Skip to main content
Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY) logoLink to Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY)
. 2021 Nov 3;32(12):2611–2620. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab368

Adolescent Binge Drinking Is Associated With Accelerated Decline of Gray Matter Volume

M A Infante 1, S C Eberson 2, Y Zhang 3,4, T Brumback 5, S A Brown 6,7, I M Colrain 8, F C Baker 9, D B Clark 10, M D De Bellis 11, D Goldston 12, B J Nagel 13, K B Nooner 14, Q Zhao 15, K M Pohl 16,17, E V Sullivan 18, A Pfefferbaum 19,20, S F Tapert 21, W K Thompson 22,23,24,
PMCID: PMC9201595  PMID: 34729592

Abstract

The age- and time-dependent effects of binge drinking on adolescent brain development have not been well characterized even though binge drinking is a health crisis among adolescents. The impact of binge drinking on gray matter volume (GMV) development was examined using 5 waves of longitudinal data from the National Consortium on Alcohol and NeuroDevelopment in Adolescence study. Binge drinkers (n = 166) were compared with non-binge drinkers (n = 82 after matching on potential confounders). Number of binge drinking episodes in the past year was linked to decreased GMVs in bilateral Desikan–Killiany cortical parcellations (26 of 34 with P < 0.05/34) with the strongest effects observed in frontal regions. Interactions of binge drinking episodes and baseline age demonstrated stronger effects in younger participants. Statistical models sensitive to number of binge episodes and their temporal proximity to brain volumes provided the best fits. Consistent with prior research, results of this study highlight the negative effects of binge drinking on the developing brain. Our results present novel findings that cortical GMV decreases were greater in closer proximity to binge drinking episodes in a dose–response manner. This relation suggests a causal effect and raises the possibility that normal growth trajectories may be reinstated with alcohol abstinence.

Keywords: adolescence, alcohol, binge drinking, brain development, cortical volume

Introduction

Adolescence is a critical developmental stage when experimentation with alcohol and other substances is often initiated. Alcohol remains the most widely used substance during adolescence with 19%, 38%, and 52% reporting past-year alcohol use by 8th, 10th, and 12th grade, respectively (Johnston et al. 2020). Although adolescents generally consume alcohol with less frequency than adults, among those who report drinking, the majority engages in binge drinking (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration 2020), defined by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (2004) as a pattern of drinking alcohol that brings the blood alcohol concentration to 0.08 g/dL or higher, which typically corresponds to consuming 5 or more drinks for men and 4 or more drinks for women within a 2-h period. Findings from the most recent Monitoring the Future survey show 14% of adolescents in the 12th grade had engaged in binge drinking in the past 2 weeks, and data from the 2019 National Survey on Drug Use and Health show 5% of adolescents aged 12–17 had engaged in past month binge drinking. Binge drinking has been identified as particularly risky for adolescents, with far more severe consequences compared with adults, including deviation from normal brain development (Zhao et al. 2021), structural and functional brain alterations (Jones et al. 2018), neuropsychological deficits (Carbia et al. 2018), and increased risk for developing alcohol as well as other substance use disorders (Chassin et al. 2002). The burden of excessive alcohol use alone on US society is estimated to be over 223 billion dollars per year (Bouchery et al. 2011).

During adolescence, the brain goes through significant maturational and organizational changes (Mills et al. 2016; Pfefferbaum et al. 2016; Tamnes et al. 2017). Structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies have shown region-specific variation of cortical development from childhood through early adulthood, with sensorimotor and occipital brain regions maturing first, followed by limbic regions important for rewards and emotion, and higher order cortical association areas, including the prefrontal cortex maturing later in development (Gogtay et al. 2004; Khundrakpam et al. 2013; Krongold et al. 2017; Sotiras et al. 2017; Nadig et al. 2021). The asynchronous development of brain regions, particularly the difference in timing of the development of the reward and control systems, may explain why adolescents are prone to risk-taking behaviors (Crews et al. 2007; Casey et al. 2008). At the same time, given the extensive nature of neurodevelopment during this period, the adolescent brain may be especially vulnerable to intoxicants such as alcohol.

Findings from cross-sectional studies have shown gray matter differences in binge drinking college-age adolescents compared with their nondrinking peers, including smaller volumes in the frontal lobe (Kvamme et al. 2016) and cerebellum (Lisdahl et al. 2013). By contrast, others have found larger cortical volumes in college-age binge drinkers compared with nondrinkers, including regions of the prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate (Doallo et al. 2014), and ventral striatum (Howell et al. 2013). Greater gray matter density in the left middle frontal gyrus among college-age binge drinkers compared with nondrinkers was also reported in a more recent study (Sousa et al. 2017). Sex differences in the effect of binge drinking on brain structure have also been documented, perhaps related to sex-related differences in genetic vulnerabilities or neurotoxic sensitivities (Kvamme et al. 2016).

Longitudinal studies suggest that accelerated decline in cortical volumes may result from heavy alcohol use during adolescence (Squeglia et al. 2014; Squeglia et al. 2015; Meda et al. 2017; Pfefferbaum et al. 2018). A study by Squeglia et al. (2014) examined brain development before and after initiation of heavy drinking, showing alcohol-associated reduction in subcortical regions as well as inferior and middle temporal structures. In another study (Meda et al. 2017), participants were categorized as heavy or light drinkers based on number of binge drinking episodes in the previous 26 weeks, with subjects in the heavy binge drinking group showing an increased rate of gray matter decline, primarily in fronto-striatal regions. Using 3 waves of data from the National Consortium on Alcohol and NeuroDevelopment in Adolescence (NCANDA), Pfefferbaum et al. (2018) examined the impact of adolescent heavy drinking on brain trajectories of gray and white matter volumes. That study compared brain trajectories of those who at the 2-year follow-up remained no/low drinkers versus those who had initiated moderate or heavy drinking by this time point. Results indicated smaller volumes of frontal, cingulate, and total gray matter in heavy drinkers compared with those in the no/low drinking group. As in Squeglia et al. (2014), youth were classified based moderate or heavy drinking pattern as opposed to binge drinking status. Understanding the impact of binge drinking on adolescent brain development of specific brain regions has important implications given the association between neurobiological changes at the structural and functional level to cognitive and behavioral changes (Jones et al. 2018).

