Abstract
In a cohort of children born very preterm (VPT), we investigated the association between breast milk feeding (BMF) initiation and its duration on cognitive development at 2 years of corrected age. Data were obtained from the Effective Perinatal Intensive Care in Europe population‐based prospective cohort of children born <32 weeks of gestation, in 11 European countries, in 2011–2012. The study sample included 4323 children. Nonverbal cognitive ability was measured applying the Parental Report of Children's Abilities, except for France where the problem‐solving domain of the Ages & Stages Questionnaire was used. Verbal cognition was based on the number of words the child could say. To determine the association between BMF (mother's own milk) and nonverbal and verbal cognition (outcome categorized as optimal and suboptimal), adjusted risk ratios (aRRs) were estimated fitting Poisson regression models, with inverse probability weights to account for nonresponse bias. Overall, 16% and 11% of the children presented suboptimal nonverbal and verbal cognition, respectively. Never BMF was associated with a significantly increased risk for suboptimal nonverbal (aRR = 1.29, 95% confidence interval [CI] = 1.09–1.53) and verbal (aRR = 1.45, 95% CI = 1.09–1.92) cognitive development compared with those ever breastfed, after adjustment for perinatal and sociodemographic characteristics. Compared with children breastfed 6 months or more, children with shorter BMF duration exhibited a statistically nonsignificant elevated aRR. VPT children fed with breast milk had both improved nonverbal and verbal cognitive development at 2 years in comparison with never breastfed, independently of perinatal and sociodemographic characteristics. This study encourages targeted interventions to promote BMF among these vulnerable children.
Keywords: breastfeeding, breast milk, cognitive development, neurodevelopment, very preterm children
In a European cohort of children born very preterm, we investigated the association between breast milk initiation and its duration on cognitive development at 2 years of corrected age. We found that never‐breastfed children faced a higher risk of having suboptimal nonverbal and verbal cognition at 2 years of corrected age when compared with those who were breastfed, independently of perinatal and sociodemographic characteristics.
Key messages
In a European cohort of children born very preterm, 16% and 11% presented suboptimal nonverbal and verbal cognitive development, respectively.
Never‐breastfed children faced a higher risk of having suboptimal nonverbal and verbal cognition at 2 years of corrected age when compared with those who were breastfed, independently of perinatal and sociodemographic characteristics.
Breastfeeding support is a modifiable factor regardless of perinatal and sociodemographic characteristics, which reinforces the importance of specifically targeted interventions to protect, promote and support breast milk feeding in neonatal intensive care units and after discharge.
1. INTRODUCTION
There is a solid evidence base showing that breast milk feeding (BMF) in healthy, full‐term infants improves cognitive abilities in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood (Horta et al., 2015; Victora et al., 2015). A meta‐analysis of 17 observational studies reported that BMF was associated with higher scores in intelligence tests (+3.4 intelligence quotient [IQ] points; Horta et al., 2015). This positive effect was also observed in a large cluster‐randomized trial designed to evaluate a BMF promotion intervention, suggesting that the association is causal: the experimental group had a 5.9 higher cluster‐adjusted mean difference in the full‐scale IQ (Kramer et al., 2008).
The presence of biological components in breast milk, such as long‐chain polyunsaturated fatty acids (especially docosahexaenoic acid and arachidonic acid) and human milk oligosaccharides, essential for the infant's brain development, might explain the association, as these components play an important role in neurogenesis and modulate infant gut microbiota composition, respectively (Agostoni, 2008; Belfort, 2018; Innis, 2004; Mayer et al., 2014). Growing evidence supports the existence of a microbiota–gut–brain axis, in which the microbiome is thought to interact with the brain through immunological, endocrine, and neural pathways, although the exact mechanisms are not yet clear (Keunen et al., 2015). Another potential mechanism is related to the children's emotional environment. Mothers who breastfeed may provide stronger attachment security and be more sensitive to the needs of the infant (Peñacoba & Catala, 2019; Tharner et al., 2012), which have been associated with brain development and better neurocognitive outcomes (Treyvaud et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2019). Furthermore, BMF can also be an indirect indicator of other parenting behaviours, which are independently associated with cognition (Horta et al., 2015), as higher maternal education and socioeconomic status are positively associated with the prevalence and duration of BMF in high‐income countries (Victora et al., 2016). Thus, the link between BMF and cognitive development may therefore be partly confounded by family and parenting characteristics (Horta et al., 2015).
Very preterm (VPT < 32 weeks of gestation) children experience a higher risk of short‐term and long‐term adverse health and developmental outcomes, including cerebral palsy, motor and cognitive delay (Ancel et al., 2015; Pierrat et al., 2017), and require more healthcare services (Seppänen et al., 2019). The importance of BMF on their neurodevelopment might be even more significant considering that VPT children are particularly vulnerable to nutrient deficits and growth faltering, presenting very specific nutritional needs compared with full‐term infants (Belfort, 2018; El Rafei et al., 2020; Koletzko et al., 2018). At the same time, preterm neonatal morbidities which affect cognition, such as brain lesions, may operate through pathways that may not be affected by the nutritional or immunological benefits of BMF. Because of these specificities, studies based on full‐term children cannot be generalized to this population (Twilhaar et al., 2018).
The evidence about the relationship between BMF and neurodevelopment in VPT infants is inconclusive, being an area of ongoing research (Koo et al., 2014; Lechner & Vohr, 2017; Miller et al., 2018). Some observational studies found an independent positive association between BMF and neurodevelopment (Beaino et al., 2011; Belfort et al., 2016; Gibertoni et al., 2020; Johnson et al., 2011; Rozé et al., 2012), whereas others found none (Furman et al., 2004; Jacobi‐Polishook et al., 2016; Pinelli et al., 2003). One randomized clinical trial of 363 very low birthweight (VLBW) infants found that the use of supplemental donor milk compared with preterm formula, as a supplement to mother's milk during hospitalization, did not improve neurodevelopment at 18 months' corrected age (CA; O'Connor et al., 2016), raising further questions about the differences between donor human milk and mother's own milk (Hård et al., 2019). Moreover, it remains unclear if prolonged BMF for more than 6 months has an influence on the neurodevelopmental outcome in VPT.
Recent systematic reviews and meta‐analysis based on children born VPT and/or VLBW revealed that different methodological approaches such as population inclusion criteria (according to gestational age and/or birthweight), age at assessment, BMF indicators/definitions (e.g., exclusive vs. any BMF; BMF considered during hospital stay, at or after hospital discharge; and different cutoffs for the duration), methods and instruments for assessing and classifying neurodevelopment, the cognitive abilities considered as outcome, as well as design limitations (e.g., sample size) make a direct comparison between studies difficult (Koo et al., 2014; Lechner & Vohr, 2017; Miller et al., 2018; Sentenac, Boutron, et al., 2020).
Taking into account the inconsistent findings and also the lack of data on the total duration of BMF in this vulnerable population, we aimed to investigate the association of BMF (mother's own milk) initiation and its duration with neurodevelopment, specifically on nonverbal and verbal cognition at 2 years of CA in a large prospective cohort of VPT children from 11 European countries.
2. METHODS
2.1. Data source
This study used data from the Effective Perinatal Intensive Care in Europe (EPICE) project, which recruited a geographically defined population‐based prospective cohort of VPT infants (22–31 completed weeks of gestational age), born in 19 regions from 11 European countries (Zeitlin et al., 2020). As previously described, data were collected during 12 consecutive months in 2011–2012, starting between March and July 2011 (Zeitlin et al., 2020), except for the three French regions where inclusions lasted 6 months as part of the study Etude Epidémiologique sur les Petits Ages Gestationnels (EPIPAGE)‐2 (Ancel et al., 2015).
Data on maternal, obstetric and infant characteristics, and on the neonatal course were retrieved from obstetrical and neonatal patient records using a pretested standardized questionnaire with common definitions filled in by medical staff until discharge from the hospital and later verified by the research team. At 2 years of CA, data on child's health and neurodevelopment outcomes, growth, health service utilization, BMF and sociodemographic characteristics were collected using a structured questionnaire completed by parents. This questionnaire was translated into the different national languages, backtranslated in English and pretested in all the regions (Zeitlin et al., 2020).
2.2. Study population
Of the 7900 VPT live births identified, 6792 infants were discharged alive (86.0%). Thirty‐one infants died before 2 years CA, leaving 6761 eligible infants for follow‐up at 2 years CA, and parents provided information for 4426 children (65.5%). As the response rate in the Northern Region in the United Kingdom was very low (27.2%, 103/379), this region was excluded from this analysis because of concern about a potentially biased sample, in accordance with other studies at 2 years CA in this cohort (Bonnet et al., 2019; Seppänen et al., 2019). After excluding the UK Northern region, follow‐up rates differed across countries, ranging from 47.2% in Belgium to 99.3% in Estonia. The final study sample included 4323 children (67.7% of those eligible; Figure S1).
2.3. Data on BMF (exposure)
Data on BMF was collected through the parental questionnaire at 2 years CA, considering only mother's own milk, independently of route of administration (nasogastric tube, bottle and/or directly at the breast). Infants were considered to have ever been breastfed if the answer was yes to the question ‘Was your child breast fed?’. BMF duration was defined as the age of the child, reported in months of chronological age, at which BMF was stopped completely, regardless of if it was exclusive or partial. We categorized the BMF duration into five groups: (1) never breastfed; (2) between >0 and <2 months; (3) between 2 and <4 months; (4) between 4 and <6 months; and (5) 6 months or more.
2.4. Data on nonverbal and verbal cognitive development (outcome)
Regarding our outcome of interest, nonverbal and verbal cognitive abilities were measured at 2 years CA through a validated developmental assessment tool filled in by parents, the Parent Report of Children's Abilities‐Revised (PARCA‐R), which includes the short version of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory for language abilities (Johnson et al., 2004, 2008). This tool has shown excellent test–retest reliability and correlation with the Mental Development Index of the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development‐II (Cuttini et al., 2012; Johnson et al., 2008). The full PARCA‐R, which includes a nonverbal and verbal score, could not be completed in countries without a translation of the MacArthur short form available (Belgium, Denmark, the Netherlands, Poland, Portugal and Sweden; Zeitlin et al., 2020). Therefore, we additionally included questions on language acquisition, which were asked in all regions (Does your child say as many as 10 words?; Does your child say as many as 50 words?; Has your child started to combine words into short sentences yet? (e.g., Mummy up, Daddy gone, I do it). The Ages & Stages Questionnaire (ASQ) was used in French regions as this instrument was validated in France, whereas the PARCA‐R was not (Flamant et al., 2011; Zeitlin et al., 2020). The problem‐solving component of the ASQ score was used as its content is closest to the PARCA‐R, as done in previous studies in the EPICE cohort (El Rafei et al., 2021; Wolf et al., 2021).
For this study, we categorized both non‐verbal and verbal cognition as a binary outcome, optimal and suboptimal. For nonverbal cognitive development, we established a cutoff of <22 for the nonverbal cognition score of the PARCA‐R and <29.78 for the ASQ problem‐solving domain, which corresponds to <2 SDs below the mean of the standardization sample to define suboptimal nonverbal cognition (Johnson et al., 2008). A negative response to the following question ‘Does your child say as many as 10 words?’ was considered as suboptimal verbal cognition, as this is an indicator of very low expressive vocabulary (<5th percentile) (Fenson et al., 2000).
2.5. Data on perinatal and sociodemographic characteristics (covariates)
Perinatal and sociodemographic characteristics were used to describe the sample and as covariates in the analyses. We included child sex, gestational age (in completed weeks), small for gestational age (SGA) defined as birthweight <10th percentile for gestational age (severe SGA as <3rd percentile and moderate SGA as 3rd to <10th percentile) using intrauterine references developed for the cohort (Zeitlin et al., 2017), any congenital anomaly (none and nonsevere vs. severe), bronchopulmonary dysplasia (BPD) defined as oxygen dependency or respiratory support at 36 weeks postmenstrual age, Intraventricular Haemorrhage Papille's grades III or IV (IVH III–IV), cystic periventricular leukomalacia (cPVL), Retinopathy of Prematurity grades III‐V (ROP III–V), and necrotizing enterocolitis (NEC) with surgery or peritoneal drainage. As defined previously for this cohort, we used a perinatal risk composite, considering three risk groups based on perinatal characteristics, which represents the overall risk of health and developmental problems at discharge from the neonatal unit (Seppänen et al., 2019) as follows: (1) Lower risk—birth at 30–31 weeks of gestation, not SGA, no congenital anomaly and without severe neonatal morbidities (BPD, IVH III–IV, cPVL, ROP III–V, NEC with surgery or peritoneal drainage); (2) Moderate risk—birth at 28–29 weeks' gestation or SGA, or any nonsevere congenital anomaly, but no severe neonatal morbidities; and (3) Higher risk—birth before 28 weeks of gestation or at least one severe neonatal morbidity (BPD, IVH III–IV, cPVL, ROP III–V, NEC with surgery or peritoneal drainage) or severe congenital anomaly.
We adjusted for CA at assessment (in months), categorized in three categories (<22 months CA, 22–26 months CA, and >26 months CA). The CA corresponds to the chronological age (when the questionnaire was filled by parents) minus the number of weeks that infant was born early. For example, a child with 24 months of chronological age who was born 3 months (12 weeks) early would have a CA of 21 months. Using infant's CA is particularly important for the development outcomes in children born VPT to give an accurate assessment of the developmental abilities and is recommended in the assessment of VPT development at 2 years of age (Gould et al., 2021).
We assessed maternal and sociodemographic characteristics, including mother's age at delivery (≤24, 25–34, or ≥35 years), mother's country of birth (native‐born, born in Europe and born outside Europe as in the UK data were available only on ethnicity), family situation at 2 years CA (mother living with partner vs. single caregiver or other family situation), parity at delivery and type of pregnancy (singleton vs. multiple). We also included the mother's highest achieved educational level collected at the 2 years CA based on the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED) 2011 definition and categorized in three groups, considering the cross‐national differences in educational systems: (1) lower level (ISCED Level 0–2: lower secondary); (2) intermediate level (ISCED Level 3–5: upper or postsecondary, nontertiary or short cycle tertiary); and (3) higher level (ISCED Level 6–8: bachelor degree or higher; UNESCO Institute for Statistics, 2012).
2.6. Statistical analysis
Responders and nonresponders' characteristics were compared considering all eligible infants for follow‐up at 2 years CA, excluding infants from UK Northern region (n = 6382). We calculated the Wald's test from the logistic regression adjusted for country because of differences in follow‐up rates. The effects of potential bias due to attrition were accounted for using the inverse probability weighting method in all analyses to give higher weight to children with characteristics of non‐responders to obtain a sample that reflects the full set of observations (Bonnet et al., 2019; Seaman & White, 2013; Seaman et al., 2012). The probability of follow‐up at 2 years CA (yes or no) was estimated with a multivariate logistic regression model including all variables available at baseline associated with nonresponse, as detailed elsewhere (Bonnet et al., 2019; Seppänen et al., 2020). The weight was computed using the total sample of eligible infants for follow‐up at 2 years CA (n = 6382). Before predicting the inverse probability weights, missing values from variables included in the regression model were imputed with multiple chained equations (20 complete data sets generated) to have a more stable weight (Seaman & White, 2013; Seaman et al., 2012). Data were assumed to be missing at random. All results presented in this study are based on weighted sample.
BMF initiation, as well as nonverbal and verbal cognitive development were described by the study sample characteristics and proportions compared performing Wald's test from the logistic regression adjusted for country, entered as a categorical variable. To estimate the association between BMF and nonverbal and verbal cognition (optimal vs. suboptimal), crude risk ratios (RR) and adjusted RRs (aRRs) with 95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) were estimated by fitting multilevel mixed‐effects generalized linear regression models with a log link, Poisson distribution and robust SEs. This model directly estimates the RRs and provides covariate aRRs and associated SEs for binary outcomes (Zou & Donner, 2013). RRs were preferred to odds ratios as suboptimal cognition is not a rare event in VPT infants (incidence of 10% or more) and our study used a prospective cohort design. We used multilevel models with random intercepts at the country and mother levels to consider the correlation of infants within countries and between siblings, respectively. To determine whether there is a dose–response relationship by BMF duration, we performed a test for trend.
Variables selected for the adjusted models were based on the scientific literature and previous findings from the EPICE cohort (Bonnet et al., 2019; Draper et al., 2020; Sentenac, Johnson, et al., 2020; Wilson et al., 2018), including characteristics that were related or plausibility related with both BMF and cognitive development (potential confounders). In addition, stratified analyses were performed to assess the association of BMF with nonverbal and verbal cognition by perinatal risk group (as an indicator of the overall risk of health and developmental problems) and maternal educational level (as an indicator of socioeconomic disadvantage), comparing the total crude RRs to the stratum‐specific RRs.
Statistical analyses were performed using STATA version 15.1 software (Stata Corporation).
3. RESULTS
Characteristics associated with loss to follow‐up at 2 years CA are provided in detail in Table S1. Children born from singleton pregnancies and to younger, multiparous and migrant women were less likely to be followed up. Children who received BMF at discharge were more likely to participate at 2 years CA.
Table 1 shows the infant and maternal characteristics of ever breastfed children (weighted distributions). We observed that proportions of never‐breastfed children were higher for younger, multipara, single and lower educational level mothers.
Table 1.
Any BMF, according to the infant, neonatal morbidity, maternal and socio‐demographic characteristics
Any breast milk feeding (n = 4305) | |||
---|---|---|---|
Never (n = 952; 23.1%) | Ever (n = 3353; 76.9%) | ||
Characteristics | n (%)a | n (%)a | p b |
Infant and neonatal morbidity | |||
Child sex | |||
Male | 504 (23.0) | 1775 (77.0) | 0.872 |
Female | 448 (23.1) | 1578 (76.9) | |
Gestational age at birth | |||
22–25 Weeks | 84 (26.1) | 239 (73.9) | 0.007 |
26–27 Weeks | 192 (27.2) | 563 (72.8) | |
28–29 weeks | 238 (20.7) | 906 (79.3) | |
30–31 Weeks | 438 (22.5) | 1645 (77.5) | |
SGA | |||
<3rd Percentile | 213 (23.9) | 683 (76.1) | 0.704 |
3rd to <10th Percentile | 115 (24.4) | 396 (75.6) | |
≥10th Percentile | 624 (22.6) | 2274 (77.4) | |
BPD | |||
No | 781 (22.2) | 2897 (77.8) | <0.001 |
Yes | 142 (27.1) | 388 (72.9) | |
ROP Stages III–V | |||
No | 883 (22.5) | 3213 (77.5) | <0.001 |
Yes | 60 (39.0) | 99 (61.0) | |
IVH III–IV/cPVL | |||
No | 871 (22.9) | 3133 (77.1) | 0.234 |
Yes | 70 (25.6) | 187 (74.4) | |
NEC needing surgery or peritoneal drainage | |||
No | 931 (23.0) | 3308 (77.0) | 0.037 |
Yes | 21 (30.6) | 45 (69.4) | |
Any congenital anomaly | |||
None | 850 (22.5) | 3093 (77.5) | 0.023 |
Nonsevere | 81 (27.1) | 231 (72.9) | |
Severe | 21 (46.6) | 28 (53.4) | |
Perinatal risk | |||
Lower | 222 (21.5) | 907 (78.5) | <0.001 |
Moderate | 326 (20.3) | 1301 (79.7) | |
Higher | 379 (27.4) | 1062 (72.6) | |
Mother and sociodemographic characteristics | |||
Mother's age at delivery | |||
≤24 Years | 159 (29.3) | 373 (70.7) | <0.001 |
25–34 Years | 508 (21.2) | 1998 (78.8) | |
≥35 Years | 282 (23.6) | 975 (76.4) | |
Parity at delivery | |||
0 Previous births | 508 (20.0) | 2078 (80.0) | <0.001 |
1 Previous birth | 248 (24.8) | 792 (75.2) | |
2 Or more previous births | 181 (29.5) | 454 (70.5) | |
Type of pregnancy | |||
Singleton | 680 (24.8) | 2201 (75.2) | <0.001 |
Multiple | 272 (19.3) | 1152 (80.7) | |
Mother's country of birth | |||
Native‐born | 714 (22.9) | 2563 (77.1) | 0.134 |
Born elsewhere in Europe | 51 (31.0) | 147 (69.0) | |
Born outside Europe | 131 (22.0) | 449 (78.0) | |
Mother's educational level | |||
Low level (ISCED 0–2) | 278 (34.7) | 558 (65.3) | <0.001 |
Intermediate level (ISCED 3–5) | 391 (22.6) | 1366 (77.4) | |
High level (ISCED 6–8) | 214 (14.3) | 1347 (85.7) | |
Family situation | |||
Mother living with partner | 812 (22.0) | 3026 (78.0) | <0.001 |
Single mother or other family situation | 102 (28.8) | 280 (71.2) | |
Country (region[s]) | |||
Belgium (Flanders) | 59 (21.7) | 246 (78.3) | <0.001 |
Denmark (Eastern) | 10 (6.2) | 170 (93.8) | |
Estonia (whole country) | 19 (13.8) | 119 (86.2) | |
France (Burgundy, Northern, Ile‐de‐France) | 357 (37.2) | 629 (62.8) | |
Germany (Hesse, Saarland) | 111 (30.7) | 319 (69.3) | |
Italy (Emilia, Lazio, Marche) | 183 (28.1) | 549 (71.9) | |
The Netherlands (East‐Central) | 29 (14.1) | 199 (85.9) | |
Poland (Wielkopolska) | 69 (36.8) | 130 (63.2) | |
Portugal (Lisbon, Northern) | 52 (15.1) | 350 (84.9) | |
Sweden (Stockholm) | 7 (5.0) | 156 (95.0) | |
UK (East Midlands, Yorkshire and Humber)c | 56 (14.3) | 486 (85.7) |
Abbreviations: BMF, breast milk feeding; BPD, bronchopulmonary dysplasia; CA, corrected age; cPVL, cystic periventricular leukomalacia; ISCED, International Standard Classification of Education 2011; IVH, intraventricular haemorrhage; NEC, necrotizing enterocolitis; ROP, retinopathy of prematurity; SGA, small for gestational age.
Proportions were calculated using inverse probability weights to account for nonresponse bias.
Wald's test from logistic regression adjusted for country.
UK Northern region was excluded from this analysis.
Of the 4092 children with available information, 16.0% presented suboptimal nonverbal cognitive development (weighted sample). This proportion varied among countries, with the lowest rate found in the Netherlands (10.6%) and the highest in Poland (24.8%; Table 2). For verbal cognition, 11.1% (n = 4134) of the children presented suboptimal ability (weighted sample) and this proportion ranged from 7.7% in Sweden to 19.1% in Poland (Table 2).
Table 2.
Nonverbal and verbal cognitive development, according to infant, neonatal morbidity, maternal and socio‐demographic characteristics
Nonverbal cognition (n = 4092) | Verbal cognition (n = 4134) | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Optimal (n = 3474; 84.0%) | Suboptimal (n = 618; 16.0%) | Optimal (n = 3691; 89.9%) | Suboptimal (n = 443; 11.1%) | |||
Characteristics | n (%)a | n (%)a | p b | n (%)a | n (%) a | p b |
Infant and neonatal morbidity | ||||||
Child sex | ||||||
Male | 1775 (81.1) | 388 (18.9) | <0.001 | 1868 (85.9) | 300 (14.1) | <0.001 |
Female | 1699 (87.4) | 230 (12.6) | 1823 (92.3) | 143 (7.7) | ||
Gestational age at birth | ||||||
22–25 Weeks | 229 (72.3) | 83 (27.7) | <0.001 | 244 (76.8) | 68 (23.2) | <0.001 |
26–27 Weeks | 576 (80.0) | 128 (20.0) | 599 (84.2) | 108 (15.8) | ||
28–29 Weeks | 930 (83.8) | 165 (16.2) | 1016 (91.0) | 93 (9.0) | ||
30–31 Weeks | 1739 (87.3) | 242 (12.7) | 1832 (91.3) | 174 (8.7) | ||
SGA | ||||||
<3rd Percentile | 697 (80.9) | 155 (19.1) | 0.013 | 727 (84.5) | 126 (15.5) | <0.001 |
3rd to <10th Percentile | 425 (86.1) | 66 (13.9) | 442 (89.9) | 47 (10.1) | ||
≥10th Percentile | 2352 (84.6) | 397 (15.4) | 2522 (90.1) | 270 (9.9) | ||
BPD | ||||||
No | 3025 (85.5) | 473 (14.5) | <0.001 | 3207 (90.4) | 328 (9.6) | <0.001 |
Yes | 368 (73.0) | 135 (27.0) | 399 (78.2) | 110 (21.8) | ||
ROP Stages III–V | ||||||
No | 3334 (85.7) | 556 (14.3) | <0.001 | 3529 (89.5) | 402 (10.5) | <0.001 |
Yes | 100 (64.5) | 55 (35.5) | 117 (73.9) | 39 (26.1) | ||
IVH III–IV/cPVL | ||||||
No | 3286 (85.8) | 524 (14.2) | <0.001 | 3464 (89.9) | 387 (10.1) | <0.001 |
Yes | 153 (59.4) | 87 (40.6) | 188 (74.7) | 51 (25.3) | ||
NEC needing surgery or peritoneal drainage | ||||||
No | 3433 (84.3) | 595 (15.7) | <0.001 | 3638 (89.0) | 432 (11.0) | 0.251 |
Yes | 41 (66.3) | 23 (33.7) | 53 (83.8) | 11 (16.2) | ||
Any congenital anomaly | ||||||
None | 3207 (84.6) | 544 (15.4) | 0.025 | 3407 (89.5) | 383 (10.5) | 0.004 |
Nonsevere | 231 (78.0) | 63 (22.0) | 248 (83.0) | 49 (17.0) | ||
Severe | 35 (77.4) | 11 (22.6) | 35 (78.3) | 11 (21.7) | ||
Perinatal risk | ||||||
Lower | 970 (89.9) | 104 (10.1) | <0.001 | 1023 (93.4) | 72 (6.6) | <0.001 |
Moderate | 1348 (86.0) | 208 (14.0) | 1431 (91.2) | 138 (8.8) | ||
Higher | 1064 (76.7) | 296 (23.3) | 1138 (83.3) | 228 (16.7) | ||
Corrected age at assessment | ||||||
≤21 Months | 41 (83.9) | 8 (16.1) | <0.001 | 44 (80.7) | 11 (19.3) | 0.052 |
22–26 Months | 3053 (83.1) | 579 (16.9) | 3211 (88.8) | 389 (11.2) | ||
≥27 Months | 366 (92.8) | 29 (7.2) | 419 (90.6) | 43 (9.4) | ||
Mother and socio‐demographic characteristics | ||||||
Mother's age at delivery | ||||||
≤24 Years | 398 (82.4) | 81 (17.6) | 0.556 | 427 (86.6) | 66 (13.4) | 0.518 |
25–34 Years | 2027 (83.9) | 363 (16.1) | 2167 (89.9) | 244 (10.1) | ||
≥35 Years | 1043 (85.3) | 172 (14.7) | 1092 (89.5) | 128 (10.5) | ||
Parity at delivery | ||||||
0 Previous births | 2131 (85.8) | 340 (14.2) | <0.001 | 2252 (90.3) | 241 (9.7) | 0.010 |
1 Previous birth | 830 (83.6) | 158 (16.4) | 880 (88.1) | 119 (11.9) | ||
2 Or more previous births | 476 (78.4) | 117 (21.6) | 522 (86.7) | 80 (13.3) | ||
Type of pregnancy | ||||||
Singleton | 2297 (83.2) | 436 (16.8) | 0.075 | 2461 (89.9) | 296 (11.1) | 0.923 |
Multiple | 1177 (85.7) | 182 (14.3) | 1230 (89.0) | 147 (11.0) | ||
Mother's country of birth | ||||||
Native‐born | 2681 (84.6) | 467 (15.4) | 0.139 | 2888 (90.1) | 308 (9.9) | 0.003 |
Born elsewhere in Europe | 162 (84.4) | 28 (15.6) | 156 (87.5) | 26 (12.5) | ||
Born outside Europe | 430 (81.1) | 91 (18.9) | 440 (84.4) | 81 (15.6) | ||
Mother's educational level | ||||||
Low level (ISCED 0–2) | 646 (82.4) | 130 (17.6) | 0.006 | 673 (85.1) | 118 (14.9) | <0.001 |
Intermediate level (ISCED 3–5) | 1420 (83.2) | 268 (16.8) | 1532 (89.9) | 172 (10.1) | ||
High level (ISCED 6–8) | 1316 (86.0) | 200 (13.8) | 1389 (91.3) | 132 (8.7) | ||
Family situation | ||||||
Mother living with partner | 3154 (84.5) | 550 (15.5) | 0.115 | 3345 (89.6) | 384 (10.4) | 0.024 |
Single mother or other family situation | 297 (80.8) | 59 (19.2) | 319 (85.1) | 51 (14.9) | ||
Country (region[s]) | ||||||
Belgium (Flanders) | 246 (80.9) | 57 (19.1) | 0.001 | 273 (90.1) | 26 (9.9) | 0.009 |
Denmark (Eastern) | 152 (85.2) | 27 (14.8) | 162 (90.6) | 17 (9.4) | ||
Estonia (whole country) | 123 (89.1) | 15 (10.9) | 126 (91.3) | 12 (8.7) | ||
France (Burgundy, Northern, Ile‐de‐France) | 734 (86.2) | 116 (13.8) | 729 (86.7) | 110 (13.3) | ||
Germany (Hesse, Saarland) | 376 (86.6) | 53 (13.4) | 397 (90.5) | 36 (9.5) | ||
Italy (Emilia, Lazio, Marche) | 624 (84.9) | 107 (15.1) | 649 (89.1) | 80 (10.9) | ||
The Netherlands (East‐Central) | 206 (89.4) | 21 (10.6) | 205 (90.2) | 20 (9.8) | ||
Poland (Wielkopolska) | 149 (75.2) | 48 (24.8) | 162 (80.9) | 37 (19.1) | ||
Portugal (Lisbon, Northern) | 344 (85.0) | 60 (15.0) | 362 (91.6) | 34 (8.4) | ||
Sweden (Stockholm) | 130 (79.9) | 33 (20.1) | 151 (92.3) | 13 (7.7) | ||
UK (East Midlands, Yorkshire and Humber)c | 390 (81.1) | 81 (18.9) | 475 (87.7) | 58 (12.3) |
Abbreviations: BPD, bronchopulmonary dysplasia; CA, corrected age; cPVL, cystic periventricular leukomalacia; ISCED, International Standard Classification of Education 2011; IVH, intraventricular haemorrhage; NEC, necrotising enterocolitis; ROP, retinopathy of prematurity; SGA, small for gestational age.
Proportions were calculated using inverse probability weights to account for nonresponse bias.
Wald's test from logistic regression adjusted for country.
UK Northern region was excluded from this analysis.
Suboptimal nonverbal and verbal ability at 2 years CA were more frequent in males, children born before 28 weeks of gestational age, and at higher perinatal risk (Table 2). When mothers were born outside Europe and with lower educational levels, the proportions of children with suboptimal non‐verbal and verbal cognition were higher.
As demonstrated in Table 3, never‐breastfed children faced a higher risk of suboptimal nonverbal cognition than those who had ever been breastfed (RR = 1.48, 95% CI: 1.11–1.98). Similarly, when stratified by perinatal risk group, never BMF was associated with an increased risk in the three groups, with a gradient from the lower to higher risk groups (Lower: RR = 1.02, 95% CI: 0.58–1.78; Moderate: RR = 1.21, 95% CI: 0.97–1.51; Higher: RR = 1.57, 95% CI: 1.12–2.18; Table S2). The stratified analysis by maternal educational level found that never BMF was associated with an increased risk of suboptimal non‐verbal cognition in all groups, with a significant association in the intermediate level group (RR = 1.55, 95% CI: 1.01–2.38; Table S3). Overall, after adjustment for sex, CA at assessment, and perinatal risk (Model 1), never BMF remained associated with an increased risk for suboptimal nonverbal cognition when compared with ever breastfed children (aRR = 1.39, 95% CI: 1.11–1.75; Table 3). Similarly, when adjusted only for sociodemographic characteristics (maternal age, parity, type of pregnancy, mother's country of birth, mother's educational level and family situation), without perinatal risk included in the model, children who were never breastfed presented 1.40 times more risk to have suboptimal non‐verbal cognition (95% CI: 1.13–1.72). This effect persisted after taking into consideration simultaneously perinatal, maternal and sociodemographic characteristics (aRR = 1.29, 95% CI: 1.09–1.53; Table 3, Model 2). The association between BMF duration and non‐verbal cognition was stronger among shorter duration categories when compared with infants breastfed until 6 months or more, with a dose–response relationship.
Table 3.
Prevalence and RRs for nonverbal cognitive development, by BMF initiation and duration
Nonverbal cognitive development | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Optimal | Suboptimal | Crude RRb | Adjusted RRb (95% CI) | ||
n (%)a | n (%)a | (95% CI) | Model 1 | Model 2 | |
BMF initiation | (n = 4075) | (n = 3959) | (n = 3592) | ||
Never breastfed | 713 (79.8) | 170 (20.2) | 1.48 (1.11–1.98) | 1.39 (1.11–1.75) | 1.29 (1.09–1.53) |
Ever breastfed | 2749 (85.4) | 443 (14.6) | Reference | Reference | Reference |
BMF duration | (n = 3870) | (n = 3765) | (n = 3422) | ||
Never breastfed | 713 (79.8) | 170 (20.2) | 1.58 (1.17–2.13) | 1.52 (1.17–1.97) | 1.42 (1.09–1.85) |
>0 to <2 Months | 379 (82.8) | 72 (17.2) | 1.26 (0.80–1.98) | 1.49 (0.98–2.26) | 1.44 (0.89–2.34) |
2 to <4 Months | 761 (86.4) | 115 (13.6) | 0.97 (0.74–1.27) | 0.97 (0.75–1.26) | 1.01 (0.74–1.37) |
4 to <6 Months | 485 (86.5) | 73 (13.5) | 0.99 (0.71–1.38) | 0.94 (0.68–1.31) | 0.95 (0.63–1.43) |
≥6 Months | 959 (86.4) | 143 (13.6) | Reference | Reference | Reference |
p | 0.007 | <0.001 | 0.003 |
Note: Model 1: Adjusted for child sex, CA at assessment and perinatal risk. Model 2: Adjusted for child sex, CA at assessment, perinatal risk, maternal age, parity, type of pregnancy, mother's country of birth, mother's educational level and family situation.
Abbreviations: BMF, breast milk feeding (mother's own milk); CA, corrected age; CI, confidence interval; RR, risk ratio.
Proportions were calculated using inverse probability weights to account for nonresponse bias.
RRs were derived from the weighted sample (inverse probability of participating at the 2 years CA follow‐up), multilevel mixed‐effects generalized linear regression models with random intercepts at the country and mother level.
As shown in Table 4, we observed that children who were never breastfed presented 1.73 times more risk to have suboptimal verbal cognition (95% CI: 1.19–2.52). The stratified analysis by perinatal risk found that never BMF was associated with an increased risk of suboptimal verbal cognition in the three risk groups, with a significant association in the moderate risk group (RR = 2.17, 95% CI: 1.23–3.82; Table S2). In the same way, never BMF was also associated with an increased risk in all maternal educational level groups, with a significant association in both lower and intermediate groups (RR = 1.70, 95% CI: 1.02–2.82; RR = 1.76, 95% CI: 1.09–2.84, respectively; Table S3). Overall, this increased risk for never‐breastfed children remained significant after adjustment for sex, CA at assessment and perinatal risk (aRR = 1.59, 95% CI: 1.16–2.19; Table 4, Model 1). When adjusted only for sociodemographic characteristics (without perinatal risk included in the model), children who were never breastfed presented 1.59 times more risk to have suboptimal verbal cognition (95% CI: 1.13–2.24). After incorporating perinatal, maternal and sociodemographic characteristics (Model 2), significant differences persisted, even with slight changes in point estimates (aRR = 1.45, 95% CI: 1.09–1.92). We estimated crude RRs and aRRs considering the BMF duration and it was possible to verify a dose–response relationship (Table 4).
Table 4.
Prevalence and RRs for verbal cognitive development, by BMF initiation and duration
Verbal cognitive development | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Optimal | Suboptimal | Crude RRb | Adjusted RRb (95% CI) | ||
n (%)a | n (%)a | (95% CI) | Model 1 | Model 2 | |
BMF initiation | (n = 4118) | (n = 3999) | (n = 3625) | ||
Never breastfed | 753 (84.3) | 134 (15.7) | 1.73 (1.19‐2.52) | 1.59 (1.16‐2.19) | 1.45 (1.09‐1.92) |
Ever breastfed | 2926 (90.4) | 305 (9.6) | Reference | Reference | Reference |
BMF duration | (n = 3894) | (n = 3787) | (n = 3440) | ||
Never breastfed | 753 (84.3) | 134 (15.7) | 1.78 (1.23–2.59) | 1.65 (1.19–2.31) | 1.50 (1.12–2.00) |
>0 to <2 Months | 416 (89.4) | 47 (10.6) | 1.18 (0.77–1.79) | 1.42 (1.00–2.03) | 1.21 (0.93–1.59) |
2 to <4 Months | 798 (91.3) | 77 (8.7) | 0.93 (0.78–1.10) | 0.92 (0.76–1.13) | 0.97 (0.74–1.26) |
4 to <6 Months | 516 (92.8) | 43 (7.2) | 0.80 (0.50–1.27) | 0.72 (0.46–1.12) | 0.80 (0.49–1.31) |
≥6 Months | 1010 (90.8) | 100 (9.2) | Reference | Reference | Reference |
p | 0.017 | 0.005 | 0.011 |
Note: Model 1: Adjusted for child sex, CA at assessment and perinatal risk. Model 2: Adjusted for child sex, CA at assessment, perinatal risk, maternal age, parity, type of pregnancy, mother's country of birth, mother's educational level and family situation.
Abbreviations: BMF, Breast milk feeding (mother's own milk); CA, corrected age; CI, confidence interval; RR, Risk ratio.
Proportions were calculated using inverse probability weights to account for nonresponse bias.
RRs were derived from the weighted sample (inverse probability of participating at the 2 years CA follow‐up), multilevel mixed‐effects generalized linear regression models with random intercepts at the country and mother level.
A sensitivity analysis was carried out considering only children hospitalized in neonatal intensive care units (NICUs) without human bank milk/donor milk available. After adjustment for potential confounding factors, the risk was 1.70 (95% CI: 1.46–1.97) for nonverbal and 1.78 (95% CI: 1.49–2.14) for verbal suboptimal cognitive development, compared with those ever breastfed.
4. DISCUSSION
In a population‐based cohort of children born VPT in Europe, we found that 16% and 11% of the children presented suboptimal nonverbal and verbal cognitive development, respectively. Never‐breastfed children faced a higher risk to have suboptimal cognition than those who had ever been breastfed, independently of perinatal and sociodemographic characteristics. When considering BMF duration, we observed a dose–response relationship with higher risks among short durations compared with longer durations.
Our findings support previous research indicating that BMF has an independent association with neurodevelopment for VPT children (Beaino et al., 2011; Gibertoni et al., 2020; Johnson et al., 2011; Rozé et al., 2012). Two independent cohorts of VPT infants in France, Loire Infant Follow‐up Team and Etude Epidémiologique sur les Petits Ages Gestationnels (EPIPAGE), showed that any BMF at discharge was associated with a reduction in the odds of suboptimal neurodevelopment at 2 and at 5 years of age, respectively (Rozé et al., 2012). Also, in the EPIPAGE cohort, it was observed that any BMF at discharge was highly associated with lower odds of mild and severe cognitive deficiencies at 5 years (Beaino et al., 2011). Another study from the United Kingdom and Ireland, using data from the EPICure cohort, revealed that receiving any breast milk was a protective factor for reading scores at 11 years (Johnson et al., 2011). Recently, a study from Italy demonstrated that exclusively formula‐fed infants at discharge from the NICU had significantly lower neurodevelopment trajectories from 3 to 24 months of CA compared with those receiving exclusively human milk or mixed (Gibertoni et al., 2020). Data obtained from the United Kingdom Millennium cohort at 5 years showed a significant difference in mean score between children who were breastfed and who were never breastfed, after adjustment for potential confounders: in term children (≥37 weeks of gestation), a two‐point increase in score for picture similarities (breastfed ≥ 4 months) and naming vocabulary (breastfed ≥6 months); in preterm children (28–36 weeks of gestation), a four‐point increase for naming vocabulary (breastfed ≥4 months) and picture similarities (breastfed ≥2 months), and a six‐point increase for pattern construction (breastfed ≥2 months; Quigley et al., 2012).
The mechanisms linking BMF to neurodevelopment are still unclear, and even more so in VPT infants. Cognitive development is a complex multifactorial issue for VPT infants, depending on biological, clinical and environmental events (Beaino et al., 2011; Draper et al., 2020; Gibertoni et al., 2020; Linsell et al., 2015; Sentenac, Johnson, et al., 2020; Twilhaar et al., 2018) and BMF practices reflect many of these factors (Bonnet et al., 2019). A systematic review performed to identify the factors for poor cognitive development in children born VPT or VLBW found that the influence of perinatal risk factors on cognitive development appears to diminish over time as other social and environmental factors become more important (Linsell et al., 2015). In our study, although risk (Model 1 the RRs decreased consecutively when we adjusted for perinatal) and additionally for maternal and sociodemographic characteristics (Model 2), never BMF remained associated with a significantly increased risk for suboptimal nonverbal (RR = 1.29) and verbal (RR = 1.45) cognition comparing with those ever breastfed. This result is particularly important for clinical practice highlighting the crucial role of BMF as a minimum standard for newborn care, being a tool to mitigate the adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes among these vulnerable infants. Studies of BMF practices in neonatal units, which are known to be effective for supporting VPT mothers, show there is a large margin for improvement to promote BMF in many countries (Cuttini et al., 2019; Rodrigues et al., 2018; Wilson et al., 2018).
Acting on modifiable risk factors that interfere with optimal neurodevelopment, such as those related to BMF, is important because despite many advances in perinatal care in the last decades, adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes among children born VPT are not declining over time (Pascal et al., 2018; Pierrat et al., 2017; Twilhaar et al., 2018). A meta‐analysis from Blencowe et al. (2013) based on articles with a median birth year of 2000 or later, estimated that 52% of extremely preterm (born at <28 weeks) and 24% of VPT infants (born at 28–31 weeks) develop a certain degree of some neurodevelopmental impairment. A recent meta‐analysis of 71 studies showed a large (0.86 SD) difference in intelligence scores between VPT children and full‐term controls at age 5 years or older, in children born between 1990 and 2008 (Twilhaar et al., 2018). Pascal et al. (2018) estimated a pooled prevalence of cognitive delay of 16.9% (95% CI: 10.4–26.3) in VPT or VLBW infants born after 2006 and evaluated from 18 months CA until 6 years. Pooled estimates of five meta‐analyses affirmed the robust conclusion that VPT birth is associated with a lower IQ, compared with full‐term children (Sentenac, Boutron, et al., 2020).
4.1. Strengths and limitations
The strengths of this study include the use of a large population‐based prospective cohort, its geographic spread and range of sociocultural contexts, and standardized data collection tools, with common definitions, across 11 European countries. Although a developmental assessment done by a trained professional is typically considered the gold standard, we used the PARCA‐R, which is a cost‐efficient, accurate, norm‐referenced and standardized measure of children's development at 2 years of age (Johnson et al., 2008, 2019). PARCA‐R has concurrent validity with examiner‐administered developmental tests and excellent test–retest reliability (Cuttini et al., 2012; Johnson et al., 2004, 2008). However, it may be more vulnerable to misclassification error as parents may over or under‐report child achievements.
Another strength of our study is the information available on the duration of BMF until the age of 2 years CA. To the best of our knowledge, there is not any published study from contemporary cohorts of children born VPT (<32 weeks) reporting the association between BMF duration and neurodevelopment outcomes. Most previous studies focused on BMF at time of discharge (any BMF vs. exclusive formula) or during hospitalization period (ever vs. never), all of them with smaller samples sizes and being from single country‐cohorts (Beaino et al., 2011; Gibertoni et al., 2020; Johnson et al., 2011; Rozé et al., 2012). In addition, we used both nonverbal and verbal cognitive development as outcomes, reinforcing the impact of our results. However, for verbal cognition we used a question about vocabulary production, reflecting a very low expressive vocabulary (<10 words), as a validated tool for assessing language was not available for all countries (Zeitlin et al., 2020).
Our study also has other limitations. About 35% of the infants discharged alive were not followed up at 2 years CA. Nevertheless, we were able to compare responders with nonresponders using data at baseline and took into consideration potential bias related to cohort attrition by using inverse probability weighting methods. Sensitivity analyses showed that results were not substantially affected by weighting for nonresponse (data not presented), as observed previously in the EPICE cohort (Bonnet et al., 2019; Seppänen et al., 2020).
In addition, we did not collect detailed infant feeding data, namely, we have no information about the total duration of exclusive BMF, type of formula used and route of BMF administration. The duration of BMF was collected retrospectively when the child was 2 years CA, which may lead to some potential recall bias, although previous studies reported good validity of maternal recall of BMF duration (Amissah et al., 2017). Also, this potential recall bias was mitigated by verifying the data reported by parents with the information on the type of feeding at discharge from hospital abstracted from clinical records, which made it possible to validate partially this information, in accordance with another study already published using EPICE data at 2 years CA (Bonnet et al., 2019).
In the EPICE cohort, 34% of units reported using human bank milk/donor milk for all VPT infants when the mother's own milk was not available (Rodrigues et al., 2021). Thus, within children classified as never breastfed (mother's own milk), some of them may have received donor milk. A sensitivity analysis was carried out considering only children hospitalized in NICUs without human bank milk/donor milk available and we found that the association was even stronger for both non‐verbal and verbal suboptimal cognitive development, compared with those ever breastfed.
Finally, maternal intelligence, which is an important potential confounder of the association between BMF and cognitive development, was not measured. However, in a recent meta‐analysis of studies that controlled for maternal intelligence, BMF remained associated with a gain in performance in IQ testing (Horta et al., 2015).
5. CONCLUSION
BMF is associated with both non‐verbal and verbal cognitive development of VPT children at 2 years CA, independently of perinatal and sociodemographic characteristics. The most important conclusion is that any amount of BMF is better than none. Considering that any intervention that has the potential to increase cognitive ability among these most vulnerable children is a significant window of opportunity, our results reinforce the importance of specifically targeted interventions to protect, promote and support BMF in NICUs and after discharge. Further research is needed to understand the impact of BMF initiation and continuation at long‐term neurodevelopmental outcomes in this vulnerable population.
EPICE (EFFECTIVE PERINATAL INTENSIVE CARE IN EUROPE) RESEARCH GROUP
BELGIUM: Flanders (E. Martens, G. Martens, P. Van Reempts); DENMARK: Eastern Region (K. Boerch, A. Hasselager, L. Huusom, O. Pryds, T. Weber); ESTONIA (L. Toome, H. Varendi); FRANCE: Burgundy, Ile‐de France and Northern Region (P. Y. Ancel, B. Blondel, A. Burguet, P. H. Jarreau, P. Truffert); GERMANY: Hesse (R. F. Maier, B. Misselwitz, S. Schmidt), Saarland (L. Gortner); ITALY: Emilia Romagna (D. Baronciani, G. Gargano), Lazio (R. Agostino, D. DiLallo, F. Franco), Marche (V. Carnielli), M. Cuttini; NETHERLANDS: Eastern & Central (C. Koopman‐Esseboom, A. Van Heijst, J. Nijman); POLAND: Wielkopolska (J. Gadzinowski, J. Mazela); PORTUGAL: Lisbon and Tagus Valley (L. M. Graça, M. C. Machado), Northern region (C. Rodrigues, T. Rodrigues), H. Barros; SWEDEN: Stockholm (A. K. Bonamy, M. Norman, E. Wilson); UK: East Midlands and Yorkshire and Humber (E. Boyle, E. S. Draper, B. N. Manktelow), Northern Region (A. C. Fenton, DWA Milligan); INSERM, Paris (J. Zeitlin, M. Bonet, A. Piedvache).
CONFLICTS OF INTEREST
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
ETHICAL APPROVALS
Informed consent was obtained from all parents or legal representatives included in the follow‐up study. Ethical approvals were obtained in each country as required by national legislation. The European study was also approved by the French Advisory Committee on Use of Health Data in Medical Research and the French National Commission for Data Protection and Liberties.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Study concept and design were done by Carina Rodrigues, Jennifer Zeitlin and Henrique Barros. All authors (including authors listed in EPICE Research Group) contributed to the acquisition, analysis or interpretation of data. The first draft of the manuscript was done by Carina Rodrigues. Also, the statistical analysis was done by Carina Rodrigues, supervised by Jennifer Zeitlin and Henrique Barros. Lastly, the critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content and approval of the final version of the manuscript was done by all authors (including authors listed in EPICE Research Group).
Supporting information
Supporting information.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors are grateful to the parents and infants enrolled in the EPICE cohort and acknowledge all members of the staff in the Departments of Obstetrics and Neonatology from the hospitals in the EPICE regions. The research received funding from the European Union's Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007‐2013) under grant agreement number 259882 and received funding from the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under grant agreement number 633724. Additional funding is acknowledged from the following regions: France (French Institute of Public Health Research/Institute of Public Health and its partners the French Health Ministry, the National Institute of Health and Medical Research, the National Institute of Cancer and the National Solidarity Fund for Autonomy; grant ANR‐11‐EQPX‐0038 from the National Research Agency through the French Equipex Programme of Investments in the Future; and the PremUp Foundation); Germany (PRIMAL Consortium, German Ministry for Education and Research); Poland (2012–2015 allocation of funds for international projects from the Polish Ministry of Science and Higher Education); Sweden (Stockholm County Council [ALF‐project and Clinical Research Appointment] and by the Department of Neonatal Medicine, Karolinska University Hospital), and UK (funding for The Neonatal Survey from Neonatal Networks for East Midlands and Yorkshire and the Humber regions). This study received additional funding from PESSOA Programme (2017–2018)—Hubert Curien Programme, under the Transnational cooperation between Portugal (FCT) and France (CAMPUS FRANCE). This study was also funded by national funding from the Foundation for Science and Technology—FCT, under the Unidade de Investigação em Epidemiologia–Instituto de Saúde Pública da Universidade do Porto (EPIUnit) (UIDB/04750/2020); the PhD Grant SFRH/BD/111794/2015 (Carina Rodrigues) was cofunded by the FCT and the POCH/FSE Program.
Rodrigues, C. , Zeitlin, J. , Zemlin, M. , Wilson, E. , Pedersen, P. , Barros, H. , & Effective Perinatal Intensive Care in Europe (EPICE) Research Group (2022). Never‐breastfed children face a higher risk of suboptimal cognition at 2 years of corrected age: A multinational cohort of very preterm children. Maternal & Child Nutrition, 18, e13347. 10.1111/mcn.13347
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
Data available on request due to privacy/ethical restrictions.
REFERENCES
- Agostoni, C. (2008). Role of long‐chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in the first year of life. Journal of Pediatrics Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 47(Suppl 2), S41–S44. 10.1097/01.mpg.0000338811.52062.b2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amissah, E. A. , Kancherla, V. , Ko, Y. A. , & Li, R. (2017). Validation study of maternal recall on breastfeeding duration 6 years after childbirth. Journal of Human Lactation, 33(2), 390–400. 10.1177/0890334417691506 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ancel, P. Y. , Goffinet, F. , EPIPAGE‐ Writing, G. , Kuhn, P. , Langer, B. , Matis, J. , Hernandorena, X. , Chabanier, P. , Joly‐Pedespan, L. , Lecomte, B. , Vendittelli, F. , Dreyfus, M. , Guillois, B. , Burguet, A. , Sagot, P. , Sizun, J. , Beuchée, A. , Rouget, F. , Favreau, A. , … Kaminski, M. (2015). Survival and morbidity of preterm children born at 22 through 34 weeks' gestation in France in 2011: Results of the EPIPAGE‐2 cohort study. JAMA Pediatrics, 169(3), 230–238. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.3351 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beaino, G. , Khoshnood, B. , Kaminski, M. , Marret, S. , Pierrat, V. , Vieux, R. , Thiriez, G. , Matis, J. , Picaud, J. C. , Rozé, J. C. , Alberge, C. , Larroque, B. , Bréart, G. , & Ancel, P. Y. , EPIPAGE Study Group . (2011). Predictors of the risk of cognitive deficiency in very preterm infants: The EPIPAGE prospective cohort. Acta Paediatrica, 100(3), 370–378. 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2010.02064.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belfort, M. B. (2018). Human milk and preterm infant brain development. Breastfeeding Medicine: The Official Journal of the Academy of Breastfeeding Medicine, 13(S1), S23–S25. 10.1089/bfm.2018.29079.mbb [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belfort, M. B. , Anderson, P. J. , Nowak, V. A. , Lee, K. J. , Molesworth, C. , Thompson, D. K. , Doyle, L. W. , & Inder, T. E. (2016). Breast milk feeding, brain development, and neurocognitive outcomes: A 7‐year longitudinal study in infants born at less than 30 weeks' gestation. Journal of Pediatrics, 177, 133–139. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.06.045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blencowe, H. , Lee, A. C. , Cousens, S. , Bahalim, A. , Narwal, R. , Zhong, N. , Chou, D. , Say, L. , Modi, N. , Katz, J. , Vos, T. , Marlow, N. , & Lawn, J. E. (2013). Preterm birth‐associated neurodevelopmental impairment estimates at regional and global levels for 2010. Pediatric Research, 74(Suppl 1), 17–34. 10.1038/pr.2013.204 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonnet, C. , Blondel, B. , Piedvache, A. , Wilson, E. , Bonamy, A. E. , Gortner, L. , Rodrigues, C. , van Heijst, A. , Draper, E. S. , Cuttini, M. , & Zeitlin, J. , EPICE Research Group . (2019). Low breastfeeding continuation to 6 months for very preterm infants: A European multiregional cohort study. Maternal and Child Nutrition, 15(1), e12657. 10.1111/mcn.12657 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cuttini, M. , Ferrante, P. , Mirante, N. , Chiandotto, V. , Fertz, M. , Dall'Oglio, A. M. , Coletti, M. F. , & Johnson, S. (2012). Cognitive assessment of very preterm infants at 2‐year corrected age: Performance of the Italian version of the PARCA‐R parent questionnaire. Early Human Development, 88(3), 159–163. 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.07.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cuttini, M. , Croci, I. , Toome, L. , Rodrigues, C. , Wilson, E. , Bonet, M. , Gadzinowski, J. , Di Lallo, D. , Herich, L. C. , & Zeitlin, J. , EPICE Research Group . (2019). Breastfeeding outcomes in European NICUs: Impact of parental visiting policies. Archives of Disease in Childhood. Fetal and Neonatal Edition, 104(2), F151–F158. 10.1136/archdischild-2017-314723 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Draper, E. S. , Zeitlin, J. , Manktelow, B. N. , Piedvache, A. , Cuttini, M. , Edstedt Bonamy, A. K. , Maier, R. , Koopman‐Esseboom, C. , Gadzinowski, J. , Boerch, K. , van Reempts, P. , Varendi, H. , & Johnson, S. J. , EPICE Group . (2020). EPICE cohort: Two‐year neurodevelopmental outcomes after very preterm birth. Archives of Disease in Childhood. Fetal and Neonatal Edition, 105(4), 350–356. 10.1136/archdischild-2019-317418 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- El Rafei, R. , Jarreau, P. H. , Norman, M. , Maier, R. F. , Barros, H. , Reempts, P. V. , Pedersen, P. , Cuttini, M. , & Zeitlin, J. , EPICE Research Group . (2020). Variation in very preterm extrauterine growth in a European multicountry cohort. Archives of Disease in Childhood. Fetal and Neonatal Edition, 106, 316–323. 10.1136/archdischild-2020-319946 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- El Rafei, R. , Jarreau, P. H. , Norman, M. , Maier, R. F. , Barros, H. , Van Reempts, P. , Pedersen, P. , Cuttini, M. , Costa, R. , Zemlin, M. , Draper, E. S. , & Zeitlin, J. , EPICE Research Group . (2021). Association between postnatal growth and neurodevelopmental impairment by sex at 2 years of corrected age in a multi‐national cohort of very preterm children. Clinical Nutrition, 40(8), 4948–4955. 10.1016/j.clnu.2021.07.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fenson, L. , Pethick, S. , Renda, C. , Cox, J. L. , Dale, P. S. , & Reznick, J. S. (2000). Short‐form versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21(1), 95–116. 10.1017/S0142716400001053 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Flamant, C. , Branger, B. , Nguyen The Tich, S. , de la Rochebrochard, E. , Savagner, C. , Berlie, I. , & Roze, J. C. (2011). Parent‐completed developmental screening in premature children: a valid tool for follow‐up programs. PLoS ONE, 6(5), e20004. 10.1371/journal.pone.0020004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Furman, L. , Wilson‐Costello, D. , Friedman, H. , Taylor, H. G. , Minich, N. , & Hack, M. (2004). The effect of neonatal maternal milk feeding on the neurodevelopmental outcome of very low birth weight infants. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 25(4), 247–253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibertoni, D. , Sansavini, A. , Savini, S. , Locatelli, C. , Ancora, G. , Perrone, E. , Ialonardi, M. , Rucci, P. , Fantini, M. P. , Faldella, G. , & Corvaglia, L. (2020). Neurodevelopmental trajectories of preterm infants of Italian native‐born and migrant mothers and role of neonatal feeding. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(12), 4588. 10.3390/ijerph17124588 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gould, J. F. , Fuss, B. G. , Roberts, R. M. , Collins, C. T. , & Makrides, M. (2021). Consequences of using chronological age versus corrected age when testing cognitive and motor development in infancy and intelligence quotient at school age for children born preterm. PLoS ONE, 16(9), e0256824. 10.1371/journal.pone.0256824 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hård, A. L. , Nilsson, A. K. , Lund, A. M. , Hansen‐Pupp, I. , Smith, L. E. H. , & Hellström, A. (2019). Review shows that donor milk does not promote the growth and development of preterm infants as well as maternal milk. Acta Paediatrica, 108(6), 998–1007. 10.1111/apa.14702 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horta, B. L. , Loret de Mola, C. , & Victora, C. G. (2015). Breastfeeding and intelligence: A systematic review and meta‐analysis. Acta Paediatrica, 104(467), 14–19. 10.1111/apa.13139 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Innis, S. M. (2004). Polyunsaturated fatty acids in human milk: An essential role in infant development. Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology, 554, 27–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobi‐Polishook, T. , Collins, C. T. , Sullivan, T. R. , Simmer, K. , Gillman, M. W. , Gibson, R. A. , Makrides, M. , & Belfort, M. B. (2016). Human milk intake in preterm infants and neurodevelopment at 18 months corrected age. Pediatric Research, 80(4), 486–492. 10.1038/pr.2016.114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, S. , Wolke, D. , & Marlow, N. (2008). Developmental assessment of preterm infants at 2 years: Validity of parent reports. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 50(1), 58–62. 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.02010.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, S. , Wolke, D. , Hennessy, E. , & Marlow, N. (2011). Educational outcomes in extremely preterm children: Neuropsychological correlates and predictors of attainment. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36(1), 74–95. 10.1080/87565641.2011.540541 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, S. , Bountziouka, V. , Brocklehurst, P. , Linsell, L. , Marlow, N. , Wolke, D. , & Manktelow, B. N. (2019). Standardisation of the Parent Report of Children's Abilities‐Revised (PARCA‐R): A norm‐referenced assessment of cognitive and language development at age 2 years. Lancet Child and Adolescent Health, 3(10), 705–712. 10.1016/s2352-4642(19)30189-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, S. , Marlow, N. , Wolke, D. , Davidson, L. , Marston, L. , O'Hare, A. , Peacock, J. , & Schulte, J. (2004). Validation of a parent report measure of cognitive development in very preterm infants. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 46(6), 389–397. 10.1017/s0012162204000635 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keunen, K. , van Elburg, R. M. , van Bel, F. , & Benders, M. J. (2015). Impact of nutrition on brain development and its neuroprotective implications following preterm birth. Pediatric Research, 77(1‐2), 148–155. 10.1038/pr.2014.171 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koletzko, B. , Fewtrell, M. S. , Domellöf, M. , Embleton, N. , Gruszfeld, D. , Lapillonne, A. , McNulty, A. , & Szitanyi, P. (2018). European standards of care for newborn health: Written standards of nutritional practice. ECFNI. [Google Scholar]
- Koo, W. , Tank, S. , Martin, S. , & Shi, R. (2014). Human milk and neurodevelopment in children with very low birth weight: A systematic review. Nutrition Journal, 13, 94. 10.1186/1475-2891-13-94 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kramer, M. S. , Aboud, F. , Mironova, E. , Vanilovich, I. , Platt, R. W. , Matush, L. , Igumnov, S. , Fombonne, E. , Bogdanovich, N. , Ducruet, T. , Collet, J. P. , Chalmers, B. , Hodnett, E. , Davidovsky, S. , Skugarevsky, O. , Trofimovich, O. , Kozlova, L. , & Shapiro, S. , Promotion of Breastfeeding Intervention Trial (PROBIT) Study Group . (2008). Breastfeeding and child cognitive development: New evidence from a large randomized trial. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(5), 578–584. 10.1001/archpsyc.65.5.578 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lechner, B. E. , & Vohr, B. R. (2017). Neurodevelopmental outcomes of preterm infants fed human milk: A systematic review. Clinics in Perinatology, 44(1), 69–83. 10.1016/j.clp.2016.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linsell, L. , Malouf, R. , Morris, J. , Kurinczuk, J. J. , & Marlow, N. (2015). Prognostic factors for poor cognitive development in children born very preterm or with very low birth weight: A systematic review. JAMA Pediatrics, 169(12), 1162–1172. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.2175 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayer, E. A. , Knight, R. , Mazmanian, S. K. , Cryan, J. F. , & Tillisch, K. (2014). Gut microbes and the brain: paradigm shift in neuroscience. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(46), 15490–15496. 10.1523/jneurosci.3299-14.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller, J. , Tonkin, E. , Damarell, R. A. , McPhee, A. J. , Suganuma, M. , Suganuma, H. , Middleton, P. F. , Makrides, M. , & Collins, C. T. (2018). A systematic review and meta‐analysis of human milk feeding and morbidity in very low birth weight infants. Nutrients, 10(6), 707. 10.3390/nu10060707 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Connor, D. L. , Gibbins, S. , Kiss, A. , Bando, N. , Brennan‐Donnan, J. , Ng, E. , Campbell, D. M. , Vaz, S. , Fusch, C. , Asztalos, E. , Church, P. , Kelly, E. , Ly, L. , Daneman, A. , & Unger, S. , GTA DoMINO Feeding Group . (2016). Effect of supplemental donor human milk compared with preterm formula on neurodevelopment of very low‐birth‐weight infants at 18 months: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 316(18), 1897–1905. 10.1001/jama.2016.16144 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pascal, A. , Govaert, P. , Oostra, A. , Naulaers, G. , Ortibus, E. , & Van den Broeck, C. (2018). Neurodevelopmental outcome in very preterm and very‐low‐birthweight infants born over the past decade: A meta‐analytic review. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 60(4), 342–355. 10.1111/dmcn.13675 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peñacoba, C. , & Catala, P. (2019). Associations between breastfeeding and mother‐infant relationships: A systematic review. Breastfeeding Medicine: The Official Journal of the Academy of Breastfeeding Medicine, 14(9), 616–629. 10.1089/bfm.2019.0106 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pierrat, V. , Marchand‐Martin, L. , Arnaud, C. , Kaminski, M. , Resche‐Rigon, M. , Lebeaux, C. , Bodeau‐Livinec, F. , Morgan, A. S. , Goffinet, F. , Marret, S. , & Ancel, P. Y. , EPIPAGE Writing Group . (2017). Neurodevelopmental outcome at 2 years for preterm children born at 22 to 34 weeks' gestation in France in 2011: EPIPAGE‐2 cohort study. BMJ, 358, j3448. 10.1136/bmj.j3448 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinelli, J. , Saigal, S. , & Atkinson, S. A. (2003). Effect of breastmilk consumption on neurodevelopmental outcomes at 6 and 12 months of age in VLBW infants. Advances in Nneonatal Care: Official Journal of the National Association of Neonatal Nurses, 3(2), 76–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quigley, M. A. , Hockley, C. , Carson, C. , Kelly, Y. , Renfrew, M. J. , & Sacker, A. (2012). Breastfeeding is associated with improved child cognitive development: A population‐based cohort study. Journal of Pediatrics, 160(1), 25–32. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2011.06.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues, C. , Severo, M. , Zeitlin, J. , & Barros, H. (2018). The type of feeding at discharge of very preterm infants: Neonatal intensive care units policies and practices make a difference. Breastfeeding Medicine: The Official Journal of the Academy of Breastfeeding Medicine, 13(1), 50–59. 10.1089/bfm.2017.0135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues, C. , Zeitlin, J. , Wilson, E. , Toome, L. , Cuttini, M. , Maier, R. F. , Pierrat, V. , Barros, H. , & EPICE Research, G. (2021). Managing mother's own milk for very preterm infants in neonatal units in 11 European countries. Acta Paediatrica, 110(1), 123–126. 10.1111/apa.15518 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rozé, J. C. , Darmaun, D. , Boquien, C. Y. , Flamant, C. , Picaud, J. C. , Savagner, C. , Claris, O. , Lapillonne, A. , Mitanchez, D. , Branger, B. , Simeoni, U. , Kaminski, M. , & Ancel, P. Y. (2012). The apparent breastfeeding paradox in very preterm infants: Relationship between breast feeding, early weight gain and neurodevelopment based on results from two cohorts, EPIPAGE and LIFT. BMJ Open, 2(2), e000834. 10.1136/bmjopen-2012-000834 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seaman, S. R. , & White, I. R. (2013). Review of inverse probability weighting for dealing with missing data. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 22(3), 278–295. 10.1177/0962280210395740 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seaman, S. R. , White, I. R. , Copas, A. J. , & Li, L. (2012). Combining multiple imputation and inverse‐probability weighting. Biometrics, 68(1), 129–137. 10.1111/j.1541-0420.2011.01666.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sentenac, M. , Boutron, I. , Draper, E. S. , Kajantie, E. , Maier, R. F. , Wolke, D. , & Zeitlin, J. (2020). Defining very preterm populations for systematic reviews with meta‐analyses. JAMA Pediatrics, 174(10), 997–999. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.0956 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sentenac, M. , Johnson, S. , Charkaluk, M. L. , Sëppanen, A. V. , Aden, U. , Cuttini, M. , Maier, R. , Mannamaa, M. , & Zeitlin, J. (2020). Maternal education and language development at 2 years corrected age in children born very preterm: Results from a European population‐based cohort study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 74, 346–353. 10.1136/jech-2019-213564 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seppänen, A. V. , Sauvegrain, P. , Draper, E. S. , Toome, L. , El Rafei, R. , Petrou, S. , Barros, H. , Zimmermann, L. , Cuttini, M. , & Zeitlin, J. , SHIPS Research Group . (2020). Parents' ratings of post‐discharge healthcare for their children born very preterm and their suggestions for improvement: A European cohort study. Pediatric Research, 89, 1004–1012. 10.1038/s41390-020-01120-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seppänen, A. V. , Bodeau‐Livinec, F. , Boyle, E. M. , Edstedt‐Bonamy, A. K. , Cuttini, M. , Toome, L. , Maier, R. F. , Cloet, E. , Koopman‐Esseboom, C. , Pedersen, P. , Gadzinowski, J. , Barros, H. , & Zeitlin, J. , Effective Perinatal Intensive Care in Europe (EPICE) Research Group . (2019). Specialist health care services use in a European cohort of infants born very preterm. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 61(7), 832–839. 10.1111/dmcn.14112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tharner, A. , Luijk, M. P. , Raat, H. , Ijzendoorn, M. H. , Bakermans‐Kranenburg, M. J. , Moll, H. A. , Jaddoe, V. W. , Hofman, A. , Verhulst, F. C. , & Tiemeier, H. (2012). Breastfeeding and its relation to maternal sensitivity and infant attachment. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 33(5), 396–404. 10.1097/DBP.0b013e318257fac3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treyvaud, K. , Doyle, L. W. , Lee, K. J. , Ure, A. , Inder, T. E. , Hunt, R. W. , & Anderson, P. J. (2016). Parenting behavior at 2 years predicts school‐age performance at 7 years in very preterm children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 57(7), 814–821. 10.1111/jcpp.12489 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Twilhaar, E. S. , Wade, R. M. , de Kieviet, J. F. , van Goudoever, J. B. , van Elburg, R. M. , & Oosterlaan, J. (2018). Cognitive outcomes of children born extremely or very preterm since the 1990s and associated risk factors: A meta‐analysis and meta‐regression. JAMA Pediatrics, 172(4), 361–367. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2017.5323 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- UNESCO Institute for Statistics . (2012). International standard classification of education (ISCED) 2011. UNESCO Institute for Statistics. [Google Scholar]
- Victora, C. G. , Horta, B. L. , Loret de Mola, C. , Quevedo, L. , Pinheiro, R. T. , Gigante, D. P. , Gonçalves, H. , & Barros, F. C. (2015). Association between breastfeeding and intelligence, educational attainment, and income at 30 years of age: A prospective birth cohort study from Brazil. The Lancet Global Health, 3(4), e199–e205. 10.1016/s2214-109x(15)70002-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Victora, C. G. , Bahl, R. , Barros, A. J. , França, G. V. , Horton, S. , Krasevec, J. , Murch, S. , Sankar, M. J. , Walker, N. , & Rollins, N. C. , Lancet Breastfeeding Series Group . (2016). Breastfeeding in the 21st century: Epidemiology, mechanisms, and lifelong effect. Lancet, 387(10017), 475–490. 10.1016/s0140-6736(15)01024-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang, Q. , Zhang, H. , Wee, C. Y. , Lee, A. , Poh, J. S. , Chong, Y. S. , Tan, K. H. , Gluckman, P. D. , Yap, F. , Fortier, M. V. , Rifkin‐Graboi, A. , & Qiu, A. (2019). Maternal sensitivity predicts anterior hippocampal functional networks in early childhood. Brain Structure and Function, 224(5), 1885–1895. 10.1007/s00429-019-01882-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, E. , Edstedt Bonamy, A. K. , Bonet, M. , Toome, L. , Rodrigues, C. , Howell, E. A. , Cuttini, M. , & Zeitlin, J. , EPICE Research Group . (2018). Room for improvement in breast milk feeding after very preterm birth in Europe: Results from the EPICE cohort. Maternal and Child Nutrition, 14(1). 10.1111/mcn.12485 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolf, H. T. , Weber, T. , Schmidt, S. , Norman, M. , Varendi, H. , Piedvache, A. , Zeitlin, J. , & Huusom, L. D. (2021). Mode of delivery and adverse short‐ and long‐term outcomes in vertex‐presenting very preterm born infants: A European population‐based prospective cohort study. Journal of Perinatal Medicine, 49(7), 923–931. 10.1515/jpm-2020-0468 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeitlin, J. , Bonamy, A. E. , Piedvache, A. , Cuttini, M. , Barros, H. , Van Reempts, P. , Mazela, J. , Jarreau, P. H. , Gortner, L. , Draper, E. S. , & Maier, R. F. , EPICE Research Group . (2017). Variation in term birthweight across European countries affects the prevalence of small for gestational age among very preterm infants. Acta Paediatrica, 106(9), 1447–1455. 10.1111/apa.13899 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeitlin, J. , Maier, R. F. , Cuttini, M. , Aden, U. , Boerch, K. , Gadzinowski, J. , Jarreau, P. H. , Lebeer, J. , Norman, M. , Pedersen, P. , Petrou, S. , Pfeil, J. M. , Toome, L. , van Heijst, A. , Van Reempts, P. , Varendi, H. , Barros, H. , & Draper, E. S. , EPICE and SHIPS Research Group . (2020). Cohort profile: Effective perinatal intensive care in Europe (EPICE) very preterm birth cohort. International Journal of Epidemiology, 49, 372–386. 10.1093/ije/dyz270 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zou, G. Y. , & Donner, A. (2013). Extension of the modified Poisson regression model to prospective studies with correlated binary data. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 22(6), 661–670. 10.1177/0962280211427759 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Supporting information.
Data Availability Statement
Data available on request due to privacy/ethical restrictions.