Abstract
The purpose of this paper is to highlight the value for speech-language pathologists of considering the overall stuttering condition—including speakers’ experiences during and around moments of stuttering—in treatment with individuals who stutter. We first highlight a framework for conceptualizing the entirety of the stuttering condition. We then present recent research and clinical perspectives about stuttering to support the claim that speech-language pathologists who account for individual differences in how their clients experience stuttering are better positioned to treat stuttering more effectively. Ultimately, this will yield better treatment outcomes and help clinicians achieve greater gains in quality of life for their clients who stutter.
For decades, researchers and clinicians have highlighted the importance of describing and conceptualizing stuttering in its many facets. Much of this work has focused on disruptions or “disfluencies” that listeners may observe in the speech of people who stutter; these disfluencies are typically referred to as stuttering or stuttering behavior (Conture, 1990; E. B. Cooper, 1968; Gregory et al., 2003; Wingate, 1964, 2001; Yairi, 2001; Yairi & Ambrose, 2005). From the perspective of a person who stutters, however, the experience of stuttering involves far more than the production of stuttered speech (E. B. Cooper, 1968, 1977; Johnson, 1961b; Sheehan, 1970; Sheehan & Sheehan, 1984; Tichenor & Yaruss, 2018, 2019b; Van Riper, 1982; Yaruss, 1998; Yaruss & Quesal, 2004). The focus of this paper is to highlight how understanding these broader aspects of living with stuttering can help speech-language pathologists improve their therapy and interactions with people who stutter. (Note that in this paper we are referring to “Childhood Onset Fluency Disorder,” sometimes called “developmental” stuttering, to differentiate it from other types of stuttering with neurogenic or psychogenic origin.)
Understanding the Stuttering Condition
The term adverse impact is often used to describe the broad range of negative sequelae that may be related to a living life as a person who stutters (Tichenor & Yaruss, 2019b; Yaruss & Quesal, 2004). One useful framework for understanding adverse impact is the International Classification of Functioning, Disability, and Health (ICF), developed by the World Health Organization (WHO, 2001). This framework has been adopted by the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA) as reflecting the scope of practice of speech-language pathologists (ASHA, 2016). The ICF as applied to stuttering (Tichenor & Yaruss, 2019b; Yaruss, 1998, 2007; Yaruss & Quesal, 2004) conceptualizes the stuttering condition in terms of several interdependent areas. These have been depicted in graphical format by Yaruss and colleagues (2019b; 1998; 2004), with the most recent update—based on extensive data collected from hundreds of people who stutter—developed by Tichenor & Yaruss (2019b). A schematized adaptation of the Tichenor & Yaruss graphic is presented in Figure 1 as a guide for the components of the overall stuttering experience described in this paper.
The first component of the Tichenor & Yaruss adaptation of the ICF for stuttering is the presumed etiology underlying the condition. This etiology is likely genetic in many cases (Kraft & Yairi, 2012), though interactions with environmental and experiential factors are certainly involved. Such genetic and epigenetic differences are presumed to contribute to neurological differences that have been widely documented in both children and adults who stutter (see Etchell et al., 2017, for review). Notably, etiological factors are not accounted for in the ICF as defined by the WHO, for the ICF is designed to reflect the experience of living with health conditions. Nevertheless, these etiological aspects of stuttering are included in the adaptation of the ICF for stuttering because it is useful for researchers and clinicians alike to recognize that the origins of the stuttering condition are found in these genetic, epigenetic, and neurological differences and not, for example, in psychological differences (Yaruss, 1998).
The consequences of these underlying etiological differences are known in the ICF as impairments that may affect body function or body structure. In the study of stuttering, these impairments reflect the various differences in motor, linguistic, emotional, cognitive, and temperamental processes that have been well-documented in the stuttering research literature (see Bloodstein et al., 2021). These functional differences interact to directly cause the primary symptom of those impairments (Kolk, 1990, 1991; Kolk & Heeschen, 1990) as experienced and described by people who stutter, that is, the sensation of a “loss of control” or of “being stuck” when attempting to speak (Perkins, 1990; Tichenor & Yaruss, 2018, 2019b). (This aspect of the experience of stuttering is discussed in more detail in the second section of this paper.) The ways in which a person reacts to this primary impairment symptom dictates what a listener hears or an observer sees when a speaker is experiencing a moment of stuttering.
When a person who stutters experiences this sensation of being stuck, out of control, or unable to speak in the way they wish to speak, then they may react in various ways. These individual reactions are described by the next component of the ICF, personal context. Examples of personal context include affective, behavioral, and cognitive reactions that may occur as a result of the impairment. These reactions, which are described in more detail in the next section, are highly individualized across people who stutter; none should be viewed as universal or true for every person who stutters. As a person continues to live with stuttering, these affective, behavioral, and cognitive reactions interact, resulting in increased observable stuttering severity (Tichenor & Yaruss, 2018, 2019b). Negative reactions often become so deeply ingrained that they seem to be automatic or habitual; yet, they remain learned coping reactions that a person develops as a result of living with stuttering (Brutten & Shoemaker, 1967). With appropriate treatment, support, and personal experiences, however, these reactions can be changed; indeed, this is a common goal for many therapeutic approaches to stuttering.
Personal Context: Affective Reactions
Affective reactions may include feelings of fear, anger, embarrassment, guilt, or shame (Beilby, 2014; Boyle, 2018; Corcoran & Stewart, 1998; Daniels & Gabel, 2004; Murphy, 1999; Sheehan, 1953, 1970), as well as heightened levels of anxiety in some individuals (Alm, 2014; Craig et al., 2003; Iverach et al., 2018). These affective components can be greatly debilitating, with some individuals even describing their experience of moments of stuttering in terms of emotional pain, feelings of hopelessness, or exhaustion (Tichenor & Yaruss, 2018). Negative affective reactions are reported as common experiences across individuals as they live their lives as people who stutter (Ahlbach & Benson, 1994; Reitzes & Reitzes, 2012). When asked how frequently individuals experience these affective reactions in moments of stuttering, many adults report often or always feeling ashamed (45%), embarrassed (53%), or emotionally drained (49%) (Tichenor & Yaruss, 2019a). These data indicate that negative affective reactions are commonly experienced but also highly individualized in form and occurrence.
Personal Context: Behavioral Reactions
Behavioral reactions, which occur as a speaker attempts to regain control or maintain outwardly fluent speech, can take the form of commonly identified stuttering behaviors, such as repetitions, prolongations, and blocks (Perkins, 1990; Tichenor & Yaruss, 2018, 2019b). In particular, physical tension has been long described a common behavioral reaction (Snidecor, 1955; Tichenor et al., 2017). Behavioral reactions can also be seen in other ways. For example, many people who stutter also avert their gaze when stuttering due to presumed shame or guilt experienced by the person (Manning & Dilollo, 2018; Wingate, 1964). Such behaviors have been hypothesized to occur as a coping or protective mechanism (Iverach et al., 2017), though they directly influence perceived stuttering severity (Tatchell et al., 1983). Covert or interiorized behavioral reactions are also quite common (Constantino et al., 2017; E. Douglass & Quarrington, 1952; J. E. Douglass et al., 2019; Murphy, Quesal, et al., 2007). These include behaviors that are not easily observed by a listener, such as avoidance of sounds, words, or situations or substituting words when speaker perceives that they might stutter (Tichenor & Yaruss, 2018). A recent study of more than 500 adults who stutter revealed that nearly 50% of speakers engaged in covert behaviors at least some of the time, and 10 to 20% of the respondents indicated that they often or always engage in covert behaviors (Tichenor & Yaruss, 2019a). Thus, both overt and covert behavioral reactions are quite commonly experienced by people who stutter.
Personal Context: Cognitive Reactions
Cognitive reactions include the thoughts that a person might have regarding their speech and stuttering. Common example of cognitive reactions include rumination, also known as repetitive negative thinking (RNT), as well as anticipation about future speaking or stuttering experiences (Constantino et al., 2017; Jackson et al., 2015, 2018; Tichenor & Yaruss, 2020a). RNT is the learned habit of engaging in recurrent, negative, and self-reflective thoughts about one’s life, concerns, or experiences (Ehring et al., 2011; Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991, 2004; Watkins, 2008). Such thoughts can be intrusive and often result in decreased quality of life (Ehring et al., 2011; Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008; Wrosch & Scheier, 2003). Tichenor & Yaruss (2020) found that adults who stutter who engage in high rates of RNT experience significantly more adverse impact related to stuttering than those who engage in RNT to a lesser degree. Anticipation is the sense that one may soon experience a moment of stuttering (Bloodstein et al., 2021; Jackson, 2015; Shapiro, 1999; Starkweather & Givens-Ackerman, 1997; Van Riper, 1982). According to Bloodstein, anticipation is reinforced by past communication experiences, leading to increased fear in subsequent speaking situations (Bloodstein, 1972, 1975; Brocklehurst et al., 2013). Other researchers have referred to anticipation-related thoughts and feelings in more cognitive/affective terms relating to anxiety (Alm, 2004; Craig et al., 2003; Messenger et al., 2004) or negative evaluation by others (Brundage et al., 2017). Yaruss and Tichenor (2020a) suggested that anticipation, as most often discussed in the stuttering literature, may be meaningfully viewed as a stuttering-specific manifestation of RNT: as a moment of stuttering is anticipated, an individual may experience disruptive thoughts about their speaking abilities or about potentially negative listener reactions. Such anticipatory thoughts thereby develop as a natural consequence of experiencing negative communication interactions.
Environmental Context
Whereas personal context in the ICF reflects the individual speaker’s reactions, environmental context reflects the influence of other people in the speaker’s life. The environmental context can influence the speaker via a conversation partner or, more generally, via society as a whole. This interaction between environment and speaker further influences the personal reactions discussed above. For example, many people who stutter have discussed the negative influence that (real or perceived) speaking pressure places on them (Ahlbach & Benson, 1994; Reitzes & Reitzes, 2012). Broader societal influences also include stigma against stuttering (Boyle, 2013, 2015, 2018; Boyle & Blood, 2015; Boyle & Fearon, 2018) and negative public opinions about stuttering and people who stutter (St. Louis, 2020; St. Louis et al., 2016). Society often displays people who stutter in a negative light, perpetuating the ideal of stuttering as something abnormal (Constantino, 2018). Like other components of the ICF, these environmental reactions influence an individual’s personal reactions. Experiencing societal judgments about stuttering can lead to personal reactions such as self-stigma, or the feeling that a person does not meet societal expectations (e.g., fluency) due to their own failings or limitations (Boyle, 2018). Feeling external speaking pressure may also cause a person to increase their physical tension when speaking, and this, in turn, can increase observable stuttering severity and overall difficulty with communicating (Tichenor et al., 2017; Tichenor & Yaruss, 2018). Thus, the environmental context has a significant effect on a person’s overall experience of stuttering. Unfortunately, this effect is often detrimental to the lives of people who stutter.
Activity Limitations and Participation Restrictions
The impairment(s), the personal reactions, and the environmental reactions, can all combine to affect a person’s ability to live life as they wish. These real-world consequences are described in the fourth section of the ICF, activities and participation. A considerable body of research has highlighted various activity limitations and participation restrictions that people who stutter may experience. For children, negative personal and environmental reactions to stuttering can lead to difficulties in school performance and in extracurricular activities, as well as in their ability to build relationships and develop individual autonomy. Other activity limitations and participation restrictions include being steered toward lower-status occupations (Gabel et al., 2004; McAllister et al., 2012), experiencing financial loss (Blumgart et al., 2010; Gabel et al., 2004; Gerlach et al., 2018) job loss (Constantino et al., 2017), and enduring social rejection (Davis et al., 2002; Van Borsel et al., 2011). Taken together, these aspects of adverse impact negatively affect a person’s overall quality of life, and the implications for people who stutter can be pervasive and far-reaching (Craig et al., 2009; Klompas & Ross, 2004; Koedoot et al., 2011).
Summary: the ICF as Applied to Stuttering
The sensation of being stuck or of losing control, the speaker’s personal reactions, the environmental context, and the activity limitations and participation restrictions, combine together to describe the overall experience of stuttering (Tichenor & Yaruss, 2019b; Yaruss, 1998; Yaruss & Quesal, 2004). Numerous lines of research continue to further define and specify various aspects of the stuttering condition, adding further detail and examples that help people who stutter, scientists, and clinicians recognize that, “stuttering is more than just stuttering” (Yaruss, 2007, p. 314). In other words, although stuttering may involve disfluent speech, it also involves numerous other experiential aspects that are within the scope of speech-language pathologists to address (ASHA, 2016).
Unfortunately, this fundamental truth about stuttering—and its implications for stuttering therapy—is still debated in some discussions about how speech-language pathologists can help people who stutter (see Nippold, 2012). Focusing on fluency as the key characteristic of the stuttering condition leads to several potential concerns (Tichenor et al, in press). For example, researchers who require participants to exhibit a certain level of observably disfluent speech as an inclusion criterion for a study may obtain results that apply more to disfluent speech than to people who stutter (Tichenor & Yaruss, 2019a). Likewise, clinicians who focus intervention primarily on fluency miss the opportunity to ensure that their intervention decreases the burden of living with stuttering. While treatment may include working on stuttering behaviors and even fluency, it most certainly should include more than those aspects. Thus, a broader consideration of the many ways that stuttering can be experienced will help researchers and clinicians ensure that their study, assessment, and treatment of stuttering addresses the entire experience of the condition and results in a reduction in the overall impact of stuttering on the life of the person who stutters.
Understanding the Moment of Stuttering Itself
While the field’s understanding of the overall stuttering condition has developed over the years, relatively less has been written about the moment of stuttering itself (i.e., how a person experiences and reacts to feeling stuck or out of control when speaking). A better understanding how people experience the moment of stuttering can lead to a deeper understanding of how speakers cope with stuttering. This, in turn, can directly lead to more effective treatment of not only the speech behaviors associated with stuttering but also the negative sequelae described in the prior section. One explanation for this relative lack of research into the moment of stuttering is the fact that stuttering has almost exclusively been defined through the eyes and ears of listeners. Johnson (1959) stated, “It is to be appreciated that there is no standard operational definition of stuttering or stutterer. Generally speaking, the word stuttering signifies a judgment made by the listener” (p. 10; emphasis added). In considering the types of observable behaviors that caused a person to be judged as stuttering, he stated that syllable repetition and sound prolongation were reported more frequently in the stuttering group rather than the control group, and that those types of speech behaviors are evidently more likely to be classified as stuttering at least by listeners within our own culture (Johnson, 1959). This research was one of the first attempts at defining stuttering in terms of observable speech-related behaviors.
Over many decades, ongoing work in this area has further refined the demarcation between what behaviors are considered to be stuttered and what behaviors are not considered to be stuttered (Johnson, 1959, 1961a, 1961c). For example, Wingate (1964) labeled repetitions/prolongations of sounds, syllables, and monosyllable words as stuttering while delegating other aspects (e.g., tension, body movements, and struggle) to the status of so-called accessory features (also commonly referred to as “secondary” behaviors). Conture (1990) distinguished between within-word and between-word disfluencies, suggesting that the former (generally) reflected the speech behaviors demonstrated by people who stutter and the latter (generally) reflected the speech behaviors of people who do not stutter. In a similar fashion, Gregory and colleagues discussed this demarcation (i.e., speech behaviors demonstrated by people who stutter vs. speech that people who do not stutter are sometimes observed to demonstrate) in terms of “less-typical” and “more-typical” disfluencies (Gregory et al., 2003; Gregory & Hill, 1999). More-typical disfluencies are those more likely to be demonstrated by the general population of people who do not stutter. Many current authors use the terms “stuttering-like” and “non-stuttering-like” or “other” disfluencies (Yairi & Ambrose, 2005). Importantly, all of these efforts are based on listener judgements (see Conture, 1990; Gregory et al., 2003; Gregory & Hill, 1999; Teesson et al., 2003; Wingate, 1964; Yairi, 1996, 2001; Yairi & Ambrose, 2005); relatively little input has been drawn from the perspective of speakers who stutter themselves. Although such classifications are widely used, numerous authors have highlighted various challenges with listener-based assessment of stuttering in both research and clinical contexts. These include the variability of the observable behavior (Constantino et al., 2016; Tichenor & Yaruss, 2020b), as well as poor reliability and questionable validity of listener judgments (see Brundage et al., 2006; Cordes & Ingham, 1994, 1999; Hall et al., 1987; Kully & Boberg, 1988; Martin & Haroldson, 1992; Tichenor et al., 2017). These concerns raise the possibility that the underlying assumption—that the moment of stuttering can be accurately defined based on what a listener sees or hears—may not be valid.
Not all authors have focused on listener-based definitions of stuttering; some individuals have directly considered the experience of people who stutter (Perkins, 1990; Quesal, 1989; Tichenor & Yaruss, 2018, 2019b). The idea that the core or essential experience of stuttering reflects a loss of control has its origins in rebuttals to listener-defined paradigms in which certain behaviors were defined as stuttering while other behaviors were defined as “normal” or non-stuttered. Perkins was among the first to propose that the actual difference between stuttered disfluencies and non-stuttered disfluencies is the internal experience of the speaker (Perkins, 1983) and not the external appearance of the behavior as observed by a listener. In referencing the experiences of people who stutter, Perkins said, “an indisputable reality for people who consider themselves to be stutterers is that they feel as they lose control of their speech when they stutter” (Perkins, 1984, p. 247). Perkins defined stuttering as, “a temporary or overt or covert loss of control of the ability to move forward fluently in the execution of linguistically formulated speech” (Perkins, 1984, emphasis added). Perkins later elaborated on his original point by differentiating between voluntary and automatic control, using the example of how fluency-enhancing strategies affect speech production. When using fluency-enhancing strategies, a person may appear to a listener as if they are speaking fluently even though they may be speaking with greater effort (Perkins, 1984). Therefore, any non-spontaneous fluency that they may demonstrate or experience is not the same as the spontaneously fluent speech of people who do not stutter (Constantino et al., 2020). Dayalu and Kalinowski (2002) referred to this effect as “pseudofluency” (p. 87). Relatedly, people may experience stuttering covertly, that is they may feel the loss of control in their speech even without displaying any overt disruption in speech that can be observed by a listener (J. E. Douglass et al., 2018; Tichenor & Yaruss, 2018). In both of these cases, a person may be demonstrating perceptibly or observably fluent speech so far as a listener can judge, even though stuttering is actually happening.
Therefore, using the apparent absence of observably “stuttered” speech as an indicator that a person does not stutter or for evaluating therapy outcomes limits our understanding of that person’s stuttering to surface characteristics only (Jackson et al., 2012). Tichenor et al. (in press) have argued that such a view may restrict a person from being included in the community of people who stutter, raise barriers that make it more difficult for that person to access therapy (e.g., if a certain percentage or severity of observable stuttering is needed in order to determine eligibility for therapy in a school setting or for third-party payment), or increase the risk that a person will be discharged from therapy prematurely because they appear to be speaking fluent when they may actually be avoiding speaking altogether. (Such factors are further complicated by the variability of observable stuttering behavior.) Thus, surface definitions of the moment of stuttering not only ignore the broader experience of the condition described by the ICF; they also ignore the reality of the speaker’s experience of that moment.
Perkins’ work has inspired investigations seeking to further specify what speakers experience during moments of stuttering. For example, Tichenor and Yaruss (2018) conducted a qualitative exploration of the moment of stuttering with 13 adults who stutter. Results corroborated many past assumptions about the moment of stuttering. Specifically, respondents reported that their experience of the moment of stuttering involves more than just disrupted speech; it also involves cognitive/affective components, such as fear, shame, guilt, anxiety that occur both while they are experiencing stuttering and over broader stretches of time. Respondents echoed Perkins’s suggestion that the moment of stuttering involves a feeling being stuck or the sensation of losing control. Importantly, this sensation was discussed as a core or essential feature, that is, an aspect of the experience that causes the affective, behavioral, and cognitive reactions described above. Respondents also indicated that the moment of stuttering is associated with various forms of management as speakers attempt to modify, control, prevent, or hide their stuttering from others. In developing the essential structure from their thematic analyses (i.e., the core experience of the moment of stuttering that was common across all participants), Tichenor and Yaruss concluded that,
The sensation of being stuck occurs when speakers know what they want to say but are unable to say it. This sensation does not directly correspond to the behaviors that listeners may observe (e.g., repetitions, prolongations, and blocks). Rather, speakers may react to the sensation of being stuck with tension or struggle, and these behaviors might be observable. They might also experience being stuck without observable reaction until the sensation passes. Other possible reactions include attempting to restart speaking, prolonging or repeating a sound to hold the conversational floor, avoiding sounds or words, changing words or the message in an attempt to postpone or prevent stuttering, or holding the conversational floor through other conversational means (e.g., starter or filler words). Speakers may also respond by using speaking strategies (e.g., reducing tension) through methods learned in treatment or independently. They may also respond by focusing on acceptance or spontaneity (p. 1189).
Larger follow-up studies have replicated and expanded these findings. Tichenor and Yaruss (2019a) surveyed 502 adults who stutter to determine what thoughts, feelings, and behaviors they experience during and around moments of stuttering. Results confirmed that speakers alone are privy to the internal sensation of being stuck or out of control when speaking. Thus, these data support the conclusion by Moore and Perkins (1990, p. 377) that, “listeners do not have the valid information on which to base their judgments” of what constitutes a moment of stuttering. Listeners can only perceive that stuttering has happened based on what the speaker does in response to the feeling of loss of control and they can only do this if the person responds in an outwardly visible way. If a person does not respond in a visible way, then stuttering might well have happened, but the listener might not know. This can occur if the speaker changed words, paused, discontinued speaking, or used some other covert behavior to mask or hide the underlying disruption.
The ways in which people who stutter respond to the feeling of loss of control are variable across situations and over time (Constantino et al., 2016; Tichenor & Yaruss, 2020b), and highly influenced by each speaker’s personal experiences. For example, Tichenor and Yaruss (2019a) asked adults who stutter what their goals were when speaking. Analyses determined that responses fell along two factors: (a) to be more fluent/not stutter when speaking and (b) to say what they want to say regardless of stuttering. The authors found that the more likely a person is to have the goal of speaking more fluently/not stuttering, the more likely they are to experience adverse impact related to stuttering. Adults who stutter whose goal is to be more fluent/not stutter are significantly more likely to avoid situations, sounds, or words, to push or struggle with tension, or to experience shame, guilt, or fear associated with stuttering. A person whose goal is to be more open about stuttering and to say what they want to say regardless of how fluent they might be showed the opposite pattern of results. That is, they were less likely to avoid situations, sounds, words, to push or struggle with tension, or to experience shame, guilt, or fear. Thus, a person’s goal when speaking significantly influences how they react to the sensation of being stuck or of losing control when speaking.
This line of research has significant implications for speech-language pathologists who work with individuals who stutter: Unless we come to understand who a person is, how they approach speaking, how they manage the condition in their life, and more, then we cannot truly understand their experiences related to stuttering. Considering these (and other) fundamental aspects of a person’s experiences is therefore critical information for planning effective intervention. Without such consideration, treatment approaches will be more generic and likely less effective because they will not address the speaker’s unique experiences and coping patterns.
Implications for Stuttering Assessment and Treatment
Historically, much of stuttering therapy has focused on decreasing the occurrence or severity of overt stuttering behaviors, using techniques involving speech modification (e.g., techniques for enhancing speech fluency) or stuttering modification (e.g., techniques for reducing tension or struggle surrounding moments of stuttering). Treatments targeting observable fluency have been shown to be effective at increasing perceptibly fluent speech in both children and adults who stutter (de Sonneville-Koedoot et al., 2015; Jones et al., 2005; Neumann et al., 2017). Many authors have also described the benefits of stuttering modification in increasing the ease of speaking (Van Riper, 1973). Together, these two approaches comprise the standard of speech treatment for people who stutter (Bloodstein et al., 2021). Yet, just as stuttering involves more than disfluent speech (Yaruss, 2007), treatment for stuttering should also address more than just disfluent speech in order to decrease negative experiences associated with the condition (Yaruss et al., 2012).
Following the components of the ICF model can help clinicians understand how each person experiences stuttering in their own way. Thus, treatment should focus on: the ways a person responds to the sensation of being stuck or of losing control when speaking, the specific forms of adverse impact that the person experiences, the reactions of those in that person’s environment, and the unique activity limitations or participation restrictions they experience in their lives. Following are several examples of how clinicians can tailor therapy to address specific aspects of adverse impact that their clients may experience.
Decreasing Negative Personal Reactions
As noted above, many people who stutter experience negative affective, behavioral, and cognitive reactions associated with stuttering; as they come to better understand these reactions, they can change them and thereby reduce this aspect of the adverse impact of stuttering. Clinicians should recognize that changing negative reactions can be difficult, in part because it is completely normal and understandable for people who stutter to have difficulty coping with the repeated sensation of feeling stuck, as well as the repeated negative reactions from listeners. Unique speaking situations occur many times each day, and the effect of habitual repeated negative experiences can be significant. The variability and unpredictability of the speaker’s experience of the loss of control compounds these negative reactions (Tichenor & Yaruss, 2020b). Moreover, these reactions develop over a lifetime, so it is expected that it will take time and effort to change them. Fortunately, several approaches to therapy can be used to decrease the frequency and severity of these reactions. Following are a few current examples.
Mindfulness.
One approach for helping speakers reduce their negative reactions during a moment of stuttering is mindfulness. Mindfulness is a broad term that can be used to refer to a number of techniques or processes broadly aimed at, “increasing awareness and responding skillfully to mental processes that contribute to emotional distress and maladaptive behavior” (Bishop et al., 2004, p. 230). Boyle (2011) provided an apt tutorial for how speech-language pathologists can use mindfulness strategies, such as increased attention, to decrease adverse impact in their clients who stutter. Having a client attend to what they are doing when they are feeling stuck (e.g., by increasing their attention to the moment of stuttering) can help to counteract continued use of less-helpful behavioral reactions, such as closing their eyes or tensing their muscles. Speech-language pathologists can also help speakers decrease such behavioral reactions by guiding clients to increase their attention to what they are doing, thinking, or feeling during moments of stuttering. For example, a SLP might help a client “freeze” or stay in a moment of stuttering while it is happening to give the speaker the time and opportunity to think more deeply about that moment of stuttering (Guitar, 2019; Reardon-Reeves & Yaruss, 2013). As the speaker becomes more aware of their physical behaviors (e.g., tensing), thoughts (e.g., “I can’t do it.”) and feelings (e.g., fear) during that moment of stuttering, they can explore whether those reactions are helpful or necessary. As the speaker’s understanding of these behaviors, thoughts, and feelings expands further, they can seek to modify them, and this can lead to moments of stuttering that are physically easier or less uncomfortable. Such modifications can ultimately lead to changes in the way the person speaks or stutters; they can also lead to changes in how a person thinks or feels about stuttering. It is important to note that such therapy must be individualized to each speaker’s needs. For example, people who stutter show wide variations in both the location and the degree of physical tension they experience in their body when stuttering (Tichenor et al., 2017), so clinicians should not assume that a particular instruction for reducing tension would apply to all speakers. Mindfulness techniques help to ensure that treatment is appropriately individualized for each unique speaker’s experience of physical tension.
Mindfulness techniques can be particularly effective for addressing affective and cognitive reactions to stuttering. As noted above, many people who stutter engage in repetitive negative, with thoughts such as “I am stuttering (or I am about to stutter), and that is bad,” “I am stuttering, and people are thinking poorly of me,” or “I am stuttering, and I should feel bad that I stutter.” One key principle of mindfulness that can help in such situations is nonjudgement, or being “non-evaluative about ones experiences in the present moment” (Dekeyser et al., 2008, p. 1236). Practicing nonjudgement involves letting thoughts occur without feeling the emotions that may previously have been attached to those thoughts. Thus, the automatic thought of “I am stuttering, and that is bad” can instead become, simply, “I am stuttering.” Detaching or defusing the emotion from the thought (Anderson & Levy, 2009; Jha et al., 2010; Wells & Papageorgiou, 2004) can help to reduce the impact of negative automatic thoughts. There is growing research that mindfulness-based therapies are effective at decreasing the burden of living with stuttering (Beilby et al., 2012; Cheasman, 2013; Gupta, 2015; Gupta et al., 2016; Palasik & Hannan, 2013); A clinician whose therapy incorporates mindfulness-based techniques, such as staying in a moment and practicing nonjudgement, can help clients to decrease both the frequency and severity of RNT (see Tichenor & Yaruss, 2020a, for discussion). Again, this effect can only be achieved through consideration of what individual clients are experiencing; clinicians must assess each client’s specific repetitive negative thoughts before initiating the process of changing RNT in therapy.
Desensitization.
Another aspect of therapy that can be used to decrease the frequency and severity of negative personal reactions is desensitization, or the process of minimizing adverse reactions by gradually exposing oneself to uncomfortable stimuli (Foa & Kozak, 1986; Rapee et al., 2000; Wolpe, 1958). Desensitization has been used in stuttering therapy for many years (see Murphy, Yaruss, et al., 2007; Reardon-Reeves & Yaruss, 2013; Van Riper, 1973). Voluntarily stuttering, also known as pseudostuttering, intentional stuttering, bouncing, or negative practice (Dunlap, 1932; Gregory, 2003; Guitar, 2019; Ham, 1990; Ingham, 1984; Manning & Dilollo, 2018; Nicholas, 2015; Sheehan, 1970; Sheehan & Voas, 1957; Van Riper, 1973), is among the most commonly used desensitization strategies in stuttering therapy. Voluntary stuttering involves the intentional production of speech behaviors that sound like actual stuttering behavior; this allows speakers to experience some of the discomfort associated with true stuttering but in a safer or more controlled manner or environment. As speakers learn to tolerate voluntary stuttering with less emotional distress, they can subsequently increase the tension or frequency of their “stuttering” behaviors to better replicate their actual moments of stuttering. Repeated exposure helps to increase speakers’ tolerance of actual moments of stuttering, and this, in turn, helps to decrease other affective, cognitive, and behavioral reactions. Specifically, as speakers learn to tolerate the sensation of loss of control, their emotional reactions such as panic and fear can be reduced, and this allows them to experience moments of stuttering more easily.
Desensitization can also be used to help speakers transform their responses to environmental reactions to their stuttering, such as being laughed at or hung up on while on the telephone. Clinicians can help their clients approach this through using a hierarchy that helps clients move from easier to harder situations as they practice tolerating other people’s reactions to their stuttering (e.g., Reardon-Reeves & Yaruss, 2013). Again, this hierarchy is individualized for each client; no two people will have the exact same hierarchy of feared situations. Initially, speakers should practice facing their fears in safe, controlled environments, such as the therapy room. As their comfort grows, they can move on toward more difficult, real-world situations; clinicians can help to plan and structure this aspect of therapy by working with the speaker to determine a list of increasingly difficult situations specific to the speaker. For example, if a client fears being hung up on, the clinician can start by creating opportunities for the speaker to use the phone while in the therapy room, then progress toward increasingly difficult situations, such as using the phone while calling the clinician in another room, then using the phone to call a close friend, then using the phone to call a store. A final, more challenging, task may involve more “real-world” types of tasks, such as calling restaurants with the hope of being hung up on so that the speaker can develop resilience to that feared situation. Repeated exposure (and being hung up on numerous times) helps to lessen the negative emotional reaction, and this helps to reduce the overall adverse impact of stuttering (and the fear of stuttering) on the speaker’s life.
Cognitive Therapy
Mindfulness and desensitization are just some of the tools that SLPs can use to help speakers reduce negative reactions to stuttering. Cognitive therapy approaches, such as Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy (CBT) and Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) (Beilby et al., 2012; Beilby & Byrnes, 2012; Blood, 1995; Cheasman, 2013; Emerick, 1988; Gupta et al., 2016; Helgadóttir et al., 2014; Kelman & Wheeler, 2015; Menzies et al., 2008, 2009; Palasik & Hannan, 2013), have also been shown to be valuable for decreasing negative personal reactions to stuttering. For example, CBT can be used to decrease the frequency or severity of negative thoughts, feelings, and behaviors related to stuttering. The underlying principle of CBT is that thoughts, feelings, and behaviors are interconnected (Beck, 1967, 1976; Ellis, 1957, 1962). Therefore, when a negative thought is altered, the associated feelings and behaviors are also altered. The process of CBT includes identifying negative thoughts, collecting evidence related to those thoughts, and ultimately changing those less helpful thoughts to more helpful thoughts. For example, a negative thought might be, “no one likes me because I stutter.” Through CBT, the individual might examine the evidence behind that thought (e.g., creating a list of friends who like them regardless of the stuttering). This evidence is then used to evaluate the original thought to determine if it is valid. For example, a person might realize that they have many friends who do not actually care whether or not they stutter. This gives speakers the opportunity to then replace that negative thought with a more neutral or positive thought, thereby decreasing their negative reactions surrounding stuttering. Several studies have shown that CBT is effective at decreasing feelings of anxiety and increasing positive reactions to stuttering (Kelman & Wheeler, 2015; Menzies et al., 2008; Murphy, Yaruss, et al., 2007; St Clare et al., 2009).
ACT is rooted in the philosophy that negative thoughts do not necessarily have to be changed; instead, they can be acknowledged and accepted. Through ACT, speakers can learn that negative thoughts and emotions can be present, but they do not have to dictate how a person acts or reacts (Fletcher & Hayes, 2005; Hayes et al., 2006). A key component of ACT is mindfulness, as described above, because mindfulness helps speakers understand their actions, feelings, and thoughts more fully. Individuals are encouraged to use this deeper understanding of their experiences in support of actions that are focused on their values rather than allowing negative reactions to lead toward further negative thoughts or actions that are inconsistent with their goals. Clinician can help speakers to create meaningful goals and tailor therapy activities to meet each individual’s unique needs. For example, it may be important for a client to give a presentation at work or school. Before giving a presentation, an individual may be feeling anxious and having negative thoughts such as, “I’m so bad at giving presentations. I’m going to fail.” Through ACT, speakers can recognize that those thoughts and feelings exist; the thoughts do not necessarily have to be changed or minimized. The speaker can then show compassion towards themselves and consider an inner dialogue such as, “This presentation means a lot, so it makes sense that I’m feeling nervous. But I know this presentation is important for me to do.” Because the individual is focused on taking action regardless of the feelings and thoughts that they are having in the moment, their goal shifts from trying to eliminate the negative thoughts to simply giving the presentation. Like CBT, ACT has shown to be effective in reducing overall adverse impact of stuttering (Beilby et al., 2012; Beilby & Byrnes, 2012; Cheasman, 2013; Palasik & Hannan, 2013).
Decreasing the Influence of Environment
The environmental context component of the ICF refers to external reactions to stuttering such as societal stigma, bullying, and discrimination. Unfortunately, negative environmental influences cannot always be prevented. Ample evidence has shown that people who stutter are more likely to experience discrimination, bullying, and other negative reactions (Blumgart et al., 2010; Davis et al., 2002; Gabel et al., 2004; Gerlach et al., 2018; McAllister et al., 2012; St. Louis, 2020; St. Louis et al., 2016; Van Borsel et al., 2011). Nevertheless, therapeutic approaches can help individual speakers cope with bullying and discrimination and, ultimately, minimize the negative effects of these environmental reactions. Examples include educating others about stuttering, self-advocacy, and self-disclosing stuttering (Boyle et al., 2018; Boyle & Gabel, 2020; Byrd, Croft, et al., 2017; Byrd, McGill, et al., 2017; Croft & Byrd, 2021; McGill et al., 2018; Murphy, Yaruss, et al., 2007; Snyder et al., 2021; Yaruss et al., 2018). Educating others may be therapeutic for the person who stutters, as well, because learning to talk more openly about stuttering can help to decrease self-stigma and decrease the frequency of more covert behavioral reactions.
Self-advocacy involves speaking up for oneself and making one’s needs known. It can take many forms; the specific self-advocacy approach that a speaker chooses will be highly individualized. Some speakers may ask others to be patient with them as they speak, and others may ask for increased time to speak in order to reduce pressures on themselves. Speakers may ask for accommodations in their environment to decrease the burden that speaking places on them. These might include asking to go first when giving a presentation or talk, or asking to talk via videoconferencing rather than the telephone. Self-disclosure of stuttering involves acknowledging the fact that a person stutters. Like other aspects of therapy, self-disclosure can be accomplished in numerous ways, and clinicians need to be prepared to work with each individual client to find the strategy that is right for them. For example, some speakers may simply state, “I stutter,” when addressing a listener. Others may provide more detail: “I stutter, so it is helpful for me if you can be patient as I speak.” Voluntary stuttering (including the pseudostuttering described above) is also a form of self-disclosure, because it can make the stuttering behavior more apparent to others. Regardless of the form of self-disclosure that a speaker chooses, the very act of talking openly about stuttering helps other people in the environment to gain a better understanding of the condition. This ultimately serves to reduce stigma and discrimination. Importantly, all of these aspects of treatment are inter-related: desensitization allows a speaker to approach self-advocacy tasks; self-advocacy strategies help to educate others while empowering the speaker, and empowered speakers are more likely to engage in speaking situations and experience reduced negative reactions.
Decreasing Activity Limitations and Participation Restrictions
All of the strategies for helping people cope with the sensation of loss of control, as well as those for addressing personal and environmental reactions, combine to reduce limitations in daily activities and restrictions in participation that people who stutter may experience. To ensure that these gains translate to improvements in daily life, it is critical for clinicians to extend therapy to real-world situations (Yaruss & Reardon, 2003). Therapy cannot just stay in the therapy room; in order for benefits to be experienced in other settings, it is necessary for therapy to incorporate those other settings through the process of generalization. Similar to the process used for desensitization, clinicians can use hierarchies to help clients move from the relatively safe environment of the therapy room to more real and more difficult situations outside of the therapy room. This principle applies to a host of treatment goals that target increasing the ability to engage in life’s activities. For example, speaking on the phone may provoke unique fear or anxiety for some people who stutter. A person who stutters might be able to make the phone call with a certain sort of person (e.g., family members, friends, neighbors) but not others (e.g., strangers). Alternatively, this person might be more able to stutter openly in a noisy environment vs. a quiet one. The clinician in both of these examples can systematically manipulate the context or the environment to allow the person who stutters to be challenged while still being set up for success. In this way, tightly focused and manageable goals make the process of generalization easier, as individuals build upon success in controlled environments (such as accomplishing a task following a clinician’s model) and work up to their ultimate goals such as accomplishing the same task in the classroom or at work). Thus, planning for and promoting generalization in therapy greatly increases the impact therapy can have on the life of a client. A clinician who plans intervention based on current understandings of the impact of stuttering based upon recent research, combined with a thorough understanding of each unique client’s individual experiences related to stuttering, has the capability and the background that is necessary to effect meaningful change.
Summary
Speech-language pathologists can play an important role in decreasing the burden that too often affects the lives of people who stutter. For decades, some researchers and clinicians have acknowledged that the experience of stuttering is more than just disfluent speech. While personal experiences have long been incorporated within some approaches to stuttering therapy, there remains room for growth. This paper has outlined how the broader experience of stuttering can be conceptualized based on the ICF framework, how moments of stuttering are perceived by people who stutter, and how adverse impact related to stuttering can be more effectively addressed in therapy by considering the unique experiences of individual speakers who stutter. When speech-language pathologists account for the individualized ways that each client experiences the stuttering condition, they have the opportunity to increase the value and efficacy of their therapy and, ultimately, help to reduce the impact of stuttering on their clients’ lives.
-
The overall experience of stuttering includes:
Personal Context and the Loss of Control
Environmental Context and Activity Limitations
The Loss of Control, Personal Context, Environmental Context, Activity Limitations, and Participation Restrictions.
-
Adults who stutter whose goal is to be more fluent or not stutter are much more like to:
Avoid situations, sounds, or words.
Demonstrate increased quality of life
Be male as opposed to female
-
Which of the following is one example of a therapeutic approach that may be used to decrease negative personal reactions
Mindfulness
Fluency shaping techniques
Encouraging avoidance behaviors
-
Desensitization is defined as:
The process of using less and less physical tension to aide fluency
The process of minimizing adverse reactions by gradually exposing oneself to uncomfortable stimuli
The process of becoming less reactive to listener perceptions.
-
Educating others about stuttering, self-advocacy, and self-disclosing stuttering are all techniques and strategies aimed at:
Decreasing the influence of the environment
Increasing one’s sense of self and quality of life
Decreasing negative personal reactions
Acknowledgment
Preparation of this article was supported, in part, by a grant from the National Institutes of Health/National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (NIH/NIDCD) under award R01DC018795. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.
Footnotes
Disclosure: All authors have declared that no competing interests existed at the time of publication.
Contributor Information
Seth E. Tichenor, Speech-Language Pathology, Duquesne University.
Caryn Herring, Communicative Sciences and Disorders, Michigan State University.
J. Scott Yaruss, Communicative Sciences and Disorders, Michigan State University.
References
- Ahlbach J, & Benson V (Eds). (1994). To Say What Is Ours: The best of 13 years of letting go. (2nd ed.). The National Stuttering Project. [Google Scholar]
- Alm PA (2004). Stuttering, emotions, and heart rate during anticipatory anxiety: a critical review. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 29(2), 123–133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alm PA (2014). Stuttering in relation to anxiety, temperament, and personality: Review and analysis with focus on causality. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 40, 5–21. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2014.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson MC, & Levy BJ (2009). Suppressing unwanted memories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(4), 189–194. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01634.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- ASHA. (2016). Scope of Practice 10.1044/policy.SP2016-00343 [DOI]
- Beck AT (1967). Depression: Causes and Treatment. University of Pennsylvania Press. [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT (1976). Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders. International Universities Press. [Google Scholar]
- Beilby JM (2014). Psychosocial Impact of Living with a Stuttering Disorder : Knowing is Not Enough. Seminars in Speech and Language, 35(2), 132–143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beilby JM, & Byrnes ML (2012). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy for People Who Stutter. Perspectives on Fluency and Fluency Disorders, 22(1), 34. 10.1044/ffd22.1.34 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beilby JM, Byrnes ML, & Yaruss JS (2012). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy for adults who stutter: Psychosocial adjustment and speech fluency. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 37(4), 289–299. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2012.05.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop SR, Lau M, Shapiro S, Carlson L, Anderson ND, Carmody J, Segal ZV, Abbey S, Speca M, Velting D, & Devins G (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11(3), 230–241. 10.1093/clipsy/bph077 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Blood GW (1995). A behavioral-cognitive therapy program for adults who stutter: Computers and counseling. Journal of Communication Disorders, 28(2), 165–180. 10.1016/0021-9924(95)00008-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bloodstein O (1972). The anticipatory struggle hypothesis: Implications of research on the variability of stuttering. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 15(3), 487–499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bloodstein O (1975). Stuttering as tension and fragmentation. In Eisenson J (Ed.), Stuttering: a second symposium. Harper & Row. [Google Scholar]
- Bloodstein O, Ratner NB, & Brundage SB (2021). A Handbook on Stuttering (7th ed.). Plural Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Blumgart E, Tran Y, & Craig A (2010). An investigation into the personal financial costs associated with stuttering. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 35(3), 203–215. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2010.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyle MP (2011). Mindfulness training in stuttering therapy: A tutorial for speech-language pathologists. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 36(2), 122–129. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2011.04.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyle MP (2013). Assessment of stigma associated with stuttering: development and evaluation of the self-stigma of stuttering scale (4S). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(5), 1517–1529. 10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0280) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyle MP (2015). Identifying correlates of self-stigma in adults who stutter: Further establishing the construct validity of the self-stigma of stuttering scale (4S). Journal of Fluency Disorders, 43, 17–27. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2014.12.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyle MP (2018). Enacted stigma and felt stigma experienced by adults who stutter. Journal of Communication Disorders, 73(August 2017), 50–61. 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2018.03.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyle MP, & Blood GW (2015). Stigma and stuttering: conceptualizations, applications, and coping. In St. Louis KO (Ed.), Stuttering meets stereotype, stigma, and discrimination (pp. 43–70). West Virginia University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Boyle MP, & Fearon AN (2018). Self-stigma and its associations with stress, physical health, and health care satisfaction in adults who stutter. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 56(September 2017), 112–121. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2017.10.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyle MP, & Gabel R (2020). Toward a better understanding of the process of disclosure events among people who stutter. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 63(August 2019), 105746. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2020.105746 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyle MP, Milewski KM, & Beita-Ell C (2018). Disclosure of stuttering and quality of life in people who stutter. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 58(June), 1–10. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2018.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brocklehurst PH, Lickley RJ, & Corley M (2013). Revisiting Bloodstein’s Anticipatory Struggle Hypothesis from a psycholinguistic perspective: A Variable Release Threshold hypothesis of stuttering. Journal of Communication Disorders, 46(3), 217–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brundage SB, Bothe AK, Lengeling AN, & Evans JJ (2006). Comparing judgments of stuttering made by students, clinicians, and highly experienced judges. J Fluency Disord, 31(4), 271–283. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2006.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brundage SB, Winters KL, & Beilby JM (2017). Fear of negative evaluation, trait anxiety, and judgment bias in adults who stutter. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 26(2), 498–510. 10.1044/2017_AJSLP-16-0129 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brutten EJ, & Shoemaker DJ (1967). The modification of stuttering. Prentice-Hall. [Google Scholar]
- Byrd CT, Croft R, Gkalitsiou Z, & Hampton E (2017). Clinical utility of self-disclosure for adults who stutter: Apologetic versus informative statements. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 54, 1–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byrd CT, McGill M, Gkalitsiou Z, & Cappellini C (2017). The effects of self-disclosure on male and female perceptions of individuals who stutter. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 26(1), 69–80. 10.1044/2016_AJSLP-15-0164 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheasman C (2013). A mindful approach to stammering. In Cheasman C, Everard R, & Simpson S (Eds.), Stammering Therapy from the Inside (pp. 227–265). J & R Press. [Google Scholar]
- Constantino CD (2018). What Can Stutterers Learn from the Neurodiversity Movement? Seminars in Speech and Language, 39(4), 382–395. 10.1055/s-0038-1667166 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Constantino CD, Eichorn N, Buder EH, Beck JG, & Manning WH (2020). The Speaker ‘ s Experience of Stuttering: Measuring Spontaneity. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 1–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Constantino CD, Leslie P, Quesal R, & Yaruss JS (2016). A preliminary investigation of daily variability of stuttering in adults. Journal of Communication Disorders, 60, 39–50. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Constantino CD, Manning WH, & Nordstrom SN (2017). Rethinking covert stuttering. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 53, 26–40. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2017.06.00 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conture EG (1990). Childhood stuttering: What is it and who does it? In Cooper J (Ed.), Research Needs in Stuttering: Roadblocks and Future Directions: ASHA Reports. Number 18 (pp. 2–14). American Speech, Language, and Hearing Association. [Google Scholar]
- Cooper EB (1968). A Therapy Process for the Adult Stutterer. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 3. 10.1044/jshd.3303.246 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper EB (1977). Controversies about stuttering therapy. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 2, 75–86. 10.1016/0094-730X(77)90011-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Corcoran JA, & Stewart M (1998). Stories of stuttering: A qualitative analysis of interview narratives. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 23(4), 247–264. 10.1016/S0094-730X(98)00020-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cordes, & Ingham RJ (1994). The reliability of observational data: II. Issues in the identification and measurement of stuttering events. J Speech Hear Res, 37(2), 279–294. 10.1044/jshr.3702.279 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cordes, & Ingham RJ (1999). Effects of time-interval judgment training on real-time measurement of stuttering. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42(4), 862–879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig A, Hancock K, Tran Y, & Craig M (2003). Anxiety Levels in People who Stutter: A Randomized Population Study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 1197–1207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig, Blumgart E, & Tran Y (2009). The impact of stutteirng on the quality of life in adults who stutter. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 34(2), 21–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Croft RL, & Byrd CT (2021). Does the clinical utility of self-disclosure of stuttering transcend culturally and linguistically diverse populations? International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 23(5), 548–558. 10.1080/17549507.2020.1861326 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daniels DE, & Gabel RM (2004). The impact of stuttering on identity construction. Topics in Language Disorders, 24(3), 200–215. 10.1097/00011363-200407000-00007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Davis S, Howell P, & Cooke F (2002). Sociodynamic relationships between children who stutter and the non-stuttering classmates. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 43(7), 939–947. 10.1111/1469-7610.00093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dayalu VN, & Kalinowski J (2002). Pseudofluency in adults who stutter: The Illusory Outcome of Therapy. Perceptual And https://Advancement.Shu.Edu/Give/Give-Online Motor Skills, 84, 87–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Sonneville-Koedoot C, Stolk E, Rietveld T, & Franken M-C (2015). Direct versus Indirect Treatment for Preschool Children who Stutter: The RESTART Randomized Trial. Plos One, 10(7), e0133758. 10.1371/journal.pone.0133758 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dekeyser M, Raes F, Leijssen M, Leysen S, & Dewulf D (2008). Mindfulness skills and interpersonal behaviour. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(5), 1235–1245. 10.1016/j.paid.2007.11.018 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Douglass E, & Quarrington B (1952). The differentiation of interiorized and exteriorized secondary stuttering. J Speech Hear Disord, 17(4), 377–385. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/13023789 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Douglass JE, Constantino CD, Alvarado J, Verrastro K, & Smith K (2019). Qualitative investigation of the speech-language therapy experiences of individuals who covertly stutter. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 61(August), 105713. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2019.105713 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Douglass JE, Schwab M, & Alvarado J (2018). Stuttering to Overtly Stuttering. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 27(October), 1235–1243. 10.1044/2018_AJSLP-ODC11-17-0190 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunlap K (1932). Habits: Their Making and Unmaking. Liverright Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Ehring T, Zetsche U, Weidacker K, Wahl K, Schönfeld S, & Ehlers A (2011). The Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ): Validation of a content-independent measure of repetitive negative thinking. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 42(2), 225–232. 10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.12.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis A (1957). Rational psychotherapy and individual psychology. Journal of Individual Psychology, 13, 38–44. 10.1080/00221309.1958.9710170 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis A (1962). Reason and Emotion in Psychotherapy. Stuart. [Google Scholar]
- Emerick LL (1988). Counseling Adults who Stutter: A Cognitive Approach. Seminars in Speech and Language, 9(3), 257–267. 10.1055/s-0028-1082470 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Etchell AC, Civier O, Ballard KJ, & Sowman PF (2017). A systematic literature review of neuroimaging research on developmental stuttering between 1995 and 2016. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2017.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher L, & Hayes SC (2005). Relational frame theory, acceptance and commitment therapy, and a functional analytic definition of mindfulness. Journal of Rational Emotive and Cognitive Behavior Therapy, 23(4), 315–336. 10.1007/s10942-005-0017-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Foa EB, & Kozak MJ (1986). Emotional processing of fear. Psychological Bulletin, 99(1), 20–35. https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/bd73/152fde2070b539f2c15a0f754efa472ecbee.pdf [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabel RM, Blood GW, Tellis GM, & Althouse MT (2004). Measuring role entrapment of people who stutter. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 29(1), 27–49. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2003.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerlach H, Totty E, Subramanian A, & Zebrowski P (2018). Stuttering and Labor Market Outcomes in the United States. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61(7), 1649–1663. 10.1044/2018_jslhr-s-17-0353 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gregory HH, Campbell JH, Gregory CB, & Hill DG (2003). Stuttering therapy: Rationale and procedures. Pearson Education, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Gregory HH, & Hill D (1999). Differential Evaluation—Differential Therapy for Stuttering Children. In Curlee RF (Ed.), Stuttering and related disorders of fluency (2nd ed., pp. 22–42). Thieme. [Google Scholar]
- Guitar B (2019). Stuttering: An Integrated Approach to It’s Nature and Treatment (5th ed.). Wolters Kluwer. [Google Scholar]
- Gupta SK (2015). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in early adolescents who stutter. Delhi Psychiatry Journal, 18(2), 452–455. [Google Scholar]
- Gupta SK, Yashodhara GY, & Vasudha HH (2016). Cognitive behavior therapy and mindfulness training in the treatment of adults who stutter. The International Journal of Indian Psychology, 3(3), 78–87. https://doi.org/18.01.010/20160303 [Google Scholar]
- Hall D, Segers VD, & Conti D (1987). Inter-intrajude reliability of the stuttering severity instrument. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 12(167–173). [Google Scholar]
- Ham RE (1990). Therapy of stuttering: Preschool through adolescence. Prentice Hall. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC, Luoma JB, Bond FW, Masuda A, & Lillis J (2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(1), 1–25. 10.1016/j.brat.2005.06.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helgadóttir FD, Menzies RG, Onslow M, Packman A, & O’Brian S (2014). A standalone Internet cognitive behavior therapy treatment for social anxiety in adults who stutter: CBTpsych. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 41(C), 47–54. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2014.04.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ingham RJ (1984). The nonbehavioral preparadigm’s contribution to stuttering treatment. In Stuttering and behavior therapy: Current status and experimental foundations (pp. 47–67). College-Hill Press. [Google Scholar]
- Iverach L, Jones M, Lowe R, O’Brian S, Menzies RG, Packman A, & Onslow M (2018). Comparison of adults who stutter with and without social anxiety disorder. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 56(January), 55–68. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2018.03.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iverach L, Rapee RM, Wong QJJ, & Lowe R (2017). Maintenance of Social Anxiety in Stuttering : American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 26(May), 540–557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson ES (2015). Variability, stability, and flexibility in the speech kinematics and acoustics of adults who do and do not stutter. City University of New York. [Google Scholar]
- Jackson ES, Gerlach H, Rodgers NH, & Zebrowski PM (2018). My Client Knows That He’s about to Stutter: How Can We Address Stuttering Anticipation during Therapy with Young People Who Stutter? Seminars in Speech and Language, 39(4), 356–370. 10.1055/s-0038-1667164 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson ES, Quesal R, & Yaruss JS (2012). What is stuttering: Revisited. Paper Presented at the International Stuttering Awareness Day Online Conference, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Jackson ES, Yaruss JS, Quesal R, Terranova V, & Whalen DH (2015). Responses of adults who stutter to the anticipation of stuttering. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 1–25. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2015.05.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jha AP, Stanley EA, Kiyonaga A, Wong L, & Gelfand L (2010). Examining the Protective Effects of Mindfulness Training on Working Memory Capacity and Affective Experience. Emotion, 10(1), 54–64. 10.1037/a0018438 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson W (1959). The onset of stuttering: Research findings and implications. U of Minnesota Press. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson W (1961a). Measurements of oral reading and speaking rate and disfluency of adult male and female stutterers and nonstutterers. The Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, Monogram Supplement, 7, 1–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson W (1961b). Stuttering and what you can do about it. Minnesota Press. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson W (1961c). Stuttering and what you can do about it. The Interstate. [Google Scholar]
- Jones M, Onslow M, Packman A, Williams S, Ormond T, Schwarz I, & Gebski V (2005). Randomised controlled trial of the Lidcombe programme of early stuttering intervention. British Medical Journal, 331(7518), 659–661. 10.1136/bmj.38520.451840.E0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelman E, & Wheeler S (2015). Cognitive Behaviour Therapy with children who stutter. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 193, 165–174. 10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.03.256 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Klompas M, & Ross E (2004). Life experiences of people who stutter, and the perceived impact of stuttering on quality of life: Personal accounts of South African individuals. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 29(4), 275–305. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2004.10.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koedoot C, Bouwmans C, Franken MC, & Stolk E (2011). Quality of life in adults who stutter. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44(4), 429–443. 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2011.02.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolk HHJ (1990). Stuttering as Corrective Adaptation: Technical Report 90–06. The Nijmegen Institute for Cognition Research and Information Technology. [Google Scholar]
- Kolk HHJ (1991). Is stuttering a symptom of adaptation or of impairment. Speech Motor Control and Stuttering, 131–140. [Google Scholar]
- Kolk HHJ, & Heeschen C (1990). Adaptation symptoms and impairment symptoms in Broca’s aphasia. Aphasiology, 4(3), 221–231. [Google Scholar]
- Kraft SJ, & Yairi E (2012). Genetic bases of stuttering: The state of the art, 2011. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 64(1), 34–47. 10.1159/000331073 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kully D, & Boberg E (1988). An investigation of the inter-clinic agreement in the identification of fluent and stuttered syllables. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 13, 309–318. [Google Scholar]
- Manning WH, & Dilollo A (2018). Clinical decision making in fluency disorders (4th ed.). Plural Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Martin R, & Haroldson SK (1992). Stuttering and speech naturalness: audio and audiovisual judgments. J Speech Hear Res, 35(3), 521–528. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1376781 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McAllister J, Collier J, & Shepstone L (2012). The impact of adolescent stuttering on educational and employment outcomes: Evidence from a birth cohort study. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 37(2), 106–121. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2012.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGill M, Siegel J, Nguyen D, & Rodriguez S (2018). Self-report of self-disclosure statements for stuttering. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 58, 22–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menzies RG, O’Brian S, Onslow M, & Packman A (2008). An Experimental Clinical Trial of a Cognitive-Behavior Therapy Package for Chronic Stuttering. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 51(6), 1451–1464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menzies RG, Onslow M, Packman A, & O’Brian S (2009). Cognitive behavior therapy for adults who stutter: A tutorial for speech-language pathologists. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 34(3), 187–200. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2009.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Messenger M, Onslow M, Packman A, & Menzies R (2004). Social anxiety in stuttering: Measuring negative social expectancies. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 29(3), 201–212. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2004.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy WP (1999). A preliminary look at shame, guilt, and stuttering. In Bernstein-Ratner N & Healy C (Eds.), Stuttering research and practice: Bridging the gap (pp. 131–143). Lawrence Erlbaum Associate. [Google Scholar]
- Murphy WP, Quesal R, & Gulker H (2007). Covert Stuttering. Perspectives on Fluency and Fluency Disorders, 17, 4–9. [Google Scholar]
- Murphy WP, Yaruss JS, & Quesal RW (2007). Enhancing treatment for school-age children who stutter. II. Reducing bullying through role-playing and self-disclosure. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 32(2), 139–162. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2007.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neumann K, Euler HA, Bosshardt HG, Cook S, Sandrieser P, & Sommer M (2017). Clinical practice guideline: The pathogenesis, assessment and treatment of speech fluency disorders. Deutsches Arzteblatt International, 114(22–23), 383–390. 10.3238/arztebl.2017.0383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicholas A (2015). Solution focused brief therapy with children who stutter. Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences, 193, 209–216. 10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.03.261 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Nippold MA (2012). When a School-Age Child Stutters, Let’s Focus on the Primary Problem. Speech, Language, and Hearing Services in Schools, 43(4), 549–551. 10.1044/0161-1461(2012/12-0054) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nolen-Hoeksema S (1991). Responses to Depression and Their Effects on the Duration of Depressive Episodes. J Abnorm Psychol, 100(4), 569–582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nolen-Hoeksema S (2004). The Response Styles Theory. In Papageorgiou C & Wells A (Eds.), Depressive Rumination NATURE, THEORY AND TREATMENT (pp. 107–123). John Wiley & Sons Ltd. [Google Scholar]
- Nolen-Hoeksema S, Wisco B, & Lyubomirsky S (2008). Rethinking Rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 400–424. 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palasik S, & Hannan J (2013). The Clinical Applications of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy With Clients Who Stutter. Perspectives on Fluency & Fluency Disorders, 23(2), 54–69. http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=jlh&AN=2012510181&site=ehost-live [Google Scholar]
- Perkins WH (1983). The problem of definition: Commentary on” stuttering.”. 10.1044/jshd.4803.246b [DOI]
- Perkins WH (1984). Stuttering as a Categorical EventBarking up the Wrong Tree—Reply to Wingate. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 49(4), 431–434. 10.1044/jshd.4904.431 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Perkins WH (1990). What is stuttering? Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 370–382. 10.1044/jshd.5503.370 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quesal R (1989). Stuttering research: Have we forgotten the stutterer? Journal of Fluency Disorders, 14(3), 153–164. 10.1016/0094-730X(89)90033-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rapee RM, Wignall A, Psych M, Hudson JL, & Schniering CA (2000). Treating anxious children and adolescents: an evidence-based approach. New Harbinger Publications, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Reardon-Reeves N, & Yaruss JS (2013). School-Age Stuttering Therapy: A Practical Guide. Stuttering Therapy Resources, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Reitzes P, & Reitzes D (2012). Stuttering: Inspiring Stories and Professional Wisdom. StutterTalk, Inc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shapiro D. (1999). Stuttering Intervention: A Collaborative Journey to Fluency. Pro-Ed. [Google Scholar]
- Sheehan JG (1953). Theory and treatment of stuttering as an approach-avoidance conflict. The Journal of Psychology, 36(1), 27–49. 10.1080/00223980.1953.9712875 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sheehan JG (1970). Stuttering: Research and therapy.
- Sheehan JG, & Sheehan V (1984). Avoidance-reduction therapy: A response suppression hypothesis. In Perkins WH (Ed.), Stuttering Disorders. Thieme-Stratton. [Google Scholar]
- Sheehan JG, & Voas RB (1957). Stuttering as conflict: 1. Comparison of therapy techniques involving approach and avoidance. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 22(5), 714–723. 10.1044/jshd.2205.714 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snidecor J (1955). Tension and facial appearance in stuttering. In Johnson W (Ed.), Stuttering in children and adults (p. 377). University of Minnesota Press. [Google Scholar]
- Snyder G, Manahan A, McKnight P, & Kornisch M (2021). The Effects of Written Stuttering Disclosure on the Perceptions of a Child Who Stutters. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 52(4), 1031–1048. 10.1044/2021_lshss-20-00119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- St. Louis KO (2020). Comparing and predicting public attitudes toward stuttering, obesity, and mental illness. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 29(4), 2023–2038. 10.1044/2020_AJSLP-20-00038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- St. Louis KO, Sønsterud H, Junuzović-Žunić L, Tomaiuoli D, Del Gado F, Caparelli E, Theiling M, Flobakk C, Helmen LN, Heitmann RR, Kvenseth H, Nilsson S, Wetterling T, Lundström C, Daly C, Leahy M, Tyrrell L, Ward D, & Wesierska M (2016). Public attitudes toward stuttering in Europe: Within-country and between-country comparisons. Journal of Communication Disorders, 62, 115–130. 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2016.05.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- St Clare T, Menzies RG, Onslow M, Packman A, Thompson R, & Block S (2009). Unhelpful thoughts and beliefs linked to social anxiety in stuttering development of a measure. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 44(3), 338–351. 10.1080/13682820802067529 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Starkweather CW, & Givens-Ackerman J (1997). Stuttering. Pro-ed. [Google Scholar]
- Tatchell RH, van den Berg S, & Lerman JW (1983). Fluency and eye contact as factors influencing observers’ perceptions of stutterers. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 8(3), 221–231. 10.1016/0094-730X(83)90036-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Teesson K, Packman A, & Onslow M (2003). The Lidcombe behavioral data language of stuttering. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46(4), 1009–1015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tichenor SE, Leslie P, Shaiman S, & Yaruss JS (2017). Speaker and Observer Perceptions of Physical Tension During Stuttering. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica : Official Organ of the International Association of Logopedics and Phoniatrics (IALP), 69(4), 180–189. 10.1159/000486032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tichenor SE, & Yaruss JS (2018). A Phenomenological Analysis of the Experience of Stuttering. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 27, 1180–1194. 10.1044/2018_AJSLP-ODC11-17-0192 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tichenor SE, & Yaruss JS (2019a). Group Experiences and Individual Differences in Stuttering. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 62, 4335–4350. 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-19-00138 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tichenor SE, & Yaruss JS (2019b). Stuttering as defined by adults who stutter. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62(12). 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-19-00137 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tichenor SE, & Yaruss JS (2020a). Repetitive negative thinking, temperament, and adverse impact in adults who stutter. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 29(1), 201–215. 10.1044/2019_AJSLP-19-00077 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tichenor SE, & Yaruss JS (2020b). Variability of stuttering: Behavior and impact. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 30(1), 75–88. 10.1044/2020_AJSLP-20-00112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tichenor SE, Constantino C, Yaruss JS (in press). A Point of View about Fluency. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Borsel J, Brepoels M, & De Coene J (2011). Stuttering, attractiveness and romantic relationships: The perception of adolescents and young adults. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 36(1), 41–50. 10.1016/j.jfludis.2011.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Riper C (1973). The treatment of stuttering. Prentice-Hall. [Google Scholar]
- Van Riper C (1982). The nature of stuttering. Prentice Hall. [Google Scholar]
- Watkins ER (2008). Constructive and Unconstructive Repetitive Thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 163–206. 10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wells A, & Papageorgiou C (2004). Metacognitive Therapy for Depressive Rumination. In Papageorgiou C & Wells A (Eds.), Depressive Rumination NATURE, THEORY AND TREATMENT (pp. 259–273). Wiley & Sons Ltd. [Google Scholar]
- WHO. (2001). International classification of functioning, disabilities, and health. World Health Organization. [Google Scholar]
- Wingate ME (1964). A Standard Definition of Stuttering. J Speech Hear Disord, 29, 484–489. 10.1044/jshd.2904.484 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wingate ME (2001). SLD is not stuttering. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 44(2), 381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolpe J (1958). No Psychotherapy by Reciprocal Inhibition. Stanford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Wrosch C, & Scheier MF (2003). Personality and quality of life: The importance of optimism and goal adjustment. Quality of Life Research, 12(SUPPL. 1), 59–72. 10.1023/A:1023529606137 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yairi E (1996). Applications of disfluencies in measurements of stuttering. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 39(2), 402–404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yairi E (2001). What is Stuttering. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(June 2001), 585–597. 10.1044/1092-4388(2001/046) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yairi E, & Ambrose N (2005). Early Childhood Stuttering For Clinicians By Clinicians. Pro-ed. [Google Scholar]
- Yaruss JS (1998). Describing the consequences of disorders: stuttering and the International Classification of Impairments, Disabilities, and Handicaps. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 41(2), 249–257. 10.1044/jslhr.4102.249 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yaruss JS (2007). Application of the ICF in fluency disorders. Seminars in Speech and Language, 28(4), 312–322. 10.1055/s-2007-986528 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yaruss JS, Coleman CE, & Quesal R (2012). Stuttering in School-Age Children: A Comprehensive Approach to Treatment. Language, Speech & Hearing Services in Schools, 43(4), 536–548. 10.1044/0161-1461(2012/11-0044)b [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yaruss JS, & Quesal R (2004). Stuttering and the international clasification of functioning, disability, and health (ICF): an update. Journal of Communication Disorders, 37, 35–52. 10.1016/S0021-9924(03)00052-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yaruss JS, & Reardon NA (2003). Fostering generalization and maintenance in school settings. Seminars in Speech and Language, 24(1), 33–40. 10.1055/s-2003-37385 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yaruss JS, Reeves N, & Herring C (2018). How Speech-Language Pathologists Can Minimize Bullying of Children Who Stutter. Seminars in Speech and Language, 39(4), 342–355. 10.1055/s-0038-1667163 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]