To date, the age- and time-dependent effects of binge drinking on adolescent brain development have not been well characterized. Large-scale longitudinal studies with several years of data collection are needed to explicate the effects of binge drinking, including its proximal and longer-term effects on the brain. To begin to address these issues, the present study expands on the Pfefferbaum et al. (2018) analysis in 3 ways. First, we assessed the relationship of adolescent binge drinking with gray matter volume (GMV) trajectories across 5 extant waves of longitudinal data from the NCANDA study, encompassing a longer time period, broader age ranges, and a larger variation in drinking behaviors than in prior reports using NCANDA data. Second, we leveraged the NCANDA cohort sequential design to assess whether strength of any associations of binge drinking with GMV trajectories varied by baseline age. Third, we examined whether models that are sensitive to number of binge drinking episodes (“dose”) and temporal proximity of binge drinking to outcomes would explain more variation than models that incorporate binge drinking in other ways (as discrete indicators and/or as cumulative measures). Based on previous findings, we predicted that binge drinking would lead to a steeper decline in GMV, particularly in frontal and cingulate regions than would be detected in no-to-low drinkers. NCANDA’s large sample size, number of longitudinal assessments, and cohort sequential design thus allow for an unprecedented investigation into the nature of the association of adolescent alcohol use and subsequent trajectories of cortical GMV development across adolescence and into young adulthood.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Data came from the NCANDA. The NCANDA cohort consists of 831 12–21 year-olds enrolled across 5 sites (Duke University, University of Pittsburgh Medical Center [UPMC], Oregon Health & Science University [OHSU], University of California San Diego [UCSD], and SRI International) and followed for 5 years (Brown et al. 2015). Participants were recruited through local schools and targeted catchment-area calling. The study follows a cohort sequential design, which recruited youth in 3 age bands (12–14, 15–17, and 18–21 years), enabling examination of a broad developmental window due to between-subject variation in baseline age (“age cohort”). Prior to study entry, most of the sample had not engaged in binge drinking (n = 121). Youth at risk for heavy drinking (i.e., early experimentation with alcohol, family history of substance use disorder, externalizing or internalizing symptoms) were overrecruited and comprised 50% of the baseline sample.

Exclusion criteria included lack of English fluency, MRI contraindications, serious medical conditions (e.g., traumatic brain injury [TBI] with loss of consciousness >30 min), noncorrectable sensory impairments, current serious Axis I psychiatric disorder that may influence study completion (e.g., psychosis), and early developmental problems (e.g., known exposure to prenatal alcohol or other drugs) (Brown et al. 2015). The research protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Boards at each site. At each visit, participants (or the parent or legal guardian for minors) provided written informed consent, and written informed assent was obtained from minors. Participants and their parents were compensated for participation.

After informed consent, at baseline and each follow-up year participants completed a comprehensive assessment of substance use, psychiatric symptoms, functioning in major life domains, neuropsychological testing, and neuroimaging (Brown et al. 2015). This included the Customary Drinking and Drug use Record (Brown et al. 1998) to characterize past and current alcohol and other substance use, reported on past-year use frequency, maximum number of drinks in a drinking episode, and number of binge drinking episodes (i.e., 5 or more drinks for males or 4 or more drinks for females on an occasion). The number of binge drinking episodes in the prior year at each annual follow-up assessment was the primary (longitudinal) independent variable of interest.

Participants who reported having had at least one lifetime binge episode at baseline (n = 121) were excluded from analyses. Because the focus of this paper is longer-term temporal variation in binge drinking, we also excluded participants who had not completed all 5 (baseline and 4 annual follow-up) visits to allow sufficient within-person assessments of binge drinking to characterize its association with concurrent and subsequent GMVs. (Note, we performed sensitivity analyses of the impact of this and other design decisions on resulting associations.) This resulted in a final pool of n = 344 subjects for analyses. Of these, n = 178 (52%) reported never having had a binge drink episode over the 5 visits and n = 166 (48%) reported binge drinking at least once during the study period subsequent to baseline. Participant characteristics are reported in Table 1.

Table 1.

Participant characteristics (% or mean) for subjects with at least one binge drinking episode during the study period (binge drinkers) versus those with no binge drinking episodes (controls) before and after matching on baseline covariates

Sex (% male) Binge drinkers (n = 166) Before matching controls (n = 178) Post-matching controls (n = 82)*
53.0 49.4 54.9
Baseline visit age in years (SD) 16.5 (1.9) 15.0 (2.4) 15.9 (2.4)
Site
 Site A (%) 7.8 12.9 14.6
 Site B (%) 10.2 7.9 8.5
 Site C (%) 14.5 22.5 13.4
 Site D (%) 24.7 21.9 28.0
 Site E (%) 42.8 34.8 35.4
Race
 Asian (%) 7.8 5.6 9.8
 Caucasian (%) 80.7 70.0 81.7
 African-American 6.6 18.0 1.2
 Other (%) 4.8 6.7 7.3
Socioeconomic status+ (SD) 17.0 (2.3) 16.2 (2.7) 16.9 (2.2)
Baseline ICV++ (SD) 0.1 (1.0) −0.2 (1.0) 0.1 (0.9)

*Matching done with replacement. Number of matches of each non-binge drinking subject with a binge drinking subject: 0: n = 96; 1: n = 39; 2: n = 23; 3: n = 8; 4: n = 7; 5: n = 2; 6: n = 2; 7: n = 1. Note: multiply matched participants only appear once in this table and in analyses. No differences are significant post-matching.

+Highest level of education achieved by either parent at baseline.

++Total ICV standardized to have zero mean and unit variance.

Binge drinkers (participants with a binge drinking episode at any of the visits) were then matched with non-binge drinkers (subjects with no binge drinking episodes at any visit) on potential confounders, including sex, age, race/ethnicity, highest level of education completed by either parent, site, and baseline (pre-binge drinking) intracranial volume (ICV). Matching was performed via a genetic search algorithm, implemented in the R package Matching using the Match function (Sekhon 2011; Sekhon and Grieve 2012; Diamond and Sekhon 2013). Because the pool of non-binge drinkers to select from was only slightly larger than the number of binge drinkers, we performed matching with replacement to improve the post-match balance on these potential confounders, although each matched subject appears only once in analyses even if they were selected in more than one match. Table 1 displays the pre- and post-match summaries of potential confounders of binge and non-binge drinkers, generally demonstrating improved balance for potential confounders. We report the number of times controls appeared in matches in Table 1. All matching variables were also included in subsequent analyses to control for any residual imbalances.

MRI Data Acquisition and Analysis

A high-resolution structural MRI protocol and the use of 3 T scanners were consistent across sites. Three sites (UCSD, SRI, and Duke University) used GE MR750 and 2 sites (UPMC and OHSU) used Siemens TIM-Trio scanners. T1-weighted and T2-weighted images were acquired in the sagittal plane. NCANDA used the same acquisition protocols for the Siemens and the same for GE sites (see (Pfefferbaum et al. 2016; Pfefferbaum et al. 2018) for details). The resulting structural MRIs were processed by the same pipeline (Pfefferbaum et al. 2018) resulting in image inhomogeneity corrected, skull stripped T1-weighted MRI and corresponding ICV defined according the SRI24 atlas (Rohlfing et al. 2010; Rohlfing et al. 2014). The skull stripped images were first processed via cross-sectional FreeSurfer (Fischl 2012) followed by the longitudinal stream (Reuter et al. 2012) to the estimate the GMVs of cortical regions of interest (ROIs) defined according to the Desikan–Killiany atlas (Desikan et al. 2006). For each of the 34 ROIs, the volumes (expressed in mm3) were combined across hemispheres to produce one value. We initially performed sensitivity analyses to assess left and right hemisphere volumes separately for each of the 34 R0Is (i.e., 68 regions total) and found little difference between left and right hemispheres. We thus decided to present results of the bilaterally averaged results here.

Statistical Analyses

Primary analyses consisted of 34 separate linear mixed-effects models (LMEs), one for each bilaterally averaged Desikan–Killiany GMV ROI as the dependent variable, each standardized to have zero mean and unit standard deviation (SD). The primary independent variable of interest was “number of binge drinking episodes in the prior year” (bingeij) for the ith subject at the jth visit (j = 1, …, 5). Because of the long tail in number of episodes (Fig. 1), bingeij was log transformed (after adding one to avoid taking the log of zero) and then mean centered at zero. Age was entered into the LME as 2 terms to reflect the cohort sequential aspect of the NCANDA study design. Between-subject variation in ages was captured by the baseline age of the subject across the 5 visits (age0i). Within-subject change in age (aged,ij) was captured by subtracting age0i from the ith subject’s age at visit j. Each of these terms (bingeij, age0i, and aged,ij) was entered into LMEs along with their 2-way interactions. Random intercepts were included for subject and for family. Variables used in matching were also included as fixed effects to control for any residual imbalances after matching. We assessed the significance of binge drinking on GMVs by fitting the same model without bingeij or its interactions and using a Likelihood Ratio Test (LRT). All reported LRT P values were 2-sided and Bonferroni-corrected for 34 comparisons (P = 0.05/34 = 0.0015).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Number of binge drinking episodes in the prior year by age (N = 242).

We next performed a second set of analyses by comparing model fits of coding binge drinking in several ways to examine the degree to which temporal proximity or “dose” (number of binge drinking episodes) was relevant for predicting differences in GMVs between binge drinkers and non-binge drinkers. Coding binge drinking episodes as an indicator (yes/no) removes any information about dose, whereas coding binge drinking episodes as a cumulative measure removes information about temporal proximity. Specifically, we fit these models: M0) a base model without any binge drinking variable; M1) number of binge drinking episodes in the prior year (bingeij,), identical to the primary analyses, which preserves temporal proximity and dose information; M2) cumulative number of binge drinking episodes (binge_cumij), equal to the log-transformed sum of the number of current and past binge drinking episodes; M3) binge_catij, equal to one if the participant had one or more binge drinking episodes in the last year and zero otherwise; M4) current or past binge drinking (binge_pastij), equal to one if the participant had a binge drinking episodes at the current visit or at any past visit and zero otherwise; M5) ever binge drank (binge_everi) equal to one if the participant had at least one binge drink in any visit (including future visits) and zero otherwise; and M6) log-transformed total number of binge drinking episodes across all 5 visits (binge_toti). These different ways of coding binge drinking were entered into separate LMEs (7 coding × 34 volumes = 238 total models) for each of the GMV’s as described above for bingeij and the fits were compared using the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC, (Akaike 1974). All statistical analyses were conducted using R Version 3.6.3. (R Core Team 2020).

Results

Demographic characteristics of the sample at baseline (N = 344) by binge drinking status are presented in Table 1. After matching with replacement, the final sample consisted of 242 subjects: 166 binge drinkers with mean (SD) age 16.5 (1.9) years, 53% male, and 82 non-binge drinkers with mean age 15.9 (2.4) years, 54% male. Table 2 gives the distribution of binge drinking by age cohort for each yearly follow-up assessment. Figure 1 displays a spaghetti plot of binge drinking trajectories across all 5 yearly assessments. Information on participants’ cannabis and tobacco use can be found in Supplementary Table 1.

Table 2.

Mean number (range, SD) of binge drinking episodes in past year by post-baseline follow-up year (Year 1 to Year 4 follow-up) by age cohort for N = 166 participants with at least one binge drinking episode over the NCANDA study period to date

Baseline age cohort (years) Mean number of binge drinking episode in the past year (range, SD)
Year 1 Year 2 Year 3 Year 4
Cohort 1 (12–14) 1.4 ([0,25], 4.7) 2.3 ([0,32], 6.3) 2.9 ([0,40], 7.1) 8.6 ([0,102], 16.9)
Cohort 2 (15–17) 0.7 ([0,12], 1.7) 4.4 ([0,45], 8.6) 7.5 ([0,60], 12.2) 15.5 ([0,114], 23.7)
Cohort 3 (18–21) 3.7 ([0,20], 6.2) 5.9 ([0,52], 10.6) 12.7 ([0,144], 27.2) 18.1 ([0,170], 36.4)

In the first set of 34 LMEs, the LRTs of models with bingeij versus models without binge, 26 were significant after a Bonferroni correction. LME fixed-effect regression coefficient estimates for the main parameters of interest (along with their Wald test P values and 95% confidence intervals) as well as the overall model LRT statistics, and P values are presented in Table 3. Fixed effects including all covariates are presented in Supplementary Tables 26.

Table 3.

LME coefficients of interest and P values for GMV in ROIs

Desikan region LRT* (P value) log(binge drinking episodes in past year + 1) log(binge drinking episodes in past year+1) × baseline age
β P value β P value
Frontal lobe
Superior frontal 59.2 (<0.001) −0.05 <0.001 0.01 <0.001
Rostral middle frontal 36.3 (<0.001) −0.05 <0.001 0.01 <0.001
Caudal middle frontal 70.5 (<0.001) −0.05 <0.001 0.02 <0.001
Pars opercularis 46.5 (<0.001) −0.03 <0.001 0.01 <0.001
Pars triangularis 33.5 (<0.001) −0.04 <0.001 0.01 0.002
Pars orbitalis 37.6 (<0.001) −0.05 <0.001 0.01 0.002
Lateral orbitofrontal 19.5 (<0.001) −0.04 <0.001 0.01 0.006
Medial orbitofrontal 8.7 (0.034) −0.03 0.006 0.01 0.129
Precentral 45.8 (<0.001) −0.03 <0.001 0.02 <0.001
Paracentral 31.1 (<0.001) −0.03 <0.001 0.01 <0.001
Frontal pole 18.6 (<0.001) −0.06 <0.001 0.01 0.076
Parietal lobe
Superior parietal 26.4 (<0.001) −0.03 0.002 0.01 <0.001
Inferior parietal 30.0 (<0.001) −0.04 <0.001 0.01 <0.001
Supramarginal 47.2 (<0.001) −0.05 <0.001 0.01 <0.001
Postcentral 29.2 (<0.001) −0.03 0.004 0.01 <0.001
Precuneus 24.6 (<0.001) −0.03 0.001 0.01 0.002
Temporal lobe
Superior temporal 38.6 (<0.001) −0.04 <0.001 0.01 <0.001
Middle temporal 36.1 (<0.001) −0.06 <0.001 0.01 0.007
Inferior temporal 37.0 (<0.001) −0.05 <0.001 0.01 0.005
Banks of the superior temporal sulcus 26.9 (<0.001) −0.03 <0.001 0.01 0.002
Fusiform 30.8 (<0.001) −0.03 <0.001 0.01 0.001
Transverse temporal 12.2 (0.007) −0.02 0.044 0.01 0.019
Entorhinal 7.6 (0.056) −0.02 0.029 0.00 0.341
Temporal pole 14.1 (0.003) −0.04 0.003 0.01 0.112
Parahippocampal 10.7 (0.014) −0.01 0.0448 0.00 0.127
Occipital lobe
Lateral occipital 27.6 (<0.001) −0.04 <0.001 0.01 0.002
Lingual 23.2 (<0.001) −0.03 <0.001 0.01 0.013
Cuneus 21.8 (<0.001) −0.02 0.003 0.01 0.004
Pericalcarine 19.1 (<0.001) 0.01 0.174 0.01 0.019
Cingulate
Rostral anterior cingulate 17.7 (0.001) −0.02 0.042 0.01 0.003
Caudal anterior cingulate 5.8 (0.124) −0.00 0.754 0.00 0.313
Posterior cingulate 8.5 (0.036) −0.01 0.256 0.00 0.479
Isthmus 6.7 (0.086) −0.01 0.062 0.00 0.146
Insula 17.4 (0.001) −0.02 0.002 0.00 0.110

*LRT is for full model with respect to a base model with the same covariates but without binge drinking or its interactions with baseline age or change in age. Full model results including covariates are included in Supplementary Tables 26.

Cells shaded in gray shows significance after Bonferroni correction (P = 0.05/34 = 0.0015).

Spaghetti plots of trajectories and model fits for superior frontal GMV are displayed in Figure 2 (fits for the all Desikan ROIs, qualitatively similar, are shown in the Supplementary Figures 15). In all models, GMVs were smaller as a function of both, age0i and aged,ij. In 33 of 34 LMEs, the main effects of bingeij were negative (20 with P < 0.05/34, with the remaining 14 not significantly different from zero). The 2-way interactions of bingeij × agem,i were positive for all models (with 12 of 34 having P values < 0.05/34), indicating attenuated association of volumes with number of binge drinking episodes in older subjects. Subsequent models including the 3-way interaction of age0i and aged,ij and bingeij (not shown) did not significantly improve fits for any of the 34 regions. Sensitivity analyses examining left and right Desikan ROIs separately (also not shown) revealed no systematic differences between hemispheres.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Left panel: Caudal middle frontal GMV trajectories for non-binge drinkers (black) and binge drinkers (red). Plots for all 34 bilaterally averaged Desikan regions, qualitatively similar, are displayed in Supplementary Figures 15. Heavy lines are predicted mean trajectories from LMEs for non-binge drinkers (heavy black line) and binge drinkers (heavy red line) for the 3 age cohorts (12–14 years, 15–17 years, 18–21 years at baseline). Note: baseline age was entered as a continuous variable in LMEs, the division into age cohorts is for display purposes only. Right panel: -loge(P value) from LRT for 34 bilaterally averaged Desikan regions outlined in black. The color coding indicates regions showing significant GMV declines related to binge metrics, where brighter yellow indicates stronger effects.

We examined the potential differential effects of binge drinking for males and females by including an interaction of sex with bingeij and testing these versus models with no interaction using LRTs. None of the interactions were significant (unadjusted Ps = 0.04–0.97). We also examined whether TBIs, childhood trauma (Adverse Childhood Experiences Scale [ACES]), or psychopathology (Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment [ASEBA]) accounted for differences in binge versus non-binge drinkers in GMVs. When rerunning base models including these 3 covariates and testing the addition of binge drinking (bingeij), LRTs gave identical results (26 of 34 Desikan GMVs significant after Bonferroni correction). Distributions of TBIs, ASEBA, and ACES are given in the Supplementary Tables. Finally, sensitivity analyses of the full sample (non-binge drinking at baseline but no exclusion for not completing all 5 visits and no matching but including the matching variables as covariates, n = 710) were consistent with the results based on the participants with all 5 visits.

The AIC results of the second set of LMEs are presented in Table 4. For each set of 6 models including binge drinking (M1–M6), the AIC for the base model M0 was subtracted to highlight the improvement of model fit of including binge drinking from the model with no binge drinking term: lower (here, more negative) AIC indicates better model fit than that for M0. The temporally proximal dimensional model (M1) was the best fitting for 26 of 34 regions. Of the remaining regions, 6 had M2 as the best-fitting model (though AICs for these models were close to that of M1). For the remaining 2, M3 was the best-fitting model. Besides M0, the worst-fitting models were thus M4–M6. In summary, including both “dose” and temporal proximity were conjointly informative and strongly improved upon fits of models ignoring this information for most outcomes.

