pressure therapy on drowsy driving in a large clinic-based obstructive sleep apnea cohort. J Clin Sleep Med. 2019;15:1613–1620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Antic NA, et al. The effect of CPAP in normalizing daytime sleepiness, quality of life, and neurocognitive function in patients with moderate to severe OSA. Sleep. 2011;34:111–119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Troussière A, et al. Treatment of sleep apnoea syndrome decreases cognitive decline in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol, Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2014;85:1405–1408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Morris JC. Clinical dementia rating: a reliable and valid diagnostic and staging measure for dementia of the Alzheimer type. Int Psychogeriatr. 1997;9:173–176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Fagan AM, et al. Inverse relation between in vivo amyloid imaging load and cerebrospinal fluid Aβ42 in humans. Ann Neurol. 2006;59:512–519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Volluz KE, et al. Correspondence of CSF biomarkers measured by lumipulse assays with amyloid PET. In: 2021 Alzheimer’s Association International Conference ALZ; 2021. [Google Scholar]
- 52. Certification, Compliance Division Office of Transportation, Air Quality U.S. Environmental protection agency. On-board diagnostic (OBD) regulations and requirements: questions and answers. Cincinnati, OH: National Service Center for Environmental Publications (NSCEP); 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 53. Babulal GM, et al. A naturalistic study of driving behavior in older adults and preclinical Alzheimer disease: A pilot study. J Appl Gerontol. 2019;38:277–289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Babulal GM, et al. Development and interval testing of a naturalistic driving methodology to evaluate driving behavior in clinical research. F1000Research. 2016;5:1716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Roe CM, et al. Reaction to a pandemic: social distancing and driving among older adults during COVID-19. J Appl Gerontol. 2021;40:263–267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Bayat S, et al. COVID-19 and preclinical Alzheimer disease: driving, mobility, activity and experiences of older adults in the United States. In: 2021 Alzheimer’s Association International Conference ALZ; 2021. [Google Scholar]
- 57. Roe CM, et al. A 2.5-year longitudinal assessment of naturalistic driving in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2019;68:1625–1633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Nilius G, et al. A randomized controlled trial to validate the Alice PDX ambulatory device. Nat Sci Sleep. 2017;9:171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Toedebusch CD, et al. Multi-modal home sleep monitoring in older adults. J Vis Exp. 2019;(143):e58823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Iber C. The AASM manual for the scoring of sleep and associated events: rules, terminology and technical specification: version 2.1. Westchester, IL:American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2014.
- 61. Lucey BP, et al. Comparison of a single-channel EEG sleep study to polysomnography. J Sleep Res. 2016;25:625–635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep. 1991;14:540–545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Aschenbrenner AJ, et al. Influence of tau PET, amyloid PET, and hippocampal volume on cognition in Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2018;91:e859–e866. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Grober E, et al. Screening for dementia by memory testing. Neurology. 1988;38:900–900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Tombaugh TN, et al. Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1999;14:167–177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Armitage SG. An analysis of certain psychological tests used for the evaluation of brain injury. Psychol Monogr. 1946;60:i. [Google Scholar]
- 67. Kraemer HC, et al. Centring in regression analyses: a strategy to prevent errors in statistical inference. Int J Met Psychiatr Res. 2004;13:141–151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Publications; 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 69. Dalal D, et al. Some common myths about centering predictor variables in moderated multiple regression and polynomial regression. Organ Res Met. 2011;15:339–362. [Google Scholar]
- 70. Dawson JF. Moderation in management research: What, why, when, and how. J Bus Psychol. 2014;29:1–19. [Google Scholar]
- 71. Alkan U, et al. Effects of surgery for obstructive sleep apnea on cognitive function and driving performance. Sleep Breath. 2021; 25(3):1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Trost M, et al. Assessment of ability to drive in obstructive sleep apnea patients using a driving simulator. Somnologie. 2019;23:36–42. [Google Scholar]
- 73. Vakulin A, et al. Brain mitochondrial dysfunction and driving simulator performance in untreated obstructive sleep apnea. J Sleep Res. 2021;3482:e1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Babulal GM, et al. Creating a driving profile for older adults using GPS devices and naturalistic driving methodology. F1000Research. 2016;5:2376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Lucey BP, et al. Amyloid-beta diurnal pattern: possible role of sleep in Alzheimer’s disease pathogenesis. Neurobiol Aging. 2014;35:S29–S34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Lucey BP. It’s complicated: the relationship between sleep and Alzheimer’s disease in humans. Neurobiol Dis. 2020;144:105031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Sleep Medicine Adult Obstructive Sleep Apnea Task Force. Clinical guideline for the evaluation, management and long-term care of obstructive sleep apnea in adults. J Clin Sleep Med. 2009;5:263–276. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]