Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2022 Jun 24;17(6):e0268121. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0268121

Developing health and environmental warning messages about red meat: An online experiment

Lindsey Smith Taillie 1,2,*, Carmen E Prestemon 1, Marissa G Hall 1,3,4, Anna H Grummon 5,6, Annamaria Vesely 2, Lindsay M Jaacks 7
Editor: John W Apolzan8
PMCID: PMC9231779  PMID: 35749387

Abstract

Introduction

The United States has among the highest per capita red meat consumption in the world. Reducing red meat consumption is crucial for minimizing the environmental impact of diets and improving health outcomes. Warning messages are effective for reducing purchases of products like sugary beverages but have not been developed for red meat. This study developed health and environmental warning messages about red meat and explored participants’ reactions to these messages.

Methods

A national convenience sample of US red meat consumers (n = 1,199; mean age 45 years) completed an online survey in 2020 for this exploratory study. Participants were randomized to view a series of either health or environmental warning messages (between-subjects factor) about the risks associated with eating red meat. Messages were presented in random order (within-subjects factor; 8 health messages or 10 environmental messages). Participants rated each warning message on a validated 3-item scale measuring perceived message effectiveness (PME), ranging from 1 (low) to 5 (high). Participants then rated their intentions to reduce their red meat consumption in the next 7 days.

Results

Health warning messages elicited higher PME ratings than environmental messages (mean 2.66 vs. 2.26, p<0.001). Health warning messages also led to stronger intentions to reduce red meat consumption compared to environmental messages (mean 2.45 vs. 2.19, p<0.001). Within category (health and environmental), most pairwise comparisons of harms were not statistically significant.

Conclusions

Health warning messages were perceived to be more effective than environmental warning messages. Future studies should measure the impact of these messages on behavioral outcomes.

Introduction

Per capita red meat consumption in the United States is among the highest in the world. On average, adults in the US consume 284 grams of unprocessed red meat per week [1] (approximately 2.5-quarter pound hamburger patties), nearly three times the recommended level identified in the landmark 2019 EAT-Lancet report Healthy Diets from Sustainable Food Systems [2]. Unprocessed red meat (defined as meat from mammals, e.g., beef, pork, lamb, goat) contains some beneficial nutrients, including protein and micronutrients such as iron and vitamin B12 [3]. However, in the US, the vast majority of the population consumes adequate protein [4] and vitamin B12 [5]. Additionally, while iron deficiency anemia is the most common nutrient deficiency in the US, prevalence is relatively low at 10.4% for females and 5.2% for males [6]. At the same time, high intake of red meat consumption is associated with a number of poor health outcomes, including increased risk of several types of cancer, type 2 diabetes, high blood pressure, high cholesterol, and premature death [7,8]. These relationships are particularly strong for processed red meats that are salted, cured, fermented, smoked or otherwise processed for flavor or preservation [9]. According to the American Cancer Society, it is unknown if there is a safe level of red meat consumption [10]. In lieu of knowledge of safe consumption levels, organizations like the American Cancer Society and the American Diabetes Association recommend limiting red meat consumption in favor of poultry, fish, or plant-based proteins [10,11].

Red meat production is also damaging to the environment. Livestock production is responsible for 14.5% of anthropogenic greenhouse gas (GHG) emissions globally [12]. Among animal products, red meat is particularly damaging; for example, beef contributes approximately five times as much GHG emissions compared to chicken [13]. Red meat production also damages the environment through deforestation and excess water use [14]. International reports by experts on food systems and climate change, including the EAT-Lancet report and the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC) report, indicate a drastic reduction in red meat consumption in high-income countries is needed to prevent further damage to the climate [15].

Given this evidence, public health practitioners and policymakers have sought strategies for reducing red meat consumption in the US. One potential population-level strategy is to use front-of-package labels to inform consumers about health and environmental effects of red meat. For example, several countries have mandatory or voluntary carbon labelling on food products, and given the large carbon footprint of red meat in particular, these labels may specifically discourage red meat consumption [16]. Additionally, several private companies across the globe have also implemented similar optional “green label” certification for products [17]. One online experiment found that providing consumers with information about GHG emissions of vegetarian and meat-containing soups reduced intentions to purchase the meat-containing soups [18]. Studies of similar carbon footprint labels and behavioral nudges on meat products in cafeteria settings in Europe have also found reductions of meat-purchasing behaviors [1921].

However, these studies focused on numerical labels about carbon emissions, rather than warning messages that make a direct statement about the health or environmental harms of a product. Previous research has highlighted the promise of warning labels to reduce purchases and consumption of tobacco products [22,23], sugary beverages [24,25], and junk food and alcohol [26]. Yet, currently there is very little knowledge about warning labels on red meat. Specifically, limited research has compared health and environmental warning messages side by side or identified the most promising health and environmental harms to include in warning messages. We recently published a study comparing two specific health warnings (about cancer and early death) and two specific environmental warnings (about environmental damage and carbon emissions) [27]; here, we describe the exploratory research that led to the development of the specific warnings used in that study.

Research is also needed to assess potential differences in responses to the warning messages based on demographic characteristics, in order to inform policies to promote equity. A systematic review on consumer perspectives towards environmental concerns of red meat consumption found that females are more likely than males to be willing to reduce meat consumption after being exposed to environmental harm messages [28]. Individuals with higher education levels tend to comprehend high-literacy sustainability and carbon labels compared to those with less education [29]. Other demographic characteristics might also affect responses to red meat warnings, but limited research has explored this question.

The objective of this study was to develop health and environmental warning messages about red meat consumption and examine US consumers’ reactions to these messages, with the overarching goal of identifying which messages hold the most promise for use in future studies to test behavioral impact. We further aimed to assess potential differences in message reactions across demographic groups. Our primary outcome was perceived message effectiveness (PME), a measure that has been used extensively in similar experiments to identify the potential impact of food and beverage warning labels on consumers [27,3034]. PME is a measure that is sensitive enough to detect small differences between warnings yet is also predictive as to messages’ ability to change actual behaviors [35].

Methods

Message development

We designed messages using a multistage process similar to studies to evaluate health warnings for other products [31,36]. First, we conducted a review of the literature on the health and environmental outcomes associated with red meat to select outcomes. Outcomes were included if recent meta-analyses, systematic reviews, or expert consensus statements indicated an association between red meat consumption and the outcome (Table 1). High-quality longitudinal cohort studies are included in the below table if they were published after the most recent systematic review or meta-analysis on the topic.

Table 1. Health and environmental outcomes selected for warning messages and supporting literature.

Health Outcome Key Citations Professional Association Dietary Recommendation for Red Meat Consumption
Type 2 diabetes [37,38] American Diabetes Association [39]:
Recommendation: “limiting intake” of red meat for diabetes prevention.
Several types of cancer
Colon and rectal cancer
Colon cancer
[4042] American Cancer Society [43]:
Notes: “It is not known if there is a safe level of consumption for either red or processed meats.”
Recommendation: “choosing protein foods such as fish, poultry, and beans more often than red meat, and for people who eat processed meat products to do so sparingly, if at all.”
Cardiovascular disease
Heart damage
[44] American Heart Association [45]:
Notes: “In general, red meats (beef, pork and lamb) have more saturated (bad) fat than chicken, fish and vegetable proteins such as beans. Saturated and trans fats can raise your blood cholesterol and make heart disease worse.”
Recommendation: “Minimize processed red meats like bacon, ham, salami, sausages, hot dogs, beef jerky and deli slices…Choose nonfried fish, shellfish, poultry without the skin, and trimmed lean meats, no more than 5.5 ounces, cooked, per day.”
Stroke [46,47] The American Stroke Association is a division of the American Heart Association; see above recommendations from the American Heart Association [45].
Early death [48,49] N/A no professional association
Environmental Outcome Key Citations
Climate change/ global warming [12,50]
Climate change, which leads to extreme weather events [51]
Water pollution [50,52]
Carbon footprint/greenhouse gas emissions [13]
Deforestation [15]
Water shortages [52,53]
[harms the] environment/ planet [2,50]

Based on this literature review, we created 14 health messages and 16 environmental messages. We then convened an Expert Advisory Committee comprised of experts in nutrition, environment, food policy, agriculture policy, and law to review and provide feedback on both the warning message wording and scientific evidence supporting each of the harms. From this meeting, we created a final list of eight health messages and ten environmental messages to be used in this experiment. The number of messages selected for health and environment reflected the number of distinct topics for which we found scientific support (Table 1), as well as discussions with the expert advisory committee. The health harms were: diabetes; several types of cancer; colon cancer; colon and rectal cancer; cardiovascular disease; heart damage; stroke; and early death (e.g., “Eating red meat increases your risk of early death”). Environmental harms were: climate change; climate change, which leads to extreme weather events; global warming; carbon footprint; greenhouse gas emissions; water pollution; water shortages; deforestation; environment; and planet. We chose to include two messages about climate change (one general and one that linked climate change to extreme weather events) to assess whether consumers have stronger reactions to a climate-related message when a concrete environmental outcome (like extreme weather) is used, as the general population often has difficulty with understanding terminology related to climate change [54]. The messages tested are presented in Table 2.

Table 2. Text of health and environmental warning messages tested in online experiment.

Health Warning Messages Environmental Warning messages
Eating red meat increases your risk of… Eating red meat…
type 2 diabetes. contributes to climate change.
several types of cancer. contributes to climate change, which leads to extreme weather events.
colon cancer. contributes to global warming.
colon cancer and rectal cancer. increases your carbon footprint.
cardiovascular disease. increases your greenhouse gas emissions.
heart damage. contributes to water shortages.
stroke. increases water pollution.
early death. increases deforestation.
harms the environment.
harms the planet.

To create stimuli for the survey, we mimicked current nutrient warning labels on food products in Latin America with regards to color (black) and shape (octagonal) [55]. We used an octagon-shaped label and included the prefix ‘WARNING’ because these features have been found to be effective in attracting attention and maximizing message effectiveness among US adults [31]. Example stimuli are shown in Fig 1.