Table 4.

AIC of 6 model comparisons of coding binge drinking

Desikan region (volume) Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6
Frontal lobe
Superior frontal −53.57 −53.12 −31.06 −30.04 −10.85 −11.47
Rostral middle frontal −30.97 −29.20 −19.90 −18.79 −5.44 −4.33
Caudal middle frontal −64.96 −59.07 −35.32 −36.69 −14.16 −14.99
Pars opercularis −40.80 −38.33 −23.82 −20.65 −3.19 −3.77
Pars triangularis −27.59 −24.71 −10.92 −10.33 −1.51 −2.28
Pars orbitalis −32.40 −31.53 −19.22 −16.89 −11.08 −12.48
Lateral orbitofrontal −14.14 −11.57 −8.91 −4.75 −2.08 −0.30
Medial orbitofrontal −2.19 −3.75 0.81 0.97 2.44 3.03
Precentral −40.53 −42.86 −26.04 −23.27 −3.83 −2.42
Paracentral −25.30 −23.97 −15.49 −13.16 −1.24 −4.85
Frontal pole −12.95 −14.60 −3.79 −2.96 2.98 0.99
Parietal lobe
Superior parietal −20.52 −16.50 −8.98 −7.96 −1.94 −2.73
Inferior parietal −24.08 −19.06 −14.59 −10.96 −2.85 −3.83
Supramarginal −40.67 −32.90 −21.53 −18.93 −3.76 −5.90
Postcentral −22.53 −18.73 −13.02 −10.46 −2.06 −1.45
Precuneus −18.45 −16.14 −11.09 −9.03 −1.47 −1.58
Temporal lobe
Superior temporal −32.41 −29.92 −16.55 −14.72 −4.31 −3.42
Middle temporal −29.63 −21.62 −14.12 −11.39 −2.94 −1.15
Inferior temporal −30.99 −26.99 −13.70 −10.08 0.42 −0.45
Banks of the superior temporal sulcus −20.96 −16.18 −17.17 −16.33 −3.57 0.59
Fusiform −24.80 −25.03 −10.59 −9.15 0.32 −1.47
Transverse temporal −5.89 −7.79 −5.18 −7.61 2.63 2.58
Entorhinal −1.69 −0.43 −1.24 1.32 2.80 1.69
Temporal pole −8.22 −4.51 −0.76 2.91 3.81 3.81
Parahippocampal −4.78 −7.40 −4.25 −3.52 3.79 3.22
Occipital lobe
Lateral occipital −21.34 −17.63 −7.72 −4.83 −2.57 −4.99
Lingual −17.30 −15.26 −7.36 −5.55 −7.22 −5.95
Cuneus −64.96 −11.33 −4.66 −4.06 −6.19 −9.20
Pericalcarine −13.14 −9.15 −4.36 −6.17 −3.35 −1.27
Cingulate
Rostral anterior cingulate −11.44 −8.70 −2.38 −0.58 0.42 −1.40
Caudal anterior cingulate 0.60 0.40 0.40 −0.03 4.20 5.70
Posterior cingulate −3.01 −3.09 −3.62 −1.13 1.38 0.46
Isthmus −0.80 −0.50 1.17 2.93 0.75 2.31
Insula −11.42 −10.01 −10.58 −4.86 −3.20 −5.74

Model 1: log of number of binge drinking episodes in the prior year (df = 20).

Model 2: log of cumulative number of binge drinking episodes to current year (df = 20).

Model 3: equal to one if any binge drinking episodes in past year and zero otherwise (df = 20).

Model 4: equal to one if any binge drinking episodes at current or any past visit and zero otherwise (df = 20).

Model 5: equal to one if any binge drinking episodes ever (including future visits) and zero otherwise (df = 20).

Model 6: log of total number of binge drinking episodes across all 5 visits (df = 20).

All AICs are with respect to Model 0 with no binge drinking variable included (df = 17).

Cells shaded in gray are the lowest AICs for that particular region.

Discussion

The goal of this study was to build on existing research to advance our understanding of the age- and time-dependent effects of binge drinking on adolescent brain development. Using 5 waves of longitudinal data from the NCANDA study with its cohort-sequential design, we examined the extent to which binge drinking in adolescence disturbs the development of regional GMVs. We specifically focused on binge drinking given the high rates of binge drinking among adolescents (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration 2020), preliminary evidence for the negative effects of binge drinking on the adolescent brain (Jones et al. 2018), and the limited longitudinal neuroimaging studies in this area. We found that binge drinking was associated with decrease GMVs relative to non-binge drinkers. Specifically, our results revealed greater number of binge drinking episodes in the prior year was associated with decline in 26 of 34 regions, with the strongest effects in the frontal lobe. Contrary to our hypothesis, after correction for multiple comparisons, we did not find significant effects of binge drinking in cingulate regions. Differences in methodological approaches including the careful matching procedure used in the current study and measure of binge drinking as a continuous variable might account for some of the observed differences. Further, we found the association between GMVs and binge drinking was attenuated among older participants. Finally, findings from our comparative analyses revealed that models coding binge drinking as a dimensional, temporally proximal variable better fit the data versus models ignoring this information. Taken together, these results provide evidence for a dose–response relationship of binge drinking with greater reduction in GMVs in younger than older youth and evidence for time-dependent associations, with temporally proximal number of binge drinks more predictive of reduced gray matter than cumulative or total lifetime binge drinks.

Our results showing decreased GMVs concurrent with binge drinking episodes are consistent with prior longitudinal studies on moderate to heavy drinking youth (Squeglia et al. 2014; Squeglia et al. 2015; Meda et al. 2017; Pfefferbaum et al. 2018). The association of binge drinking with smaller GMVs may be mediated by neurotoxic effects related to accelerated gray matter pruning, as suggested by Pfefferbaum et al. (2018). Alternatively, preexisting differences may account for the association of brain developmental trajectories and binge drinking. In particular, preexisting vulnerabilities in the inhibitory control system have been associated with alcohol initiation and other risk-taking behaviors among adolescents (Norman et al. 2011; Whelan et al. 2012; Wetherill et al. 2013; Cheetham et al. 2014; Heitzeg et al. 2014). To minimize the potential effect of preexisting vulnerabilities, binge and non-binge drinker participants in the current study were matched on various sociodemographic variables and total brain volume. Moreover, that temporal proximity of binge drinking improved model performance suggests the observed associations are not purely due to preexisting differences. Evidence from neuropsychological studies examining the effects of binge drinking provides support to the potential negative implications of the accelerated declines in GMV observed in this study and a potential for associated cognitive sequelae (Carbia et al. 2018; Lees et al. 2019). We found the strongest associations between effects of binge drinking and accelerated decline of GMV in frontal-executive control regions and in temporal lobe regions involved in memory processes. In line with our findings, deficits in various executive functioning domains have been observed among binge drinking adolescents and young adults (Goudriaan et al. 2007; Parada et al. 2012; Mota et al. 2013; Gil-Hernandez and Garcia-Moreno 2016; Gil-Hernandez et al. 2017). Similarly, studies have found binge drinking to be associated with memory deficits (Parada et al. 2011; Nguyen-Louie et al. 2016; Carbia et al. 2017).

Although the neurotoxic effects of alcohol on the developing brain have been widely reported, our study furthered prior findings by identifying a differential association of GMV with respect to age cohort. While an accelerated decline in GMVs among binge drinkers was observed across all age cohorts in contrast to their non-binge drinking peers, we found the association with binge drinking was accelerated in younger drinkers and attenuated in older youth. This attenuation of effects in older participants can be clearly observed in the model fits presented in Figure 2. Our results reinforce the increase risk of negative outcomes associated with early initiation of alcohol and in particular binge drinking.

This study has several strengths, including 5 yearly longitudinal assessments in a cohort sequential design, enabling ascertainment of age cohort and temporal and dose associations between binge drinking and brain development. Moreover, the large sample size allowed for matching on key sociodemographic factors and baseline (pre-binge drinking) total brain volume while maintaining adequate statistical power to estimate effects and test for associations.

Despite these strengths, our findings should be interpreted in light of limitations. Our study did not examine behavioral data to capture the longitudinal association between binge drinking episodes and cognition, nor how any effects of binge drinking on the brain may mediate this relationship. Additionally, as with all observational studies, there may be confounders we did not include in the analyses that bias observed associations between binge drinking and gray matter trajectories, including unobserved differences in confounding effects across age cohorts.

Site differences in GMVs were not completely accounted for by controlling for age, sex, parental education, and race. We thus included site in models along with these sociodemographic factors. Part of these site differences may have been due to scanner manufacturer (site A and D had a Siemens scanner and the others have a GE scanner), with Siemens scanners having consistently lower GMV estimates. Note, since each site has one scanner, controlling for site effects in analyses simultaneously controls for scanner effects.

Finally, there was insufficient within-subject variation among binge drinkers (e.g., onset of binge drinking followed by cessation) to enable direct assessment of the impact of abstinence on the potential for recovery of GMVs. However, the impact of abstinence on developmental trajectories will likely become estimable as the NCANDA study collects more annual follow-up assessments.

In summary, we found that binge drinking during adolescence resulted in accelerated decrease in GMVs above and beyond the expected decreases from the normal maturational process in youth. This decrease was greater in younger bingers and largely attenuated in later adolescence. Further, our novel findings that cortical GMV decreases were greater in closer proximity to reported drinking episodes in a dose–response manner suggests a causal effect and raises the possibility that normal growth trajectories may be reinstated with alcohol abstinence. Given the implications of our findings, future studies with additional time points and increased temporal variation in binge drinking patterns (including prolonged abstinence) will be needed to examine any potential recovery effects.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary_Figure_1-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Figure_2-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Figure_3-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Figure_4-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Figure_5-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Table_1_su-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Table_2_Frontal-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Table_3_Parietal-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Table_4_Temporal-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Table_5_Occipital-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Table_6_Cingulate_and_insula-rev_bhab368

Contributor Information

M A Infante, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA.

S C Eberson, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA.

Y Zhang, Division of Biostatistics, Herbert Wertheim School of Public Health and Human Longevity Science, University of California, San Diego, USA; Population Neuroscience and Genetics Lab, University of California, San Diego, USA.

T Brumback, Department of Psychological Science, Northern Kentucky University, Kentucky, USA.

S A Brown, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA; Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA.

I M Colrain, Center for Health Sciences, SRI International, Menlo Park, CA, USA.

F C Baker, Center for Health Sciences, SRI International, Menlo Park, CA, USA.

D B Clark, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA.

M D De Bellis, Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA.

D Goldston, Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA.

B J Nagel, Departments of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, Oregon Health & Sciences University, Portland, OR, USA.

K B Nooner, Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina Wilmington, Wilmington, NC, USA.

Q Zhao, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA.

K M Pohl, Center for Health Sciences, SRI International, Menlo Park, CA, USA; Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA.

E V Sullivan, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA.

A Pfefferbaum, Center for Health Sciences, SRI International, Menlo Park, CA, USA; Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA.

S F Tapert, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA.

W K Thompson, Division of Biostatistics, Herbert Wertheim School of Public Health and Human Longevity Science, University of California, San Diego, USA; Population Neuroscience and Genetics Lab, University of California, San Diego, USA; Department of Radiology, University of California, San Diego, USA.

Funding

U.S. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism of the National Institutes of Health and National Institute on Drug Abuse, the National Institute of Mental Health, and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (grants: U24 AA021697 to A.P. and K.M.P., U24 AA021695 to S.A.B and S.F.T., U01 AA021692 to S.F.T., U01 AA021696 to F.C.B. and M.dZ., U01 AA021681 to D.G., U01 AA021690 to D.B.C., U01 AA021691 to B.J.N.).

Notes

Data were collected from January 13, 2013 to January 15, 2019 study and were based on the formal locked data releases NCANDA_PUBLIC_4Y_REDCAP_V02* and NCANDA_PUBLIC_4Y_STRUCTURAL_V01**, which are distributed to the public according to the NCANDA Data Distribution agreement***.

* Pohl KM, Sullivan EV, Podhajsky S, Baker FC, Brown SA, Clark DB, Colrain IM, Goldston D, Nagel BJ, Nooner KB, Tapert SF, Pfefferbaum A. The NCANDA_PUBLIC_4Y_REDCAP_V02 Data Release of the National Consortium on Alcohol and NeuroDevelopment in Adolescence (NCANDA). Sage Bionetworks Synapse. https://dx.doi.org/10.7303/syn24226662.

* * Pohl KM, Sullivan EV, Pfefferbaum A. The NCANDA_PUBLIC_4Y_STRUCTURAL_V01 Data Release of the National Consortium on Alcohol and NeuroDevelopment in Adolescence (NCANDA). Sage Bionetworks Synapse. https://dx.doi.org/10.7303/syn22216457.

* * *  https://www.niaaa.nih.gov/ncanda-data-distribution-agreement. Conflict of Interest: All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest that could inappropriately influence, or be perceived to influence, this work.

References

  1. Akaike  H. 1974. A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans Autom Control. 19:716–723. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bouchery  EE, Harwood  HJ, Sacks  JJ, Simon  CJ, Brewer  RD. 2011. Economic costs of excessive alcohol consumption in the U.S., 2006. Am J Prev Med. 41:516–524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Brown  SA, Brumback  T, Tomlinson  K, Cummins  K, Thompson  WK, Nagel  BJ, De Bellis  MD, Hooper  SR, Clark  DB, Chung  T, et al.  2015. The National Consortium on alcohol and NeuroDevelopment in adolescence (NCANDA): a multisite study of adolescent development and substance use. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 76:895–908. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Brown  SA, Myers  MG, Lippke  L, Tapert  SF, Stewart  DG, Vik  PW. 1998. Psychometric evaluation of the Customary Drinking and Drug Use Record (CDDR): a measure of adolescent alcohol and drug involvement. J Stud Alcohol. 59:427–438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Carbia  C, Cadaveira  F, Caamano-Isorna  F, Rodriguez-Holguin  S, Corral  M. 2017. Binge drinking during adolescence and young adulthood is associated with deficits in verbal episodic memory. PLoS One. 12:e0171393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Carbia  C, Lopez-Caneda  E, Corral  M, Cadaveira  F. 2018. A systematic review of neuropsychological studies involving young binge drinkers. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 90:332–349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Casey  BJ, Jones  RM, Hare  TA. 2008. The adolescent brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1124:111–126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chassin  L, Pitts  SC, Prost  J. 2002. Binge drinking trajectories from adolescence to emerging adulthood in a high-risk sample: predictors and substance abuse outcomes. J Consult Clin Psychol. 70:67–78. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cheetham  A, Allen  NB, Whittle  S, Simmons  J, Yucel  M, Lubman  DI. 2014. Volumetric differences in the anterior cingulate cortex prospectively predict alcohol-related problems in adolescence. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 231:1731–1742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Crews  F, He  J, Hodge  C. 2007. Adolescent cortical development: a critical period of vulnerability for addiction. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 86:189–199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Desikan  RS, Segonne  F, Fischl  B, Quinn  BT, Dickerson  BC, Blacker  D, Buckner  RL, Dale  AM, Maguire  RP, Hyman  BT, et al.  2006. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 31:968–980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Diamond  A, Sekhon  JS. 2013. Genetic matching for estimating causal effects: a general multivariate matching method for achieving balance in observational studies. Rev Econ Stat. 95:932–945. [Google Scholar]
  13. Doallo  S, Cadaveira  F, Corral  M, Mota  N, López-Caneda  E, Holguín  SR. 2014. Larger mid-dorsolateral prefrontal gray matter volume in young binge drinkers revealed by voxel-based morphometry. PLoS One. 9:e96380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Fischl  B. 2012. FreeSurfer. Neuroimage. 62:774–781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Gil-Hernandez  S, Garcia-Moreno  LM. 2016. Executive performance and dysexecutive symptoms in binge drinking adolescents. Alcohol. 51:79–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Gil-Hernandez  S, Mateos  P, Porras  C, Garcia-Gomez  R, Navarro  E, Garcia-Moreno  LM. 2017. Alcohol binge drinking and executive functioning during adolescent brain development. Front Psychol. 8:1638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gogtay  N, Giedd  JN, Lusk  L, Hayashi  KM, Greenstein  D, Vaituzis  AC, Nugent  TF  3rd, Herman  DH, Clasen  LS, Toga  AW, et al.  2004. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 101:8174–8179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Goudriaan  AE, Grekin  ER, Sher  KJ. 2007. Decision making and binge drinking: a longitudinal study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 31:928–938. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Heitzeg  MM, Nigg  JT, Hardee  JE, Soules  M, Steinberg  D, Zubieta  JK, Zucker  RA. 2014. Left middle frontal gyrus response to inhibitory errors in children prospectively predicts early problem substance use. Drug Alcohol Depend. 141:51–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Howell  NA, Worbe  Y, Lange  I, Tait  R, Irvine  M, Banca  P, Harrison  NA, Bullmore  ET, Hutchison  WD, Voon  V. 2013. Increased ventral striatal volume in college-aged binge drinkers. PLoS One. 8:e74164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Johnston  LD, Miech  RA, O’Malley  PM, Bachman  JG, Schulenberg  JE, Patrick  ME. 2020. Monitoring the future national survey results on drug use 1975–2019: overview, key findings on adolescent drug use. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan. [Google Scholar]
  22. Jones  SA, Lueras  JM, Nagel  BJ. 2018. Effects of binge drinking on the developing brain. Alcohol Res. 39:87–96. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Khundrakpam  BS, Reid  A, Brauer  J, Carbonell  F, Lewis  J, Ameis  S, Karama  S, Lee  J, Chen  Z, Das  S, et al.  2013. Developmental changes in organization of structural brain networks. Cereb Cortex. 23:2072–2085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Krongold  M, Cooper  C, Bray  S. 2017. Modular development of cortical gray matter across childhood and adolescence. Cereb Cortex. 27:1125–1136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kvamme  TL, Schmidt  C, Strelchuk  D, Chang-Webb  YC, Baek  K, Voon  V. 2016. Sexually dimorphic brain volume interaction in college-aged binge drinkers. Neuroimage Clin. 10:310–317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Lees  B, Mewton  L, Stapinski  LA, Squeglia  LM, Rae  CD, Teesson  M. 2019. Neurobiological and cognitive profile of young binge drinkers: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rev. 29:357–385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lisdahl  KM, Thayer  R, Squeglia  LM, McQueeny  TM, Tapert  SF. 2013. Recent binge drinking predicts smaller cerebellar volumes in adolescents. Psychiatry Res. 211:17–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Meda  SA, Dager  AD, Hawkins  KA, Tennen  H, Raskin  S, Wood  RM, Austad  CS, Fallahi  CR, Pearlson  GD. 2017. Heavy drinking in college students is associated with accelerated gray matter volumetric decline over a 2 year period. Front Behav Neurosci. 11:176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Mills  KL, Goddings  AL, Herting  MM, Meuwese  R, Blakemore  SJ, Crone  EA, Dahl  RE, Guroglu  B, Raznahan  A, Sowell  ER, et al.  2016. Structural brain development between childhood and adulthood: convergence across four longitudinal samples. Neuroimage. 141:273–281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Mota  N, Parada  M, Crego  A, Doallo  S, Caamano-Isorna  F, Rodriguez Holguin  S, Cadaveira  F, Corral  M. 2013. Binge drinking trajectory and neuropsychological functioning among university students: a longitudinal study. Drug Alcohol Depend. 133:108–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Nadig  A, Seidlitz  J, McDermott  CL, Liu  S, Bethlehem  R, Moore  TM, Mallard  TT, Clasen  LS, Blumenthal  JD, Lalonde  F, et al.  2021. Morphological integration of the human brain across adolescence and adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 118:e2023860118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism . 2004. NIAAA council approves definition of binge drinking. Bethesda, MD: NIAAA Newsletter. [Google Scholar]
  33. Nguyen-Louie  TT, Tracas  A, Squeglia  LM, Matt  GE, Eberson-Shumate  S, Tapert  SF. 2016. Learning and memory in adolescent moderate, binge, and extreme-binge drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 40:1895–1904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Norman  AL, Pulido  C, Squeglia  LM, Spadoni  AD, Paulus  MP, Tapert  SF. 2011. Neural activation during inhibition predicts initiation of substance use in adolescence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 119:216–223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Parada  M, Corral  M, Caamano-Isorna  F, Mota  N, Crego  A, Holguin  SR, Cadaveira  F. 2011. Binge drinking and declarative memory in university students. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 35:1475–1484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Parada  M, Corral  M, Mota  N, Crego  A, Rodríguez Holguín  S, Cadaveira  F. 2012. Executive functioning and alcohol binge drinking in university students. Addict Behav. 37:167–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Pfefferbaum  A, Kwon  D, Brumback  T, Thompson  WK, Cummins  K, Tapert  SF, Brown  SA, Colrain  IM, Baker  FC, Prouty  D, et al.  2018. Altered brain developmental trajectories in adolescents after initiating drinking. Am J Psychiatry. 175:370–380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Pfefferbaum  A, Rohlfing  T, Pohl  KM, Lane  B, Chu  W, Kwon  D, Nolan Nichols  B, Brown  SA, Tapert  SF, Cummins  K, et al.  2016. Adolescent development of cortical and white matter structure in the NCANDA sample: role of sex, ethnicity, puberty, and alcohol drinking. Cereb Cortex. 26:4101–4121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. R Core Team . 2020. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
  40. Reuter  M, Schmansky  NJ, Rosas  HD, Fischl  B. 2012. Within-subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis. Neuroimage. 61:1402–1418. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Rohlfing  T, Cummins  K, Henthorn  T, Chu  W, Nichols  BN. 2014. N-CANDA data integration: anatomy of an asynchronous infrastructure for multi-site, multi-instrument longitudinal data capture. J Am Med Inform Assoc. 21:758–762. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Rohlfing  T, Zahr  NM, Sullivan  EV, Pfefferbaum  A. 2010. The SRI24 multichannel atlas of normal adult human brain structure. Hum Brain Mapp. 31:798–819. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Sekhon  JS. 2011. Multivariate and propensity score matching software with automated balance optimization: the matching package for R. Journal of Statistical Software.  42:1–52. 10.18637/jss.v042.i07 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Sekhon  JS, Grieve  RD. 2012. A matching method for improving covariate balance in cost-effectiveness analyses. Health Econ. 21:695–714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Sotiras  A, Toledo  JB, Gur  RE, Gur  RC, Satterthwaite  TD, Davatzikos  C. 2017. Patterns of coordinated cortical remodeling during adolescence and their associations with functional specialization and evolutionary expansion. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 114:3527–3532. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Sousa  SS, Sampaio  A, Marques  P, Gonçalves  ÓF, Crego  A. 2017. Gray matter abnormalities in the inhibitory circuitry of young binge drinkers: a voxel-based morphometry study. Front Psychol. 8:1567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Squeglia  LM, Rinker  DA, Bartsch  H, Castro  N, Chung  Y, Dale  AM, Jernigan  TL, Tapert  SF. 2014. Brain volume reductions in adolescent heavy drinkers. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 9:117–125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Squeglia  LM, Tapert  SF, Sullivan  EV, Jacobus  J, Meloy  MJ, Rohlfing  T, Pfefferbaum  A. 2015. Brain development in heavy-drinking adolescents. Am J Psychiatry. 172:531–542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration . 2020. Key substance use and mental health indicators in the United States: results from the 2019 National Survey on Drug Use and Health. Rockville, MD: Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. [Google Scholar]
  50. Tamnes  CK, Herting  MM, Goddings  AL, Meuwese  R, Blakemore  SJ, Dahl  RE, Guroglu  B, Raznahan  A, Sowell  ER, Crone  EA  et al.  2017. Development of the cerebral cortex across adolescence: a multisample study of inter-related longitudinal changes in cortical volume, surface area, and thickness. J Neurosci. 37:3402–3412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Wetherill  RR, Squeglia  LM, Yang  TT, Tapert  SF. 2013. A longitudinal examination of adolescent response inhibition: neural differences before and after the initiation of heavy drinking. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 230:663–671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Whelan  R, Conrod  PJ, Poline  JB, Lourdusamy  A, Banaschewski  T, Barker  GJ, Bellgrove  MA, Buchel  C, Byrne  M, Cummins  TD, et al.  2012. Adolescent impulsivity phenotypes characterized by distinct brain networks. Nat Neurosci. 15:920–925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Zhao  Q, Sullivan  EV, Honnorat  N, Adeli  E, Podhajsky  S, De Bellis  MD, Voyvodic  J, Nooner  KB, Baker  FC, Colrain  IM, et al.  2021. Association of heavy drinking with deviant fiber tract development in frontal brain systems in adolescents. JAMA Psychiat. 78:407–415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary_Figure_1-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Figure_2-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Figure_3-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Figure_4-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Figure_5-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Table_1_su-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Table_2_Frontal-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Table_3_Parietal-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Table_4_Temporal-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Table_5_Occipital-rev_bhab368
Supplementary_Table_6_Cingulate_and_insula-rev_bhab368

Articles from Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY) are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES