Abstract
Aims:
As populations are ageing worldwide, it is important to identify strategies to promote successful ageing. We investigate how working conditions throughout working life are associated with successful ageing in later life.
Methods:
Data from two nationally representative longitudinal Swedish surveys were linked (n=674). In 1991, respondents were asked about their first occupation, occupations at ages 25, 30, 35, 40, 45 and 50 years and their last recorded occupation. Occupations were matched with job exposure matrices to measure working conditions at each of these time points. Random effects growth curve models were used to calculate intra-individual trajectories of working conditions. Successful ageing, operationalised using an index including social and leisure activity, cognitive and physical function and the absence of diseases, was measured at follow-up in 2014 (age 70 years and older). Multivariable ordered logistic regressions were used to assess the association between trajectories of working conditions and successful ageing.
Results:
Intellectually stimulating work; that is, substantive complexity, in the beginning of one’s career followed by an accumulation of more intellectually stimulating work throughout working life was associated with higher levels of successful ageing. In contrast, a history of stressful, hazardous or physically demanding work was associated with lower levels of successful ageing.
Conclusions:
Promoting a healthy workplace, by supporting intellectually stimulating work and reducing physically demanding and stressful jobs, may contribute to successful ageing after retirement. In particular, it appears that interventions early in one’s employment career could have positive, long-term effects.
Keywords: Work-related stress, substantive complexity, physical working conditions, accumulation, de-accumulation, successful ageing, longitudinal
Background
As populations are ageing, it is important to promote health in later life. Working conditions play an important role in shaping health [1–4]. However, evidence is unclear as to whether work has positive or negative impacts on health and wellbeing in later life [3–6]. We will address this by investigating how the accumulation of physical and psychosocial working conditions over the life course relates to successful ageing in older adults.
The long-term impact of psychosocial working conditions on cognitive function later in life is well documented. Intellectually stimulating jobs have a protective effect on cognitive function and dementia at older ages [3, 4, 7]. In contrast, stressful jobs are linked with cognitive decline and dementia [7, 8]. Growing evidence shows that job strain is associated with limitations in physical function later in life [5, 9]. However, existing studies tend to use relatively crude measures of working conditions (such as one specific type of working condition) or static measures (e.g. main lifetime employment assessed once) [3, 4, 9, 10], and usually focus on one or two discreet outcomes later in life [3–5, 8–10]. Such approaches may underestimate long-term consequences of working life on health in later life [5]. To redress this, we model the accumulation and de-accumulation of diffe-rent working conditions throughout working life. Following Ferraro and Morton [11] we define accumulation as a process of amassing one or more things, whether desirable or undesirable, within domains of interest. In this paper the phenomena of interest are various types of occupational characteristics; that is, whether jobs are complex (i.e. intellectually stimulating), hazardous, physically demanding or stressful. Moreover, Ferraro and Morton note that there is a relative lack of research on the ‘accumulation of advantage’ and ‘de-accumulation’. We address both in this paper by looking at both increasing and decreasing trajectories of both poor working conditions; for example, hazardous jobs and good working conditions; that is, intellectually stimulating jobs. Another limitation of much of the existing research is the focus on single health outcomes. This ignores the complexity of health and wellbeing in later life. In contrast we use a multidimensional measure of successful ageing. Although there is not an agreed upon definition of successful ageing, the consensus is that it is ageing without chronic diseases while maintaining high cognitive and physical function [12, 13]. In contrast to ‘normal ageing’ it is presented as what may be possible if people are able to maintain healthy lifestyles and reserve capacities over the life course [14].
We hypothesise that those who start with poor working conditions and experience an accumulation of disadvantage will be least likely to age successfully, and those who start with good working conditions and experience an accumulation of advantage will be most likely to age successfully.
Methods
Data
Data from the Level of Living Survey (LNU) 1991 and the Swedish Panel Study of Living Conditions of the Oldest Old (SWEOLD) 2014 [15] were linked. The LNU is a nationally representative sample of people aged 18 to 75 years (79.1% response rate). LNU participants who had reached at least 70 years of age by 2014 were invited to participate in SWEOLD (84.3% response rate). Mixed interviews (0.5% of our analytical sample) and proxy interviews (3.6% of our analytical sample) were used to avoid non-response due to impaired cognition/communication or poor health. Proxies were typically a spouse, close relative, or healthcare professional.
