Skip to main content
Journal of Intelligence logoLink to Journal of Intelligence
. 2022 May 31;10(2):33. doi: 10.3390/jintelligence10020033

Dietary Intake of Polyphenols Enhances Executive/Attentional Functioning and Memory with an Improvement of the Milk Lipid Profile of Postpartum Women from Argentina

Agustín Ramiro Miranda 1,2, Mariela Valentina Cortez 1,2, Ana Veronica Scotta 1,2, Elio Andrés Soria 1,3,*
PMCID: PMC9224741  PMID: 35736005

Abstract

Puerperium may lead to memory and executive/attentional complaints that interfere with women’s daily life. This might be prevented by dietary compounds, such as neuroprotective polyphenols. Their bioactivity depends on their effects on lipid metabolism in different tissues, such as the brain, fat, and breast. Thus, a polyphenol-related cognitive improvement may be associated with changes of lipids in human milk, which are key for infant neurodevelopment. A cross-sectional study was conducted on 75 postpartum women from Córdoba (Argentina), involving several neuropsychological tests. Diet was registered to identify polyphenol intake and food pattern adherence, with sociodemographic and other psychological variables (insomnia, stress, subjective cognitive complaints) being also studied. Triacylglycerols, cholesterol, and their oxidative forms were analyzed as milk biomarkers. Multivariate statistical methods were applied. Results confirmed that women who consumed polyphenols presented better executive/attentional performance (i.e., higher correct responses, conceptual level responses, complete categories, verbal fluency; lower attentional interferences, and perseverative errors) and word retention with lower interference. Polyphenols were positively associated with milk lipids, which were higher in women with better cognition. Furthermore, they had lower oxidized triacylglycerols. In conclusion, polyphenolic intake during postpartum may improve executive/attentional functioning, memory, and milk lipid profile.

Keywords: breast feeding, diet, language tests, lipids, mental health, oxidative stress, polyphenols, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, Stroop Test, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

1. Introduction

Cognitive functioning during puerperium depends on neuropsychological adaptations with important psychosocial consequences for women, which require special attention in postpartum health care (Carrizo et al. 2020). Furthermore, women’s vulnerability was triggered by the impact on daily life derived from the COVID-19 pandemic, leading to stress, postpartum depression, insomnia, and cognitive impairment (Miranda et al. 2021b; Miranda et al. 2022a).

Executive/attentional functioning involves several processes, such as inhibition, planning, mental shifting, decision taking, updating, and dual-task coordination (Gilsoul et al. 2019; Tull et al. 2012). Puerperium also leads to memory adaptations, involving changes in neural networks and functional connectivity (Bak et al. 2020; Shin et al. 2018). Specific instruments are necessary to assess these changes. Complaints on these spheres compromise different aspects of women’s lives, such as nurture, breastfeeding, work, and study (Miranda et al. 2020b; Scotta et al. 2022). These concerns involve a persistent decline in memory, linguistic, attentional, and executive domains in women with previously normal frontal-executive functioning (Hohman et al. 2011; Mendonça et al. 2016). Thus, it is necessary to study preventative factors that support effective mental health measures.

Among these factors, nutrition is a crucial multitarget determinant of human health and well-being. In this sense, the diet provides several bioactive compounds that can prevent neuropsychological compromise. Polyphenols are promising candidates because they exert a wide range of neurotrophic effects (Miranda et al. 2018). These are heterogeneous compounds grouped into different chemical families, such as flavonoids (e.g., flavonols), and non-flavonoids (e.g., phenolic acids). They are present in plant foods, such as glycosides or aglycones (Singla et al. 2019). Although their bioactivity was thoroughly documented, the relevance of polyphenols as part of a habitual diet is still unclear due to the complexity of human nutrition and metabolism (Luca et al. 2020). Nonetheless, previous research suggested that the dietary intake of certain polyphenol families enhances memory during postpartum (Miranda et al. 2021a). Thus, HJ-Biplot, an innovative methodological approach in nutritional epidemiology, will be applied herein. This statistical method was recently developed to evaluate polyphenols comprehensively and their impact on lactating women (Miranda et al. 2022b).

Dietary polyphenols regulate lipid metabolism, which underlies several molecular mechanisms in the brain, fat, liver, endocrine system, muscles, and gut (Matacchione et al. 2020). Polyphenols are responsible for the Mediterranean diet’s benefits, such as preventing metabolic and neurological diseases that involve low-grade inflammation, oxidative stress, and anomalies in lipid metabolism (Milošević et al. 2021). Nonetheless, their involvement in the mammary gland, which may present unique responsiveness to these compounds, is partially unknown. Certain antioxidant effects on human milk lipids were reported (Poniedziałek et al. 2018; Valls-Bellés et al. 2021). Although the physiological mechanisms remain unclear, they may protect against peroxidative damage-related lipid degradation, upregulate gene expression related to lipid metabolism in the mammary gland, modulate neuroendocrine pathways, and enhance milk lipid clearance (Codini et al. 2020; Downing et al. 2017; Lin et al. 2018). Thus, additional biochemical studies are still needed because lipids are indispensable nutrients for healthy child development (Brink and Lönnerdal 2020).

Polyphenols target molecular pathways of lipid metabolism that are common to the brain and breast (Ehrmann et al. 2002; Senthil et al. 2021). Therefore, changes in the milk content of triacylglycerols and cholesterol may reflect a more complex internal response, which can participate in a cognitive improvement. Moreover, maternal behavior and milk production share neuroendocrine mediators, which determine lactating physiology and cognition (Jurek and Neumann 2018; Kiełbasa et al. 2021). Thus, this work aimed to establish the effects of dietary polyphenols on lactating women, based on the hypothesis that polyphenol intake during postpartum enhances executive/attentional functioning and memory in association with a quantitative and qualitative improvement of the human milk lipids.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design and Participants

This cross-sectional, analytical study was performed on 75 lactating women from Córdoba province (Argentina), who were interviewed face-to-face by health professionals trained in neuropsychology and nutrition assessment in primary care units of the public health system, the Maternal-Neonatal Hospital “Prof. Dr. Ramón Carrillo” and the Medical Sciences’ facilities of the National University of Cordoba. The inclusion criteria were: adult (≥18 years old), Córdoba inhabitant, postpartum (first six months), and breastfeeding. Exclusion criteria were: pregnancy, alcohol and drug abuse, currently active disease, neuropathology, and psychological conditions (e.g., depression).

Women signed an informed consent to be included voluntarily in this epidemiological study, approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the National Hospital of Clinics (National University of Cordoba), in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and current national legislation. Approval codes were RENIS-IS000548, RENIS-IS001262, RENIS-IS002045 (national registration), REPIS-145, REPIS-2654, and REPIS-5554 (province registration).

2.2. Interview

The assessments were carried out on working days (between 8 a.m. and 6 p.m.) and took place in a quiet test room at a hospital, faculty, or private setting. Participants were individually tested by trained technicians in neuropsychology (for the cognitive assessment) and in nutrition (for the nutritional assessment) in a single session lasting, on average, 120 min. Regarding the cognitive assessment, the participants responded to the tasks in the same order: first the participants answered the Stroop task, then the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, then they were assessed with the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, and finally the Verbal Fluency Tasks. The rooms were sound-proofed and light controlled, had adequate ventilation and temperature, and the women were provided with comfortable furniture (chair and desk), as well as a babysitter to take care of their children during the assessment. The neuropsychological assessment was performed before the nutritional assessment.

2.2.1. General Evaluation

The following sociodemographic data were collected using an ad hoc questionnaire: age (years), educational level (<12 or ≥12 years of formal instruction), employment (formally employed or informally employed/unemployed), marital status (single or in a couple), number of births (primiparous or multiparous), gestational age at delivery (<37 weeks -preterm or ≥37 weeks -term-), time of milk sampling (08.00 to 12.00, 12.00 to 16.00, or 16.00 to 20.00), practice of exclusive breastfeeding (yes or no), and postpartum duration (days). In addition, the type of physical activity, minutes and times per week, were registered to calculate the metabolic equivalents (MET) (Roman-Viñas et al. 2010).

2.2.2. Cognitive Assessment

All of the participants were neuropsychologically assessed with a set of validated tests to evaluate attention, language, memory, and executive functions.

Attention

The Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT- Stroop 1935) is an instrument widely used in clinics and research that provides measures of inhibition, selective attention, processing speed, and mental flexibility (Rivera et al. 2015). Each one of its three sheets has 100 randomly organized elements in five columns. The first two sheets represent the “congruent condition” in which the participants should read the names of the “red”, “green”, and “blue” colors printed in black ink (first sheet “word”-W-) and name the ink color (red, green, and blue) in which “xxxx” (second sheet, “color”-C-) are printed. On the contrary, the “incongruous condition” is presented in the third sheet (“word-color”-WC-), in which the words of color are printed with a different color ink on the name of the color (for example, the word “blue” is printed with red ink) (Scarpina and Tagini 2017). For each sheet, participants were instructed to read from top to bottom and from left to right as quickly as possible, and the score derived from the number of items correctly appointed in 45 s. An interference rate (I) was calculated as I = WC − [(W × C)/(W + C)] (Rivera et al. 2015; Scarpina and Tagini 2017). Successful execution on the third page requires that subjects intentionally concentrate their attention to an aspect of the stimulus (print color), while inhibiting attention from the other (meaning of the word). SCWT has good psychometric properties for Latin American Spanish speakers (Rodríguez Barreto et al. 2016).

Memory

The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) for Spanish speakers was used to assess episodic memory (Ferreira Correia and Campagna Osorio 2014). This widely known test, developed by Rey (1958), is considered easy and quick to administer. RAVLT consists of presenting orally a list of 15 unrelated words in five consecutive trials (Burin et al. 2007). Then, a new list is read with 15 distractors (List B), and the person must remember them immediately. Next, participants are required to remember the words of the first list (Trial A6-postinterference free recall-). After 30 min, the participant must again remember the words of the first list (Trial A7-long term delayed free recall-), and finally identify them on a printed sheet with 75 words (visual recognition). For this study, a set of traditional and novel scores were obtained from RAVLT (Alviarez-Schulze et al. 2022; Miranda et al. 2021a; Tyagi et al. 2021).

Executive Functions

The manual version of the Wisconsin Card Classification Test (WCST) was used to assess executive functioning (De la Cruz 2001; Heaton et al. 1993). This task-switching instrument originally developed by Berg (1948), consists of 4 reference cards and 128 response cards, which are divided into two decks of 64 cards each. The cards contain geometric figures, which vary in shape (crosses, circles, triangles, or stars), color (red, blue, yellow, or green), and number (one, two, three, or four). The participant must match the cards with one of the four reference cards. The sorting criterion is not indicated, and after the subject’s response, the evaluator indicates whether the card was correctly or incorrectly matched, but still does not reveal the principle of correct classification. Therefore, this forces the subject to repeat the responses until he/she discovers the classification rule. Once the subject has correctly classified ten consecutive cards, the criterion changes without prior notice, so that the subject must modify their classification criteria to identify the new one. Thus, this instrument is widely used to measure higher-level cognitive processes related to the prefrontal cortex (e.g., attention, perseverance, working memory, abstract reasoning, cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control, and set shifting) (Faustino et al. 2021). For this purpose, traditional WCST scores were used for the analysis of executive functions (Faustino et al. 2020). This instrument proved to be reliable and valid for the study of executive functions with local normative data for Spanish speakers (Miranda et al. 2020a).

Language

Linguistic functioning was evaluated using four verbal fluency tasks (VFT): two phonological tasks with direct instructions (initial letter P and F), one semantics (animals), and one phonological with indirect instructions (words without “A”- excluded letter A-). VFT was systematized by Borkowski et al. (1967). The application of the VFT followed the procedure proposed by Marino and Alderete (2010). The time allotted for each test was one minute. All of the words were recorded on a spreadsheet and each correct answer was scored with one point. Intrusions (words that do not belong to the required semantic field or do not comply with the phonological instruction) and perseverations (the repetition of a previous response) were considered incorrect (Lezak et al. 2012). VFTs are a useful tool to evaluate different capacities and cognitive functions related to language, such as lexical-semantic memory and executive/attentional processes (Cardoso Silva de Souza et al. 2020). VFTs have adequate psychometric properties and there are normative data available for several VFTs for Spanish speakers (Marino and Alderete 2010).

2.2.3. Nutritional Assessment

A validated food frequency questionnaire (FFQ) was applied to record the usual diet during the last 12 months. The FFQ comprises 127 foods available in the country, grouped according to their nutritional profile and origin. Questions were about frequency (times per month/week/day, as appropriate) and usual portion size of each food consumed (three categories: small, medium, and large). This instrument has shown adequate levels of validity and reproducibility for the studied population (Cortez et al. 2021). A validated photographic atlas was used, based on the standard portion sizes in Argentina to help participants describe the amount of food consumed. Next, data were analyzed using the Phenol-Explorer database (version 3.6) (Rothwell et al. 2013). This database provides values for more than 500 different polyphenols in more than 400 foods in different forms, including raw, cooked, and processed foods. The intake was calculated as mg/day. Only polyphenols with a medium daily intake ≥5 mg/d were considered for the analyses (Kesse-Guyot et al. 2012). This cut off point was previously demonstrated to present health effects (Miranda et al. 2022b).

Nutritional assessment included anthropometric measures: self-reported height (meters) and weight (kilograms) were recorded to calculate body mass index (BMI); and body fat percentage (BFP) was measured by hand-held bioelectrical impedance analysis (Cortez et al. 2021).

2.3. Milk Sampling and Lipid Measurement

The sampling protocol of Cortez and Soria (2016) was followed. Ten mL of human milk was delivered by each woman by manual expression into sterile collection bottles. Samples were immediately stored at −20 °C for transportation, and then at −80 °C to avoid lipid deterioration by enzymatic activity until the analyses (George et al. 2018). Triacylglycerols and cholesterol were measured by spectrophotometry at 540 nm, using a Promega’s GloMax Multi Microplate Multimode Reader (USA).

2.3.1. Triacylglycerols

Triacylglycerols were estimated by a widely used enzymatic/chromogenic method provided by a GT Lab commercial kit under the manufacturer’s instructions (GPO/PAP, Triglicéridos Liquid PlusTM, GT Lab, Rosario, Argentina), who established the corresponding analytical conditions. The assay involves the following enzymes: lipase (which hydrolyzes triglycerides to glycerol and fatty acids); glycerol kinase (which phosphorylates to glycerol, forming glycerol-1-phosphate); glycerol phosphate oxidase (which oxidizes to glycerol 1-phosphate, forming dihydroxyacetone-phosphate and hydrogen peroxide); and peroxidase (which in the presence of 4-aminophenazone forms a pink color quinonimine from hydrogen peroxide) (Kumari et al. 2020). Briefly, 3 parts of milk were mixed with 300 parts of a reactant solution for 5 min at 37 °C. Triacylglycerol content per liter (g/L) was then calculated by measuring the reaction at 540 nm and using the corresponding standard.

2.3.2. Cholesterol

Milk cholesterol concentration was estimated using an enzymatic/colorimetric method provided by a GT Lab kit, following the manufacturer’s instructions (CHOD/PAP, Colesterol Liquid PlusTM, GT Lab, Rosario, Argentina), who established the corresponding analytical conditions. Firstly, cholesterol esters are hydrolyzed by cholesterol esterase. Subsequently, the free cholesterol is oxidized by cholesterol oxidase to Δ4-cholestenone. Under peroxidase action, the hydrogen peroxide produced in the previous reaction oxidizes the chromophore 4-aminophenazone, in the presence of phenol, to the red dye compound 4-(p-benzoquinone-monoimino)-phenazone (Kumari et al. 2020). It was measured at 540 nm to calculate mg/L using the corresponding standard, after 3 parts of milk and 300 parts of a reactant solution were mixed for 5 min at 37 °C.

2.3.3. Oxidized Lipids

Lipoperoxides are products of the oxidative damage of fatty acids and cholesterol in cell membranes and lipoproteins (Elisia and Kitts 2011). To determine their concentration in human milk, a commercial kit was used, according to the manufacturer’s instructions (PeroxiDetectTM, Sigma-Aldrich, Saint Louis, MO, USA), who established the corresponding analytical conditions. One part of HM, 0.1 parts of 25 mM ferrous ammonium sulfate (prepared in 2.5 M sulfuric acid), and 10 parts of a reactant (125 μM xylenol orange with 4 mM butylated hydroxytoluene) were mixed for 30 min at 37 °C to reveal the optical density of lipoperoxides at 540 nm per liter of milk (OD/L). For the lipid hydroperoxides, the measuring procedure was similar to that of lipoperoxides, but the reactant was replaced by 125 μM xylenol orange in 100 mM sorbitol. Given that triacylglycerols are the form of storage and transportation of the principal targets of lipid peroxidation (polyunsaturated fatty acids), the quotient between lipoperoxides and triacylglycerols were calculated as OD/g (Pérez-Rodríguez et al. 2015). On the other hand, oxysterols are oxidized cholesterol derivatives that can be generated in the human organism through different oxidation processes. The quotient between the lipoperoxides and cholesterol indicated non-polar oxysterols (e.g., esterified cholesterol), while the hydroperoxides/cholesterol quotient indicated polar oxysterols (e.g., non-esterified cholesterol or lipoprotein) (Brown and Jessup 2009), with the level of oxysterols being reported as OD/mg.

2.4. Other Measures

2.4.1. Maternal Insomnia

The Spanish version of the Insomnia Severity Index (ISI) was used to evaluate the nature, severity, and impact of insomnia during the last month (Morin et al. 2011). This seven item self-report instrument is a traditional questionnaire used in insomnia research (Manzar et al. 2021). Each item was responded using a five-point Likert scale (0 to 4, where 0 = no problem and 4 = very severe problem) and the scores were transformed on a scale ranging from 0 to 28, with the highest scores indicating more severe insomnia and impact on quality of life. In the current study, the ISI showed good reliability (McDonald’s omega coefficient = 0.733).

2.4.2. Maternal Adherence to Dietary Patterns

Adherence to dietary patterns was evaluated using dietary indices for Argentinian lactating women. Adherence was calculated using three indices, based on the patterns identified by Cortez et al. (2021), which includes the following:

  • Macro-nutritional Dietary Index (MDI): Snacks; processed meats; legumes; dairy products; cheese; whole grains; and candies;

  • Phytochemical Dietary Index (PDI): vegetables A (5% of carbohydrate content); vegetables B (10% of carbohydrate content); fruits; and fatty fruits;

  • Energetic Dietary Index (EDI): vegetables C (20% of carbohydrate content); animal fats; vegetable oils; sugary drinks; and refined grains.

The dietary intake of each group was divided into deciles, with each decile being scored from 1 (lowest) to 10 (highest), whereas no intake received 0 points. The scoring system was adjusted for food groups with low intake frequency (i.e., snacks). Finally, points were totaled: MDI ranged from 0–41 points; PDI from 0–35; and EDI from 0–39, with higher scores indicating greater dietary pattern adherence.

In order to evaluate the quality of fats, carbohydrates, and proteins in the maternal diet, the Fat Quality Index (FQI) and the Protein to Carbohydrate Ratio (PCR) were calculated (Miranda et al. 2022b).

2.4.3. Subjective Memory Complaints

Participants responded to a Spanish version of the Memory Failures of Everyday questionnaire (MFE) (Lozoya-Delgado et al. 2012), which explores subjective memory complaints and related cognitive processes, such as attention, perceptual recognition, and language (Rodríguez-Blázquez et al. 2022). This instrument consists of 30 items about memory failures during situations and activities that take place in everyday life, which must be rated using a five-point Likert scale (0 to 4, 0 = never and 4 = always). In this study, MFE showed excellent reliability (McDonald’s omega coefficient = 0.924).

2.4.4. Subjective Executive Complaints

The Executive Complaint Questionnaire (ECQ) was used (Mías 2010). This questionnaire explores the subjective complaints of executive functions. The questionnaire has 15 items and participants must judge to what extent a series of daily life behaviors concerning mental functioning occur (Miranda et al. 2020c). Each item was rated on a five-point Likert scale (0 to 4, where 0 = never and 4 = always). In this work, it was found that the ECQ has a very good reliability (McDonald’s omega coefficient = 0.821).

2.5. Statistical Analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using Infostat (version 2020, InfoStat Group, Córdoba, Argentina) and Stata (version 15, StataCorp, College Station, TX, USA). Mean, standard deviation (SD), median, 25th, and 75th percentiles were calculated for numerical variables, and percentages for categorical ones. Skewness (S) and kurtosis (K) were used to assess the distribution of the neuropsychological scores. McDonald’s omega coefficient was estimated to assess the reliability of self-report questionnaires.

HJ-Biplot multivariate analyses were performed to discover the correlations between polyphenols, as well as cognitive scores. This technique provides a graphical representation of the rows and columns of a data matrix in a low-dimensional subspace where their relative positions are interpretable (Frutos Bernal et al. 2020). The markers are obtained from the singular value decomposition of the data matrix, where the rows represent individuals (women), and the columns variables (polyphenols or cognitive scores), so that both sets of markers can be overlaid in the same reference frame with the highest quality of representation. The HJ-Biplot graphic representation must be interpreted based on the following criteria (Carrasco et al. 2019):

  • The distances between points are interpreted as an inverse function of their similarities;

  • The vector length approximates the variable standard deviation;

  • The cosines of the angles between the vectors approximate the correlations between them, and the cosines of the angles between vectors and axes approximate the correlations between both;

  • The order of the orthogonal projections of the points onto a vector approximates the order of those points in that vector.

Clustering analyses, based on coordinates using the K-means method, were conducted in the HJ-Biplot models (Nieto-Librero et al. 2017). This multivariate classification technique enables the detection and description of clusters of subjects or homogeneous variables, based on the values observed within a heterogeneous set, trying to achieve the maximum possible homogeneity in each group and the significant differences between them. Similarity calculation between the centroids and data points (i.e., women) was based on cosine similarity (Carrasco et al. 2019). Contributions and qualities of representation (QLR) for each variable and cluster on the factorial plane were calculated as data fit measures. It was shown that the HJ-Biplot is a valid tool for the study of nutrition and cognitive functioning (Miranda et al. 2022b; Cadavid Ruiz et al. 2018). The HJ-Biplot analysis was performed using the MULTBIPLOT software package (multivariate analysis using BIPLOT) (Vicente-Villardón 2015).

Analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) with Fisher’s post hoc comparisons were used to test the differences in cognitive performance among the polyphenolic clusters. In addition, ANCOVA was used to analyze the effect of dietary polyphenols and cognitive status on lipids in human milk. All of the models were adjusted by potential confounders (β). The sets of potential confounders for each model were selected according to directed acyclic graphs, using the criterion of minimal sufficient adjustment for direct effects (Figure 1) (Miranda et al. 2021a). Values of 0 and 1 were adopted to establish the absence and presence, respectively, of the covariates introduced in the ANCOVA models, which combines an analysis of variance to the data and a multiple linear regression to the data to eliminate the effect of the covariates. Moreover, a dichotomous factor can be entered into a regression equation by formulating a dummy regressor with a 0/1 codification (Fox 2015). The use of several covariates reduces the variability of the data, thus increasing the statistical power, with models being fitted appropriately in accordance with sample size and prior criteria (Shieh 2020). Coefficients of determinations of the model (R2), means, standard errors, p-values (for comparisons), and Cohen’s d coefficients were reported. Effect sizes were calculated using Cohen’s d, which were interpreted as small (≥0.20), medium (≥0.50), large (≥0.80), and very large (≥1.30). Finally, post hoc analyses showed that the sample size in this study achieved more than 80% power with an alpha of 0.05, taking 8 predictors in the ANCOVA models and size effects of 0.45. The homogeneity of variance among the groups was tested by Levene’s test (Mishra et al. 2019).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Directed acyclic graph of the relationship among polyphenol intake, cognition, and human milk lipids in postpartum Argentinian women. Green squares indicate the independent (dietary polyphenol intake and cognition) and dependent variables (human milk lipids); red squares indicate minimal sufficient adjustment variables; the bold arrow indicates the relationship between the independent and dependent variables; thin arrows indicate other causal relationships.

3. Results

3.1. Sample Characteristics

Maternal age averaged 29.92 years (SD = 6.13). Most women were in a couple (93.33%), and had finished at least 12 years of formal education (75.66%). Unemployment or informal employment affected 54.66% of the participants. Regarding reproductive variables, the mean postpartum days were 95.04 (SD = 52.46), with a pregnancy duration of 38.37 weeks (SD = 2.05). Cesarean sections were presented in 55% of the sample, and 60% of the women were multiparous. The percentage of participants who practiced exclusive breastfeeding was 53.33%. In relation to the nutrition status, women had a BMI of 24.79 (5.62) Kg/m2 and a BFP of 28.88 (7.00) %, while they exhibited 384.96 (SD = 686.33) MET of physical activity. Means of dietary adherence were similar: 25.25 (SD = 6.85) for MDI, 21.89 (SD = 7.18) for PDI, and 22.81 (SD = 7.71) for EDI (Table 1).

Table 1.

Sociodemographic and reproductive characteristics of postpartum women (n = 75).

Variable Mean SD % N
Age (years) 29.92 6.13
Marital status
   In a couple 93.33 70
   Single 6.66 5
Educational level
   <12 years of formal instruction 25.33 19
   ≥12 years of formal instruction 75.66 56
Employment
   Formally employed 45.33 34
   Informally employed/unemployed 54.66 41
Postpartum duration (days) 95.04 52.46
Gestational age at delivery 38.37 2.05
   <37 weeks (preterm) 14.66 11
   ≥37 weeks (term) 85.33 64
Mode of delivery
   Cesarean section 54.66 41
   Vaginal 45.33 34
Number of births
   Primiparous 40 30
   Multiparous 60 45
Practice of exclusive breastfeeding
   No 46.66 35
   Yes 53.33 40
Breastfeeding frequency (hours) 3.01 2.72
Time of sampling
   08.00 to 12.00 62.66 47
   12.00 to 16.00 18.66 14
   16.00 to 20.00 18.66 14
BMI 24.79 5.62
BFP 28.88 7
Physical activity (MET) 384.96 686.33
Macro-nutritional Dietary Index 25.25 6.85
Phytochemical Dietary Index 21.89 7.18
Energetic Dietary Index 22.81 7.71
Fat Quality Index 27.94 23.84
Protein to Carbohydrate Ratio 0.34 0.08

Note. SD = standard deviation; BMI = body mass index; BFP = body fat percentage; MET = metabolic equivalents of task.

Table 2 describes the WCST performances. All of the scores showed levels of skewness and kurtosis ± 3, suggesting a normal distribution, except for trials to complete the first category, and the percentage of perseverative errors. Similarly, most scores in the RAVLT showed a normal approximation, except for the amount of intrusion errors, percentage of forgetting, and retroactive interference (Table 3). All of the SCWT and VFT scores showed acceptable values for skewness and kurtosis (Table 4).

Table 2.

Postpartum women’s performance in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (n = 75).

WCST Score M SD Mdn 25P–75P S K
Trials administered 100.67 25.35 93.00 76.00–128.00 0.03 −1.83
Correct responses 67.36 12.28 66.00 62.00–75.00 −0.28 1.10
Total errors 33.30 26.68 20.00 11.00–55.00 0.76 −0.77
% Total errors 29.13 18.44 20.40 14.67–42.97 0.87 −0.45
Perseverative responses 12.64 9.59 9.00 6.00–16.00 1.30 0.67
% Perseverative responses 11.62 6.88 9.33 7.14–14.13 1.09 0.63
Perseverative errors 9.79 7.43 7.00 5.00–12.00 1.33 0.77
% Perseverative errors 11.06 12.59 7.62 5.71–11.69 4.50 21.78
Non-perseverative errors 23.32 23.61 11.00 6.00–37.00 1.32 0.91
% Non-perseverative errors 20.15 18.66 11.68 7.59–28.13 1.80 3.32
Conceptual level responses 55.89 17.68 60.00 54.00–65.00 −1.34 1.65
% Conceptual level responses 61.12 25.80 73.74 42.19–82.28 −0.77 −0.65
Categories achieved 4.49 2.06 6.00 3.00–6.00 −0.95 −0.73
Global score 55.74 43.62 33.00 16.00–98.00 0.37 −1.57
Trials to complete first category 19.13 24.53 11.00 11.00–13.00 3.56 11.35
Failure to maintain set 0.70 1.18 0.00 0.00–1.00 1.91 2.92
Learning to learn −1.84 4.13 −1.26 −2.86–0.33 −1.50 2.59

Note. M = mean; SD = standard deviation; Mdn = median; 25P = 25th percentile; 75P = 75th percentile; S = Skewness; K = Kurtosis.

Table 3.

Postpartum women’s performance in the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (n = 75).

RAVLT Score M SD Mdn 25P–75P S K
Trial A1 5.68 2.02 5.00 4.00–7.00 0.52 0.26
Trial A2 8.03 2.74 8.00 6.00–9.00 0.30 −0.41
Trial A3 9.73 3.09 10.00 8.00–12.00 −0.54 −0.43
Trial A4 10.82 2.72 11.00 9.00–13.00 −1.07 1.15
Trial A5 11.48 2.62 12.00 11.00–13.00 −1.51 3.97
Σ A1–A5 45.95 11.11 48.00 37.00–53.00 −0.17 −0.50
Interference (trial B) 5.39 2.38 5.00 4.00–6.00 0.91 0.81
Post-interference (trial A6) 10.07 2.61 10.00 8.00–12.00 −0.15 −0.50
Delayed recall (trial A7) 10.19 2.86 10.00 9.00–12.00 −0.25 −0.63
Recognition (trial A8) 12.99 2.21 14.00 11.00–15.00 −1.29 1.11
Errors of repetitions 5.45 3.93 5.00 3.00–7.00 1.18 1.46
Intrusion errors 2.89 4.61 1.00 0.00–4.00 3.95 20.01
Corrected total learning 17.34 7.87 18.00 12.00–24.00 −0.56 −0.53
Learning 5.73 2.37 6.00 4.00–7.00 −1.03 2.57
Forgetting 1.31 2.20 1.00 0.00–3.00 −0.79 2.13
% of forgetting 0.12 0.23 0.10 0.00–0.25 −2.69 14.73
Forgetting speed 1.02 0.19 1.00 0.91–1.11 0.27 0.07
Retention 0.05 1.67 0.00 −1.00–1.00 −0.23 −0.20
Evocation 2.63 2.38 3.00 1.00–4.00 −0.70 2.29
Primacy 0.65 0.16 0.65 0.55–0.80 −0.24 −0.83
Recency 0.68 0.16 0.70 0.60–0.80 −0.51 −0.37
Total hit rate 0.59 0.12 0.61 0.49–0.67 −0.08 −0.59
Primacy hit rate 0.62 0.13 0.63 0.54–0.75 −0.19 −0.88
Middle hit rate 0.54 0.16 0.54 0.44–0.68 −0.16 −0.52
Recency hit rate 0.65 0.15 0.67 0.54–0.75 −0.43 −0.54
Memory Efficiency Index 1.99 0.32 2.04 1.84–2.19 −0.44 1.22
Proactive interference 1.03 0.51 1.00 0.71–1.25 1.73 4.31
Retroactive interference 0.90 0.32 0.89 0.75–1.00 4.20 24.40

Note. M = mean; SD = standard deviation; Mdn = median; 25P = 25th percentile; 75P = 75th percentile; S = Skewness; K = Kurtosis.

Table 4.

Postpartum women’s performance in the Stroop Color and Word Test, Verbal Fluency Tasks, and self-report complaints (n = 75).

Scores M SD Mdn P25–P75 S K
Stroop Color and Word Test
 Word 92.51 13.93 96.00 85.00–102.00 −0.84 0.39
 Color 67.01 10.94 69.00 59.00–74.00 −0.24 −0.17
 Word-color 40.50 8.86 41.50 35.00–47.00 −0.47 0.26
 Interference 1.87 7.71 1.54 −2.66–6.48 0.10 0.87
Verbal fluency task
 Phonological Letter P 14.03 4.38 13.00 11.00–17.00 0.48 −0.50
 Phonological Letter F 9.73 3.62 9.50 7.00–12.00 0.56 0.41
 Semantic Animals 17.55 5.68 17.00 14.00–22.00 −0.18 0.25
 Phonological Letter Excluded A 7.69 3.90 8.00 5.00–11.00 0.12 −0.03
Self-report complaints
 Insomnia Severity Index 9.78 4.77 9.00 7.00–12.00 0.44 0.10
 Executive Complaints 24.64 8.85 25.00 17.00–31.00 0.01 −0.60
 Memory Failures of Everyday 28.76 17.27 25.00 16.00–36.00 1.02 0.69

Note. M = mean; SD = standard deviation; Mdn = median; 25P = 25th percentile; 75P = 75th percentile; S = Skewness; K = Kurtosis.

3.2. Diet Characteristics

Thirty-six polyphenolic compounds were consumed at levels above 5 mg/d (Figure 2), including the following families: hydroxycinnamic acids (50%); flavanols (19.44%); flavanones (8.33%); hydroxybenzoic acids (8.33%); flavonols (5.55%); lignans (5.55%); and anthocyanin (2.77%). Figure 3 shows the HJ-Biplot graphics to visually represent the polyphenolic intake models in the factorial plane 1–2. The first model included those polyphenols consumed above 20 mg/d, and explained the 79.3% of variance (Figure 3a). These compounds showed adequate levels of contributions to the plane 1–2, and were represented with adequate quality (QLR > 200) (Supplementary Materials: Table S1). Three women clusters were identified in this model: C1 (women with lower polyphenolic intake, n = 34); C2 (women with higher intake of different hydroxycinnamic acids and one flavonol, n = 24); and C3 (women with higher intake of two hydroxycinnamic acids, one lignan, and one flavanone, n = 17). The qualities of representation for each cluster on the plane 1–2 exceed the value of 95%.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Daily dietary polyphenolic intake in postpartum women from Córdoba, Argentina (n = 75), with data being expressed as mean and standard errors.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

HJ-Biplot representation (axis 1–2) for clusters of polyphenols consumed above 20 mg/d (a) and between 5 and 20 mg/d (b) by Argentinian postpartum women.

On the other hand, Figure 3b shows the HJ-Biplot representation on the main first plane for the polyphenols consumed between 5 and 20 mg/d, which explained the 50.19% of variance. Most of these compounds showed adequate representation qualities, except for naringenin, eriodyctiol, syringic, ellagic acid, and trans-ferulic acid (QLR < 150) (Supplementary Materials: Table S2). Three clusters were formed with qualities of representation above 90%: C1 (women with lower polyphenolic intake, n = 33); C2 (women with higher intake of different hydroxycinnamic acids, one hydroxybenzoic acid, and one lignan, n = 29); and C3 (women with higher intake of different flavanols and one hydroxybenzoic acid, n = 13).

3.3. Effects of Polyphenols on Women’s Cognition

For polyphenols > 20 mg/d, women from C3 had a significantly higher number of correct responses, conceptual level responses, percentage of conceptual level responses, and categories achieved in the WCST, with respect to C1. Moreover, C3 had lower total errors, percentage of total errors, non-perseverative responses, percentage of non-perseverative responses, and trials to complete the first category. All of these significant differences on the WCST scores presented moderate effect sizes (Cohen’s d ≥ 0.50) (Figure 4).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) scores according to polyphenolic intake clusters (>20 mg/d). ANCOVA models were carried out with Fisher’s post hoc contrast and adjusted by educational level, postpartum days, parity, dietary indices (macronutritional, phytochemical, and energetic), and insomnia. C1: n = 34; C2: n = 24; C3: n = 17. D = Cohen’s d for effect size; * p < 0.05.

Figure 5 shows the results for the clusters > 20 mg/d on RAVLT, SCWT, and VFT. In this sense, women from C1 scored lower on forgetting speed and retention when compared with C2, but higher on attentional interference (SCWT). In addition, C1 showed higher means of proactive and retroactive interference than C3. Women from C1 produced significantly fewer words on the VFT letter F than the other ones. The effect sizes were moderate. Regarding adjustment variables, educational level was associated with all of the cognitive measures (p-values < 0.05). In addition, parity (primiparous > multiparous) and EDI (β = 0.04) were related, respectively, to Stroop interference and retroactive interference (p = 0.008 for both), whilst the number of trials to complete the first category was associated with MDI (β = −1.45, p = 0.0059) and EDI (β = 0.84, p = 0.0331).

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Postpartum women’s performance in the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT), and Verbal Fluency Task (VFT) according to polyphenolic intake clusters (>20 mg/d). ANCOVA models were adjusted by educational level, postpartum days, parity, dietary indices (macronutritional, phytochemical and energetic), and insomnia, with a Fisher’s post hoc contrast. C1: n = 34; C2: n = 24; C3: n = 17. d = Cohen’s d for effect size; * p < 0.05.

There were also significant differences among the clusters for the polyphenols consumed between 5 to 20 mg/d (Figure 6). In this regard, C1 had lower scores in WCST than C3: correct responses; conceptual level responses; and failure to maintain set. Furthermore, C1 showed higher retroactive interference (RAVLT) and attentional interference (SCWT) than C3. Cluster 1 also differed from C2, which presented lower scores on color and word-color tasks, with a small effect size. Most effect sizes were medium. Concerning adjustment variables, educational level was associated with all of the cognitive measures (p < 0.05), parity with Stroop interference (primiparous > multiparous, p = 0.007), and EDI with retroactive interference (β = 0.04, p = 0.007).

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Postpartum women’s performance in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), and Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT) according to polyphenolic intake clusters (5 to 20 mg/d). ANCOVA models were adjusted by educational level, postpartum days, parity, dietary indices (macronutritional, phytochemical, and energetic), and insomnia, with a Fisher’s post hoc contrast. C1: n = 33; C2: n = 29; C3: n = 13. d = Cohen’s d for effect size; * p < 0.05.

3.4. Cognitive Profiles of Postpartum Women

First, the measures of executive/attentional functioning were analyzed together, using the HJ-Biplot technique (Figure 7a). By retaining two axes, the explained variance was 41.48% for the first axis and 16.16% for the second one, which indicated that this model explained over 50% of the data variability. Representation contributions and qualities indicated that the best subspace to interpret results was the plane 1–2, with all of the elements showing good representation (QLR > 200) (Supplementary Materials: Table S3). Three clusters were identified with good quality:

  • Cluster 1 or “higher executive/attentional performance”: This conglomerate represented 49.33% (n = 37) of the sample and was comprised of women with better scores on positive measures (e.g., conceptual level responses, learning to learn, and categories achieved). This conglomerate had a good quality of representation at the 1–2 plane (QLR = 99%);

  • Cluster 2 or “lower attentional set control”: This conglomerate (32.00% of the sample, n = 24) was comprised of women with low scoring in the SCWT and with high scores in the negative WCST indices (e.g., global score and trials to complete the first category), and had an adequate quality of representation (QLR = 98%);

  • Cluster 3 or “lower inhibition and flexibility”: Fourteen women (18.67%) comprised this cluster, and presented lower scores of inhibitory control and behavioral flexibility in the WCST (i.e., higher perseverative responses, perseverative errors, and failures to maintain set). Its representation quality was good with a QLR of 87%.

Figure 7.

Figure 7

HJ-Biplot representation for clusters in the main plane (axis 1–2) for the executive/attentional functioning (a) and memory (b) in Argentinian postpartum women. SCWT = Stroop color and word test; I = Interference; C = Color; W = Word; W/C = Word-Color; WCST = Wisconsin card sorting test; TC1 °C = Trials to complete first category; %NPE = Percentage of non-perseverative errors; %TE = Percentage of total errors; GS = Global score; TA = Trials administered; FMS = Failure to maintain set; %PR = Percentage of perseverative responses; %PE = Percentage of perseverative errors; CR = Correct responses; CLR = Conceptual level responses; CA = Categories achieved; %CLR = Percentage of conceptual level responses; LL = Learning to learn; VFT:ELA = Verbal fluency task, excluded letter A; RHR = recency hit rate; MHR = middle hit rate; TA1 = Trial A1; IT = interference trial (B); PIT = post-interference trial (A6); PHR = primacy hit rate; R = recognition; DR = delayed recall; MEI = memory efficiency index; F = forgetting; %F = percentage of forgetting; L = Learning; CTL = Corrected total learning; ΣA1–5 = sum trials A1 to A5; IR, immediate recall; VF-P, verbal fluency letter P; VF-F, verbal fluency letter F; VF-Animals, verbal fluency Animals; PrI = proactive interference; ReI = retroactive interference; TA5 = trial A5; E = evocation; Ret = retention; IE = Intrusion errors; ER = errors of repetitions; FS = forgetting speed; VFT = verbal fluency task; LF = letter F; LP = letter P; S = semantic.

Figure 7b shows the HJ-Biplot representation of memory performance. The variance explained by the first axis was 34.72%, and 14.38% by the second one. In addition, contributions and qualities of all of the RAVLT and FVT scores confirmed that this model should be interpreted at the 1–2 plane (Supplementary Materials: Table S4). Two clusters were identified: Cluster 1 or “higher memory”, and Cluster 2 or “lower memory”, which contained 54.67% (n = 41) and 45.33% (n = 34) of the women, respectively. Both clusters showed a good quality of representation in the plane 1–2 (QLR of 99% for both).

3.5. Human Milk Lipids: Effects of Dietary Polyphenols and Cognitive Status

Finally, the ANCOVA models for the human milk lipids according to the polyphenol intake clusters are displayed in Table 5. Levene’s test (p > 0.05) indicated that the variances between the groups were statistically equal for all of the chemical variables. Regarding polyphenols between 5 and 20 mg/d, C3 had a significantly higher concentration of triacylglycerols than C1 (30.52 (2.79) versus 23.76 (1.87) g/L, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.17). In addition, C2 showed higher levels of cholesterol (113.71 (11.46) versus 80.04 (10.85) mg/L, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.17). These results had medium size effects (Cohen’s d > 0.50). No other significant differences were found. Moreover, no other statistically significant associations were found between polyphenol intake and human milk lipids or adjustment variables.

Table 5.

Human milk lipids’ concentration according to polyphenolic clusters in postpartum women from Córdoba, Argentina (n = 75).

Compound Polyphenols from 5 to 20 mg/d Polyphenols > 20 mg/d
C1
(n = 33)
C2
(n = 29)
C3
(n = 13)
d C1
(n = 34)
C2
(n = 24)
C3
(n = 17)
d
Triacylglycerols (g/L) 23.76
(1.87)
25.10
(1.99)
30.52
(2.79) *
0.654 25.06
(2.03)
26.61
(2.19)
24.81
(2.98)
-
Cholesterol (mg/L) 80.04
(10.85)
113.71
(11.46) *
97.73
(16.10)
0.552 85.94
(11.51)
92.02
(16.91)
112.63
(12.38)
-
Oxidized triacylglycerols (OD/g) 335.96
(116.59)
237.66
(123.71)
248.77
(173.78)
- 275.09
(122.65)
241.49
(132.17)
349.39
(180.12)
-
Polar oxysterols (OD/mg) 92.90
(10.77)
80.39
(11.37)
84.81
(15.97)
- 91.79
(11.28)
78.72
(12.13)
88.18
(16.57)
-
Non-polar oxysterols (OD/mg) 81.31
(8.50)
59.02
(8.97)
61.65
(12.61)
- 71.11
(9.11)
62.43
(9.80)
74.85
(13.38)
-

Note. Data expressed as mean (standard error). ANCOVA models were adjusted by breastfeeding, parity, postpartum days, body fat percentage, and dietary indices (macronutritional, phytochemical and energetic), with post hoc comparisons (d = Cohen’s d for size effect; * p < 0.05). Thresholds of the effect size: small (0.20), medium (0.50), large (0.80), very large (1.30).

On the other hand, outcomes revealed that women with higher executive/attentional functioning (Cluster 1) had higher milk concentration of triacylglycerols (28.53 (1.71) g/L) than those with lower inhibition and behavioral flexibility (21.90 (8.84) g/L) and lower attentional set control (22.94 (1.98) g/L) (p < 0.05, R2 = 0.20). In addition, Cluster 1 showed a lower level of oxidized triacylglycerols (189.17 (103.55) OD/g) when compared with Cluster 3 (640.97 (172.12) OD/g) (p < 0.05, R2 = 0.22). In respect of women with better memory functioning, they showed more triacylglycerols (27.69 (1.60) g/L vs 22.83 (1.77) g/L, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.17) and lower oxidized triacylglycerols (194.52 (27.69) OD/g vs. 281.28 (30.85) OD/g, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.15). As can be seen in Table 6, the effects were medium in size in the executive-attentional functioning model (Cohen’s d > 0.50) and small in the memory model (Cohen’s d > 0.20). Concerning the variables that were included for the adjustment of ANCOVA models, insomnia was negatively related to milk cholesterol content in the executive/attentional functioning model (β = −3.13, p = 0.04, R2 = 0.22) and the memory model (β = −2.70, p = 0.07, R2 = 0.17). No other statistically significant associations were found between executive and attentional functioning and human milk lipids or adjustment variables.

Table 6.

Human milk lipids concentration according to executive/attentional functioning and memory of postpartum women from Córdoba, Argentina (n = 75).

Compound Executive/Attentional Functioning Memory
>EF
(n = 37)
<IF
(n = 14)
<AS
(n = 24)
d >M
(n = 41)
<M
(n = 34)
d
Triacylglycerols (g/L) 28.53
(1.71)
21.90
(2.84) *
22.94
(1.98) *
−0.647
−0.561
27.69
(1.60)
22.83
(1.77) *
−0.479
Cholesterol (mg/L) 90.97
(10.26)
112.34
(17.88)
96.84
(11.88)
- 94.35
(9.53)
98.36
(10.70)
-
Oxidized triacylglycerols (OD/g) 189.17
(103.55)
640.97
(172.12) *
241.97
(120.01)
0.727 194.52
(27.69)
281.28
(30.85) *
0.493
Polar oxysterols (OD/mg) 95.34
(9.77)
65.13
(17.03)
84.06
(11.32)
- 92.89
(9.10)
78.89
(10.22)
-
Non-polar oxysterols (OD/mg) 71.32
(7.97)
55.70
(13.90)
72.32
(9.24)
- 64.96
(7.37)
74.48
(8.28)
-

Note. Data expressed as mean (standard error). ANCOVA models were adjusted by breastfeeding, parity, postpartum days, body fat percentage, dietary indices (macronutritional, phytochemical, and energetic), and insomnia severity index, with post hoc comparisons (d = Cohen’s d for size effect; * p < 0.05). Thresholds of the effect size: small (0.20), medium (0.50), large (0.80), very large (1.30). >EF = cluster with higher executive functioning; <IF = cluster with lower inhibitory control and behavioral flexibility; <AS = cluster with lower attentional set control; M = memory.

Additionally, the analyses of human milk lipids according to extraction time, gestational age at delivery, breastfeeding frequency, and dietary macronutrients are displayed in Table S5 (Supplementary Materials). No associations were found in these ANCOVA models.

4. Discussion

Results showed marked interindividual variability in the daily intake of polyphenols, which were associated with better executive/attentional functioning and memory performance in postpartum women. In addition, these dietary compounds significantly correlated with the lipid profile of breast milk, with higher triacylglycerols and cholesterol concentrations. Furthermore, women with better cognitive status showed a better quantity and quality of these nutrients. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study that integrates the effect of maternal diet and cognition on human milk composition. Taken together, these results support the stated hypothesis.

In this work, we used valid instruments to measure executive/attentional functioning and memory, to include their scores in statistical analyses (Henríquez et al. 2022). Most of them showed a normal distribution, confirming their adequacy for multivariate analyses (Aminu and Shariff 2014).

The nutritional assessment reported that 36 polyphenols presented a mean intake above 5 mg/d. Hydroxycinnamic acids represented the most ingested family, in concordance with previous research about the South American diet. This diet is composed of foods and beverages rich in hydroxycinnamic acids, lignans, and some flavonoids (Carnauba et al. 2021; Rossi et al. 2018).

Nutrition regulates physiological pathways via synergistic nutrient effects (Margină et al. 2020). Polyphenols with cognitive effects were hesperetin, lariciresinol, caffeic acid, and ferulic acid (for those consumed above 20 mg/d) and gallated flavonoids (for those consumed between 5 and 20 mg/d). Women who consumed these compounds showed higher scores in executive function, attention, memory, and language. These results are supported by studies about the cognitive benefits of polyphenols in other populations. In this sense, middle-aged women’s verbal memory is enhanced by the intake of lignans (Greendale et al. 2012), which showed brain bioaccumulation and prefrontal improvement (Kreijkamp-Kaspers et al. 2007; Wang et al. 2018).

On the other hand, flavonoids are neuroprotective by increasing blood flow and strengthening brain pathways related to executive functioning, learning, and memory (Bakoyiannis et al. 2019; Williamson et al. 2018; Kennedy et al. 2017). In the current study, hesperetin, a flavanone, belonged to the cluster associated with better cognitive performance. It was shown that hesperetin, naringenin, and their metabolites, are lipophilic flavonoids that cross hematic barriers (Bakoyiannis et al. 2019). In addition, hesperetin reduces lipid peroxidation (Moghaddam and Zare 2018). In addition to this antioxidant effect, it exerts positive effects on synaptic plasticity in cortical and hippocampal neurons (Vauzour et al. 2007). Only very few clinical trials have established the cognitive effects of hesperetin, which enhances cerebral blood flow and cognition (Salehi et al. 2021).

Only a small amount of information encoded in the nervous system is consolidated and stored by the long-term memory, whereas the rest is forgotten through passive and active mechanisms (Davis and Zhong 2017). In this work, women with low intake of polyphenols presented more interference than women with a higher intake. Memory interference occurs when additional information competes before, after, or during the coding of target information (Crawford et al. 2020). Lariciresinol, hesperetin, and caffeic acid were inversely related to proactive and retroactive interference (moderate effect sizes), with the latter being also associated with gallated flavonoids. Proactive interference occurs when previous knowledge hinders the ability to acquire new information, while retroactive interference refers to the effect of new learning on previously established knowledge, impairing memory consolidation (Crawford et al. 2020). Consequently, retroactive interference is more frequent and harmful (Edwards 2010). There is little evidence about the dietary polyphenolic effect on interference phenomena. Some data show that the intake polyphenolic sources improve memory consolidation by decreasing interference (Bell and Williams 2019) and diet-induced neuroplasticity in postpartum women (Xavier et al. 2021). Other studies found non-significant results (Lamport et al. 2016a; Lamport et al. 2016b) or contradictory ones (Whyte and Williams 2015). Nevertheless, polyphenols attenuate interference and promote cognition and attention (Crawford et al. 2020), which were also improved by these compounds in the present study.

Certain hydroxycinnamic acids formed the cluster with the greatest cognitive outcome, together with hesperetin (flavanone) and lariciresinol (lignan). However, the highly consumed chlorogenic acids, other hydroxycinnamic compounds, were less involved. In terms of cerebral function, there is little evidence about their effects on humans (Coman and Vodnar 2020), with contradictory data (Cropley et al. 2012; Camfield et al. 2013). The cognitive effects of chlorogenic acids imply mood improvement and acetylcholinesterase downregulation (Bouayed et al. 2007; Kwon et al. 2010). This evidence could partially explain the weak outcomes in postpartum women with a higher intake of chlorogenic acids. Conversely, the ferulic and caffeic acids exert well-established biological effects in the brain, supporting the results of the current work. These pro-cognitive effects depend on their higher intestinal absorption, redox-regulating activity, lipoxygenase inhibition, and glial/neuronal protection (Coman and Vodnar 2020; Singh et al. 2021; Habtemariam 2017).

Catechins were related to better cognition. They are classified into gallated and non-gallated catechins, whether they have gallic moieties or not. The gallated ones achieve more bioavailability (Chu and Pang 2018), with epigallocatechin gallate being anxiolytic, anti-amyloidogenic, and a promoter of memory, social cognition, and attention (Wightman et al. 2012). These benefits depend on their effects on brain tissue, such as antioxidant activity, improvement of parietal-frontal connectivity, neuroplasticity, and neuroreceptor modulation (Mancini et al. 2017).

Multiple effects of polyphenols on human milk and child health were described (Ríos et al. 2021). We discovered that these dietary compounds (polyphenols consumed at 5 to 20 mg/d) were positively associated with milk triacylglycerols and cholesterol, with low polyphenol intake being related to milk values below expected averages (Koletzko 2016). Polyphenols modulate lipid metabolism and adipokine secretion (Boccellino and D’Angelo 2020; Carpene et al. 2015). Experimental maternal models found metabolic and genetic effects. In this sense, polyphenols trigger breast tissue-specific gene upregulation, fatty acid bioavailability, lipogenesis, and adiponectin pathway (Caimari et al. 2017). Nonetheless, these mechanisms remain unknown in women, and in human milk.

Triacylglycerols are the main energetic component in human milk (Demmelmair and Koletzko 2018), which is produced by the mammary gland from circulating fatty acids and de novo synthesis to form fat globules (Yang et al. 2018). Numerous factors modify these processes (Yao et al. 2020), such as maternal genetics, environment, and diet (Yang et al. 2018). Adipokines and PPAR regulation determinate lipid homeostasis and inflammatory response to maintain human milk quality (Yang et al. 2018). Moreover, polyphenols target these pathways (Farràs et al. 2013). Thus, the higher lipid level found in women who consumed flavonoids may respond to these complex mechanisms (Caimari et al. 2017; Rabadan-Chávez et al. 2016; McManaman 2014).

The pharmacological potential of polyphenols, such as procyanidin, is applied against dyslipidemia (Rufino et al. 2021; Downing et al. 2017). Feng et al. (2019) also found that epigallocatechin gallate, kaempferol, and quercetin are bioactive on cholesterol metabolism by multiple gene regulation. Moreover, ellagic acid induces lipid metabolism (Kubota et al. 2019), with vegetable-based foods triggering lipid synthesis in breast tissue in an animal model (Palin et al. 2014). Lignans (lariciresinol, pinoresinol) and phenolic acids also contribute (Herchi et al. 2014; Zhong et al. 2021). Quercetin induces sensitivity to prolactin and the expression of stearoyl-CoA desaturase and fatty acid synthase in the murine mammary gland (Lin et al. 2018).

There is growing interest in the effect of psychological factors on breastfeeding (Fallon et al. 2016). However, few studies evaluated the impact of mental health on milk composition (Ziomkiewicz et al. 2021). Our study established that women with better executive/attentional functioning and memory showed a better lipid profile, characterized by increased triacylglycerols with lower oxidation. Ziomkiewicz et al. (2021) found that maternal response to stress is associated with neuroendocrine alterations and breast milk changes. Moreover, chronic stress leads to lipid oxidation (Troubat et al. 2009), with breastfeeding and cortisol levels responding to relaxation interventions (Mohd Shukri et al. 2019).

An inverse association existed between milk cholesterol concentration and insomnia severity. Given that dietary intake and fat storage were considered in multivariate analyses (confounding control), endogenous mechanisms are suggested. Lipids reach human milk from plasma and glandular biosynthesis by insomnia-sensitive mechanisms (Ontsouka and Albrecht 2014; Mohammad and Haymond 2013; Andreas et al. 2015; Fu et al. 2015; Casey et al. 2014). Although cholesterol was within a normal range, our results proposed that severe insomnia could compromise milk composition.

A limitation of this work is that specific mood indicators, which may modify milk composition, were not included (Kawano and Emori 2015; Stuebe et al. 2012). Nevertheless, clinical aspects of mood disorders, such as insomnia, were considered. Another possible limitation can be the sample size and the cross-sectional design. Although potential confounders were adjusted with adequate statistical power, longitudinal studies should replicate this research in larger samples. Although the timing of milk extraction was not significantly associated, milk chronobiology is of interest. Neuropsychological assessment between 8 a.m. and 6 p.m. may cause time-of-day bias (Venkat et al. 2020). Thus, future studies should control this factor. Some of the results have to be interpreted with caution. Future in-depth research should consider collecting milk samples at different time points (i.e., colostrum, transitional milk, and mature milk) with standardized sample collection (e.g., foremilk, hindmilk, complete breast expression, expression time). Specific analyses for human milk lipidomic are also encouraged (e.g., gas chromatography, mass spectrometry, NMR spectroscopy) (George et al. 2018).

Finally, our results reinforce the current perspective to approach puerperal health. Women’s mental health determines self-efficacy, and impacts breastfeeding behaviors and the composition of human milk (Fallon et al. 2016). Nutrient transference to this fluid is essential for the healthy growth and development of children. A complex interaction of psycho-,neuro-,immuno-endocrine responses regulates the maternal brain and mammary gland (Jonas and Woodside 2016).

5. Conclusions

In this study, polyphenolic intake during puerperium promoted two relevant aspects of the health of lactating women from Córdoba, Argentina. The first one was that the higher executive/attentional functioning and memory were related to the intake of different families of dietary polyphenols. The second one was a positive association with the milk content of triacylglycerols and cholesterol, crucial lipids for healthy infant growth and development. These findings suggest that human milk lipids can be modulated by certain dietary polyphenols, whose intake of 5–20 mg/d appears to be more suitable for future nutritional interventions. Results, therefore, supported a better cognitive status and lipid profile, which were related. Women with better cognition showed higher triacylglycerols concentration in milk with less peroxidation. In conclusion, maternal polyphenolic intake may improve executive/attentional functioning, memory, and milk lipid profile. Future studies with larger samples, repeated measures, and different methods are needed to validate these findings. The current work constitutes an innovative approach by integrating the assessment of biological, nutritional, and psychological issues of women’s health during this vital stage, and may contribute to understanding the complex nature of human milk.

Acknowledgments

ARM and AVS were supported by doctoral fellowships of Universidad Nacional de Córdoba, and MVC was supported by a postdoctoral fellowship of Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas. The authors gratefully acknowledge the participating women.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/jintelligence10020033/s1, Table S1: Contributions and representation qualities for each polyphenol consumed above 20 mg/d on the first two axes; Table S2: Contributions and representation qualities for each polyphenol consumed from 5 to 20 mg/d on the first two axes; Table S3: Contributions and representation qualities for each executive/attentional score on the first two axes; Table S4: Contributions and representation qualities for each memory score on the first two axes; Table S5: Human milk lipids according to extraction time, gestational age at delivery, breastfeeding frequency, and dietary macronutrients.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.R.M. and E.A.S.; methodology, A.R.M., M.V.C. and A.V.S.; software, A.R.M.; validation, A.R.M. and A.V.S.; formal analysis, A.R.M.; investigation, A.R.M., M.V.C. and A.V.S.; resources, E.A.S.; data curation, A.R.M., M.V.C. and A.V.S.; writing—original draft preparation, A.R.M.; writing—review and editing, A.R.M., M.V.C., A.V.S., and E.A.S.; visualization, A.R.M.; supervision, E.A.S.; project administration, E.A.S.; funding acquisition, E.A.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the National Hospital of Clinics (National University of Cordoba), in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and current national legislation. Approval codes were RENIS-IS000548, RENIS-IS001262, RENIS-IS002045 (national registration), REPIS-145, REPIS-2654, and REPIS-5554 (province registration). Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to ethical restrictions.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Funding Statement

Funding was provided by the Secretaría de Ciencia y Tecnología, Universidad Nacional de Córdoba [(grant numbers SECYT-UNC 411/2018)], and Agencia Nacional de Promoción Científica y Tecnológica [(grant numbers PICT-2016-2846 RESOL-2017-285-APN-DANPCYT#MCT)].

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  1. Alviarez-Schulze Vanessa, Cattaneo Gabriele, Pachón-García Catherine, Solana-Sánchez Javier, Tormos Josep M., Pascual-Leone Alvaro, Bartrés-Faz David. Validation and Normative Data of the Spanish Version of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test and Associated Long-Term Forgetting Measures in Middle-Aged Adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2022;14 doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.809019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aminu Ibrahim Murtala, Shariff Mohd Noor Mohd. Strategic orientation, access to finance, business environment and SMEs performance in Nigeria: Data screening and preliminary analysis. European Journal of Business and Management. 2014;6:124–32. [Google Scholar]
  3. Andreas N. J., Kampmann B., Le-Doare K. Mehring. Human breast milk: A review on its composition and bioactivity. Early Human Development. 2015;91:629–35. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2015.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bak Yunjin, Nah Yoonjin, Han Sanghoon, Lee Seung-Koo, Shin Na-Young. Altered neural substrates within cognitive networks of postpartum women during working memory process and resting-state. Scientific Reports. 2020;10:9110. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-66058-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bakoyiannis Ioannis, Daskalopoulou Afrodite, Pergialiotis Vasilios, Perrea Despina. Phytochemicals and cognitive health: Are flavonoids doing the trick? Biomedicine and Pharmacotherapy. 2019;109:1488–97. doi: 10.1016/j.biopha.2018.10.086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bell Lynne, Williams Claire. A pilot dose–response study of the acute effects of haskap berry extract (Lonicera caerulea L.) on cognition, mood, and blood pressure in older adults. European Journal of Nutrition. 2019;58:3325–34. doi: 10.1007/s00394-018-1877-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Berg Esta. A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. The Journal of General Psychology. 1948;39:15–22. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1948.9918159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Boccellino Mariarosaria, D’Angelo Stefania. Anti-obesity effects of polyphenol intake: Current status and future possibilities. International Journal of Molecular Sciences. 2020;21:5642. doi: 10.3390/ijms21165642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Borkowski John G., Benton Arthur L., Spreen Otfried. Word fluency and brain damage. Neuropsychologia. 1967;5:135–40. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(67)90015-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bouayed Jaouad, Rammal Hassan, Dicko Amadou, Younos Chafique, Soulimani Rachid. Chlorogenic acid, a polyphenol from Prunus domestica (Mirabelle), with coupled anxiolytic and antioxidant effects. Journal of the Neurological Sciences. 2007;262:77–84. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2007.06.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brink Lauren, Lönnerdal Bo. Milk fat globule membrane: The role of its various components in infant health and development. The Journal of Nutritional Biochemistry. 2020;85:108465. doi: 10.1016/j.jnutbio.2020.108465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brown Andrew, Jessup Wendy. Oxysterols: Sources, cellular storage and metabolism, and new insights into their roles in cholesterol homeostasis. Molecular Aspects of Medicine. 2009;30:111–22. doi: 10.1016/j.mam.2009.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Burin Débora, Drake Marina, Harris Paula. Evaluación Neuropsicológica en Adultos. Paidós SAICF; Buenos Aires: 2007. [Google Scholar]
  14. Cadavid Ruiz Natalia, Egido Jaime, Galindo-Villardón María Purificación, Río Pablo del. Advantages of using HJ-biplot analysis in executive functions studies. Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa. 2018;34:e3426. doi: 10.1590/0102.3772e3426. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Caimari Antoni, Mariné-Casadó Roger, Boqué Noemí, Crescenti Anna, Arola Lluís, Bas Josep Maria Del. Maternal intake of grape seed procyanidins during lactation induces insulin resistance and an adiponectin resistance-like phenotype in rat offspring. Scientific Reports. 2017;7:12573. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-12597-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Camfield David, Silber Beata, Scholey Andrew, Nolidin Karen, Goh Antoniette, Stough Con. A randomised placebo-controlled trial to differentiate the acute cognitive and mood effects of chlorogenic acid from decaffeinated coffee. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e82897. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cardoso Silva de Souza Brenda, Teixeira Tailah de Oliveira Barreiros, Silva Liara Dias da, Satler Corina, Cera Maysa Luchesi. Verbal fluency of younger and older adults from the Federal District: Proposed normative values. Audiology-Communication Research. 2020;25 doi: 10.1590/2317-6431-2020-2315. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Carnauba Renata, Hassimotto Neuza, Lajolo Franco. Estimated dietary polyphenol intake and major food sources of the Brazilian population. British Journal of Nutrition. 2021;126:441–48. doi: 10.1017/S0007114520004237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Carpene Christian, Gomez-Zorita Saioa, Deleruyelle Simon, Carpene Marie-Anne. Novel strategies for preventing diabetes and obesity complications with natural polyphenols. Current Medicinal Chemistry. 2015;22:150–64. doi: 10.2174/0929867321666140815124052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Carrasco Gonzalo, Molina Jose-Luis, Patino-Alonso María-Carmen, Castillo Marisela del Carmen, Vicente-Galindo María-Purificación, Galindo-Villardón María-Purificación. Water quality evaluation through a multivariate statistical HJ-Biplot approach. Journal of Hydrology. 2019;577:123993. doi: 10.1016/j.jhydrol.2019.123993. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Carrizo Eugenia, Domini Julieta, Quezada Romina, Serra Silvana Valeria, Soria Elio Andrés, Miranda Agustín Ramiro. Variaciones del estado cognitivo en el puerperio y sus determinantes: Una revisión narrativa. Ciencia & Saude Coletiva. 2020;25:3321–34. doi: 10.1590/1413-81232020258.26232018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Casey Theresa, Crodian Jennifer, Donkin Shawn S., Plaut Karen. Continuously changing light-dark phase decreases milk yield, fat, protein and lactose in dairy cows. Advances in Dairy Research. 2014;2:119. doi: 10.4172/2329-888X.1000119. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Chu Kai On, Pang Calvin. Pharmacokinetics and Adverse Effects of Drugs—Mechanisms and Risks Factors. IntechOpen; London: 2018. Pharmacokinetics and Disposition of Green Tea Catechins. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Codini Michela, Tringaniello Carmela, Cossignani Lina, Boccuto Antonio, Mirarchi Alessandra, Cerquiglini Laura, Troiani Stefania, Verducci Giuseppa, Patria Federica Filomena, Conte Carmela, et al. Relationship between fatty acids composition/antioxidant potential of breast milk and maternal diet: Comparison with infant formulas. Molecules. 2020;25:2910. doi: 10.3390/molecules25122910. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Coman Vasile, Vodnar Dan. Hydroxycinnamic acids and human health: Recent advances. Journal of the Science of Food and Agriculture. 2020;100:483–99. doi: 10.1002/jsfa.10010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Cortez Mariela Valentina, Miranda Agustín Ramiro, Scotta Ana Veronica, Aballay Laura Rosana, Soria Elio Andrés. Patrones alimentarios de mujeres argentinas durante el puerperio en relación con factores socioeconómicos y sanitarios. Revista Médica del Instituto Mexicano del Seguro Social. 2021;59:7–16. doi: 10.24875/RMIMSS.M21000047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Cortez Mariela Valentina, Soria Elio Andrés. The effect of freeze-drying on the nutrient, polyphenol, and oxidant levels of breast milk. Breastfeeding Medicine. 2016;11:551–54. doi: 10.1089/bfm.2016.0102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Crawford Lindsay, Li Hong, Zou Liye, Wei Gao-Xia, Loprinzi Paul. Hypothesized mechanisms through which exercise may attenuate memory interference. Medicina. 2020;56:129. doi: 10.3390/medicina56030129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Cropley Vanessa, Croft Rodney, Silber Beata, Neale Chris, Scholey Andrew, Stough Con, Schmitt Jeroen. Does coffee enriched with chlorogenic acids improve mood and cognition after acute administration in healthy elderly? A pilot study. Psychopharmacology. 2012;219:737–49. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2395-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Davis Ronald L., Zhong Yi. The biology of forgetting—A perspective. Neuron. 2017;95:490–503. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.05.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. De la Cruz María Victoria. Manual del Test de Clasificación de Tarjetas de Wisconsin (WCST) TEA Ediciones; Madrid: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  32. Demmelmair Hans, Koletzko Berthold. Lipids in human milk. Best Practice & Research Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 2018;32:57–68. doi: 10.1016/j.beem.2017.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Downing Laura E., Edgar Daniel, Ellison Patricia A., Ricketts Marie-Louisse. Mechanistic insight into nuclear receptor-mediated regulation of bile acid metabolism and lipid homeostasis by grape seed procyanidin extract (GSPE) Cell Biochemistry and Function. 2017;35:12–32. doi: 10.1002/cbf.3247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Edwards William. Motor Learning and Control: From Theory to Practice. Cengage Learning; Wadsworth: 2010. [Google Scholar]
  35. Ehrmann Jirí, Vavrusová Nicol, Collan Yrjo, Kolár Zdenek. Peroxisome proliferator-activated receptors (PPARs) in health and disease. Biomedical Papers of the Medical Faculty of the University Palacky, Olomouc, Czechoslovakia. 2002;146:11–14. doi: 10.5507/bp.2002.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Elisia Ingrid, Kitts David D. Quantification of hexanal as an index of lipid oxidation in human milk and association with antioxidant components. Journal of Clinical Biochemistry and Nutrition. 2011;49:147. doi: 10.3164/jcbn.10-142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Fallon Victoria, Groves Rachael, Halford Jason Christian Grovenor, Bennett Kate Mary, Harrold Joan Allison. Postpartum anxiety and infant-feeding outcomes: A systematic review. Journal of Human Lactation. 2016;32:740–58. doi: 10.1177/0890334416662241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Farràs Marta, Valls Rosa M., Fernández-Castillejo Sara, Giralt Montserrat, Solà Rosa, Subirana Isaac, Motilva María José, Konstantinidou Valentini, Covas María Isabel, Fitó Montserrat. Olive oil polyphenols enhance the expression of cholesterol efflux related genes in vivo in humans. A randomized controlled trial. The Journal of Nutritional Biochemistry. 2013;24:1334–1339. doi: 10.1016/j.jnutbio.2012.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Faustino Bruno, Oliveira Jorge, Lopes Paulo. Normative scores of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in a sample of the adult Portuguese population. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult. 2020;27:1–8. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2020.1810040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Faustino B., Oliveira J., Lopes P. Diagnostic precision of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in assessing cognitive deficits in substance use disorders. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult. 2021;28:165–72. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2019.1607737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Feng Juan, Yang Jian, Chang Yujun, Qiao Liansheng, Dang Honglei, Luo Kun, Guo Hongyan, An Yannan, Ma Chengmei, Shao Hong, et al. Caffeine-free hawk tea lowers cholesterol by reducing free cholesterol uptake and the production of very-low-density lipoprotein. Communications Biology. 2019;2:1–18. doi: 10.1038/s42003-019-0396-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Ferreira Correia A., Osorio I. Campagna. The Rey auditory verbal learning test: Normative data developed for the venezuelan population. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2014;29:206–15. doi: 10.1093/arclin/act070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Fox John. Applied Regression Analysis and Generalized Linear Models. Sage Publications; Thousand Oaks: 2015. [Google Scholar]
  44. Frutos Bernal Elisa, Rey Ángel Martin del, Villardón María Purificación Galindo. Analysis of madrid metro network: From structural to HJ-biplot perspective. Applied Sciences. 2020;10:5689. doi: 10.3390/app10165689. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Fu Manjie, Zhang Lingsong, Ahmed Azza, Plaut Karen, Haas David M., Szucs Kinga, Casey Theresa M. Does Circadian Disruption Play a Role in the Metabolic-Hormonal Link to Delayed Lactogenesis II? Frontiers in Nutrition. 2015;2:4. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2015.00004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. George Alexandra D., Gay Melvin C., Trengove Robert D., Geddes Donna T. Human milk lipidomics: Current techniques and methodologies. Nutrients. 2018;10:1169. doi: 10.3390/nu10091169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Gilsoul Jessica, Simon Jessica, Hogge Michaël, Collette Fabienne. Do attentional capacities and processing speed mediate the effect of age on executive functioning? Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition. 2019;26:282–317. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2018.1432746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Greendale Gail A., Huang Mei-Hua, Leung Katherine, Crawford Sybil L., Gold Ellen B., Wight Richard, Waetjen Elaine, Karlamangla Arun S. Dietary phytoestrogen intakes and cognitive function during the menopause transition: Results from the SWAN phytoestrogen study. Menopause. 2012;19:894–903. doi: 10.1097/gme.0b013e318242a654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Habtemariam Solomon. Protective effects of caffeic acid and the Alzheimer’s brain: An update. Mini Reviews in Medicinal Chemistry. 2017;17:667–74. doi: 10.2174/1389557516666161130100947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Heaton Robert K., Chelune Gordon J., Talley Jack L., Kay Gary G., Curtiss Glenn. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual: Revised and Expanded. Psychological Assessment Resources; Odessa: 1993. [Google Scholar]
  51. Henríquez Fernando, Cabello Victoria, Baez Sandra, Souza Leonardo Cruz de, Lillo Patricia, Martínez-Pernía David, Olavarría Loreto, Torralva Teresa, Slachevsky Andrea. Multidimensional Clinical Assessment in Frontotemporal Dementia and Its Spectrum in Latin America and the Caribbean: A Narrative Review and a Glance at Future Challenges. Frontiers in Neurology. 2022;12:768591. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.768591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Herchi Wahid, Arráez-Román David, Trabelsi Hajer, Bouali Intidhar, Boukhchina Sadok, Kallel Habib, Segura-Carretero Antonio, Fernández-Gutierrez Alberto. Phenolic compounds in flaxseed: A review of their properties and analytical methods. An overview of the last decade. Journal of Oleo Science. 2014;63:7–14. doi: 10.5650/jos.ess13135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hohman Timothy J., Beason-Held Lori L., Lamar Melissa, Resnick Susan M. Subjective cognitive complaints and longitudinal changes in memory and brain function. Neuropsychology. 2011;25:125–30. doi: 10.1037/a0020859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Jonas Wibke, Woodside Barbara. Physiological mechanisms, behavioral and psychological factors influencing the transfer of milk from mothers to their young. Hormones and Behavior. 2016;77:167–81. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.07.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Jurek Benjamin, Neumann Inga D. The Oxytocin Receptor: From Intracellular Signaling to Behavior. Physiological Reviews. 2018;98:1805–908. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00031.2017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kawano Atsuko, Emori Yoko. The relationship between maternal postpartum psychological state and breast milk secretory immunoglobulin A level. Journal of the American Psychiatric Nurses Association. 2015;21:23–30. doi: 10.1177/1078390314566882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Kennedy David O., Jackson Philippa A., Forster Joanne, Khan Julie, Grothe Torsten, Perrinjaquet-Moccetti Tania, Haskell-Ramsay Crystal F. Acute effects of a wild green-oat (Avena sativa) extract on cognitive function in middle-aged adults: A double-blind, placebo-controlled, within-subjects trial. Nutritional Neuroscience. 2017;20:135–51. doi: 10.1080/1028415X.2015.1101304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kesse-Guyot Emmanuelle, Fezeu Léopold, Andreeva Valentina A., Touvier Mathilde, Scalbert Augustin, Hercberg Serge, Galan Pilar. Total and specific polyphenol intakes in midlife are associated with cognitive function measured 13 years later. The Journal of Nutrition. 2012;142:76–83. doi: 10.3945/jn.111.144428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kiełbasa Anna, Gadzała-Kopciuch Renata, Buszewski Boguslaw. Cytokines-Biogenesis and Their Role in Human Breast Milk and Determination. International Journal of Molecular Sciences. 2021;22:6238. doi: 10.3390/ijms22126238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Koletzko Berthold. Human milk lipids. Annals of Nutrition & Metabolism. 2016;69:28–40. doi: 10.1159/000452819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kreijkamp-Kaspers Sanne, Kok Linda, Grobbee Diederick, Haan Edward De, Aleman André, Schouw Yvonne T. Van Der. Dietary phytoestrogen intake and cognitive function in older women. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2007;62:556–62. doi: 10.1093/gerona/62.5.556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Kubota Shizuka, Tanaka Yuma, Nagaoka Satoshi. Ellagic acid affects mRNA expression levels of genes that regulate cholesterol metabolism in HepG2 cells. Bioscience, Biotechnology, and Biochemistry. 2019;83:952–59. doi: 10.1080/09168451.2019.1576498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Kumari Suchitra, Bahinipati Jyotirmayee, Pradhan Tapaswini, Sahoo Durgesh P. Comparison of test performance of biochemical parameters in semiautomatic method and fully automatic analyzer method. Journal of Family Medicine and Primary Care. 2020;9:3994–4000. doi: 10.4103/jfmpc.jfmpc_94_20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Kwon Seung-Hwan, Lee Ha-Kyung, Kim Ji-Ah, Hong Sa-Ik, Kim Hyoung-Chun, Jo Tae-Hyung, Park Young-In, Lee Chong-Kil, Kim Yong-Bin, Lee Seok-Yong, et al. Neuroprotective effects of chlorogenic acid on scopolamine-induced amnesia via anti-acetylcholinesterase and anti-oxidative activities in mice. European Journal of Pharmacology. 2010;649:210–17. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2010.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Lamport Daniel J., Lawton Clare L., Merat Natasha, Jamson Hamish, Myrissa Kyriaki, Hofman Denise, Chadwick Helen K., Quadt Frits, Wightman JoLynne D., Dye Louise. Concord grape juice, cognitive function, and driving performance: A 12-wk, placebo-controlled, randomized crossover trial in mothers of preteen children. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2016a;103:775–83. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.115.114553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Lamport Daniel J., Pal Deepa, Macready Anna L., Barbosa-Boucas Sofia, Fletcher John M., Williams Claire M., Spencer Jeremy PE, Butler Laurie T. The effects of flavanone-rich citrus juice on cognitive function and cerebral blood flow: An acute, randomised, placebo-controlled cross-over trial in healthy, young adults. British Journal of Nutrition. 2016b;116:2160–68. doi: 10.1017/S000711451600430X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Lezak Muriel Deutsch, Howieson Diane B., Bigler Erin D., Tranel Daniel. Neuropsychological Assessment. 5th ed. Oxford University Press; New York: 2012. [Google Scholar]
  68. Lin Man, Wang Na, Yao Bei, Zhong Yao, Lin Yan, You Tianhui. Quercetin improves postpartum hypogalactia in milk-deficient mice via stimulating prolactin production in pituitary gland. Phytotherapy Research. 2018;32:1511–20. doi: 10.1002/ptr.6079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Lozoya-Delgado Paz, Ruiz-Sánchez José María de León, Pedrero-Perez Eduardo J. Validation of a cognitive complaints questionnaire for young adults: The relation between subjective memory complaints, prefrontal symptoms and perceived stress. Revista de Neurología. 2012;54:137–50. doi: 10.33588/rn.5403.2011283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Luca Simon Vlad, Macovei Irina, Bujor Alexandra, Miron Anca, Skalicka-Woźniak Krystyna, Aprotosoaie Ana Clara, Trifan Adriana. Bioactivity of dietary polyphenols: The role of metabolites. Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition. 2020;60:626–59. doi: 10.1080/10408398.2018.1546669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Mancini Edele, Beglinger Cristoph, Drewe Jürgen, Zanchi Davide, Lang Undine E., Borgwardt Stefan. Green tea effects on cognition, mood and human brain function: A systematic review. Phytomedicine. 2017;34:26–37. doi: 10.1016/j.phymed.2017.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Manzar Md Dilshad, Jahrami Haitham A., Bahammam Ahmed S. Structural validity of the Insomnia Severity Index: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2021;60:101531. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2021.101531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Margină Denisa, Ungurianu Anca, Purdel Carmen, Nițulescu George Mihai, Tsoukalas Dimitris, Sarandi Evangelia, Thanasoula Maria, Burykina Tatyana I., Tekos Fotis, Buha Aleksandra, et al. Analysis of the intricate effects of polyunsaturated fatty acids and polyphenols on inflammatory pathways in health and disease. Food and Chemical Toxicology. 2020;143:111558. doi: 10.1016/j.fct.2020.111558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Marino Julián C., Alderete Ana María. Valores Normativos de Pruebas de Fluidez Verbal Categoriales, Fonológicas, Gramaticales y Combinadas y Análisis Comparativo de la Capacidad de Iniciación. Revista Neuropsicología, Neuropsiquiatría y Neurociencias. 2010;10:82–93. [Google Scholar]
  75. Matacchione Giulia, Gurău Felicia, Baldoni Simone, Prattichizzo Francesco, Silvestrini Andrea, Giuliani Angelica, Pugnaloni Armanda, Espinosa Emma, Amenta Francesco, Bonafè Massimiliano, et al. Pleiotropic effects of polyphenols on glucose and lipid metabolism: Focus on clinical trials. Ageing Research Reviews. 2020;61:101074. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2020.101074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. McManaman James L. Lipid transport in the lactating mammary gland. Journal of mammary Gland Biology and Neoplasia. 2014;19:35–42. doi: 10.1007/s10911-014-9318-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Mendonça Marcelo D., Alves Luísa, Bugalho Paulo. From Subjective Cognitive Complaints to Dementia: Who is at Risk?: A Systematic Review. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and other Dementias. 2016;31:105–14. doi: 10.1177/1533317515592331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Mías Carlos D. Quejas de Memoria y Deterioro Cognitivo Leve: Concepto, Evaluación y Prevención. Encuentro; Córdoba: 2010. [Google Scholar]
  79. Milošević Maja, Arsić Aleksandra, Cvetković Zorica, Vučić Vesna. Memorable Food: Fighting Age-Related Neurodegeneration by Precision Nutrition. Frontiers in Nutrition. 2021;8:688086. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2021.688086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Miranda Agustín Ramiro, Albrecht Claudia, Cortez Mariela Valentina, Soria Elio Andrés. Pharmacology and toxicology of polyphenols with potential as neurotropic agents in non-communicable diseases. Current Drug Targets. 2018;19:97–110. doi: 10.2174/1389450117666161220152336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Miranda Agustín Ramiro, Sierra Juliana Franchetto, Roulet Amparo Martínez, Rivadero Luisina, Serra Silvana Valeria, Soria Elio Andrés. Age, education and gender effects on Wisconsin card sorting test: Standardization, reliability and validity in healthy Argentinian adults. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition. 2020a;27:807–25. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2019.1693491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Miranda Agustín Ramiro, Rivadero Luisina, Bruera Jorge Ángel, Villarreal Viriginia, Bernio Laura Yhicel, Baydas María de Los Ángeles, Brizuela Mónica Liliana, Serra Silvana Valeria. Examining the relationship between engagement and perceived stress-related cognitive complaints in the Argentinian working population. Europe’s Journal of Psychology. 2020b;16:12–31. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v16i1.1832. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Miranda Agustín Ramiro, Rivadero Luisina, Serra Silvana Valeria, Soria Elio Andrés. Multidomain self-report assessment of fronto-executive complaints in Spanish-speaking adults. Psychology and Neuroscience. 2020c;13:357–74. doi: 10.1037/pne0000187. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  84. Miranda Agustín Ramiro, Cortez Mariela Valentina, Scotta Ana Veronica, Rivadero Luisina, Serra Silvana Valeria, Soria Elio Andrés. Memory enhancement in Argentinian women during postpartum by the dietary intake of lignans and anthocyanins. Nutrition Research. 2021a;85:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.nutres.2020.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Miranda Agustín Ramiro, Scotta Ana Veronica, Cortez Mariela Valentina, Soria Elio Andrés. Triggering of postpartum depression and insomnia with cognitive impairment in Argentinian women during the pandemic COVID-19 social isolation in relation to reproductive and health factors. Midwifery. 2021b;102:103072. doi: 10.1016/j.midw.2021.103072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Miranda Agustín Ramiro, Cortez Mariela Valentina, Scotta Ana Veronica, Soria Elio Andrés. COVID-19-related stress in postpartum women from Argentina during the second wave in 2021: Identification of impairing and protective factors. Midwifery. 2022a;108:103290. doi: 10.1016/j.midw.2022.103290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Miranda Agustín Ramiro, Scotta Ana Veronica, Cortez Mariela Valentina, González-García Nerea, Galindo-Villardón María Purificación, Soria Elio Andrés. Association of dietary intake of polyphenols with an adequate nutritional profile in postpartum. Preventive Nutrition & Food Science. 2022b;27:20–36. doi: 10.3746/pnf.2022.27.1.20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Mishra Prabhaker, Singh Uttam, Pandey Chandra M., Mishra Prishadarshni, Pandey Gaurav. Application of student’s t-test, analysis of variance, and covariance. Annals of Cardiac Anaesthesia. 2019;22:407. doi: 10.4103/aca.ACA_94_19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Moghaddam Akbar Hajizadeh, Zare Mahboobeh. Neuroprotective effect of hesperetin and nano-hesperetin on recognition memory impairment and the elevated oxygen stress in rat model of Alzheimer’s disease. Biomedicine & Pharmacotherapy. 2018;97:1096–101. doi: 10.1016/j.biopha.2017.11.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Mohammad Mahmoud A., Haymond Morey W. Regulation of lipid synthesis genes and milk fat production in human mammary epithelial cells during secretory activation. American Journal of Physiology. Endocrinology and Metabolism. 2013;305:E700–16. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.00052.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Mohd Shukri Nurul Husna, Wells Jonathan, Eaton Simon, Mukhtar Firdaus, Petelin Ana, Jenko-Pražnikar Zala, Fewtrell Mary. Randomized controlled trial investigating the effects of a breastfeeding relaxation intervention on maternal psychological state, breast milk outcomes, and infant behavior and growth. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2019;110:121–30. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqz033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Morin Charles M., Belleville Geneviève, Bélanger Lynda, Ivers Hans. The Insomnia Severity Index: Psychometric indicators to detect insomnia cases and evaluate treatment response. Sleep. 2011;34:601–8. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.5.601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Nieto-Librero A. B., Sierra C., Vicente-Galindo M. P., Ruíz-Barzola O., Galindo-Villardón M. P. Clustering Disjoint HJ-Biplot: A new tool for identifying pollution patterns in geochemical studies. Chemosphere. 2017;176:389–96. doi: 10.1016/j.chemosphere.2017.02.125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Ontsouka Edgar C., Albrecht Christiane. Cholesterol transport and regulation in the mammary gland. Journal of Mammary Gland Biology and Neoplasia. 2014;19:43–58. doi: 10.1007/s10911-014-9316-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Palin Marie France, Côrtes Cristiano, Benchaar Chaouki, Lacasse Pierre, Petit Hélène V. mRNA Expression of lipogenic enzymes in mammary tissue and fatty acid profile in milk of dairy cows fed flax hulls and infused with flax oil in the abomasum. British Journal of Nutrition. 2014;111:1011–20. doi: 10.1017/S0007114513003589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Pérez-Rodríguez Lorenzo, Romero-Haro Ana A., Sternalski Audrey, Muriel Jaime, Mougeot Francois, Gil Diego, Alonso-Alvarez Carlos. Measuring oxidative stress: The confounding effect of lipid concentration in measures of lipid peroxidation. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology. 2015;88:345–51. doi: 10.1086/680688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Poniedziałek Barbara, Rzymski Piotr, Pięt Malgorzata, Gąsecka Monika, Stroińska Anna, Niedzielski Przemyslaw, Mleczek Miroslaw, Rzymski Pawel, Wilczak Maciej. Relation between polyphenols, malondialdehyde, antioxidant capacity, lactate dehydrogenase and toxic elements in human colostrum milk. Chemosphere. 2018;191:548–54. doi: 10.1016/j.chemosphere.2017.10.098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Rabadan-Chávez Griselda, Quevedo-Corona Lucia, Garcia Angel Miliar, Reyes-Maldonado Elba, Jaramillo-Flores Maria. Cocoa powder, cocoa extract and epicatechin attenuate hypercaloric diet-induced obesity through enhanced β-oxidation and energy expenditure in white adipose tissue. Journal of Functional Foods. 2016;20:54–67. doi: 10.1016/j.jff.2015.10.016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  99. Rey André. L’examen Clinique en Psychologie. [The Clinical Examination in Psychology] Presses Universitaries De France; Paris: 1958. [Google Scholar]
  100. Ríos Jimena, Valero-Jara Viviana, Thomas-Valdés Samanta. Phytochemicals in breast milk and their benefits for infants. Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition. 2021:1–16. doi: 10.1080/10408398.2021.1906627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Rivera Diego, Perrin Paul B., Stevens Lillian Flores, Garza María Teresa, Weil Carlos, Saracho Carmen Patricia, Rodriguez Walter, Rodríguez-Agudelo Yaneth, Rábago Brenda, Weiler Gudrun, et al. Stroop color-word interference test: Normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. NeuroRehabilitation. 2015;37:591–624. doi: 10.3233/NRE-151281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Rodríguez Barreto Lucía Carlota, Pulido Ninfa del Carmen, Roa Carlos Alejandro Pineda. Propiedades psicométricas del Stroop, test de colores y palabras en población colombiana no patológica. Universitas Psychologica. 2016;15:255–72. doi: 10.11144/Javeriana.upsy15-2.ppst. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  103. Rodríguez-Blázquez Carmen, Ayala-García Alba, Forjaz Maria Joao, García-Herranz Sara, Venero César, Rodríguez-Fernández Raquel, Díaz-Mardomingo María del Carmen. Validation of the Spanish version of the Memory Failures of Everyday questionnaire in older adults using Rasch analysis. Geriatrics and Gerontology International. 2022;22:332–37. doi: 10.1111/ggi.14364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Roman-Viñas Blanca, Serra-Majem Lluis, Hagströmer Maria, Ribas-Barba Lourdes, Sjöström Michael, Segura-Cardona Ramon. International physical activity questionnaire: Reliability and validity in a Spanish population. European Journal of Sport Science. 2010;10:297–304. doi: 10.1080/17461390903426667. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  105. Rossi María Constanza, Bassett María Natalia, Samman Norma Cristina. Dietary nutritional profile and phenolic compounds consumption in school children of highlands of Argentine Northwest. Food Chemistry. 2018;238:111–16. doi: 10.1016/j.foodchem.2016.12.065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Rothwell Joseph A., Perez-Jimenez Jara, Neveu Vanessa, Medina-Remon Alexander, M’hiri Nouha, García-Lobato Paula, Manach Claudine, Knox Craig, Eisner Roman, Wishart David S., et al. Phenol-Explorer 3.0: A major update of the Phenol-Explorer database to incorporate data on the effects of food processing on polyphenol content. Database. 2013:2013. doi: 10.1093/database/bat070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Rufino Ana T., Costa Vera M., Carvalho Félix, Fernandes Eduarda. Flavonoids as antiobesity agents: A review. Medicinal Research Reviews. 2021;41:556–85. doi: 10.1002/med.21740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Salehi Bahare, Cruz-Martins Natália, Butnariu Monica, Sarac Ioan, Bagiu Iulia-Cristina, Ezzat Shahira M., Wang Jinfan, Koay Aaron, Sheridan Helen, Adetunji Charles Oluwaseun, et al. Hesperetin’s health potential: Moving from preclinical to clinical evidence and bioavailability issues, to upcoming strategies to overcome current limitations. Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition. 2021:1–16. doi: 10.1080/10408398.2021.1875979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Scarpina Federica, Tagini Sofía. The Stroop Color and Word Test. Frontiers in Psychology. 2017;8:557. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Scotta Ana Veronica, Cortez Mariela Valentina, Miranda Agustín Ramiro. Insomnia is associated with worry, cognitive avoidance and low academic engagement in Argentinian university students during the COVID-19 social isolation. Psychology, Health & Medicine. 2022;27:199–214. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2020.1869796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Senthil Renganathan, Sakthivel Manokaran, Usha Singaravelu. Structure-based drug design of peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor gamma inhibitors: Ferulic acid and derivatives. Journal of Biomolecular Structure & Dynamics. 2021;39:1295–311. doi: 10.1080/07391102.2020.1740790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Shin Na-Young, Bak Yunjin, Nah Yoonjin, Han Sanghoon, Kim Dong Joon, Kim Se Joo, Lee Jong Eun, Lee Sang Guk, Lee Seung Koo. Disturbed retrieval network and prospective memory decline in postpartum women. Scientific Reports. 2018;8:5476. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-23875-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Singh Yash Pal, Rai Himanshu, Singh Gourav, Singh Gireesh Kumar, Mishra Sunil, Kumar Saroj, Srikrishna S., Modi Gyan. A review on ferulic acid and analogs based scaffolds for the management of Alzheimer’s disease. European Journal of Medicinal Chemistry. 2021;215:113278. doi: 10.1016/j.ejmech.2021.113278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Shieh Gwowen. Power analysis and sample size planning in ANCOVA designs. Psychometrika. 2020;85:101–20. doi: 10.1007/s11336-019-09692-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Singla Rajeev K., Dubey Ashok K., Garg Arun, Sharma Ramesh K., Fiorino Marco, Ameen Sara M., Haddad Moawiya A., Al-Hiary Masnat. Natural polyphenols: Chemical classification, definition of classes, subcategories, and structures. Journal of AOAC International. 2019;102:1397–400. doi: 10.5740/jaoacint.19-0133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Stuebe Allison M., Grewen Karen, Pedersen Cort A., Propper Cathi, Meltzer-Brody Samantha. Failed lactation and perinatal depression: Common problems with shared neuroendocrine mechanisms? Journal of Women’s Health. 2012;21:264–72. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2011.3083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Stroop John Ridley. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1935;18:643. doi: 10.1037/h0054651. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  118. Troubat Nicolas, Fargeas-Gluck Marie-Agnes, Tulppo Mikko, Dugué Benoit. The stress of chess players as a model to study the effects of psychological stimuli on physiological responses: An example of substrate oxidation and heart rate variability in man. European Journal of Applied Physiology. 2009;105:343–49. doi: 10.1007/s00421-008-0908-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Tull Matthe T., Maack Danielle J., Viana Andres G., Gratz Kim L. Behavioral inhibition and attentional network functioning. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy. 2012;41:1–4. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2011.614273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Tyagi Rahul, Arvind Harshita, Goyal Manoj, Anand Akshay, Mohanty Manju. Working Memory Alterations Plays an Essential Role in Developing Global Neuropsychological Impairment in Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy. Frontiers in Psychology. 2021;11:613242. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.613242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Valls-Bellés Victoria, Abad Cristina, Hernández-Aguilar María Teresa, Nacher Amalia, Guerrero Carlos, Baliño Pablo, Romero Francisco J., Muriach María. Human Milk Antioxidative Modifications in Mastitis: Further Beneficial Effects of Cranberry Supplementation. Antioxidants. 2021;11:51. doi: 10.3390/antiox11010051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Vauzour David, Vafeiadou Katerina, Rice-Evans Catherine, Williams Robert J., Spencer Jeremy P. Activation of pro-survival Akt and ERK1/2 signalling pathways underlie the anti-apoptotic effects of flavanones in cortical neurons. Journal of Neurochemistry. 2007;103:1355–67. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2007.04841.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Venkat Nanditha, Sinha Meenakshi, Sinha Ramanjan, Ghate Jayshri, Pande Babita. Neuro-Cognitive Profile of Morning and Evening Chronotypes at Different Times of Day. Annals of Neurosciences. 2020;27:257–65. doi: 10.1177/0972753121990280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Vicente-Villardón José Luis. MultBiplot . A Package for Multivariate Analysis Using Biplots. Departamento de Estadística, Universidad de Salamanca; Salamanca: 2015. [Google Scholar]
  125. Wang Zhuo, You Linjun, Cheng Yan, Hu Kaiyong, Wang Zhanbo, Cheng Yanan, Yang Jin, Yang Yong, Wang Guangji. Investigation of pharmacokinetics, tissue distribution and excretion of schisandrin B in rats by HPLC–MS/MS. Biomedical Chromatography. 2018;32:e4069. doi: 10.1002/bmc.4069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Whyte Adrian R., Williams Claire M. Effects of a single dose of a flavonoid-rich blueberry drink on memory in 8 to 10 y old children. Nutrition. 2015;31:531–34. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2014.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Wightman Emma L., Haskell Crystal F., Forster Joanne S., Veasey Rachel C., Kennedy David O. Epigallocatechin gallate, cerebral blood flow parameters, cognitive performance and mood in healthy humans: A double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover investigation. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental. 2012;27:177–86. doi: 10.1002/hup.1263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Williamson Gary, Kay Colin D., Crozier Alan. The bioavailability, transport, and bioactivity of dietary flavonoids: A review from a historical perspective. Comprehensive Reviews in Food Science and Food Safety. 2018;17:1054–112. doi: 10.1111/1541-4337.12351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Xavier Soniya, Soch Alita, Younesi Simin, Malik Sajida, Spencer Sarah, Sominsky Luba. Maternal diet before and during pregnancy modulates microglial activation and neurogenesis in the postpartum rat brain. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2021;98:185–97. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2021.08.223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Yang Dengbao, Huynh HoangDinh, Wan Yihong. Milk lipid regulation at the maternal-offspring interface. Seminars in Cell and Developmental Biology. 2018;81:141–48. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2017.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Yao Junpeng, Hu Pengcheng, Zhu Yanhong, Xu Yingyan, Tan Qingsong, Liang Xufang. Lipid-Lowering Effects of Lotus Leaf Alcoholic Extract on Serum, Hepatopancreas, and Muscle of Juvenile Grass Carp via Gene Expression. Frontiers in Physiology. 2020;11:1480. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2020.584782. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Zhong Jing, Liang Yuxuan, Chen Yongchun, Zhang Jiawei, Zou Xiaoying, Deng Jie, Wang Da, Sun Yuanming, Li Meiying. Study and Experimental Validation of the Functional Components and Mechanisms of Hemerocallis citrina Baroni in the Treatment of Lactation Deficiency. Foods. 2021;10:1863. doi: 10.3390/foods10081863. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Ziomkiewicz Anna, Babiszewska Magdalena, Apanasewicz Anna, Piosek Magdalena, Wychowaniec Patrycja, Cierniak Agnieszka, Barbarska Olga, Szołtysik Marek, Danel Dariusz, Wichary Szymon. Psychosocial stress and cortisol stress reactivity predict breast milk composition. Scientific Reports. 2021;11:1–14. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-90980-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to ethical restrictions.


Articles from Journal of Intelligence are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES