Abstract
For many research questions in perinatal epidemiology, gestational age is a mediator that features the causal pathway between exposure and outcome. A mediator is an intermediate variable between an exposure and outcome, which is influenced by the exposure on the causal pathway to the outcome. Therefore, conventional analyses that adjust, stratify, or match for gestational age or its proxy (eg, preterm vs term deliveries) are problematic. This practice, which is entrenched in perinatal research, induces an overadjustment bias. Depending on the causal question, it may be inappropriate to adjust (or condition) for a mediator, such as gestational age, by either design or statistical analysis, but its effect can be quantified through causal mediation analysis. In an exposition of such methods, we demonstrated the relationship between the exposure and outcome and provided a formal analytical framework to quantify the extent to which a causal effect is influenced by a mediator. We reviewed concepts of confounding and causal inference, introduced the concept of a mediator and illustrated the perils of adjusting for a mediator in an exposure-outcome paradigm for a given causal question, adopted causal methods that call for an evaluation of a mediator in a causal exposure effect on the outcome, and discussed unmeasured confounding assumptions in mediation analysis. Furthermore, we reviewed other developments in the causal mediation analysis literature, including decomposition of a total effect when the mediator interacts with the exposure (4-way decomposition), methods for multiple mediators, mediation methods for case-control studies, mediation methods for time-to-event outcomes, sample size and power analysis for mediation analysis, and available software to apply these methods. To illustrate these methods, we provided a clinical example to estimate the risk of perinatal mortality (outcome) concerning placental abruption (exposure) and to determine the extent to which preterm delivery (mediator; a proxy for gestational age) plays a role in this causal effect. We hoped that the adoption of mediation methods described in this review will move research in perinatal epidemiology away from biased adjustments of mediators toward a more nuanced quantification of effects that pose unique challenges and provide unique insights in our field.
Keywords: causal analysis, collider bias, epidemiology, mediation analysis, perinatal research, unmeasured confounding
Introduction
This review provides an exposition of methods for causal mediation analysis and illustrated the influence of mediating variables with real-life applications in perinatal epidemiology.
Furthermore, we introduced the concept of causal mediation through the following scenario, drawing on the concepts espoused by Hernán.1 Consider a pregnant person at 28 weeks’ gestation who experiences heavy vaginal bleeding and frequent uterine contractions, requiring urgent evaluation on labor and delivery. The patient undergoes an emergent cesarean delivery for suspected placental abruption (exposure), which results in a live-born neonate, who unfortunately dies a week later (outcome). If this case had been managed differently, deferred delivery may or may not have resulted in stillbirth but may have averted the neonatal death (all else being equal). This reasoning is termed “counterfactual” (or contrary to the fact).
The example illustrates several salient issues. First, abruption determines gestational age (GA) at delivery,2–4 and GA, in turn, is a powerful predictor of mortality.5–7 Therefore, it is likely that the abruption-preterm delivery-perinatal mortality association is causal. Second, in searching for clues into the cause of neonatal death, both abruption and preterm delivery (independently or abruption resulting in preterm delivery) are plausible candidates. Did the abruption cause the neonatal death or was early delivery the cause? Or were both factors involved? In other words, was the abruption serious enough to warrant an emergent cesarean delivery (leading to preterm delivery at 28 weeks’ gestation), which, in turn, led to the neonatal demise? If so, how large was the effect of abruption on neonatal death, and how much of the effect on neonatal death was owing to abruption leading to preterm delivery? An analysis that seeks to answer these questions and estimate causal effects are approached through mediation analysis.
Mediation analysis provides a formal analytical framework to understand the causal effect of an exposure (eg, placental abruption) on the outcome (eg, perinatal mortality) and the role of mediators (eg, preterm delivery) that operate within this paradigm.8,9 Table 1 presents a glossary of common epidemiologic terms relevant to causal analysis.
TABLE 1.
Glossary of general epidemiologic terms relevant to causal analysis
Terms | Definition |
---|---|
Association | An association is a statistical relationship between 2 variables that co-occur. Associations can occur between variables in the presence or absence of a causal relationship. |
Confounder | A confounder is a variable that is a common “cause” of both the exposure and outcome and is not on the causal pathway between exposure and outcome. The influence of confounders requires adjustment to quantify an association between exposure and outcome to address confounding bias. Confounding bias occurs when there is a failure to adjust for common causes of both the exposure and outcome. |
Intermediate | An intermediate is a variable that exerts influence on the causal pathway between the exposure and outcome. Directed acyclic graphs illustrate the influence of intermediates in the causal pathway. |
Unmeasured confounder | An unmeasured confounder is a “hidden bias” that may account for an observed association owing to variables for which data were not ascertained or yet unrealized variables. |
Mediator | A mediator is an intermediate variable between an exposure and the outcome, which is influenced by the exposure on the causal pathway to the outcome. A mediation analysis quantifies the extent that an exposure affects the outcome through a specific mediated pathway vs one that is independent of this pathway. |
Overadjustment | An overadjustment is a type of bias that is introduced when there is adjustment (or stratification or matching) for a mediator. This adjustment no longer results in an estimate of the total effect (but rather a form of a direct effect) and often leads to counterintuitive and/or paradoxical results. |
Collider | A collider is a variable on the causal pathway that is a common “effect” of both the exposure and outcome. Any adjustment or stratification or matching on a collider induces a collider stratification bias, also referred to as a form of selection bias or bias owing to conditioning on a collider. An adjustment for a collider can result in distorted causal effects between the exposure and outcome. |
All definitions were adapted from VanderWeele.8
This review is devoted to an exposition of mediation analysis in perinatal epidemiology for clinician-researchers. The objectives were to (1) review the concepts of confounding and causal inference, (2) introduce the concept of a mediator and illustrate the perils of adjusting for this mediator in an exposure-outcome paradigm, (3) present an overview of causal mediation methods, and (4) discuss unmeasured confounding assumptions in a mediation analysis and methods to address unmeasured confounding. We further discussed recent developments in the mediation analysis literature, including decomposition of the total effect (TE) in the presence of a mediator when the mediator also interacts with the exposure (called the 4-way decomposition), methods for multiple mediators, mediation methods for case-control studies, and mediation analysis for time-to-event outcomes. In addition, we provided a brief discussion of the sample size and power analysis for causal mediation analysis and review of available software to accomplish mediation analysis. Furthermore, we used this single example to illustrate several causal mediation methods. Currently, it will suffice to declare that the illustration of mediation analysis is built on the premise that preterm delivery is the single mediator of the abruption-perinatal mortality association. Methods to address multiple mediators are presented later.
Causal Directed Acyclic Graphs
Evaluation of causal effects rests on certain assumptions regarding unmeasured confounding, among others. Directed acyclic graph (DAG) is a causal graph that characterizes assumptions about how exposures, outcomes, confounders, mediators, and unmeasured confounders are interrelated and help identify potential biases (Shrier and Platt10 provides an overview). We distinguished 3 forms of biases: confounding bias, bias owing to an adjustment for a causal intermediate, and collider stratification bias (Figure 1).
FIGURE 1. Simple directed acyclic graphs depicting the relationship between an exposure and outcome in the presence of confounding, overadjustment, and collider.
An example for each of the 3 scenarios are shown based on placental abruption (ABPL exposure [A]) and Mort (outcome [Y]) in the presence of confounders (C) and GA (mediator [M]) on the causal pathway and 2 unmeasured confounders, U (PTL) and V (Chorio). Panel A refers to a causal scenario with confounding; panel B refers to a causal scenario with overadjustment; and panel C refers to a causal scenario with a collider. Note that there are other scenarios under which a collider bias may be induced. Adapted from Cole et al11 and Ananth and Schisterman.12
ABPL, placental abruption; Chorio, chorioamnionitis; GA, gestational age; Mort, perinatal mortality; PTL, preterm labor.
Confounding bias is said to occur when there is a failure to adjust for common causes of the exposure and outcome. A simple definition of the confounder is a variable that is causally associated with the exposure and outcome and does not feature on the causal pathway between the exposure and outcome. In contrast, a mediator (also referred to as an “intermediate”) is a variable that does feature on the causal pathway between the exposure and outcome. Another serious bias is a collider stratification bias, a form of selection bias. This is introduced when an adjustment is made for a variable that is affected by the exposure and shares common causes with the outcome.13–15
DAGs provide a visual guide to analyses—they facilitate an intuitive and easy-to-understand graphical approach—by conceptualizing the complex relationship among exposure, outcome, and the myriad variables that influence the causal structure.
Motivating Example
We began with a motivation of the causal question: to estimate the risk of perinatal mortality in a subpopulation experiencing abruption, relative to what would have happened if that subpopulation had not experienced abruption (called the “total effect” [TE]), and determine the extent to which preterm delivery (a proxy for GA) plays a role in this causal effect. Returning to the abruption example, we used data from the Collaborative Perinatal Project (1959–1966), which includes 50,395 singleton births, to examine the causal effect of placental abruption (exposure) on perinatal mortality (outcome; defined as stillbirth plus deaths within the first week of life). The causal question, which is illustrated in a simplified DAG that depicts this relationship, is described in Figure 2 and will be used throughout this review.
FIGURE 2. Simplified directed acyclic graph depicting the relation between placental abruption (A) and perinatal mortality (Y) with gestational age as the mediator (M) and unmeasured confounders (U).
Perinatal mortality rates among patients with abruption and patients with nonabruption pregnancies were 264.8 and 24.7 per 1000 births, respectively (risk ratio [RR], 10.72; 95% confidence interval [CI], 9.52–12.70) (Table 2). This is defined as the RR of TE and refers to the effect of ignoring potential mediators. Patients with abruption were delivered earlier than patients with nonabruption pregnancies to avoid maternal and fetal compromises.12,16,17 Clinical experience suggests that abruption and preterm delivery exert effects on perinatal mortality. Preterm delivery—regardless of abruption—confers an increased risk of perinatal mortality.18,19 Simultaneously, abruption shortens GA at delivery (and increases the likelihood of preterm delivery)17,20–22 as abruption may trigger the onset of spontaneous labor or may lead to a clinician-initiated intervention.3,23
TABLE 2.
Perinatal mortality rates among pregnancies with and without placental abruption: Collaborative Perinatal Project, 1959 to 1966
Placental abruption |
No placental abruption |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Perinatal mortality | Total births | Number (rate per 1000) | Total births | Number (rate per 1000) | Risk ratio (95% confidence interval) |
Overall | 1031 | 273 (264.8) | 49,346 | 1219(24.7) | 10.72 (9.52–12.70) |
| |||||
Term deliveriesa | 589 | 57 (96.8) | 42,232 | 463 (11.0) | 8.83 (6.78–11.49) |
| |||||
Preterm deliveriesa | 442 | 216(488.7) | 7114 | 756 (106.3) | 4.60(4.08–5.19) |
| |||||
Adjusted for preterm deliverya | — | — | — | — | 5.21 (4.64–5.85) |
Caveat emptor: We recognized the potential for bias in this analysis owing to the stratification and adjustment for a causal intermediate (preterm delivery), potentially leading to a collider bias. The intent was purely for an illustration of the concept.
It is tempting to examine the effect between abruption and perinatal mortality within the strata of GA groups (eg, preterm gestation vs term gestation) or adjust the effect for GA. There seems to be an entrenched bias that favors the adjustment of GA as a confounder. However, when the TE is adjusted for or stratified based on preterm delivery status, an “over-adjustment” bias is introduced.11,14,24 Investigators sometimes estimate an effect by adjusting away for a mediator. In doing so, they are no longer estimating the TE but instead estimating a direct effect, which can be biased.25
Although perinatal mortality rates sharply decline with increasing GA among both the abruption and nonabruption groups, the decline is less among patients with abruption pregnancies (Figure 3). Furthermore, abruption confers increased perinatal mortality risk at every gestational week, with progressively increasing RR with advancing gestation. When adjustments are made for preterm delivery, the effect is stronger at term (≥37 weeks’ gestation) than at preterm gestations (Table 2). When adjusted for preterm delivery, the strength of the causal effect drops by half compared with the estimate of RR not adjusted for preterm delivery. This counterintuitive result does not estimate the TE, but rather reflects the unintended introduction of an overadjustment bias that occurs when an adjustment is made for a mediator.
FIGURE 3. Gestational age-specific perinatal mortality rates among women with and without placental abruption: Collaborative Perinatal Project, 1959 to 1966.
Caveat emptor: We recognized the potential for bias in this analysis owing to the stratification and adjustment for a causal intermediate (preterm delivery), potentially leading to a collider bias. The intent was purely for an illustration of the concept.
This example sets the stage for a closer look at GA (or preterm delivery) as a potential mediator in a causal framework. An adjustment for preterm delivery in this example is referred to as an overadjustment problem in perinatal epidemiology.26,27 Stratification, adjustment, or matching on a mediator opens a “backdoor” path through which a portion of the TE goes through unmeasured confounders.11,14 Furthermore, failure to account for the backdoor path through correction for unmeasured confounding will render the causal effect biased and invalid. In effect, such an adjustment will result in a biased causal effect, as doing so will result in a common “effect” on both abruption and perinatal mortality that remains unaccounted for in the analysis.
With this backdrop, we delved to reevaluate our causal question. The objective was to address the causal question: Does placental abruption increase the risk of perinatal mortality, and how much of the increase in mortality risk is because of preterm delivery (whether via obstetrical intervention or spontaneous preterm delivery)? The implicit issue in this causal question was to describe whether (and to what extent) the mortality risk is because of early delivery or to other mechanisms that are independent of GA.
Mediation methods provide the analytical framework to account for the impact of mediating variables, such as GA, to estimate the causal effect of an exposure on the outcome. Mediation disentangles the TE into 2 components: an estimate that operates through the mediator, also called the mediated or the indirect effect, and an estimate that operates independent of the mediator, also called the direct effect.
The remainder of this review was devoted to a nontechnical exposition of mediation methods. To the statistically brilliant reader, we provided the algebraic backdrop and formulas for mediation analysis in the Appendix. We focused on mediation methods devoted to a binary exposure and binary outcome, as is common in most of the research in perinatal epidemiology. For other types of exposures and outcomes, the interested reader is referred to the collection of works by VanderWeele8 and Hernán and Robins.9
Components of Mediation Parameters
Six causal parameters were potentially identifiable in the mediation analysis (Table 3).
TABLE 3.
Definitions of causal mediation parameters based on the counterfactual framework
Causal parameter | Definition | Interpretation based on the placental abruption-perinatal mortality example |
---|---|---|
TE | TE refers to the average exposure (treatment) effect if everyone were to have been exposed (treated) compared with if everyone were unexposed (untreated). | The causal association between placental abruption and perinatal mortality after adjustment for confounders |
CDE | CDE provides a contrast of the effect of an exposure on the outcome, at a fixed level of the mediator. Note that for a binary mediator, there will be 2 CDE estimates: one where the mediator is fixed to present, and the other where the mediator is fixed to absent. | The effect of the abruption on perinatal mortality if the preterm delivery were set to what it would have been among nonabruption births |
NDE | NDE expresses how much the outcome would change, on average, if the exposure were set at level “exposed” vs “unexposed,” but the mediator was kept at the level it would have taken in the absence of the exposure. | The perinatal mortality risk between those with and without abruption if, in both cases, delivery was delayed to the preterm delivery status that would have occurred without abruption |
NIE | NIE expresses the change in the outcome under those exposed, on average, if the mediator was changed to what it would be in the absence of the exposure. | Among abruption births, the mortality risk if we were able vs unable to delay delivery to what it would have been without abruption |
PM | PM provides an estimate of the proportion of the TE that is explained (or accounted) by the mediator. Another way of expressing PM is “how much of the effect of the exposure on the outcome is because of the effect of the exposure on the mediator.” PM is estimated as NIE/TE. | The proportion of the increased abruption-perinatal mortality association that is explained (or mediated) through preterm delivery |
PE | One definition for PE is the proportion of the effect of the exposure on the outcome that could be eliminated by intervening to set the mediator to being present or absent. In other words, PE is the proportion of the TE that might be blocked by intervening on the mediator. PE is estimated as (TE-CDEm)/TE. | The proportion of the effect of abruption on perinatal mortality if we were to fix delivery at term gestations. Another way to interpret PE is the proportion of the abruption-perinatal mortality association that can be eliminated by delivery every birth at term gestation |
Table 4 provides further details about the interpretation based on the placental abruption-perinatal mortality example.
CDE, controlled direct effect; NDE, natural direct effect; NIE, natural indirect effect; PE, proportion eliminated; PM, proportion mediated; TE, total effect.
Total effect
The TE is the overall causal effect of the exposure on the outcome, without accounting for mediating variables. In other words, TE is a contrast of the outcome risk between those exposed and those unexposed, regardless of downstream mediating variables.
Controlled direct effect
The controlled direct effect (CDE) is the estimate of the expected effect that is directly attributable to the exposure when the mediator is held constant at a fixed value. This proportion of the effect does not operate through the mediator. Intuitively, the CDE is useful for examining whether there are any pathways for the exposure independent of, or other than, the mediator.
Natural direct effect
The natural direct effect (NDE) provides an estimate of the effect of the exposure on the outcome if the mediator were set to what it would have been without the exposure. The NDE is an estimate of the direct effect of the exposure on the outcome that is independent of the mediator.
Natural indirect effect
The natural indirect effect (NIE) is the estimate of the effect of the exposure that operates through the mediator. Because the effect operates through the mediator, its effects are termed “indirect.”
Proportion mediated
Proportion mediated (PM) is the proportion of the TE that is explained or accounted by the mediator. It is estimated as the ratio of the natural indirect effect to the TE (PM=NIE/TE). The PM measures what would happen to the effect of the exposure on the outcome if we were to somehow disable the pathway from the exposure to the causal intermediate.28 The PM is particularly insightful for policy-relevant recommendations.28 An important caveat with PM is if the TE and NIE are in the opposite directions of the null (eg, 1.0 for the relative risk or 0.0 for the risk difference), then the PM will be negative. Interpretation of the mediated proportion in such circumstances gets complicated and problematic.
Proportion eliminated
Proportion eliminated (PE) is the proportion of the TE that could be eliminated by removing the pathway from the exposure to the outcome through the mediator at a fixed (or given) level. The PE provides a causal estimate of the proportion of the effect that we could block by intervening on the mediator.28
Further broader discussions of mediation methods and theoretical details are presented in the Appendix.
Unmeasured Confounding Assumptions for Mediation Analysis
Drawing valid causal inferences from observational data rests on several unverifiable (and some verifiable) assumptions, including positivity29,30 (ie, every combination observed confounders are represented in both the exposed and unexposed groups), exchangeability31 (ie, no unmeasured confounding and no selection bias; discussed below), consistency (ie, well-defined exposure and mediator), lack of measurement error in the variables, and correct model specification. Another important assumption pertains to the “cross-world” assumption, which implies that there is independence between the (counterfactual) outcome and mediator values.32 This assumption is challenging to verify in empirical data and has not been considered further.
For the CDE, NDE, and NIE to be valid and unbiased, several assumptions regarding unmeasured confounding are necessary.25,33 Even if the TE is unconfounded, the partitioning of the TE through mediation effects can result in the direct and indirect effects being biased because of unmeasured confounding.34 The identification of CDE, NDE, and NIE requires at least 4 “no unmeasured confounding” assumptions.35–37
Two unmeasured confounding assumptions are required to estimate the CDE. These include (1) that there is no unmeasured confounding of the exposure (conditional on observed confounders) on the response and (2) that there is no unmeasured confounding of the mediator (conditional on the exposure and observed confounders) on the response. Failure to adjust the analyses for confounders of the mediator-response relationship (in addition to adjustment for confounders of the exposure-response relationship) can lead to biased estimation of CDE.38 These 2 assumptions are essential to estimate CDE of the exposure on the response, but 2 additional assumptions are necessary to estimate NDE and NIE. These include (1) no unmeasured confounding of the exposure (conditional on observed confounders)-mediator scenario and (2) no unmeasured confounding of the mediator response scenario (conditional on observed confounders).35–37
Interpretation of Causal Parameters
To illustrate these causal parameters, we returned to the example of placental abruption-perinatal mortality with preterm delivery as a binary mediator. The interpretation of the mediation parameters holds insofar as the identifiability assumptions are satisfied. The RRs of TE, NDE, and NIE were 11.30 (95% CI, 9.84–12.73), 6.07 (95% CI, 5.15–7.22), and 1.86 (95% CI, 1.67–2.09), respectively, for the causal effect of abruption on perinatal mortality, adjusted for several confounders (Table 4). The risk of perinatal mortality was substantially higher among patients with abruption than patients with nonabruption pregnancies, with slightly more than half of the TE being an indirect effect mediated through preterm delivery (PM of 51%). In other words, about half of the effects of the increased risk of perinatal deaths owing to abruption were because of preterm delivery, and about half of the TEs were explained through other, yet undiscovered, pathways (other than preterm delivery).
TABLE 4.
Association between placental abruption and risk of perinatal mortality: mediation effects by preterm delivery (<37 weeks’ gestation; binary mediator) and gestational age (continuous mediator): Collaborative Perinatal Project, 1959 to 1966
Risk ratio (95% CI) |
Percentage (95% CI) |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Causal mediators | Total effect | Controlled direct effect among term births | Natural direct effect | Natural indirect effect | Proportion mediated | Proportion eliminated |
Preterm delivery | ||||||
| ||||||
Unadjusted | 10.72 (9.58–12.07) | 8.83 (6.70–11.32) | 6.21 (5.34–7.20) | 1.73 (1.57–1.92) | 47 (40–53) | 64 (56–73) |
| ||||||
Adjusted | 11.30(9.84–12.73) | 8.58 (6.47–11.30) | 6.07(5.15–7.22) | 1.86 (1.67–2.09) | 51 (44–57) | 66 (57–75) |
| ||||||
Gestational age | ||||||
| ||||||
Unadjusted | 10.39(9.00–11.97) | 8.16(6.76–9.81) | 6.69 (5.66–7.89) | 1.56 (1.47–1.65) | 39 (35–44) | 43 (38–48) |
| ||||||
Adjusted | 10.41 (8.90–12.08) | 7.97 (6.55–9.83) | 6.59 (5.53–7.94) | 1.58 (1.49–1.69) | 41 (36–45) | 44 (39–49) |
Causal effects were adjusted for confounding effects of maternal age, primiparity, maternal race, education, single marital status, smoking before and during pregnancy, prepregnancy body mass index, chronic hypertension, clinic vs private patient, and socioeconomic status. The 95% CIs were estimated based on the bias-corrected bootstrap resampling method (with 2000 replications). We used the CAUSALMED procedure in SAS (version 9.4; SAS Institute, Cary, NC) to estimate the causal estimates reported in the table
CI, confidence interval
In addition, we illustrated the interpretation of causal parameters when the mediator was continuous (GA). The analysis indicates that the RRs for the NDE and NIE were 6.59 (95% CI, 5.53–7.94) and 1.58 (95% CI, 1.49–1.69), respectively (Table 4). This suggested that 41% of the TEs of abruption on perinatal mortality was mediated through GA. Is the mediated effect similar across the GA range, or might GA show stronger mediation effects, for instance, at earlier GAs compared with later GAs? Allowing more flexible modeling options for the continuous mediator (in the mediator model) may offer different insights.39
Methods to Assess the Unmeasured Confounding Assumptions
There are several sensitivity analysis methods to test these “no unmeasured confounding” assumptions,34,40,41 but we described 2 simple approaches. The first approach to evaluate the role of unmeasured confounding is to estimate a bias factor,34 which indicates how different the true (unobserved) RRs for the TE, NIE, and NDE will be and in the presence of unmeasured confounding.42 The bias-corrected estimates of RRs are then obtained by dividing the observed RR, and the CI limits by the bias factor, and multiplying the indirect effect estimate and CI limits by the bias factor. The bias is the largest factor (beyond adjustments for measured confounders) by which the unmeasured confounder could reduce an observed RR estimate. This method, accomplished through a formal sensitivity analysis, is fairly straightforward to implement.
The second method is the estimation of an E value,43 which indicates the extent to which the confounder-adjusted RRs for the TE may also be biased because of unmeasured confounders. The E value is defined as the minimum strength of association (on the RR scale) that an unmeasured confounder would need to have with both the exposure and outcome to fully explain the exposure outcome association, after adjusting for measured confounders.43 A second assessment is to evaluate how large an E value should be (on the RR scale) to move the lower 95% CI estimate if the RR is >1 or to move the upper 95% CI estimate if the RR <1 to cross the null. Intuitively, a large E value (relative to the observed confounder-adjusted RR) will indicate that it is unlikely for unmeasured confounding to wipe out the observed association. In contrast, a small E value will indicate that even a small unmeasured confounding is sufficient to account for the observed associations. Computation of the E value can be accomplished using an online program freely available at https://www.evaluecalculator.com.43,44
To illustrate the potential impact of unmeasured confounding, the E values for the estimate of RR and the lower 95% CI of the RR in the abruption-perinatal mortality example for the TE were 22.09 and 19.17, respectively. These estimates allow a qualitative assessment of how large an effect of the unmeasured confounders must be, over and above the observed confounders, to reduce the observed RR toward the null. Furthermore, the E value provides a lower limit of 95% CI estimate to cross the null of the observed RR. In this case, the results of the sensitivity analysis illustrate that unmeasured confounding is highly unlikely to contribute to the adjusted RRs, strengthening the validity of the observed observations.
An important caveat that undermines the relevance of unmeasured confounding is the lack of insights as to what these unmeasured confounders are. These unmeasured confounders may be variables that we fail to collect data on or unknown variables as we lack a theoretical hypothesis to justify their importance. We recognized this limitation and emphasized that the “no unmeasured confounding” assumption is relevant to mediation methods but suggested that there is a wider need for the application of these sensitivity analyses to other observational studies in perinatal epidemiology.45
Other Developments in the Mediation Literature
There have been several other important developments in the mediation literature. These include decomposition of a causal effect in the presence of a mediator when the mediator interacts with the exposure (4-way decomposition), methods for multiple mediators, mediation methods for case-control studies, mediation methods for time-to-event outcomes, and sample size and power analysis for mediation analysis. These methods are described in the Appendix, including an exposition of available software applications.
Discussion
In this review, we described mediation methods that call for an evaluation of the contribution of mediating variables in the causal effect of an exposure on the outcome. We provided statistical descriptions of several mediation methods and reviewed statistical software in the Appendix. In addition, we provided guidelines for clinicians by “translating” key technical issues into a relatable real-world example, using the placental abruption-perinatal mortality causal estimate to examine the mediation effects of GA.
GA exists on the causal pathway for many, if not most, outcomes of interest in perinatal research, yet there is an entrenched bias that favors adjustment as a confounder. In this review, we illustrated some of the perils that arise when adjustments are made for GA in the abruption-perinatal mortality paradigm. The practice introduces an overadjustment bias that obscures the true causal effect. When such research questions are considered through a mediation lens, a more robust analysis can quantify the direct effect of the exposure (abruption) and characterize the indirect effect of the mediator (GA) to provide causal insights into the outcomes of interest. Furthermore, the analysis can estimate the PM and PE, providing insight into the magnitude of the effect attributed to the mediator. These estimates offer a deeper introspection of what pathways (other than the mediator in question) through which the exposure may affect the outcome.46 Consequently, the results of mediation analysis can generate new research ideas that may provide novel insights and innovative therapeutic targets for common obstetrical and perinatal complications.
There are many applications of mediation analysis in perinatal epidemiology, and the choice of causal parameters rests on the scientific goals of the analysis. If the intent is to understand the causal structure and mechanisms by which an exposure operates on the causal pathway to an outcome, then mediation analysis to estimate the direct and indirect (mediated) effects may be useful. In contrast, if the goal is to understand policy implications, then the mediation analysis to estimate CDE is particularly relevant.47 For instance, Mendola et al48 demonstrated the usefulness of CDE to examine the causal effect of preeclampsia on neonatal outcomes and the role of preterm delivery as a mediator. Although there is some ambiguity in the broader applied literature of mediation methods regarding the choice of the causal parameter (Naimi et al49 examined this topic), the results of mediation analysis can provide insights into diverse research questions across perinatal research.
This review provides the analytical framework to help perinatal researchers determine whether the TE of the exposure on the outcome is influenced by mediators and, if so, methods to quantify the effect of mediators. We encourage discussions among stakeholders regarding mediators, mediation analysis, and, importantly, approach to the correct interpretation of these results. We hope that these discussions, which fall under the broad framework of causal analysis, will move research in perinatal epidemiology away from the historic disregard (intentional or unintentional) of mediating effects. -
Supplementary Material
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors thank Matthew Bicocca, MD; Suneet Chauhan, MD; Valery Danilack, PhD; Alexander Friedman, MD, MPH; Cynthia Gyamfi-Bannerman, MD, MSc; Erinn Hade, PhD; Courtney Lynch, PhD, MPH; Anthony M. Vintzileos, MD; Jason Wright, MD; and Jennifer Zeitlin, DSc, for their thoughtful review and creative suggestions, which helped improve the manuscript substantially. We also thank the 6 anonymous referees whose comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript helped us clarify many of the concepts.
C.V.A. is supported by grants from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (grant number R01-HL151551) and the National Institute for Environmental Health Sciences (grant number R01-ES033190), National Institutes of Health.
Footnotes
The authors report no conflict of interest.
REFERENCES
- 1.Hernán MA. A definition of causal effect for epidemiological research. J Epidemiol Community Health 2004;58:265–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Tikkanen M Placental abruption: epidemiology, risk factors and consequences. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand 2011;90:140–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Downes KL, Grantz KL, Shenassa ED. Maternal, labor, delivery, and perinatal outcomes associated with placental abruption: a systematic review. Am J Perinatol 2017;34: 935–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ananth CV, VanderWeele TJ. Placental abruption and perinatal mortality with preterm delivery as a mediator: disentangling direct and indirect effects. Am J Epidemiol 2011;174: 99–108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Ananth CV, Guise JM, Thorp JM Jr. Utility of antibiotic therapy in preterm premature rupture of membranes: a meta-analysis. Obstet Gynecol Surv 1996;51:324–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ananth CV, Wilcox AJ. Placental abruption and perinatal mortality in the United States. Am J Epidemiol 2001;153:332–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Wilcox AJ, Skjaerven R. Birth weight and perinatal mortality: the effect of gestational age. Am J Public Health 1992;82:378–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.VanderWeele TJ. Explanation in causal inference: methods for mediation and interaction. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2015:1–706. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hernán MA, Robins JM. Causal inference: what if. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC; 2019. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Shrier I, Platt RW. Reducing bias through directed acyclic graphs. BMC Med Res Methodol 2008;8:70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Cole SR, Platt RW, Schisterman EF, et al. Illustrating bias due to conditioning on a collider. Int J Epidemiol 2010;39:417–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hypertension in pregnancy. Report of the American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists’ Task Force on Hypertension in Pregnancy. Obstet Gynecol 2013;122:1122–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hernández-Díaz S, Schisterman EF, Hernán MA. The birth weight “paradox” uncovered? Am J Epidemiol 2006;164:1115–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hernán MA, Hernández-Díaz S, Robins JM. A structural approach to selection bias. Epidemiology 2004;15:615–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Hernán MA, Hernández-Díaz S, Werler MM, Mitchell AA. Causal knowledge as a prerequisite for confounding evaluation: an application to birth defects epidemiology. Am J Epidemiol 2002;155:176–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Joseph KS, Demissie K, Kramer MS. Obstetric intervention, stillbirth, and preterm birth. Semin Perinatol 2002;26:250–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Ananth CV, Vintzileos AM. Maternal-fetal conditions necessitating a medical intervention resulting in preterm birth. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2006;195:1557–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Basso O, Rasmussen S, Weinberg CR, Wilcox AJ, Irgens LM, Skjaerven R. Trends in fetal and infant survival following preeclampsia. JAMA 2006;296:1357–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Harmon QE, Huang L, Umbach DM, et al. Risk of fetal death with preeclampsia. Obstet Gynecol 2015;125:628–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ananth CV, Basso O. Impact of pregnancyinduced hypertension on stillbirth and neonatal mortality. Epidemiology 2010;21:118–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hutcheon JA, Lisonkova S, Joseph KS. Epidemiology of pre-eclampsia and the other hypertensive disorders of pregnancy. Best Pract Res Clin Obstet Gynaecol 2011;25:391–403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ananth CV, Vintzileos AM. Medically indicated preterm birth: recognizing the importance of the problem. Clin Perinatol 2008;35:53–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ananth CV, Berkowitz GS, Savitz DA, Lapinski RH. Placental abruption and adverse perinatal outcomes. JAMA 1999;282:1646–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Schisterman EF, Cole SR, Platt RW. Overadjustment bias and unnecessary adjustment in epidemiologic studies. Epidemiology 2009;20:488–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Robins JM, Greenland S. Identifiability and exchangeability for direct and indirect effects. Epidemiology 1992;3:143–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Vanderweele TJ, Mumford SL, Schisterman EF. Conditioning on intermediates in perinatal epidemiology. Epidemiology 2012;23:1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Wilcox AJ, Weinberg CR, Basso O. On the pitfalls of adjusting for gestational age at birth. Am J Epidemiol 2011;174:1062–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.VanderWeele TJ. Policy-relevant proportions for direct effects. Epidemiology 2013;24:175–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hernán MA, Robins JM. Estimating causal effects from epidemiological data. J Epidemiol Community Health 2006;60:578–86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Westreich D, Cole SR. Invited commentary: positivity in practice. Am J Epidemiol 2010;171: 674–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Cole SR, Frangakis CE. The consistency statement in causal inference: a definition or an assumption? Epidemiology 2009;20:3–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Andrews RM, Didelez V. Insights into the cross-world independence assumption of causal mediation analysis. Epidemiology 2021;32:209–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Pearl J Direct and indirect effects. In: Kaufmann M, ed. Proceedings of the seventeenth conference on uncertainty and artificial intelligence. San Francisco, CA; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 34.VanderWeele TJ. Bias formulas for sensitivity analysis for direct and indirect effects. Epidemiology 2010;21:540–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Valeri L, VanderWeele TJ. Mediation analysis allowing for exposure-mediator interactions and causal interpretation: theoretical assumptions and implementation with SAS and SPSS Macros. Psychol Methods 2013;18:137–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.VanderWeele TJ. Sensitivity analysis: distributional assumptions and confounding assumptions. Biometrics 2008;64:645–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.VanderWeele TJ. Mediation analysis: a practitioner’s guide. Annu Rev Public Health 2016;37:17–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Cole SR, Hernán MA. Fallibility in estimating direct effects. Int J Epidemiol 2002;31:163–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Ananth CV, Brandt JS. Disentangling the mediating effects of gestational age on neonatal outcomes: still many unresolved questions. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 2020;34:341–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Hafeman DM. Confounding of indirect effects: a sensitivity analysis exploring the range of bias due to a cause common to both the mediator and the outcome. Am J Epidemiol 2011;174:710–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Tchetgen EJ, Shpitser I. Semiparametric Theory for Causal Mediation Analysis: efficiency bounds, multiple robustness, and sensitivity analysis. Ann Stat 2012;40:1816–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Ding P, VanderWeele TJ. Sensitivity analysis without assumptions. Epidemiology 2016;27:368–77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.VanderWeele TJ, Ding P. Sensitivity analysis in observational research: introducing the E-value. Ann Intern Med 2017;167:268–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Mathur MB, Ding P, Riddell CA, VanderWeele TJ. Web site and R package for computing E-values. Epidemiology 2018;29:e45–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Ananth CV, Schisterman EF. Hidden biases in observational epidemiology: the case of unmeasured confounding. BJOG 2018;125: 644–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ananth CV. Proportion mediated in a causal mediation analysis: how useful is this measure? BJOG 2019;126:983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Basso O, Naimi AI. Commentary: from estimation to translation: interpreting mediation analysis results in perinatal epidemiology. Epidemiology 2015;26:27–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Mendola P, Mumford SL, Männistö TI, Holston A, Reddy UM, Laughon SK. Controlled direct effects of preeclampsia on neonatal health after accounting for mediation by preterm birth. Epidemiology 2015;26:17–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Naimi AI, Kaufman JS, MacLehose RF. Mediation misgivings: ambiguous clinical and public health interpretations of natural direct and indirect effects. Int J Epidemiol 2014;43:1656–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.