To minimise recall bias the questions on occupational history in LNU 1991 were only administered to participants in paid employment and aged 47–66 years (n=1211) [16]. Of these, 29 had item non-response at baseline, hence our baseline sample is 1182. Of these, 779 were re-interviewed for SWEOLD 2014; that is, a total of 403 respondents were lost at follow-up between 1991 and 2014 (dead, n=335 and other reason, n=68). Item non-response at follow-up (SWEOLD 2014, n=105) reduced the analytical sample to 674. The item non-response at follow-up was mainly because cognitive function could not be assessed through postal questionnaires (n=85) (see Figure 1). Our analytical sample consisted of participants aged 47–65 years at baseline (M=53.5) and 70–89 years at follow-up (M=76.5). The regional ethical review board in Stockholm approved the study protocol for SWEOLD 2014 (EPN Dnr 2014/1003-31/5) and for linking SWEOLD with LNU (EPN Dnr 2015/1070-31/5).
Variables
Occupational history
We created individual trajectories of working conditions over the working life (up to eight time points). This was done by matching job exposure matrices [17, 18], which are tables with average working conditions for different occupations, with data on last recorded occupation (i.e. current occupation in LNU 1991) and occupational history recorded in LNU 1991.
Occupational history was assessed with retrospective questions about the respondent’s first occupation and all jobs thereafter. The occupations were classified using the 1980 Nordic version of the three-digit international standard occupational classification codes and were created for ‘first occupation’ (regardless of age at first job), and at ages 25, 30, 35, 40, 45 and 50 years and current occupation in 1991. About 90% of the sample had complete data on occupational history. Those 10% with missing data on occupational history had missing data on occupation at age 25 years, probably because their first job occurred after the age of 25 years.
Job exposure matrices: working conditions
Physically demanding, hazardous and high strain working conditions were assessed with a matrix [17] based on average scores on working conditions for 262 occupations from the 1977 and 1979 Swedish Survey of Living Conditions (ULF). In ULF, gender-specific scores were generated for physically demanding occupations (five questions), hazardous occupations (seven questions), job control (12 questions), and job demand (two questions) (Table I). All scores in the matrix consisted of a linear composite that ranged from 0 to 10. For a more comprehensive description of the matrix, see Johnson and Stewart [17]. High strain occupations were measured based on the Karasek and Theorell [19] model as the ratio of job demands to job control.
Table I.
Dimension | Components |
---|---|
Physically demanding occupations | Unsuitable working postures Heavy lifting Work safety Physical exertion Dirt |
Hazardous occupations | Noise Heavy shaking or vibrations Cold Drafts Inadequate ventilation Bad lighting Gas, mist or smoke |
Job control | Planning of work Planning of vacations Planning of work breaks The selection of supervisors The selection of co-workers The setting of the work pace How time is used in work If there are varied work procedures If there is varied task content Flexible working hours Possibility to learn new things Experience of personal fulfilment on the job |
Job demand | Is your job hectic? Is your job psychologically demanding? |
Substantive complexity | General educational development The complexity of work with data Intellectual aptitude Numerical aptitude Verbal aptitude Temperament for repetitive and continuous processes Abstract interest in the job |
The second job exposure matrix measures substantive complexity scores [18]. The scores were derived from the Dictionary of Occupational Titles (DOT) using the 1971 Current Population Survey [18]. Substantive complexity comprises eight characteristics (Table I). These items were standardised and summed to form a substantive complexity scale, which for ease of interpretation was converted to a 0–10 range. The US occupational categories have been matched with Swedish occupational categories to create substantive complexity scores for Swedish occupations.
Lifetime trajectories of working conditions
Trajectories of working conditions were assessed with random effects growth curve models to calculate within-person change of working conditions across working life. Growth curve models handle missing data by giving more weight to individuals with the most time points. Therefore, people with incomplete data on occupational history were included, as long as they had data on first occupation and current occupation in LNU 1991. Random effects allow for variation between participants in the individual slope and intercept. The intercept was divided into low and high via median split. The slope was divided into de-accumulation, stable and accumulation. A slope was considered as de-accumulation if there was a decrease (an estimate below zero) and as accumulation if there was an increase of more than half a standard deviation above zero. A slope between zero and half a standard deviation increase was considered a stable slope. The slope and intercept were combined to create six categories: (a) starting low and de-accumulating; (b) starting low and stable; (c) starting low and accumulating; (d) starting high and de-accumulating; (e) starting high and stable; or (f) starting high and accumulating.
Outcome
Successful ageing was assessed at follow-up in 2014. It was operationalised using an index of social and cultural activities (visiting friends or relatives, socialising with friends or relatives outside the home, helping family members, going to movies/theatre/concerts/museums/exhibitions during the past 12 months), cognitive function (an abridged version of the Mini-Mental State Examination) [20], physical function (self-reported ability to walk up and down stairs, standing without support, walking 100 meters fairly briskly, problems with balance indoors, and getting up from a (kitchen) chair with arms crossed over the chest), and absence of diseases (if during the past 12 months they had cancer, diabetes, stroke, depression, hypertension, heart failure and arrhythmia, or had ever had cancer, stroke, or myocardial infarction). The subcomponents of the index were z-standardised to allow for equal representation of the three domains of successful ageing, as recommended by Rowe and Kahn [13]. As the variables were not normally distributed the index was divided into quintiles. A more detailed description of the dependent variables is available in Supplemental data.
Covariates
Information on childhood conditions was taken from the earliest possible LNU wave; that is, 1968, 1974, 1981 or 1991, to minimise recall bias: father’s social class (blue-collar workers, including small farmers and entrepreneurs without employees; and white-collar workers, including large farmers and entrepreneurs with employees and academic professionals); father’s educational attainment (compulsory or beyond); severe conflicts within the family during childhood (yes/no or not sure); and financial difficulties during upbringing (yes/no). Level of education was collected in LNU 1991 (compulsory or beyond).
Statistical analyses
Ordered logistic regression was used to analyse the association between working conditions and successful ageing (ordinal dependent variable). The proportional odds assumption was tested and fulfilled. A crude and an adjusted model were performed. The adjusted model included age (linear); sex (binary); father’s social class (binary); father’s educational attainment (binary); severe conflicts within the family during childhood (binary); financial difficulties during upbringing (binary); and level of education (binary). In a sensitivity analysis, we tested if there was an interaction between sex and working conditions for the different outcomes. The interaction term was only statistically significant for two out of the 40 interactions. Therefore, women and men were analysed together. All statistical analyses were conducted with STATA 15.
Results
Participants with a higher score of successful ageing also reported better childhood conditions, had a higher level of education, were more likely to be women and on average were younger (Table II). Bivariate analyses show that, with the exception of high strain jobs, those with more advantageous lifetime working conditions had higher successful ageing scores (Table III).
Table II.
Successful ageing a | Analytical sample, n=674 | Dead at follow-up, n=335 | Non-response, n=68 | P value b | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean (SD) | n (%) | n (%) | n (%) | ||
Age | <0.001 | ||||
Age 47–53 | 2.3 (1.3) | 377 (55.9) | 104 (31.0) | 40 (58.8) | |
Age 54–66 | 1.6 (1.4) | 297 (44.1) | 231 (69.0) | 28 (41.2) | |
Sex | <0.001 | ||||
Women | 2.1 (1.4) | 349 (51.8) | 107 (31.9) | 27 (39.7) | |
Men | 1.9 (1.4) | 325 (48.2) | 228 (68.1) | 41 (60.3) | |
Level of education | <0.001 | ||||
Compulsory | 1.7 (1.4) | 187 (27.7) | 145 (43.3) | 37 (54.4) | |
Vocational | 2.0 (1.4) | 337 (50.0) | 138 (41.2) | 16 (23.5) | |
Upper secondary | 2.4 (1.4) | 80 (11.9) | 20 (6.0) | 10 (14.7) | |
University | 2.6 (1.2) | 70 (10.4) | 32 (10.0) | 5 (7.4) | |
Father’s social class | 0.391 | ||||
Blue collar worker | 1.9 (1.4) | 380 (56.4) | 197 (58.8) | 34 (50.0) | |
White collar worker | 2.2 (1.3) | 294 (43.6) | 138 (41.2) | 34 (50.0) | |
Father’s education | 0.016 | ||||
Compulsory | 2.0 (1.4) | 496 (73.6) | 270 (80.6) | 57 (83.8) | |
Beyond compulsory | 2.1 (1.4) | 178 (26.4) | 65 (19.4) | 11 (16.2) | |
Financial hardship | 0.039 | ||||
Yes | 1.7 (1.4) | 123 (18.2) | 70 (20.9) | 21 (30.9) | |
No | 2.1 (1.4) | 551 (81.8) | 265 (79.1) | 47 (69.1) | |
Conflicts in childhood | 0.652 | ||||
Yes | 1.9 (1.4) | 74 (11.0) | 36 (10.8) | 5 (7.4) | |
No | 2.0 (1.4) | 600 (89.0) | 299 (89.3) | 63 (92.7) |
Range 0–4.
P value was calculated with Pearson’s chi-square test to compare the groups (analytical sample, dead at follow-up, non-response).
Table III.
Successful ageing a | Analytical sample, n=674 | Dead at follow-up, n=335 | Non-response, n=68 | P value b | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean (SD) | n (%) | n (%) | n (%) | ||
Complexity | 0.240 | ||||
Low/de-accumulation | 1.9 (1.4) | 84 (12.5) | 52 (15.5) | 16 (23.5) | |
Low/stable slope | 1.8 (1.4) | 92 (13.7) | 37 (11.0) | 11 (16.2) | |
Low/accumulation | 1.8 (1.4) | 153 (22.7) | 65 (19.4) | 12 (17.7) | |
High/de-accumulation | 2.0 (1.4) | 96 (14.2) | 54 (16.1) | 9 (13.2) | |
High/stable slope | 2.2 (1.4) | 127 (18.8) | 70 (20.9) | 13 (19.1) | |
High/accumulation | 2.4 (1.3) | 122 (18.1) | 57 (17.0) | 7 (10.3) | |
High strain | <0.001 | ||||
Low/de-accumulation | 2.0 (1.5) | 132 (19.6) | 61 (18.2) | 5 (7.4) | |
Low/stable slope | 1.9 (1.4) | 78 (11.6) | 38 (11.3) | 5 (7.4) | |
Low/accumulation | 2.1 (1.4) | 128 (19.0) | 53 (15.8) | 12 (17.7) | |
High/de-accumulation | 1.9 (1.4) | 216 (32.1) | 114 (34.0) | 25 (36.8) | |
High/stable slope | 2.3 (1.4) | 90 (13.4) | 51 (15.2) | 17 (25.0) | |
High/accumulation | 2.0 (1.4) | 30 (4.5) | 18 (5.4) | 4 (5.9) | |
Physically demanding | <0.001 | ||||
Low/de-accumulation | 2.3 (1.4) | 250 (37.1) | 86 (25.7) | 11 (16.2) | |
Low/stable slope | 2.6 (1.3) | 36 (5.3) | 15 (4.5) | 4 (5.9) | |
Low/accumulation | 2.1 (1.4) | 61 (9.1) | 22 (6.6) | 8 (11.8) | |
High/de-accumulation | 1.8 (1.4) | 228 (33.8) | 137 (40.9) | 31 (45.6) | |
High/stable slope | 1.7 (1.4) | 46 (6.8) | 50 (14.9) | 10 (14.7) | |
High/accumulation | 1.5 (1.5) | 53 (7.9) | 25 (7.5) | 4 (5.9) | |
Hazardous | <0.001 | ||||
Low/de-accumulation | 2.3 (1.4) | 243 (36.1) | 74 (22.1) | 16 (23.5) | |
Low/stable slope | 1.9 (1.4) | 71 (10.5) | 16 (4.5) | 4 (5.9) | |
Low/accumulation | 2.0 (1.5) | 31 (4.6) | 9 (2.7) | 3 (4.4) | |
High/de-accumulation | 1.9 (1.3) | 266 (39.5) | 183 (54.6) | 39 (57.4) | |
High/stable slope | 2.0 (1.6) | 23 (3.4) | 23 (6.9) | 4 (5.9) | |
High/accumulation | 1.6 (1.5) | 40 (5.9) | 30 (9.0) | 2 (2.9) |
Range 0–4.
P value was calculated with Pearson’s chi-square test and Fisher’s exact test to compare the groups (analytical sample, dead at follow-up, non-response).
A high initial level of substantive complexity followed by an accumulation throughout working life was associated with higher odds of successful ageing (odds ratio (OR) 2.25, 95% confidence interval (CI) 1.34, 3.80) in the adjusted model (Table IV).
Table IV.
Successful ageing (n=674) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Complexity | High strain | Physically demanding | Hazardous | |
OR (95% CI) | OR (95% CI) | OR (95% CI) | OR (95% CI) | |
Trajectories across working life (Crude model) | ||||
Low/de-accumulation | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 |
Low/stable slope | 0.91 (0.54, 1.54) | 1.12 (0.68, 1.84) | 1.40 (0.77, 2.58) | 0.57 (0.36, 0.92) |
Low/accumulation | 0.93 (0.58, 1.50) | 0.62 (0.41, 0.95) | 0.75 (0.46, 1.22) | 0.64 (0.33, 1.28) |
High/de-accumulation | 1.27 (0.75, 2.13) | 0.84 (0.58, 1.23) | 0.54 (0.39, 0.74) | 0.56 (0.41, 0.77) |
High/stable slope | 1.57 (0.96, 2.56) | 1.02 (0.63, 1.64) | 0.45 (0.26, 0.80) | 0.68 (0.30, 1.53) |
High/accumulation | 1.86 (1.14, 3.03) | 0.52 (0.26, 1.02) | 0.36 (0.21, 0.62) | 0.40 (0.22, 0.75) |
Adjusted model | ||||
Trajectories across working life (Adjusted model) | ||||
Low/de-accumulation | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 |
Low/stable slope | 0.89 (0.52, 1.51) | 1.01 (0.62, 1.66) | 1.28 (0.69, 2.36) | 0.56 (0.35, 0.91) |
Low/accumulation | 0.96 (0.59, 1.56) | 0.55 (0.36, 0.86) | 0.81 (0.49, 1.34) | 0.83 (0.42, 1.64) |
High/de-accumulation | 1.27 (0.74, 2.17) | 0.87 (0.60, 1.28) | 0.62 (0.44, 0.87) | 0.67 (0.47, 0.97) |
High/stable slope | 1.43 (0.86, 2.40) | 0.81 (0.50, 1.31) | 0.56 (0.31, 1.02) | 0.80 (0.34, 1.91) |
High/accumulation | 2.25 (1.34, 3.80) | 0.65 (0.32, 1.34) | 0.38 (0.22, 0.68) | 0.38 (0.19, 0.76) |
Adjusted model: adjusted for age, sex, educational attainment and childhood conditions assessed in 1991.
Indicators of working conditions were entered into four separate models.
Results in bold P<0.05.
High job strain was generally not statistically significantly associated with successful ageing. The exception was participants with a low initial level of high strain and an accumulation in which we observed lower odds of successful ageing (OR 0.55, 95% CI 0.36, 0.86) in the adjusted model (Table IV).
A high starting point of physically demanding or hazardous work was associated with lower odds of successful ageing, regardless of the subsequent trajectory (Table IV). In the adjusted model, an initially high starting point followed by an accumulation and a high starting point of physically demanding work followed by a de-accumulation were both associated with lower odds of successful ageing (OR 0.38, 95% CI 0.22, 0.68 and OR 0.62, 95% CI 0.44, 0.87, respectively). Although the association between a high starting point followed by a stable trajectory and successful ageing did not reach traditional thresholds of statistical significance (i.e. P<0.05), it was approaching significance (Table IV). A high initial level of hazardous work followed by an accumulation was associated with lower odds of successful ageing (OR 0.38, 95% CI 0.19, 0.76) in the adjusted model (Table IV).
Discussion
This longitudinal study investigated working conditions throughout working life in relation to successful ageing at age 70 years and beyond. In short, having higher substantive complexity at the beginning of the career, followed by an accumulation across working life, appears to be most beneficial for successful ageing. Having a high initial starting point of physically demanding or hazardous work, followed by an accumulation across working life, appears to be the least beneficial for successful ageing. Having a low initial starting point of job strain, followed by an accumulation across working life, was associated with lower levels of successful ageing.
Earlier research has shown that the effect of working conditions can reach deep into retirement [3–9]. However, the majority of these studies focus on just one health outcome. This study reveals a long-term association between both physical and psychological working conditions throughout working life and a multi-dimensional measure of successful ageing, including health, cognitive and physical function and social and leisure activities. This indicates that working life influences key aspects of later life.
The importance of taking a life course approach to predict successful ageing has been highlighted previously [11, 14, 21]. As suggested by cumulative advantage/disadvantage (CAD) theory [11] exposure to poor working conditions multiple times across working life may cause an accumulation of disadvantage with a negative influence on health later in life. Starting with a low exposure of job strain, followed by an accumulation of disadvantage, was associated with decreased chances of ageing successfully. Re-occurring experiences of job strain (chronic stress) may cause accumulated wear and tear on the body resulting in allostatic overload [22]. This may have long-term effects on the body through biochemical pathways, such as elevated levels of cortisol, which may, in turn, affect brain regions involved in cognition and memory [23]. Chronic stress may also have long-term physical impacts through physiological dysregulations that cause damage to muscles, brain, heart and vascular systems via disruptions of immune function, blood pressure, metabolism and hormone function [22]. This, in turn, increases the risk of several health conditions that pose a threat for successful ageing later in life, such as cardiovascular diseases [1]. Surprisingly, starting with a high exposure of job strain, followed by an accumulation of disadvantage, was not associated with decreased chances of ageing successfully. This is likely to be due to power issues. Still, it warrants more investigation in future research.
Those who started with a high exposure to physical or hazardous work, followed by an accumulation of disadvantage, were least likely to age successfully. High physical workload, such as heavy lifting and unsuitable working postures, has been associated with lower physical function and back pain in later life [24, 25]. Experiencing increasing levels of physical demanding and hazardous working conditions across working life may cause physical damage to the body, such as musculoskeletal pain [26] and ischaemic heart disease [1] with long-term consequences for health and function. Moreover, people with physically demanding and stressful jobs generally have poorer lifestyle habits; for example, fewer activities outside of work [19] and they smoke more [27]. They are also more likely to have lower incomes [28] and belong to a lower social class [5, 28]. This may lead to accumulated disadvantages over the life course [11].
There is a relative lack of research on ‘de-accumulation’ and ‘accumulation of advantage’ [11]. Our findings show that starting with a high exposure to physically demanding or hazardous work, compared to a low starting point, followed by a de-accumulation of disadvantage was associated with decreased chances of ageing successfully. This may indicate a change of occupation as a consequence of poor working conditions and/or poor health. Perhaps these individuals, after a long period of physically demanding or hazardous work tasks, changed their occupation, either as a result of poor health or voluntarily. Those who start their career with high substantive complexity and experience an accumulation of advantage were most likely to age successfully. Our finding of an association between intellectually stimulating jobs throughout working life and successful ageing is in line with the cognitive reserve theory [29]. This maintains that intellectually stimulating work should postpone age-related cognitive decline as the intellectually challenging occupations may function as a buffer; that is, people with high reserve can tolerate a certain degree of brain pathology. Moreover, people with good working conditions generally have healthier lifestyles; for example, greater physical, social and mental activities outside of work [19], and they do not smoke [27]. But they are also more likely to have higher incomes [10, 28] and belong to a higher social class [5, 28]. On retirement, these people may have better health and function as a result of accumulated advantage over the life course [11].
Our results should be interpreted with some caution. First, although we had a nationally representative sample with a high response rate at both baseline and follow-up, studies with the oldest old are sensitive to selective survival and attrition; that is, those with better health and life circumstances, such as working conditions, survive and continue to respond. Post hoc analyses, in which the analytical sample was compared with people who did not participate in the follow-up, confirmed this assumption (see Tables II and III). Complete responders reported better childhood circumstances, higher education and better working conditions across working life. Hence, our models are likely to underestimate the associations. However, the use of proxy interviews made it possible to represent the frailest older adults better and can partially compensate for selective non-response [30]. Second, there may also be a health selection into different types of occupations that may influence the results. We could not adjust for health before the first occupation, which would reduce the risk of reversed causality. We did, however, adjust for childhood conditions and level of education in an attempt to minimise this type of selection bias. Third, although the occupational history provided in the LNU presents a unique chance to examine working conditions across working life, this presents a risk of recall bias. To minimise such bias, all jobs held were described by the respondent one after the other in temporal order. Fourth, we were not able to adjust for other socioeconomic and lifestyle factors over the life course. Therefore, it is likely that we did not eliminate all confounders that may play a role in the relation between working conditions and successful ageing. Finally, using an average population-based matrix does not consider inter-individual variations within the same type of occupation. However, the advantage of using these matrices is that they are relatively free of bias caused by individual reporting differences and they have been shown to have good internal validity [17].
Conclusions
To conclude, as retirement ages increase internationally, the influence of work on future health could become even more important. Understanding which working conditions contribute to successful ageing is central to the design of public health policies that aim to reduce health risks over the life course. There is a growing body of research on inequalities in health and wellbeing in later life. The majority of the research focuses on just one outcome; for example, depression, physical health, quality of life, etc. However, the different dimensions of health and wellbeing tend to be associated with one another. Hence, by using a composite measure of successful ageing we captured this multi-dimensionality. Our results suggest that policy makers, employers and practitioners should not only target isolated working conditions, or isolated health outcomes in later life, but rather take a holistic approach. The results from this study highlight the importance of improving intellectual stimulation and control, as well as reducing physically demanding, hazardous and stressful jobs throughout working life to contribute to successful ageing among people 70 years and older. Targeting modifications to working conditions early in the career might have cumulative beneficial effects for successful ageing.
Supplemental Material
Supplemental material, sj-docx-1-sjp-10.1177_14034948211013279 for Life-course trajectories of working conditions and successful ageing by Charlotta Nilsen, Alexander Darin-Mattsson, Martin Hyde and Jonas W. Wastesson in Scandinavian Journal of Public Health
Acknowledgments
The author(s) would like to express their gratitude to John W. Rowe for his support and encouragement of the study, and for providing helpful comments and suggestions. The author(s) also acknowledge Ross Andel’s contribution to matching scores for substantive complexity to the Swedish context.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest: The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding: The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This work was supported by the Marcus and Marianne Wallenberg Foundation (Grant Number MMW 2016.008); and Forte, Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research (Grant Number 2019-01141 and 2016-07206).
ORCID iD: Charlotta Nilsen https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3662-5486
Supplemental material: Supplemental material for this article is available online.
References
- [1]. Åkerstedt T, Mittendorfer-Rutz E, Rahman S. Sleep disturbance and work-related mental strain: a national prospective cohort study of the prediction of subsequent long-term sickness absence, disability pension and mortality. Scand J Public Health 2020;48:888–895. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [2]. Theorell T, Jood K, Järvholm LS, et al. A systematic review of studies in the contributions of the work environment to ischaemic heart disease development. Eur J Public Health 2016;26:470–477. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [3]. Nexø MA, Meng A, Borg V. Can psychosocial work conditions protect against age-related cognitive decline? Results from a systematic review. Occup Environ Med 2016;73:487–496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [4]. Then FS, Luck T, Luppa M, et al. Systematic review of the effect of the psychosocial working environment on cognition and dementia. Occup Environ Med 2014;71:358–365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [5]. Nilsen C, Andel R, Darin-Mattsson A, et al. Psychosocial working conditions across working life may predict late-life physical function: a follow-up cohort study. BMC Public Health 2019;19:1125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [6]. Wickrama K, O’Neal CW, Kwag KH, et al. Is working later in life good or bad for health? An investigation of multiple health outcomes. J Gerontol Series B: Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2013;68:807–815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [7]. Fratiglioni L, Marseglia A, Dekhtyar S. Ageing without dementia: can stimulating psychosocial and lifestyle experiences make a difference? Lancet Neurol 2020;19:533–543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [8]. Nilsen C, Nelson ME, Andel R, et al. Job strain and trajectories of cognitive change before and after retirement. J Gerontol Series B. Epub ahead of print 24 February 2021. DOI: 10.1093/geronb/gbab033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [9]. Kulmala J, Hinrichs T, Törmäkangas T, et al. Work-related stress in midlife is associated with higher number of mobility limitation in older age – results from the FLAME study. Age 2014;36:9722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [10]. Darin-Mattsson A, Fors S, Kåreholt I. Different indicators of socioeconomic status and their relative importance as determinants of health in old age. Int J Equity Health 2017;16:1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [11]. Ferraro KF, Morton PM. What do we mean by accumulation? Advancing conceptual precision for a core idea in gerontology. J Gerontol Series B 2018;73:269–278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [12]. Kleineidam L, Thoma MV, Maercker A, et al. What is successful aging? A psychometric validation study of different construct definitions. Gerontologist 2019;59:738–748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13]. Rowe JW, Kahn RL. Successful aging. Gerontologist 1997;37:433–440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14]. Britton A, Shipley M, Singh-Manoux A, et al. Successful aging: the contribution of early-life and midlife risk factors. J Am Geriatr Soc 2008;56:1098–1105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15]. Lennartsson C, Agahi N, Hols-Salén L, et al. Data resource profile: the Swedish panel study of living conditions of the oldest old (SWEOLD). Int J Epidemiol 2014;43:731–738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16]. Jonsson JO, Mills C. The Swedish level-of-living surveys: a general overview and discription of the event history data. In: Jonsson JO, Mills C. (eds) Cradle to Grace: Life-Course Change in Modern Sweden, 1st edition. sociologypress, 2001, pp.228–242. [Google Scholar]
- [17]. Johnson JV, Stewart WF. Measuring work organization exposure over the life course with a job-exposure matrix. Scand J Work, Environ Health 1993;19:21–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [18]. Roos PA, Treiman DJ. Appendix F: DOT scales for the 1970 census classification. In: Miller AR, Treiman DJ, Cain PS, et al. (eds) Work, Jobs, and Occupations: A Critical Review of the Dictionary of Occupational Titles. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 1980, pp.336–389. [Google Scholar]
- [19]. Karasek R, Theorell T. Healthy work. Stress, productivity, and the reconstruction of working life. New York: Basic Books, 1990, p. 54. [Google Scholar]
- [20]. Ericsson MC, Gatz M, Kåreholt I, et al. Validation of abridged mini-mental state examination scales using population-based data from Sweden and USA. Eur J Ageing 2017;14:199–205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [21]. Stowe JD, Cooney TM. Examining Rowe and Kahn’s concept of successful aging: importance of taking a life course perspective. Gerontologist 2015;55:43–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [22]. McEwen BS. Central effects of stress hormones in health and disease: understanding the protective and damaging effects of stress and stress mediators. Eur J Pharmacol 2008;583:174–185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23]. Lupien SJ, Maheu F, Tu M, et al. The effects of stress and stress hormones on human cognition: implications for the field of brain and cognition. Brain Cognit 2007;65:209–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [24]. Gnudi S, Sitta E, Gnudi F, et al. Relationship of a lifelong physical workload with physical function and low back pain in retired women. Aging Clin Exp Res 2009;21:55–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [25]. Mänty M, Kouvonen A, Lallukka T, et al. Pre-retirement physical working conditions and changes in physical health functioning during retirement transition process. Scand J Work, Environ Health 2016;42:405–412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [26]. Burström L, Nilsson T, Wahlström J. Whole-body vibration and the risk of low back pain and sciatica: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int Arch Occupat Environ Health 2015;88:403–418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [27]. Radi S, Ostry A, LaMontagne AD. Job stress and other working conditions: relationships with smoking behaviors in a representative sample of working Australians. Am J Industr Med 2007;50:584–596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [28]. Galobardes B, Shaw M, Lawlor DA, et al. Indicators of socioeconomic position (part 1). J Epidemiol Community Health 2006;60:7–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [29]. Stern Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 2009;47:2015–2028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [30]. Kelfve S, Thorslund M, Lennartsson C. Sampling and non-response bias on health-outcomes in surveys of the oldest old. Eur J Ageing 2013;10:237–245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Supplemental material, sj-docx-1-sjp-10.1177_14034948211013279 for Life-course trajectories of working conditions and successful ageing by Charlotta Nilsen, Alexander Darin-Mattsson, Martin Hyde and Jonas W. Wastesson in Scandinavian Journal of Public Health