Fig 1. Example of health and environmental warning images tested in online experiment of 1,199 US adults (March 2020).

Fig 1

Participants

From March 22-March 25, 2020, we recruited a sample of 1,199 U.S. adults aged 18 years and older using Cloud Research’s Prime Panels, an online participant recruitment company commonly used by social and behavioral science researchers [56]. Eligibility criteria were being aged 18 years or older and consumption of any red meat in the past 30 days. We used an item adapted from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) to measure consumption of red meat in the past 30 days (“In the past 30 days, how often did you eat red meat? Red meat includes beef, lamb, pork, sausage, and ham. It also includes processed red meats such as bacon, hot dogs, and lunch meats. It does not include chicken, turkey, or seafood products.” Response options: Never, Less than once a week, Once a week, 2–3 times a week, 4–6 times per week, 1 time per day, 2 times per day, 3 or more times per day) [57]. No a priori power calculations were conducted for this exploratory study.

Procedures

The exploratory study was conducted online using the survey platform Qualtrics. Median completion time was 10 minutes. The main experiment employed a mixed between-within design (Fig 2). Eligible participants were randomized to view a series of either health or environmental warning messages (between-subjects factor). The within-subjects factor was the specific health or environmental harm included in the warning message. Participants randomized to the health arm viewed a series of eight health warning messages, and those randomized to the environmental arm viewed a series of ten environmental warning messages (Table 2). Similar to previous message development studies [58], this study omitted a control group, because the main purpose was to understand whether health or environmental messages were perceived as more effective relative to each other.

Fig 2. Participant flow diagram of online experiment of 1,199 U.S. adults (March 2020).

Fig 2

*Within-subjects factor was the 8 health and 10 environmental messages.

Participants viewed specific messages in random order. After viewing each message, participants answered survey questions about the message (S1 Appendix). After viewing all messages, participants were asked to select which of the messages most discouraged them from wanting to eat red meat.

Then, participants completed two additional experimental tasks to examine other important but often overlooked design choices for warning messages. First, we examined consumer reactions to differences in the framing of how the target behavior was described (behavioral framing task). All participants were randomized to view one of the following six messages: 1) Eating red meat contributes to species loss; 2) Eating too much red meat contributes to species loss; 3) Eating a lot of red meat contributes to species loss; 4) Consumption of red meat contributes to species loss; 5) Overconsumption of red meat contributes to species loss; 6) Excess consumption of red meat contributes to species loss. (Underlining is presented here to highlight differences between conditions but was not used in the survey).

Second, we examined reactions to messages with varying strength in causal language (causal variant task). Prior research on causal variants has shown that ‘causes’ is more discouraging than weaker variants such as ‘contributes to,’ ‘can contribute to,’ and ‘may contribute to’ [58]. This study aimed to test additional variations of causal language that could be applied to red meat. All participants were randomized to view one of five messages: 1) Eating red meat contributes to obesity; 2) Eating red meat may contribute to obesity; 3) Eating red meat is associated with obesity; 4) Eating red meat increases your risk of obesity; 5) Eating red meat increases your chances of obesity. (As above, underlining is presented here to highlight differences between conditions but was not used in the survey).

Measures

The primary outcome for the main experiment was PME of the health and environmental warning messages. After viewing each message, participants rated the message on a three-item PME scale. We adapted items from the UNC PME scale, which has previously been validated for use developing tobacco warnings [59]. The three PME items used in this study were: “How much does this message… 1) discourage you from wanting to eat red meat?; 2) make eating red meat seem unpleasant to you?; 3) make you concerned about the health/environmental effects of eating red meat?” The final item regarding concern about health/environmental effects was tailored to participants’ assigned condition (i.e., participants in the health warning message condition were asked about health effects and those in the environmental warning message condition were asked about environmental effects). The three PME items used five-point Likert-type response options ranging from “Not at all (coded as 1) to “A great deal” (coded as 5). Average PME (the primary outcome) was calculated by averaging responses across all three items.

Secondary outcomes for the main experiment were intentions to limit red meat consumption over the next seven days and selection of the most discouraging message in each message category (health and environmental). Intention to reduce red meat consumption in the next seven days [60] was measured using the same five-point Likert response options as PME. To measure the most discouraging message, participants were asked “Which of these messages most discourages you from wanting to eat red meat?” (underline shown in survey). The answer choices were the eight health or ten environmental warning messages (dependent upon condition).

The outcome for the behavioral framing and causal variant tasks was perceived discouragement, measured using the discouragement item of the UNC PME scale.

After completing the experiment, participants answered a series of questions on demographic characteristics as well as political affiliation and change in red meat consumption over the past 12 months.

Ethics

The University of North Carolina Chapel Hill Institutional Review Board approved this study (protocol #: 19–3349). All participants provided online written informed consent. Participants received incentives in cash, gift cards, or reward points from Prime Panels.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were conducted in Stata/SE version 14.1 (StataCorp LLC, College Station, TX). For all analyses, p<0.05 was considered statistically significant. The study protocol and hypotheses were preregistered on Aspredicted.org, prior to data collection (https://aspredicted.org/kp28j.pdf).

We used chi-square tests to check that socio-demographic characteristics were balanced across arms (Table 3). Of the nine balance tests conducted, 1 was significant: participants randomly assigned to the environmental arm were slightly less likely to report consuming less meat as compared to a year ago (24.8%) relative to participants randomly assigned to the health warning arm (23.4%) (p = 0.04).

Table 3. Characteristics of participants in online experiment of health and environmental warning messages for red meat (n = 1,199) from March 2020 in the United States.

 Characteristic  All
(n = 1,199)
Health Warnings
(n = 599)
Environmental Warnings
(n = 600)
p-value*
N (%) N (%) N (%)
Age categories n (%)  
    18–25 years 200 (16.8) 94 (15.8) 106 (17.9) 0.25
    26–34 years 214 (18.0) 114 (19.2) 100 (16.9)
    35–44 years 201 (16.9) 106 (17.8) 95 (16.0)
    45–54 years 183 (15.4) 96 (16.1) 87 (14.6)
    55–65 years 195 (16.4) 97 (16.3) 98 (16.5)
    65 years and older 195 (16.4) 88 (14.8) 107 (18.0)
Gender, n (%)
    Male  592 (49.4)  287 (47.9)  305 (50.8)  0.62
    Female  603 (50.3)  308 (51.4)  295 (49.2) 
    Non-Binary  3 (0.2)  3 (0.5)  0 (0) 
    Self-Describe  1 (0.08)  1 (0.2)  0 (0) 
Race, n (%)
    White 993 (82.3)  498 (83.1)  495 (82.5)  0.54
    Black or African American  131 (10.9)  62 (10.3)  69 (11.5) 
    Asian  30 (2.5)  30 (5.0)  30 (5.0) 
    Pacific Islander  2 (0.2)  0 (0)  2 (0.3) 
    Native American or     Alaskan Native  31 (2.6)  16 (2.7)  15 (2.5) 
    Other Race Not Listed  24 (2.0)  11 (1.8)  13 (2.2) 
Ethnicity, n (%) §
    Hispanic/Latinx  118 (9.8)  63 (10.3)  55 (9.2)  0.81
    Not Hispanic/Latinx  1081 (90.1)  536 (89.5)  545 (90.8) 
Education Level, n (%) 
    High School Degree or Less  475 (39.6)  244 (40.7)  231 (38.5)  0.97
    Associate or Technical Degree  269 (22.4)  134 (22.4)  135 (22.5) 
    4-year College Degree  314 (26.3)  146 (24.4)  168 (28.0) 
    Master’s, Graduate, or Higher  140 (11.7)  75 (12.5)  65 (10.8) 
Household Income, annual, n (%) 
    Less than $24,999  276 (23.0)  146 (24.4)  130 (21.7)  0.91
    $25,000 to $49,999  328 (27.4)  161 (20.4)  167 (27.8) 
    $50,000 to $74,999  253 (21.1)  132 (22.0)  121 (20.2) 
    $75,000 to $99,999  154 (12.8)  74 (12.4)  80 (13.3) 
    $100,000 or more  188 (15.7)  86 (13.9)  102 (16.8) 
Political Affiliation, n (%) 
    Liberal 311 (25.9)  159 (26.5)  152 (25.3)  0.06
    Moderate  522 (43.5)  251 (41.9)  271 (45.2) 
    Conservative  363 (30.3)  189 (31.5)  174 (29.0) 
Frequency of Red Meat Consumption, n (%) 
    Less than 1 per week  109 (9.1)  48 (8.0)  61 (10.2)  0.72
    1 time per week  170 (14.2)  99 (16.5)  71 (11.8) 
    2–3 times per week  552 (46.0)  268 (44.7)  284 (47.3) 
    4–6 times per week  238 (19.8)  122 (20.4)  116 (19.3) 
    1 time or more per day  130 (10.8)  62 (10.4)  68 (11.3) 
Red Meat Consumption as Compared to a Year Ago, n (%) 
    Less  379 (31.7) 184 (30.8)  195 (32.6)  0.04
    About the Same  742 (62.0)  374 (62.6)  368 (61.4) 
    More  75 (6.3)  39 (6.5)  36 (6.0) 
Believe that Human Activity is Main Cause of Climate Change, n (%) || 
    Strongly Disagree  --  --  46 (7.7)  --
    Somewhat Disagree  --  --  60 (10.0) 
    Neither Agree nor Disagree  --  --  67 (11.2) 
    Somewhat Agree  --  --  219 (36.5) 
    Strongly Agree  --  --  208 (34.7) 

*P-values were calculated using chi-square tests.

†Non-reported ages (n = 11) were set to missing.

‡Percents of race do not add up to 100% due to participants being able to select multiple races. There were 32 participants that selected multiple races. Due to small cell size, races other than Black were collapsed into an ‘other race’ category for regression analysis.

§Listed ethnicities: Mexican/Mexican American/Chicano, Cuban, Other Listed (Columbian, Dominican, Ecuadorian, Panamanian, Puerto Rican, Spanish, Venezuelan, Peruvian, Other).

||Climate change belief questions were only shown to those that were randomized to the climate warning message condition.

To assess whether PME varied by message type (health vs. environmental) in the main experiment, we used a t-test to compare PME ratings (averaged across all warning messages) between the two conditions. Results did not differ in direction or statistical significance when we used linear regression controlling for the unbalanced covariate (change in red meat consumption) (S1 Table), so we retained the unadjusted analysis. We also used t-tests to evaluate differences in intention to reduce meat consumption over the next seven days.

To compare the different messages within topic in the main experiment, we used mixed-effects linear regression, with separate models for health and for environmental messages. For both models, indicator variables were used for each warning message (excluding one), treating the intercept as random to account for repeated measures. We used postestimation commands to conduct pairwise comparisons of the predicted means, correcting for multiple comparisons. The reported p-values for these comparisons were produced using Bonferroni’s method.

To evaluate the results of the most discouraging message, we used the descriptive results of the frequency of selection of warning messages. Additionally, we conducted z-tests to assess statistical significance in the frequency of selection of the different messages.

In exploratory analyses, we used multivariate linear regression with OLS estimation to examine predictors of average PME for health and environmental messages (averaged across all warning messages shown in Table 2). Predictors were chosen based on characteristics previously found to be associated with food purchases and dietary intake overall and red meat intake in particular [61,62]. Separate regressions were conducted for health and environmental messages. The following predictors were included in the models: demographic characteristics (age, gender, race, Latinx ethnicity, income, education level, household income), political affiliation, usual red meat consumption, and change in red meat consumption in the past 12 months. For the environment model, belief in climate change was also included.

There were four participants that selected “nonbinary” or “self-describe” for their gender; these respondents were not included in the OLS regressions involving gender due to small cell size. Racial categories other than Black and white were collapsed into an “other race” category and analyzed as such in the OLS due to small sample size in other racial categories. Likewise, those who had no change in red meat consumption over the past 12 months were combined with those who had increased their red meat consumption over the past 12 months due to the fact that only 6.3% of the sample increased their red meat consumption over the past 12 months, and a dichotomous variable of “reducers” and “non-reducers” was used for analysis. In post estimation tests, no variable had a Variance Inflation Factor of 5 or greater, indicating that multicollinearity was not problematic and thus all variables were retained in the model.

For the behavioral framing and causal variant tasks, we assessed whether PME varied by message by conducting a one-way ANOVA to examine statistical differences between groups.

Results

Participants were, on average, 44.6 years old (range: 18–95). About half (50.3%) were female, and 82.3% were White (Table 3).

Differences between health and environmental messages

Health warning messages (mean PME 2.66, SD 1.18) were perceived as more effective than environmental warning messages (mean PME 2.26, SD 1.14) (p<0.001) (Table 4). Similarly, participants who viewed the health warning messages had stronger intentions to reduce red meat consumption in the next seven days (mean 2.45, SD 1.22) compared to those who viewed the environmental warning messages (mean 2.19, SD 1.17) (p<0.001).

Table 4. Perceived message effectiveness (PME)* and intention to reduce meat consumption in the next seven days* between health and environmental messages.

Measure Health Messages Environmental Messages
p-value
Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
PME 2.66 (1.18) 2.26 (1.14) <0.001
Intention to reduce meat consumption in the next 7 days 2.45 (1.22) 2.19 (1.17) <0.001

*Five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 “Not at all” to 5 “A great deal”.

†Results from t-test between health and environmental messages.

Variation of health and environmental messages (within-subjects)

Specific health harms elicited PME scores ranging from 2.46 to 2.75 (Table 5). After adjusting for multiple comparisons, nine statistically significant differences (p<0.05) between specific harms emerged. Early death and colon and rectal cancer messages elicited higher PME ratings than cardiovascular disease (p = 0.008 and p = 0.001, respectively). The diabetes message elicited lower PME ratings than all other messages (p<0.001 for all 7 comparisons).

Table 5. Perceived message effectiveness (PME)* of health and environmental harms, in descending order of PME, from an online experiment among 1,199 U.S. adults (March 2020).

Warning Message Mean Standard Deviation
Health Harm
Colon and rectal cancer 2.75A 1.28
Early death 2.73A 1.34
Several types of cancer 2.70AB 1.30
Stroke 2.69AB 1.24
Colon cancer 2.67AB 1.26
Heart damage 2.66AB 1.24
Cardiovascular disease 2.63B 1.20
Type 2 Diabetes 2.46 1.21
Environmental Harm
Environment 2.32A 1.24
Water pollution 2.30AB 1.24
Planet 2.29ABC 1.29
Climate change, which leads to extreme weather events 2.27ABC 1.23
Deforestation 2.27ABC 1.25
Global warming 2.26ABC 1.24
Greenhouse gas emissions 2.24ABC 1.20
Climate change 2.23ABC 1.23
Water shortages 2.22BC 1.27
Carbon footprint 2.20C 1.17

*Five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 “Not at all” to 5 “A great deal.”

†Means sharing a letter in the superscript are not significantly different from one another at the 5% level.

‡Results from mixed-effects linear regression, with separate models for health messages and for environmental messages.

A narrower range of PME was observed across different environmental messages (range, 2.20 to 2.32). After adjusting for multiple comparisons, there were three statistically significant differences between environmental harms (p<0.05). Environment performed better than carbon footprint (p = 0.001) and water shortages (p = 0.032). Water pollution performed better than carbon footprint (p = 0.036).

Most discouraging health and environmental messages

For health messages, early death was most frequently selected as the most discouraging message (Fig 3, Panel A; p-values presented in S2 Table). Early death was selected significantly more often than all other health messages. For environmental messages, climate change, which leads to extreme weather events, was selected the most frequently, but there were not statistically significant differences between messages in likelihood of being selected as most discouraging (Fig 3, Panel B).

Fig 3.

Fig 3

Message selected as most discouraging by U.S. adults randomized to the health (Panel A, n = 587) or environmental (Panel B, n = 584) group (March 2020). Panel A: Health, Panel B: Environment. *Message topic is presented rather than full message text for brevity. See Table 2 for complete message text. Message topics are arranged from highest to lowest PME score.

Predictors of PME

The results from the OLS regressions on personal characteristics and PME are presented in Table 6. For both health and environmental harms, messages elicited higher PME among younger adults (age 18–25) compared to older adults (age 45 and older). Messages also elicited higher PME among participants who had reduced their red meat consumption over the past year compared to those who had not reduced their consumption (b = 0.57 and 0.68 for health and environment, respectively, p < .001).

Table 6. Results of linear regression of demographic and behavioral characteristics and perceived message effectiveness (PME) of health (n = 587) and environmental messages (n = 587).

Characteristic  Health Messages
(n = 587)
Environmental Messages
(n = 587)
  Coefficient (SE) p-value Coefficient (SE) p-value
Age
    18–25  Referent Referent Referent Referent
    26–34  -0.26 (0.16) 0.10 -0.02 (0.13) 0.87
    35–44  -0.10 (0.16) 0.55 -0.08 (0.14) 0.57
    45–54  -0.32 (0.17) 0.05 -0.35 (0.14) 0.01
    55–64  -0.35 (0.17) 0.04 -0.45 (0.14) 0.001
    65 or older  -0.63 (0.17) 0.00 -0.45 (0.14) 0.001
Gender 
    Male  Referent Referent Referent Referent
    Female  0.22 (0.09) 0.02 0.11 (0.08) 0.16
Race 
    White  Referent Referent Referent
    Black  0.35 (0.16) 0.03 0.01 (0.13) 0.92
    Other race  0.19 (0.16) 0.23 0.17 (0.14) 0.21
Ethnicity 
    Not Hispanic/Latinx  Referent Referent Referent Referent
    Hispanic/Latinx  0.13 (0.15) 0.40 0.37 (0.14) 0.01
Education 
    Less than high school or high school diploma  Referent Referent Referent Referent
    Associate or technical degree  -0.10 (0.12) 0.41 -0.07 (0.12) 0.51
    4-year college degree  0.11 (0.13) 0.36 0.02 (0.10) 0.86
    Master’s degree or graduate degree  -0.02 (0.16) 0.91 0.16 (0.14) 0.26
Annual Household Income 
    $24,999 or below  Referent Referent Referent Referent
    $25,000-$49,999  0.31 (0.02) 0.02 0.20 (0.11) 0.08
    $50,000-$74,999  0.35 (0.14) 0.01 0.16 (0.12) 0.19
    $75,000-$99,999  0.07 (0.17) 0.68 0.20 (0.14) 0.15
    $100,000 or more  0.38 (0.16) 0.02 0.42 (0.13) 0.002
Political Affiliation 
    Liberal  Referent Referent Referent Referent
    Moderate  0.02 (0.11) 0.88 -0.26 (0.10) 0.009
    Conservative  -0.22 (0.12) 0.08 -0.40 (0.12) 0.001
Frequency of Red Meat Consumption in Past 30 Days 
    Less than once per week  Referent Referent Referent Referent
    1 time per week  -0.02 (0.20) 0.92 -0.49 (0.17) 0.004
    2–3 times per week  -0.20 (0.18) 0.26 -0.53 (0.14) 0.00
    4–6 times per week  -0.39 (0.20) 0.05 -0.72 (0.16) 0.00
    Once a day or more  -0.34 (0.22) 0.12 -0.69 (0.18) 0.00
Change in Red Meat Consumption in Past Year 
    Did not reduce  Referent Referent Referent Referent
    Reduced  0.57 (0.10) 0.00 0.68 (0.09) 0.00
Believe that climate change is caused by human activities 
    Strongly or somewhat disagree*  - - Referent Referent
    Neither agree nor disagree  - - 0.30 (0.15) 0.05
    Somewhat agree  - - 0.54 (0.12) 0.00
    Strongly agree  - - 0.73 (0.12) 0.00

*Strongly and somewhat disagree categories were collapsed into one category after initial regression post-hoc tests showed no significant difference in PME between these groups.

†Results from multivariate linear regression with OLS, with separate models for health messages and for environmental messages.

Health messages elicited higher PME scores among women than men (b = 0.22, p<0.05). This relationship was not statistically significant for environmental messages. Environmental messages elicited higher PME scores among Hispanic/Latinx participants compared to Not Hispanic/Latinx participants (p = 0.01); among liberal participants compared to moderate (p = 0.009) and conservative (p = 0.001) participants; and among participants with an annual household income of $100,000 or more compared with participants with an annual household income of $24,999 or below (p = 0.002). Health messages elicited higher PME scores among Black participants compared to white participants (p < .05); and among participants with a higher annual household income compared to $24,999 or below, except for $75,000-$99,999.

Behavioral framing and causal variant tasks

Perceived discouragement ratings for different causal variants ranged from 2.41 to 2.62. There were no differences in perceived discouragement between causal variants after correcting for multiple comparisons. For the behavioral framing experiment, discouragement ratings ranged from 2.32 to 2.59. As with the causal variant experiment, differences were not found to be statistically significant in this exploratory study.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is one of the first studies to develop and compare health and environmental warning messages about red meat. In our sample of US adults, we found that red meat consumers perceived health warning messages to be more effective than environmental warning messages. Moreover, across specific health warning messages, those for colon and rectal cancer and early death were perceived to be most effective. Red meat warnings for both health and the environment elicited higher perceived message effectiveness ratings among younger adults and those reporting that they had reduced their red meat consumption over the past year. The relationship between other personal characteristics and PME varied depending on warning topic (health vs. environmental). PME from health warnings was higher among Black participants compared to white participants. For environmental warnings, PME was higher among Hispanic/Latinx participants, participants with an annual household income of $100,000 or more, and liberal participants.

Warnings about red meat could help inform consumers about the health and environmental impacts of these products [63]. Current awareness of the adverse health impacts likely is low given the lack of public health campaigns focused on red meat’s health harms. Further, red meat is highly marketed. For example, the Beef Industry Council-funded ad campaign that promotes the consumption of red meat (“Beef. It’s what’s for dinner.”), administered by the U.S. Department of Agriculture, is recognized by 88% of American adults [64]. Public confusion over the health effects of red meat has been exacerbated by inconsistent scientific messaging. Most recently, for example, the Annals of Internal Medicine, a journal published by the American College of Physicians, published a review article that concluded that there was insufficient evidence to support that red or processed meat consumption was associated with poor health outcomes [65]. This article received a widespread critique by nutrition scientists due to flaws in study design and interpretation bias [66]. Despite this critique, the article garnered widespread media coverage in outlets such as the New York Times [67]. In the same year, the New York Times also covered the EAT-Lancet report, which includes recommendations to limit red meat intake, in multiple articles [68,69]. The rapid and contradictory nature of reporting around red meat may contribute to consumer confusion.

In this study, health warnings elicited higher ratings of PME than did environmental warnings. The reasons for these differences are unclear. One possible explanation is that environmental warnings elicit stronger feelings of defensiveness among consumers [70] than health warnings. For example, abstinence from meat can be viewed as threatening [71]. If consumers felt more judged by the environmental messages, perhaps this led to lower ratings of PME. Similarly, research on graphic tobacco warnings has found that some messages elicit reactance, or cognitive or emotional resistance to a message, which suppresses warnings’ effects on intentions to quit smoking [72]. More research will be needed to understand the degree to which environmental warning messages elicit reactance and potentially blunt the impact of the warnings.

Another possibility is that baseline awareness about the environmental damage caused by red meat is low, leading to lower PME. According to a recent report by the Yale Program on Climate Change Communication, only 27% of Americans think that beef contributes to global warming “a lot” and 30% think it contributes “a little” or “some” [73]. Further, the same survey found that 91% of consumers cited health as at least moderately important as a reason why they consumed plant-based foods, compared to 64% that cited reducing global warming as at least moderately important [73]. This is consistent with a previous nationally representative survey that found people who are reducing meat consumption more commonly cite health than environmental reasons, and the relationship we observed in this study is consistent with those findings [74]. Warnings on red meat may help increase public awareness and perceptions of risk of these environmental harms. In turn, higher public awareness about the environmental damages caused by red meat may increase the effectiveness of the messages. More research will be needed to understand consumers’ reactions to environmental messages about red meat, particularly as public attitudes and knowledge shift over time.

Additionally, the difference in PME ratings between the health and environmental messages could be due to differences in the types of causal variants used. All of the health warning messages included the phrase “increases your risk of,” while the environmental warnings used “contributes to,” “increases,” and “harms.” On the other hand, the results from our causal variant task suggested that the type of causal language used does not meaningfully influence PME for messages about red meat. We tested three causal variants linking red meat with one health harm, obesity (“Eating red meat is associated with, increases your risk, or increases your chances of obesity”) and found that there were no differences in PME by causal variant. However, we did not test different causal variants for other health harms, nor for any environmental harms, and thus, it would be useful for future research on messages about red meat to explore whether different causal variants are perceived as more or less effective.

Within the messages focused on health harms, there were few differences in PME between different health topics. The exception to this pattern was type 2 diabetes, elicited lower PME ratings than all other health harms tested in this study. These differences may be because other outcomes, such as cancer and mortality, may be perceived as more damaging to health than type 2 diabetes. Furthermore, these differences could be due to the fact that the general public has overall low awareness about the association between red meat intake and type 2 diabetes [63].

Similar to the results for health harms, there were few differences in PME across specific environmental harms, with little variability between the top rated and lowest rated messages. The exception to this pattern is that the environment message elicited higher PME than the carbon footprint, water shortage, and climate change messages. These results are consistent with a recently published study by Wistar et al. which also found that a general environment message was perceived as more effective at discouraging red meat consumption than other environmental topics [75]. In contrast, it was surprising that the carbon footprint message elicited lower perceived message effectiveness as many existing environmental warning labels focus on carbon footprints. Lower PME for the carbon footprint message could reflect that many consumers do not understand the meaning of carbon footprints [76,77]. Our findings that many health and environmental harms elicited similar PME highlights that practitioners and policymakers have a wide variety of options for which harms to include in warnings.

We assessed the concept of PME two different ways in this study. Although PME rating of individual warnings was the primary outcome [35], we also asked participants to select which message of all the messages would most discourage them from consuming red meat because forcing participants to select one message might amplify subtle differences in reactions to each warning. For health messages, PME ratings were highest for messages about colon and rectal cancer, followed by early death, and several types of cancer, which all had very similar PME ratings. The overall pattern of top messages was the same when participants were forced to select the most discouraging message compared to when they rated each message individually, though the differences between messages appeared to be amplified in the former. For example, a substantial margin separated the proportion of participants who selected early death as the most discouraging harm (31% of participants) compared to several types of cancer or colon and rectal cancer (selected by 17% and 13% of participants, respectively), despite these harms performing similarly in PME ratings. These differences could reflect differences in measurement approaches (rating vs. forced choice), or that the 3-item PME scale measures two constructs in addition to discouragement (concern and unpleasantness). Overall, results from both sets of questions suggest that messages focused on cancer and especially early death may be particularly promising for eliciting behavior change.

For the environmental messages, results were largely consistent regardless of whether participants were rating messages on PME or selecting the most discouraging message. Specifically, there was little variability in both PME ratings (range of mean PME scores, 2.20 to 2.32) and in the proportion of participants who selected each message as the most discouraging (range of %s, 6% to 15%). This pattern of results suggests that a variety of environment-related messages would perform similarly.

With regards to demographic and personal predictors of PME, health warning messages elicited a higher PME among Black compared to white participants. It is possible this pattern was seen because racial inequities persist in chronic diseases such as type 2 diabetes and cardiovascular disease, wherein the prevalence is higher among Black as compared to white adults [78]. This higher message effectiveness may also be because at the population level Black individuals are already reducing their meat consumption more than white individuals [79], and so may be more receptive to these types of warning messages. If this pattern of results holds in real-world settings, health warning messages on red meat could serve as a tool for reducing racial inequity in chronic diseases.

Political affiliation and belief in human-caused climate change were the strongest predictors of PME in the environmental message model. As we hypothesized, stronger beliefs in human-caused climate change were associated with higher perceived effectiveness of the environmental messages. This finding is consistent with the findings from “Climate Change and the American Diet” [73]. While these findings indicate that those who do not believe that human-caused climate change may be harder to reach with environmental messaging, only 18% of our sample strongly or somewhat disagreed with the statement that human-caused climate change is occurring. This finding is consistent with other national polls of belief in climate change that show that the majority of US residents believe in human-caused climate change [80].

Environmental messages elicited higher PME from liberals than from moderates and conservatives. This finding is consistent with climate change literature that has examined concern for climate change based on political belief [81,82]. This relationship was not significant in the analysis of health messages. Thus, health messages may be perceived as less politically controversial in the US.

In both our health and environmental analyses, messages were less effective among older adults compared to 18-25-year-olds (45 and older for environment warning messages; 55 and older for health warning messages). Young adults are the fastest growing age group for vegetarians and vegans; 12% of U.S. adults aged 18–29 report eating mostly vegetarian or vegan, compared to 5% of U.S. adults aged 50 and older [79]. Though our survey contained only red meat consumers, younger people may be more willing to reduce their meat consumption, perhaps contributing to their stronger perceptions that warning messages would be effective at reducing red meat consumption.

For both health and environmental messages, participants who had not reduced their red meat consumption over the past year rated messages as less effective than participants who had reduced their red meat consumption. Higher self-reported consumption of red meat was significantly associated with lower PME ratings for environmental messages. Again, this relationship between lower red meat consumption and higher PME for environmental messages was consistent with the recent study published by Wistar et al. [75]. No such relationship between red meat intake and PME emerged for health messages.

All causal variants and behavioral frames performed similarly. These findings suggest messages can take a variety of forms and perform well. These findings are important because subtle differences in warning language could affect warnings’ legal viability in the US; if a variety of warnings are similarly effective, policymakers can pursue those most likely to clear potential legal challenges.

This research was conducted with the goal of informing future policies to inform consumers about the harms of red meat and discourage consumption. Although warning labels on other food products, such as sugary beverages, have been successful in several countries, policies focused on red meat have not been implemented, so legal and political feasibility in the US must be considered. In the US, warning may face First Amendment challenges [83]. Furthermore, the strong political power of the meat industry [83,84], as well as deep-rooted ideologies of carnism and neoliberalism [84], could make implementing meat warnings politically challenging.

Although perceived effectiveness is a marker for messages’ potential to change behavior, more research is needed to assess whether red meat warnings influence red meat purchasing and consumption behaviors. To our knowledge, only one study has investigated the impact of health and environmental warnings on outcomes related to red meat purchases [27]. That study found that participants perceived health warnings and combined environmental and health warnings as more effective than environmental warnings. However, the warnings did not affect participants’ selections in a product choice task. Given that previous research has shown that perceived message effectiveness is predictive of behavioral change [35], it is unclear why even the health warnings did not reduce selection of red meat in that study. One possibility is that the behavioral task may not have been properly powered to detect differences [27], or that the simplistic nature of an online choice experiment does not reflect how warnings would influence purchasing decisions in the real world. Future research would be useful to understand whether such warnings would discourage meat purchases in a more realistic food retail environment.

Strengths of this study include the large sample of US residents and use of a randomized experiment. Limitations includes the use of a convenience sample, though the Cloud Research sample has been shown to more closely reflect the US population on sociodemographic characteristics than other online samples [56]. Furthermore, this was an exploratory study without an a priori power calculation, and it is possible that there were differences in consumer responses to messages that we were not powered to detect. Additional limitations include that this study did not assess behavioral outcomes, such as purchases of red meat, although previous research on communication campaigns has found that PME, our primary outcome, is predictive of behavioral change [85]. Further research will clarify how consumers alter food purchases in response to red meat warnings. Another limitation is that data were collected in late March 2020, while coronavirus was spreading across the US. The spread of coronavirus may have elicited higher PME in health warning messages, particularly early death, given heightened concern about health harms during this time period. Public health agencies widely communicated the elevated risk of severe COVID-19 infection and death for individuals with pre-existing conditions [86]. Many of these pre-existing conditions are associated with diet, such as type 2 diabetes and cardiovascular disease [87]. Additionally, research has found that the COVID-19 pandemic increased mortality salience, leading to increased health behavior intentions [88,89]. Future research should examine how the salience of health and mortality risks caused by the coronavirus pandemic may impact reactions to health warning messages.

Conclusions

Reducing red meat consumption in the US is critical for human and planetary health. This study developed several evidence-based health and environmental warning messages for red meat. Results of this exploratory study suggest that health warning messages are perceived to be more effective than environmental messages. Nonetheless, the inclusion of environmental waring messages is an opportunity to educate consumers about the environmental harms of consuming red meat. Future work will clarify the impact of these messages on meat purchases and consumption.

Supporting information

S1 Appendix. Codebook.

(DOCX)

S1 Table. Adjusted perceived message effectiveness (PME)* and intention to reduce meat consumption in the next seven days* between Health and Environmental Messages.

*Five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 “Not at all” to 5 “A great deal” †Results from linear regression, adjusting for meat consumption as compared to a year ago.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. P-Values from Z-Tests comparing messages selected as most discouraging by U.S. adults randomized to the health (n = 587) or environmental (n = 584) group.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Christina Chauvenet for project management, statistical analysis, and manuscript drafting assistance. The authors thank Hannah Rayala for the design of the warning images. The authors also thank Isabella Higgins and Sarah Frank for feedback on survey design and methodology.

Data Availability

All data files are available from the Harvard Dataverse database (https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/KE1KLH).

Funding Statement

This work was funded through a grant from the Wellcome Trust, grant ID #216042/Z/19/Z. Grant #K01HL147713 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute of the NIH supported MGH's time on the paper. We are grateful to the Carolina Population Center and its NIH Center grant (P2C HD050924) for general support. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Zeng L, Ruan M, Liu J, Wilde P, Naumova EN, Mozaffarian D, et al. Trends in Processed Meat, Unprocessed Red Meat, Poultry, and Fish Consumption in the United States, 1999–2016. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2019;119(7):1085–98.e12. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2019.04.004 PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6689198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Willett W, Rockström J, Loken B, Springmann M, Lang T, Vermeulen S, et al. Food in the Anthropocene: the EAT-Lancet Commission on healthy diets from sustainable food systems. Lancet. 2019;393(10170):447–92. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)31788-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Williams P. Nutritional composition of red meat. Nutrition & Dietetics. 2007;64(s4):S113–S9. doi: 10.1111/j.1747-0080.2007.00197.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Berryman CE, Lieberman HR, Fulgoni VL 3rd, Pasiakos SM. Protein intake trends and conformity with the Dietary Reference Intakes in the United States: analysis of the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 2001–2014. Am J Clin Nutr. 2018;108(2):405–13. Epub 2018/06/23. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqy088 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Mineva E, Sternberg M, Pfeiffer CM. Prevalence of Vitamin B-12 Deficiency in US Adults Using the Combined Indicator of Vitamin B-12 status, cB12. Current Developments in Nutrition. 2020;4(Supplement_2):1824–. doi: 10.1093/cdn/nzaa067_051 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Sun H, Weaver CM. Decreased Iron Intake Parallels Rising Iron Deficiency Anemia and Related Mortality Rates in the US Population. J Nutr. 2021;151(7):1947–55. Epub 2021/04/10. doi: 10.1093/jn/nxab064 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Pan A, Sun Q, Bernstein AM, Schulze MB, Manson JE, Stampfer MJ, et al. Red meat consumption and mortality: results from 2 prospective cohort studies. Arch Intern Med. 2012;172(7):555–63. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2011.2287 PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3712342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Wang X, Lin X, Ouyang YY, Liu J, Zhao G, Pan A, et al. Red and processed meat consumption and mortality: dose-response meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Public Health Nutr. 2016;19(5):893–905. doi: 10.1017/S1368980015002062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bouvard V, Loomis D, Guyton KZ, Grosse Y, Ghissassi FE, Benbrahim-Tallaa L, et al. Carcinogenicity of consumption of red and processed meat. Lancet Oncol. 2015;16(16):1599–600. doi: 10.1016/S1470-2045(15)00444-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Rock CL, Thomson C, Gansler T, Gapstur SM, McCullough ML, Patel AV, et al. American Cancer Society guideline for diet and physical activity for cancer prevention. CA Cancer J Clin. 2020;70(4):245–71. doi: 10.3322/caac.21591 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Protein: American Diabetes Association; [cited 2020]. Available from: https://www.diabetes.org/healthy-living/recipes-nutrition/eating-well/protein.
  • 12.Gerber PJ, Steinfeld H, Henderson B, Mottet A, Opio C, Dijkman J, et al. Tackling climate change through livestock--A global assessment of emissions and mitigation opportunities. Rome: Food and Agricultural Organization of the United Nations (FAO), 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Eshel G, Shepon A, Makov T, Milo R. Land, irrigation water, greenhouse gas, and reactive nitrogen burdens of meat, eggs, and dairy production in the United States. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111(33):11996–2001. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1402183111 PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4143028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Rotz CA, Asem-Hiablie S, Place S, Thoma G. Environmental footprints of beef cattle production in the United States. Agric Syst. 2019;169:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.agsy.2018.11.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Climate Change and Land. Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change and Land, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Boehm R, Wilde PE, Ver Ploeg M, Costello C, Cash SB. A Comprehensive Life Cycle Assessment of Greenhouse Gas Emissions from U.S. Household Food Choices. Food Policy. 2018;79:67–76. doi: 10.1016/j.foodpol.2018.05.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Liu T, Wang Q, Su B. A review of carbon labeling: Standards, implementation, and impact. Renewable and Sustainable Energy Reviews. 2016;53:68–79. doi: 10.1016/j.rser.2015.08.050 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Camilleri AR, Larrick RP, Hossain S, Patino-Echeverri D. Consumers underestimate the emissions associated with food but are aided by labels. Nat Clim Chang. 2019;9(1):53–8. doi: 10.1038/s41558-018-0354-z [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Brunner F, Kurz V, Bryngelsson D, Hedenus F. Carbon label at a university restaurant–label implementation and evaluation. Ecological Economics. 2018;146:658–67. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2017.12.012 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Elofsson K, Bengtsson N, Matsdotter E, Arntyr J. The impact of climate information on milk demand: Evidence from a field experiment. Food policy. 2016;58:14–23. doi: 10.1016/j.foodpol.2015.11.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Garnett EE, Marteau TM, Sandbrook C, Pilling MA, Balmford A. Order of meals at the counter and distance between options affect student cafeteria vegetarian sales. Nat Food. 2020;1(8):485–8. doi: 10.1038/s43016-020-0132-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Noar SM, Francis DB, Bridges C, Sontag JM, Ribisl KM, Brewer NT. The impact of strengthening cigarette pack warnings: Systematic review of longitudinal observational studies. Soc Sci Med. 2016;164:118–29. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.06.011 PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5026824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Noar SM, Hall MG, Francis DB, Ribisl KM, Pepper JK, Brewer NT. Pictorial cigarette pack warnings: a meta-analysis of experimental studies. Tob Control. 2016;25(3):341–54. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2014-051978 PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4636492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Grummon AH, Hall MG. Sugary drink warnings: A meta-analysis of experimental studies. PLoS Med. 2020;17(5):e1003120. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1003120 PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7239392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Taillie LS, Reyes M, Colchero MA, Popkin B, Corvalan C. An evaluation of Chile’s Law of Food Labeling and Advertising on sugar-sweetened beverage purchases from 2015 to 2017: A before-and-after study. PLoS Med. 2020;17(2):e1003015. Epub 2020/02/12. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1003015 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7012389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Clarke N, Pechey E, Kosite D, Konig LM, Mantzari E, Blackwell AKM, et al. Impact of health warning labels on selection and consumption of food and alcohol products: systematic review with meta-analysis. Health Psychol Rev. 2020:1–24. Epub 2020/06/10. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2020.1780147 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Taillie LS, Chauvenet C, Grummon AH, Hall MG, Waterlander W, Prestemon CE, et al. Testing front-of-package warnings to discourage red meat consumption: a randomized experiment with US meat consumers. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2021;18(1):114. Epub 2021/09/09. doi: 10.1186/s12966-021-01178-9 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC8423585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Sanchez-Sabate R, Sabaté J. Consumer attitudes towards environmental concerns of meat consumption: A systematic review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(7). doi: 10.3390/ijerph16071220 PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6479556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Grunert KG, Hieke S, Wills J. Sustainability labels on food products: Consumer motivation, understanding and use. Food policy. 2014;44:177–89. doi: 10.1016/j.foodpol.2013.12.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Hall MG, Lazard AJ, Grummon AH, Higgins ICA, Bercholz M, Richter APC, et al. Designing warnings for sugary drinks: A randomized experiment with Latino parents and non-Latino parents. Prev Med. 2021;148:106562. Epub 2021/04/21. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2021.106562 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Grummon AH, Hall MG, Taillie LS, Brewer NT. How should sugar-sweetened beverage health warnings be designed? A randomized experiment. Prev Med. 2019;121:158–66. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2019.02.010 PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6520104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Grummon AH, Reimold AE, Hall MG. Influence of the San Francisco, CA, Sugar-Sweetened Beverage Health Warning on Consumer Reactions: Implications for Equity from a Randomized Experiment. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2021. Epub 2021/09/02. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2021.07.008 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Taillie LS, Hall MG, Gomez LF, Higgins I, Bercholz M, Murukutla N, et al. Designing an Effective Front-of-Package Warning Label for Food and Drinks High in Added Sugar, Sodium, or Saturated Fat in Colombia: An Online Experiment. Nutrients. 2020;12(10). Epub 2020/10/18. doi: 10.3390/nu12103124 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7601991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Grummon AH, Hall MG, Mitchell CG, Pulido M, Mendel Sheldon J, Noar SM, et al. Reactions to messages about smoking, vaping and COVID-19: two national experiments. Tob Control. 2020. Epub 2020/11/15. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2020-055956 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7669534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Noar SM, Barker J, Bell T, Yzer M. Does Perceived Message Effectiveness Predict the Actual Effectiveness of Tobacco Education Messages? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Health Commun. 2020;35(2):148–57. Epub 2018/11/30. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2018.1547675 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6538475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Brewer NT, Jeong M, Hall MG, Baig SA, Mendel JR, Lazard AJ, et al. Impact of e-cigarette health warnings on motivation to vape and smoke. Tob Control. 2019. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2018-054878 PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6824616. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Pan A, Sun Q, Bernstein AM, Schulze MB, Manson JE, Willett WC, et al. Red meat consumption and risk of type 2 diabetes: 3 cohorts of US adults and an updated meta-analysis. Am J Clin Nutr. 2011;94(4):1088–96. Epub 2011/08/13. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.111.018978 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3173026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Schwingshackl L, Hoffmann G, Lampousi AM, Knuppel S, Iqbal K, Schwedhelm C, et al. Food groups and risk of type 2 diabetes mellitus: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Eur J Epidemiol. 2017;32(5):363–75. Epub 2017/04/12. doi: 10.1007/s10654-017-0246-y ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5506108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Healthy Food Choices Made Easy: American Diabetes Association; [August 11, 2020]. Available from: https://www.diabetes.org/nutrition/healthy-food-choices-made-easy/protein.
  • 40.Vieira AR, Abar L, Chan DSM, Vingeliene S, Polemiti E, Stevens C, et al. Foods and beverages and colorectal cancer risk: a systematic review and meta-analysis of cohort studies, an update of the evidence of the WCRF-AICR Continuous Update Project. Ann Oncol. 2017;28(8):1788–802. Epub 2017/04/14. doi: 10.1093/annonc/mdx171 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Zhao Z, Feng Q, Yin Z, Shuang J, Bai B, Yu P, et al. Red and processed meat consumption and colorectal cancer risk: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Oncotarget. 2017;8(47):83306–14. Epub 2017/11/16. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.20667 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5669970. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Kim SR, Kim K, Lee SA, Kwon SO, Lee JK, Keum N, et al. Effect of Red, Processed, and White Meat Consumption on the Risk of Gastric Cancer: An Overall and Dose(-)Response Meta-Analysis. Nutrients. 2019;11(4). Epub 2019/04/14. doi: 10.3390/nu11040826 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6520977. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.American Cancer Society Guideline for Diet and Physical Activity: American Cancer Society; [August 29, 2020]. Available from: https://www.cancer.org/healthy/eat-healthy-get-active/acs-guidelines-nutrition-physical-activity-cancer-prevention/guidelines.html. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 44.Bechthold A, Boeing H, Schwedhelm C, Hoffmann G, Knuppel S, Iqbal K, et al. Food groups and risk of coronary heart disease, stroke and heart failure: A systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis of prospective studies. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr. 2019;59(7):1071–90. Epub 2017/10/19. doi: 10.1080/10408398.2017.1392288 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Picking Healthy Proteins: American Heart Association; [August 31, 2020]. Available from: https://www.heart.org/en/healthy-living/healthy-eating/eat-smart/nutrition-basics/meat-poultry-and-fish-picking-healthy-proteins.
  • 46.Chen GC, Lv DB, Pang Z, Liu QF. Red and processed meat consumption and risk of stroke: a meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2013;67(1):91–5. Epub 2012/11/22. doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2012.180 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Kim K, Hyeon J, Lee SA, Kwon SO, Lee H, Keum N, et al. Role of Total, Red, Processed, and White Meat Consumption in Stroke Incidence and Mortality: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Prospective Cohort Studies. J Am Heart Assoc. 2017;6(9). Epub 2017/09/01. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.117.005983 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5634267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Zheng Y, Li Y, Satija A, Pan A, Sotos-Prieto M, Rimm E, et al. Association of changes in red meat consumption with total and cause specific mortality among US women and men: two prospective cohort studies. BMJ. 2019;365:l2110. doi: 10.1136/bmj.l2110 PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6559336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Schwingshackl L, Schwedhelm C, Hoffmann G, Lampousi AM, Knuppel S, Iqbal K, et al. Food groups and risk of all-cause mortality: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Am J Clin Nutr. 2017;105(6):1462–73. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.117.153148 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Poore J, Nemecek T. Reducing food’s environmental impacts through producers and consumers. Science. 2018;360(6392):987–92. doi: 10.1126/science.aaq0216 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Attribution of Extreme Weather Events in the Context of Climate Change. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Gerbens-Leenes PW, Mekonnen MM, Hoekstra AY. The water footprint of poultry, pork and beef: A comparative study in different countries and production systems. Water Resources and Industry. 2013;1–2:25–36. doi: 10.1016/j.wri.2013.03.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Vanham D, Comero S, Gawlik BM, Bidoglio G. The water footprint of different diets within European sub-national geographical entities. Nature Sustainability. 2018;1(9):518–25. doi: 10.1038/s41893-018-0133-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Bruine de Bruin W, Rabinovich L, Weber K, Babboni M, Dean M, Ignon L. Public understanding of climate change terminology. Climatic Change. 2021;167(3):37. doi: 10.1007/s10584-021-03183-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Reyes M, Garmendia ML, Olivares S, Aqueveque C, Zacarías I, Corvalán C. Development of the Chilean front-of-package food warning label. BMC Public Health. 2019;19(1):906. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-7118-1 PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6615240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Chandler J, Rosenzweig C, Moss AJ, Robinson J, Litman L. Online panels in social science research: Expanding sampling methods beyond Mechanical Turk. Behav Res Methods. 2019;51(5):2022–38. doi: 10.3758/s13428-019-01273-7 PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6797699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey Questionnaire: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2017–2018. Available from: https://wwwn.cdc.gov/nchs/nhanes/continuousnhanes/default.aspx?BeginYear=2017.
  • 58.Hall MG, Grummon AH, Maynard OM, Kameny MR, Jenson D, Popkin BM. Causal language in health warning labels and US adults’ perception: A randomized experiment. Am J Public Health. 2019;109(10):1429–33. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2019.305222 PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6727278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Baig SA, Noar SM, Gottfredson NC, Boynton MH, Ribisl KM, Brewer NT. UNC perceived message effectiveness: validation of a brief scale. Ann Behav Med. 2019;53(8):732–42. doi: 10.1093/abm/kay080 PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6636889. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Malek L, Umberger WJ, Goddard E. Committed vs. uncommitted meat eaters: Understanding willingness to change protein consumption. Appetite. 2019;138:115–26. Epub 2019/03/29. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.03.024 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Frank SM, Jaacks LM, Batis C, Vanderlee L, Taillie LS. Patterns of Red and Processed Meat Consumption across North America: A Nationally Representative Cross-Sectional Comparison of Dietary Recalls from Canada, Mexico, and the United States. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(1). Epub 2021/01/21. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18010357 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7796493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Lacko AM, Maselko J, Popkin B, Ng SW. Socio-economic and racial/ethnic disparities in the nutritional quality of packaged food purchases in the USA, 2008–2018. Public Health Nutr. 2021;24(17):5730–42. Epub 2021/01/28. doi: 10.1017/S1368980021000367 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC8313635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Grummon AH, Goodman D, Jaacks LM, Taillie LS, Chauvenet CA, Salvia MG, et al. Awareness of and reactions to health and environmental harms of red meat among parents in the United States. Public Health Nutr. 2021:1–11. Epub 2021/07/30. doi: 10.1017/S1368980021003098 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Nestle M. Food politics: how the food industry influences nutrition and health: Berkeley: University of California Press; 2002 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Johnston BC, Zeraatkar D, Han MA, Vernooij RWM, Valli C, El Dib R, et al. Unprocessed red meat and processed meat consumption: dietary guideline recommendations from the nutritional recommendations (nutrirecs) consortium. Ann Intern Med. 2019;171(10):756–64. doi: 10.7326/M19-1621 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Qian F, Riddle MC, Wylie-Rosett J, Hu FB. Red and processed meats and health risks: how strong is the evidence? Diabetes Care. 2020;43(2):265–71. doi: 10.2337/dci19-0063 PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6971786. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Kolata G. Eat Less Red Meat, Scientists Said. Now Some Believe That Was Bad Advice. The New York Times. 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Brody J. A Guide to Sustainable Eating. The New York Times. 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Sengupta S. New Diet Guidelines to Benefit People and the Plant: More Greens for All, Less Meat for Some. The New York Times. 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Koch JA, Bolderdijk JW, van Ittersum K. Can graphic warning labels reduce the consumption of meat? Appetite. 2022;168:105690. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105690 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Rothgerber H. Efforts to overcome vegetarian-induced dissonance among meat eaters. Appetite. 2014;79:32–41. Epub 2014/04/15. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.04.003 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Hall MG, Sheeran P, Noar SM, Boynton MH, Ribisl KM, Parada H Jr., et al. Negative affect, message reactance and perceived risk: how do pictorial cigarette pack warnings change quit intentions? Tob Control. 2018;27(e2):e136–e42. Epub 2017/12/19. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2017-053972 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6004228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Leiserowitz A, Ballew M, Rosenthal S, Semaan J. Climate change and the American diet. New Haven, CT: Yale Program on Climate Change Communication, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Neff RA, Edwards D, Palmer A, Ramsing R, Righter A, Wolfson J. Reducing meat consumption in the USA: a nationally representative survey of attitudes and behaviours. Public Health Nutr. 2018;21(10):1835–44. Epub 2018/03/27. doi: 10.1017/S1368980017004190 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6088533. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Wistar A, Hall MG, Bercholz M, Taillie LS. Designing Environmental Messages to Discourage Red Meat Consumption: An Online Experiment. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(5). doi: 10.3390/ijerph19052919 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Hartikainen H, Roininen T, Katajajuuri J-M, Pulkkinen H. Finnish consumer perceptions of carbon footprints and carbon labelling of food products. Journal of Cleaner Production. 2014;73:285–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jclepro.2013.09.018 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Hammond G. Time to give due weight to the ’carbon footprint’ issue. Nature. 2007;445(7125):256–. doi: 10.1038/445256b [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Cunningham T, Croft J, Liu Y, Lu H, Eke P, Giles W. Vital Signs: Racial Disparities in Age-Specific Mortality Among Blacks or African Americans--United States, 1999–2015. MMWR Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 2017;66:444–56. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm6617e1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Public views about Americans’ eating habits. Pew Research Center, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 80.U.S. Public Views on Climate and Energy Pew Research Center, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.McCright AM, Dunlap RE. The politicization of climate change and polarization in the american public’s views of global warming, 2001–2010. Sociol Q. 2011;52(2):155–94. doi: 10.1111/j.1533-8525.2011.01198.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Sintov ND, Abou-Ghalioum V, White LV. The partisan politics of low-carbon transport: Why democrats are more likely to adopt electric vehicles than Republicans in the United States. Energy Research & Social Science. 2020;68:101576. doi: 10.1016/j.erss.2020.101576 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Wilde P, Pomeranz JL, Lizewski LJ, Ruan M, Mozaffarian D, Zhang FF. Legal Feasibility of US Government Policies to Reduce Cancer Risk by Reducing Intake of Processed Meat. Milbank Q. 2019;97(2):420–48. Epub 2019/04/25. doi: 10.1111/1468-0009.12385 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6554508. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Sievert K, Lawrence M, Parker C, Baker P. Understanding the Political Challenge of Red and Processed Meat Reduction for Healthy and Sustainable Food Systems: A Narrative Review of the Literature. Int J Health Policy Manag. 2020. Epub 2020/12/11. doi: 10.34172/ijhpm.2020.238 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Noar SM, Bell T, Kelley D, Barker J, Yzer M. Perceived message effectiveness measures in tobacco education campaigns: A systematic review. Commun Methods Meas. 2018;12(4):295–313. doi: 10.1080/19312458.2018.1483017 PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6699787. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.People with Certain Medical Conditions: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2021. [December 10, 2021]. Available from: https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/need-extra-precautions/people-with-medical-conditions.html. [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Collaborators GBDD. Health effects of dietary risks in 195 countries, 1990–2017: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2017. Lancet. 2019;393(10184):1958–72. Epub 2019/04/08. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(19)30041-8 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6899507. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Liu Y, Lv X, Tang Z. The impact of mortality salience on quantified self behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic. Personality and Individual Differences. 2021;180:110972. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.110972 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Evers NFG, Greenfield PM, Evers GW. COVID-19 shifts mortality salience, activities, and values in the United States: Big data analysis of online adaptation. Hum Behav Emerg Technol. 2021;3(1):107–26. Epub 2021/04/07. doi: 10.1002/hbe2.251 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC8013295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

John W Apolzan

3 Jun 2021

PONE-D-21-09001

Developing health and environmental warning messages about red meat: An online experiment

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Taillie,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jul 18 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

John W. Apolzan, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments:

This is more of a reject with opportunity to resubmit than a major revision. Revision doesn't guarantee acceptance and both reviewers had concerns. Please thoroughly respond to all reviewer comments. Please pay close attention to R1's statistical analyses concerns. Please also provide power calculations for the trial and if primary are powered please further clarify for secondary within entire manuscript (i.e. exploratory). If not prospectively powered, clarify in abstract and throughout manuscript pilot study or exploratory. With R2, please perform a sensitivity analyses pre vs. COVID-19 to see if it affects results.

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please provide additional details regarding participant consent. In the ethics statement in the Methods and online submission information, please ensure that you have specified what type you obtained (for instance, written or verbal, and if verbal, how it was documented and witnessed). If your study included minors, state whether you obtained consent from parents or guardians. If the need for consent was waived by the ethics committee, please include this information.

3. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Comments to authors

This study investigates the effectiveness of health and environmental warning messages on consumers’ intention to reduce red meat consumption via an online experiment. The authors claim that health warning is more effective than environmental messages. The topic is timely and relevant. However, the measurement of the effectiveness is poorly designed. Based on the results resented by far, I am far from convinced that the health warning messages are more effective.

The most important concern that I have for this study is the choice of the effectiveness measurement. In the current version, the authors use perceived message effectiveness (PME) as the key measurement. PME is a self-report Likert scale about the message, e.g., I think this message discourage me from wanting to eat red meat a great deal or not at all.

If we assume that the message works via the following mechanism: (a)messages --> (b) change thoughts --> (c) change behavior (e.g., consumption frequency). What the authors measured is only the first step (from a to b) - the impacts of message on thoughts not behavior. Given that there are field studies already successfully measured the impacts of nutrition education on direct consumption outcomes, I find that a study only investigating the partial impacts of the message on people mind could provide limited solid and novel insights to the existing literature.

Further, a large amount literature has demonstrated that the hypothetical bias associated with self-report behavior intention, and such bias can be particularly large when social desirability bias exists. I believe both health and environmental message could induce social desirability bias which could exacerbate hypothetical bias.

In term of statistical analysis, I strongly disagree that authors directly calculated means of Liker scale, e.g., 5= a great deal and 1= not at all, and the average is 2.6. By taking average of those scales, the authors are assuming the distance between 1=not at all and 2 = somewhat not important are equal to the distance between 4=somewhat important and 5=a great deal, which is a very poor assumption and is also not the common practice. Corresponding to that, instead of using linear regression model (table 5), the author should use ordered logit model.

Also, I strongly disagree with the author that, because messages are randomly assigned, the balance tests are therefore not required. Balance tests are essential to demonstrate that your randomization was successful. Unsuccessful randomization is fairly common even when researchers thought they randomly assigned subjects to treatment groups.

For the experiment design, shouldn’t there be a baseline group where they received a placebo information so that their attitude can be used as the gold standard benchmark?

Minor comments

The presentation of the table requires a lot of work. The presentation of Table 1 is horrible. This single table takes 8 pages with a lot of blank areas on many pages. For table 3, the author can easily add one more column to compare the differences between Health Warning and Environmental Warnings groups as the balance test. As for table 5, there are more professional presentation to report regression results.

Reviewer #2: This study explored participants’ reactions to health and environmental warnings messages about red meat. The manuscript is well written and easy to follow. I have included general and specific notes below on areas of the paper that I believe could be strengthened:

GENERAL

You mention at the very end of your limitations section that COVID may have elicited higher PME in health warning messages, especially for early death, which seems likely given its salience during the study period. This seems to be a very large limitation of this work, and should be discussed in more detail. Did you collect any data before the nationwide quarantine (i.e., early March)? Is there any way you can do a sensitivity analysis looking at responses from earlier in the pandemic (March) compared to later (April) to see if the effectiveness of the warnings changed over time? In the discussion, I think it would be valid to spend more time reflecting on COVID’s influence on this study. Do you think health warnings will continue to be salient, given the ongoing pandemic? Is there any scientific evidence that you can cite that supports or negates the idea that the salience of health risks created by COVID may have influenced your findings?

I think this manuscript could benefit from a more in-depth discussion of the application of red meat warnings. How policy-relevant is this work? Is there any way such warnings could be legally required, either in the US or internationally? Do you foresee these warnings being used in educational materials or PSAs?

INTRODUCTION

p. 3, line 51: Can you give the reader context for what 284 grams of red meat looks like in a person’s diet, perhaps in servings of steak or burgers?

p. 3, line 60: typo: “knowledge of a safe consumption levels”

METHODS

General: Could you standardize your language throughout the methods section to have consistent terminology for your main experiment, the behavioral framing task, and the causal variant task? I found it a little confusing when you referred to your main experiment as the “between subjects” experiment, because it appears that all three of the tasks in your study were between subjects (in the second and third tasks, participants were randomized to see only one message, correct?).

p. 14, line 122: Can you explain why you tested 8 health messages and 10 environmental messages, instead of 8 and 8? Even just a few additional words at the end of the sentence in lines 122-123 would be helpful. Something like, “we created a final list of eight health messages…to be used in this experiment CHOSEN BASED ON X CRITERIA”

p. 14, lines 123-124: You list 9 health harms here (not 8), and mention colon and rectal cancer twice—could you clarify?

p. 14, line 126: Can you separate your list with semicolons to make it clear that you didn’t accidentally list climate change twice? I had to reread that sentence to understand what you were trying to say.

p. 17, line 188: Can you be more specific about your primary outcome, given that you’re assessing PME in three separate tasks? ie, add a note that it’s PME for the health/environment warning messages in the main experiment

p. 19, lines 218-222: While balance tests may be unnecessary for randomized experiments, I still think it’s useful to check for differences in demographics and if one is found, conduct a sensitivity analysis on your primary outcome to ensure it isn’t affecting your results.

p. 19, line 223: “between-subjects’ arms”: see general comment under methods section heading. Could you refer to this as your main experiment (or something like that)?

p. 19, line 228: “most discouraging message”: You had previously mentioned asking participants which message was most discouraging under Procedures lines 162-163, but I didn’t see that as an outcome of interest in the measures section.

p. 19, lines 230-236: How did you determine which predictors to include in your models?

RESULTS

Table 3: When you refer to “meat” in this table, is that red meat? If so, can you modify to “frequency of red meat consumption” and “red meat consumption as compared to a year ago”?

pp. 24-25, lines 261-269: Can you list the p-values for the significant findings you’re reporting, beyond just saying they’re <0.05? It’s hard to understand what a significant difference of 0.1 on the PME scale means without added context.

Table 4: Why was the SD for global warming so small (0.05) compared to the others (~1.2-1.3)?

Figure 3: How was the order of messages on the X axis determined? Could you order them from highest to lowest PME score (and add a note at the bottom of the figure that they’re arranged that way) so that readers can see the relationship between the discouragement outcome vs. PME outcome, given that they’re both trying to predict behavior?

p. 29, lines 290-293: There appear to be other significant differences in predictors for PME for the health and environmental message models individually that weren’t commented on in the text (e.g., income, political affiliation, ethnicity)—could you either add those results to the text or provide justification for excluding them?

DISCUSSION

General: Your main takeaway for differences between warning messages was that colon and rectal cancer and early death were most effective (based on PME), but based on your discouragement outcome, only early death was “effective”. I’m curious as to why you chose to highlight both outcomes (PME and discouragement) in the results—which do you think is most predictive of actual behavior change? What do we gain by using the other outcome?

General: I think it’s worth commenting on the varied phrasing of the messages you tested, given that your health warnings all used “increases your risk of” but the environmental warnings never used the word “risk” and instead used words like “contributes to”, “increases”, and “harms.” Is there reason to believe that the word “risk” contributed to the health warning messages’ success? Could you craft an environmental message that frames the warning as health risks to the environment? This seems worthy of mention and future research, given your findings.

p. 30, lines 307-308: I think it’s worth spending a little more time talking about significant predictors of PME for health and environment warnings, especially given ethnic and income differences that could be of use for future messaging research.

p. 30, lines 309-310: Does your study provide evidence that warnings about red meat could help inform consumers about the health and environmental impacts of products? If so, can you explicitly say that, and if not, can you temper this statement?

p. 30, lines 349-355: Couldn’t it also be possible that type 2 diabetes had the lowest PME because most people associate it with sugar and carbs, not red meat? You also found differences in the environment messages that seem worth commenting on—why do you think the environment message performed so much better than carbon footprint? This seems useful to mention given that many labels focus on carbon footprints.

p. 32, lines 356-374: Can you also speak to the other significant differences in predictors that you observed in Table 3? You didn’t mention some of these results until now, so I recommend adding them to the results section before they’re mentioned in the discussion.

p. 34, lines 394-398: You didn’t see statistically significant differences in the causal language or eating behavior language experiments, but could that be because you weren’t powered to see significant effects, given that your sample sizes were much smaller per group for those analyses? Can you provide an explanation of what your study was powered to find? If you were underpowered to detect differences in effects for these two experiments, I would recommend tempering your language here and including this as a limitation.

CONCLUSIONS

I don’t think your study found that health and environmental harms could be used in warning messages about red meat—your study created warning messages about red meat that included health and environmental harms, and you found that health warnings appeared to be more effective.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2022 Jun 24;17(6):e0268121. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0268121.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


13 Jan 2022

Dr. Emily Chenette

Editor-in-Chief

PLOS One

Dear Editor,

We thank you and the reviewers for your comments on our recently submitted manuscript titled “Developing health and environmental warning messages about red meat: An online experiment” (ID PONE-D-21-09001). We appreciate the thoughtful comments from the reviewers and believe the revised manuscript is significantly stronger for having incorporated their suggestions.

We are attaching a revised version of the manuscript with track changes. We are also attaching a table to this submission that contains point-by-point responses to the reviewers’ comments.

Another change is that co-author Christina Chauvenet has requested to be moved from the author list to the acknowledgements section as due to a job change, she can no longer to contribute to reviewing or contributing to the manuscript. We have also added Carmen Prestemon as a co-author. Carmen is a research assistant who contributed to the project during the initial phases and also contributed writing, editing, and analysis during the revision; we believe that her contributions warrant authorship.

We look forward to hearing from you regarding our submission. We would be happy to respond to any further questions and comments that you might have.

Kind regards,

Lindsey Smith Taillie, PhD

Assistant Professor, Department of Nutrition

University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

Attachment

Submitted filename: PLOS One Revision Response Letter_FINAL.docx

Decision Letter 1

John W Apolzan

16 Feb 2022

PONE-D-21-09001R1Developing health and environmental warning messages about red meat: An online experimentPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Taillie,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Apr 02 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

John W. Apolzan, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #2: Thank you for your thoughtful edits and responses. A few more thoughts:

Lines 82-83, 96-97: You say that studies have not yet examined health warnings on red meat or developed warning messages about the environmental impact of red meat, but didn’t the study below do that? I understand that this study under review informed the behavioral study cited below and that study was conducted after this one, but it still was published first so I think you need to acknowledge it. You could change this language to something like, “before conducting this study, no research had examined health warnings…” etc.

Taillie, L.S., Chauvenet, C., Grummon, A.H. et al. Testing front-of-package warnings to discourage red meat consumption: a randomized experiment with US meat consumers. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 18, 114 (2021).

Lines 197, 405: you used “casual” instead of “causal”, just double check if that happened anywhere else in the paper.

Lines 255-256: You say that you corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni’s method—can you specify whether the p-values presented in the paper are corrected, and if so, which ones?

Figure 3: I previously commented about changing this figure and although you said that you made the change, it does not appear to have changed in the revision you submitted. My original comment stands: I think it would be more useful to reorder the x axes in Panels A and B so that messages are ordered from highest to lowest PME score from left to right. You added a footnote saying that message topics are arranged from highest to lowest PME score, but they don’t appear to be.

Discussion: I know your discussion is already quite long, but I think the Taillie et al. 2021 paper cited above deserves some acknowledgement in the discussion, given that you are calling for that work to be done at the end of your paper but it already has been done and published. In that published paper you did not comment on how your findings compared to this paper (because it wasn’t published yet), so it seems important to note in this paper that although you found significant differences in this paper, you did not see those differences play out in an actual behavioral task. I know it was one single task and may not have been properly powered, but I think it is still worth discussing.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2022 Jun 24;17(6):e0268121. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0268121.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


25 Mar 2022

Thank you to the editor and reviewers for the opportunity. For specific responses to each comment, please see the response letter attached to this submission.

Attachment

Submitted filename: PLOS One Revision 2 Response Letter_FINAL.docx

Decision Letter 2

John W Apolzan

25 Apr 2022

Developing health and environmental warning messages about red meat: An online experiment

PONE-D-21-09001R2

Dear Dr. Taillie,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

John W. Apolzan, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

John W Apolzan

15 Jun 2022

PONE-D-21-09001R2

Developing health and environmental warning messages about red meat: An online experiment

Dear Dr. Taillie:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. John W. Apolzan

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Appendix. Codebook.

    (DOCX)

    S1 Table. Adjusted perceived message effectiveness (PME)* and intention to reduce meat consumption in the next seven days* between Health and Environmental Messages.

    *Five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 “Not at all” to 5 “A great deal” †Results from linear regression, adjusting for meat consumption as compared to a year ago.

    (DOCX)

    S2 Table. P-Values from Z-Tests comparing messages selected as most discouraging by U.S. adults randomized to the health (n = 587) or environmental (n = 584) group.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: PLOS One Revision Response Letter_FINAL.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: PLOS One Revision 2 Response Letter_FINAL.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All data files are available from the Harvard Dataverse database (https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/KE1KLH).


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES