Abstract
Social learning is fundamental to human development, helping individuals adapt to changing circumstances and cooperate in groups. During the formative years of adolescence, the social environment shapes people's socio-cognitive skills needed in adulthood. Although peer influence among adolescents is traditionally associated with risky and unruly conduct, with long-term negative effects on educational, economic and health outcomes, recent findings suggest that peers may also have a positive impact. Here, we present a series of experiments with 10–20-year-olds (n = 146) showing that positive and negative peer effects reflect a domain-general factor of social information use which declines during adolescence. Exposure to disobedient peers provoked rule breaking, and selfish peers reduced prosocial behaviour, particularly in early adolescence. However, compliant peers also promoted rule compliance and fair peers increased prosociality. A belief formation task further revealed that younger adolescents tend to assimilate social information, while older adolescents prioritize personal views. Our results highlight early adolescence as a key window for peer-based interventions to improve developmental trajectories.
Keywords: social learning, social influence, cooperation, belief updating, human development
1. Introduction
Learning to adequately respond to social information is a central developmental goal, essential for finding one's place in society as an independent and socially responsible adult. Observing the behaviour of others enables people to navigate the diverse interactions they face every day, helping them make socially appropriate decisions and forming accurate beliefs about the world. As children enter adolescence and prepare for adult roles in society, peers become increasingly important sources of social information. Social influence among adolescents is often associated with increased risk taking and negative outcomes (e.g. crime, alcohol abuse, taking drugs and having unprotected sex [1–11]). In the long term, running with the wrong crowd can consequently set people on negative developmental trajectories, with grave implications for health, education, social and economic success, and general well-being [12–14].
Recent research has started to explore the potential of peers to have positive effects on adolescent behaviour, indicating that peers may also reduce risk taking and promote prosociality [15–21]. This suggests that there may be a domain-general mechanism that drives social influence in different decision contexts. However, empirical evidence for this idea is limited because studies of peer influence in adolescence typically focuses on a single behavioural domain at a time, and each study involves its own particular age bracket. As a result, it remains unclear whether positive and negative peer influence on adolescents' behaviour follow the same or different developmental pathways. Furthermore, the research emphasis on risk taking—often measured with hypothetical scenarios and self-reports rather than incentivized behavioural tasks—has led to a negligence of social influence on adolescents’ behaviour in other important domains, such as rule compliance and belief formation. Understanding the development of the behavioural mechanisms of social influence in these domains, and how they relate to each other, is essential for facilitating adolescents' attunement to their social environment, reducing anti-social behaviour and promoting socially desirable outcomes [13].
In this paper, we present evidence from a set of incentivized behavioural experiments with 146 adolescents (ages 10–20, 45% female) targeting three core domains of decision-making: rule compliance, belief formation and prosociality. Our first experiment addresses social influence on rule compliance. The smooth functioning of human societies critically depends on individuals complying with rules that facilitate social coordination and curb the spreading of disorder [22,23]. Developmental studies on risk taking have provided valuable insights in the role of peer influence when adolescents face uncertainty [6,8,21,24–27], but they provide little behavioural insight into rule violations, one of the hallmarks of adolescent behaviour [28]. Our second experiment addresses social influence on belief formation. With the advent of social media, adolescents are continually exposed to social information, which helps acquiring useful knowledge [29] and make accurate decisions [30,31], but can also misinform [32] and fuel group polarization [33]. Yet, although some research has studied belief formation across adolescence [34], the role of social information in this process remains unclear. Our third experiment examines social influence on prosociality, a key building block of cooperation between individuals in society [35]. This experiment allows us to examine the robustness of recent results showing peer influence on adolescents' willingness to help others [20], and further investigate how this relates to individuals’ social information use in other decision contexts. Across all experiments, rather than relying on hypothetical scenarios or self-report, behaviour was incentivized, providing a proper test of peer influence because changing behaviour upon observing peers has real consequences [36].
2. Methods
We recruited 146 participants aged 10–20 years from the area of Berlin, Germany, with a balanced distribution across these ages and gender (table 1). We implemented a within-subjects design. Participants completed a series of computerized tasks and questionnaires on their own, in a separate room. All tasks were programmed in LIONESS Lab [37] and are available in editable form via lioness-lab.org. The tasks were administered in the participants' native language (German). At the beginning of each session, participants received general instructions; always from the same test leader, who remained available to answer questions throughout a session, but was seated in a separate room during the session.
Table 1.
age | total | male | female | figure 1 |
figure 2 | figure 3 |
figure 4 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
rule following |
belief formation | prosociality |
factor S | ||||||
bad example | good example | selfish example | fair example | ||||||
10 | 16 | 10 | 6 | 9 | 7 | 16 | 15 | 8 | 15 |
11 | 21 | 7 | 14 | 10 | 11 | 21 | 15 | 12 | 19 |
12 | 18 | 10 | 8 | 10 | 8 | 18 | 13 | 12 | 17 |
13 | 9 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 3 | 9 | 5 | 5 | 9 |
14 | 13 | 10 | 3 | 7 | 6 | 13 | 8 | 8 | 11 |
15 | 13 | 9 | 4 | 8 | 5 | 13 | 7 | 11 | 12 |
16 | 13 | 7 | 6 | 7 | 6 | 13 | 5 | 10 | 12 |
17 | 12 | 7 | 5 | 7 | 5 | 12 | 8 | 7 | 12 |
18 | 9 | 5 | 4 | 4 | 5 | 9 | 5 | 8 | 9 |
19 | 12 | 7 | 5 | 3 | 9 | 12 | 11 | 7 | 12 |
20 | 10 | 5 | 5 | 6 | 4 | 10 | 4 | 7 | 10 |
sum | 146 | 81 | 65 | 77 | 69 | 146 | 96 | 95 | 138 |
Tasks were administered in two approximately 60 min sessions, which took place approximately four weeks apart to avoid carry-over effects between sessions with similar tasks (i.e. the two treatments of the task measuring prosociality; see §2c below). To reduce carry-over effects between experiments in a session, task order was randomized within and between sessions, independently between participants. Task-specific instructions were provided on-screen (see section ‘Experimental Procedures and Materials’ in the electronic supplementary material for full experimental materials, translated from German to English).
Tasks were incentivized such that each decision was payoff-relevant, and expected hourly wages were constant across tasks. Instructions for each task concluded with compulsory comprehension questions. After completing the second session, participants received payment in cash (flat fee of €30 plus a performance-specific bonus between €5 and €16). In the instructions of all tasks, peers were introduced as other participants who previously completed the tasks. The information letter for participants pointed out that, in accordance with the rules of the local laboratory, no deception was used in our experiments. To gauge whether participants believed that the peer behaviour they observed was real, we asked participants during debriefing about their general impression of their peers in the experiment, both in writing ('What was your general impression of the previous participants?') and orally by the test leader. We did not find any indication to suggest that participants did not believe the instructions or the veracity of the social information presented to them.
(a) . Rule compliance
To measure social influence in rule compliance, we administered a stylized, animated task (adapted from [38]). Participants could choose whether or not to comply with a rule, in the absence and presence of peers. They had to move a circle figure across their screen by clicking a button. In the middle of the screen stood a red traffic light (figure 1). The instructions stated that ‘the rule is to wait until the traffic light turns green’. However, waiting was costly: participants started with an endowment of 20 points (total worth: €1.00), and each second they spent in the task, this endowment was reduced with 1 point until it reached zero. After 12 s, the traffic light turned green. Instructions and control questions made it clear that the rule was not enforced (and that there were no risks of negative consequences for violating the rule), so compliance was voluntary and against one's material self-interest.
Participants completed three iterations of the task, one of which was randomly chosen for payment after the task had completed. In the first iteration, participants completed the task alone. Based on their initial decision, we allocated them to either of two conditions that differed in the displayed movements of three peers who completed the task before. In particular, these peers always chose the opposite of a participant's own initial choice: participants who initially complied with the rule (i.e. moved after the traffic light turned green) were shown three ‘bad’ examples, peers who moved as soon as possible. Participants who initially violated the rule (i.e. moved before the traffic light turned green) were shown three ‘good examples’, peers who waited until the traffic light turned green. This set-up allows us to study age trends in switching in both conditions, but the endogenous treatment allocation limits our ability to directly compare rates of switching across conditions due to selection effects (for example, initial compliance might be correlated with choice consistency). Table 1 details the number of participants allocated to either of these endogenous treatments.
For both conditions, we measured the proportion of participants following social information, switching their behaviour from compliance to violation (or vice versa). An animated gif of the task (German original) without and with peers can be viewed here and here, respectively. The quantity of interest—measuring social influence—was whether or not a participant switched their behaviour upon observing peers choosing the opposite from their own. To test whether social influence effects would spill over into later behaviour, the task concluded with a third iteration in which participants completed the task on their own again.
(b) . Belief formation
To measure how peers impact belief formation, we administered the BEAST (figure 2), a validated task reliably measuring individuals' propensities to integrate social information in perceptual decisions [39], which has been used in adolescent samples [40,41]. The task involves judgements in a context where social values or preferences are irrelevant. For five rounds, participants observed an image with 50–60 animals for 6 s and had to estimate how many there were. After entering their first estimate (E1), participants observed social information (X) in the form of the estimate from another participant who observed the same image. Then, they made a second estimate (E2).
Social information was selected from a pre-recorded pool of 100 previous participants. We selected an estimate at intermediate distance from a participant's first estimate. In rounds 1–5 of the task, the target deviations of X from E1 were 25%, 15%, 20%, 15% and 25%, respectively, and we selected a pre-recorded estimate closest to that target [39]. This allowed for a varying yet relatively constant room for adjustment, without the social information being too far away to be ignored altogether [42]. Social information always pointed in the direction of the true value (T), and could therefore be lower or higher than E1, depending on whether the participant initially over- or underestimated the number of animals. The pre-defined target deviations imply that social information could be farther away from the true value when a participant's first estimate was close to the true value; in those cases, E1 and X bracketed T. Finally, when E1 was exactly correct, we randomly determined whether the targeted social information was lower or higher than E1. This approach using controlled social information is designed to measure individuals' propensity for social information use [39], but is less suitable for studying how social information may impact decision accuracy [43].
A participant's social information use (s) in a round is calculated as their relative estimate adjustment towards social information: s = (E2 − E1)/(X − E1). Rearranging the terms makes clear that E2 is an average of E1 and X, weighted by s: E2 = (1 − s) × E1 + s × X. In other words, s indicates the relative weight assigned to social information. We characterized a participant's social information use as their adjustment s averaged across the five rounds of the task. In our analyses, we focus on cases 0 ≤ s ≤ 1, that is, where second estimates were a weighted average of E1 and X [39]. Cases outside this range were removed from further analysis.
Participants were rewarded for accuracy. If an estimate was exactly correct, earnings were 100 points. For each animal the participant was off, we subtracted five points (in this task, 100 points equalled €0.30). This set-up ensures that participants should only use social information to adjust their first estimate if they believed it increased accuracy [39]. To avoid that participants would learn about their own performance or the usefulness of social information, they did not receive any feedback about their performance during the task.
(c) . Prosociality
To measure peer effects in prosociality, we administered an adapted version of the well-studied dictator game (figure 3) [44]. Participants received an endowment of 10 points (worth €1.00) to divide between themselves and another person, who participated in a previous session. Only whole numbers were allowed, and the allocations needed to sum up to 10 for a participant to proceed. The decision situation minimizes the scope for strategic behaviour (e.g. due to reciprocity), so that any positive donation can be interpreted as a prosocial choice. Because of its clear and simple set-up, the task is suitable to study prosociality in developmental samples [45–47].
In the task, participants make two decisions. First, participants made a donation decision alone (D1; cf. figure 3a). Participants were then matched with another previous participant, and made a second decision (D2; cf. figure 3b). Before they made this second decision, they observed social information (Ds) showing how another (third) previous participant (the ‘peer’) divided their 10 points. Then, they made a second donation decision (D2). We quantify social information use as (i) whether the participant moved towards the observed social information and (ii) the relative extent of adjustment towards social information calculated as (D2 − D1)/(DS − D1).
In a within-subject treatment manipulation—administered in two different sessions—we varied the value of DS. In the ‘selfish peer’ condition, we showed as social information the donation of a previous participant who chose to keep all 10 points to themselves (i.e. DS = 0). In the ‘fair peer’ condition, we showed a previous participant who split the points equally (i.e. DS = 5).
In the associated analyses, we only included participants whose initial decision was not identical to social information (so that any adjustments towards social information were actually possible) or moved away from social information. That is, in figure 3c, we included participants who initially donated more than 0 (66%; 96 participants); and in figure 3d, we included participants who initially donated less than 5 (65%; 95 participants; for full details about numbers of participants in the analyses in each of the experiments, table 1).
(d) . Domain generality
Our within-subject implementation of the three tasks allowed us to test the domain generality of social information use. We conducted a confirmatory factor analysis on three variables characterizing participants' social information use in each of the three tasks, using the ‘lavaan’ package [48]. For calculating a factor characterizing social information use across domains for each participant, we required a measure of social information use in each of the three tasks (rule following: did or did not follow the examples; belief formation: average adjustment towards social information in the BEAST; and prosociality: an initial donation different from the social information in at least one of the conditions).
In the rule compliance task, we used a binary variable indicating whether or not a participant followed the examples (0 = stuck with initial behaviour; 1 = conformed to opposite social information). For the belief formation task (BEAST), we used the mean relative adjustment as outlined above. For the dictator game, we exploited all variation present in our data by calculating for both conditions the participants' relative adjustment towards social information (as for the BEAST; see above); if for a participant both were available (which was the case when in both conditions, initial donations were different from social information; see section 2c), we averaged the two.
Figure 4 summarizes the standardized solution of the latent variable model. The reported χ2 assesses the overall model fit and the discrepancy between the sample and fitted covariance matrices. The associated p-value of 0.500 indicates that we cannot reject the null hypothesis that the model predictions and the data are equal. The standardized root mean square residual (SRMSR) quantifies the difference between the residuals of the sample covariance matrix and the hypothesized model.
(e) . Statistical analyses
All statistical analyses were conducted in R v. 4.0.2 [49]. In figures 1–4, dot opacity reflects the number of participants underlying each dot; the numbers of participants in each age bin (i.e. each dot) for each experimental condition are given in table 1. Lines and shades in figures 1–3 show predictions of regressions fitted to participants’ behaviour, using ‘age’ as the sole predictor (see electronic supplementary material, tables S1–S3 for full models). Dependent variables were decisions to follow social information (figure 1d,e; binomial model), mean adjustment towards social information (figure 2d; linear model), decisions to move towards social information (figure 3c,d; binomial model), and the factor S (figure 4; linear model). Due to the novelty of some of the tasks, it was not possible to produce a proper a priori power analysis. Our sample size was therefore based on reported sample sizes in recent experimental work on social influence in adolescents [21,27,50,51].
3. Results
(a) . Rule compliance
Figure 1 outlines the traffic light task and its main results. When participants first completed the task on their own, 53% (77 out of 146) complied with the rule, foregoing about €0.50 in possible earnings. This compliance rate is comparable to previous findings with this paradigm from a large adult sample (58%) [38]. We observe no age trends in this initial behaviour (electronic supplementary material, figure S1; GLM: β = −0.032, 95% CI [−0.135, 0.072], p = 0.550; electronic supplementary material, table S1, model 1).
Peer effects in rule compliance markedly declined with age (figure 1d,e; GLM: β = −0.232, 95% CI [−0.354, −0.110], p < 0.001; electronic supplementary material, table S1, model 2). Overall, when observing three bad examples, 61% of participants violated the rule after having complied first (figure 1d). A substantially smaller portion (29%) of participants who initially violated the rule chose to comply upon observing three good examples (figure 1e; GLM: β = 1.379, 95% CI [0.629, 2.130], p < 0.001; electronic supplementary material, table S1, model 2). We did not find a significant interaction effect between age and condition (GLM: β =−0.137, 95% CI [−0.381, 0.108], p = 0.275; electronic supplementary material, table S1, model 3).
Fifty-five per cent (37 out of 67) of participants who followed the peers stuck with their updated behaviour in a third iteration of the task in which they completed the task alone again, suggesting that peers had some lasting effect on compliance. Sticking with updated behaviour did not appear to depend on participants' age or peer behaviour (GLM: β = −0.146, 95% CI [−0.320, 0.027], p = 0.099; electronic supplementary material, table S1, model 4). These results suggest that adolescents are not particularly unruly per se and good examples can promote compliance, but it also appears that early adolescents are easily swayed by their peers to change their behaviour and to break a rule.
(b) . Belief formation
Figure 2 summarizes the perceptual judgement task and its main results. In the overwhelming majority of cases (94%), revised estimates were weighted averages of initial estimates and social information (electronic supplementary material, figure S2). On average, revised estimates were closer to the true value than initial estimates (paired t-test: t = 17.06; d.f. = 729, p < 0.001), which is not surprising given that social information always pointed in the right direction.
Again, social information use substantially decreased across adolescence (figure 2d; LMM: β = −0.021, 95% CI [−0.032, −0.011], p < 0.001; electronic supplementary material table S2). Ten-year-olds on average assigned about equal weight to their own first estimate and social information (y ≈ 0.5 in figure 2d). Twenty-year-olds on average assigned four times less weight to social information than to own estimates (y ≈ 0.2). These results reveal how ‘egocentric discounting’, a commonly observed phenomenon in social information use in adults [39,42,52,53], emerges during adolescence.
(c) . Prosociality
Figure 3 shows the decision situation in the dictator game and summarizes its main results. Participants first completed the task on their own. Mean initial donations across both sessions were 2.7 (s.d. = 2.3), and did not significantly differ across age (in line with [54–56]; LMM: β = −0.069, 95% CI [−0.164, 0.025], p = 0.150; electronic supplementary material, table S3, model 1).
Peer effects on donations were substantial (figure 3c,d; electronic supplementary material, figure S3). After observing a selfish example, 46% of participants who initially donated more than 0 adjusted their donation downwards, reducing average donations by 0.89 (95% CI [−1.19, −0.59]). After observing a fair example, 50% of participants who initially donated less than 5 adjusted their donation upwards, increasing average donations by 0.95 (95% CI [0.64, 1.25]; paired t-tests comparing initial and revised donations, selfish example: t = −5.816, d.f. = 145, p < 0.001; fair example: t = 5.988, d.f. = 145, p < 0.001).
Peer effects in prosociality strongly decreased with age: older adolescents were less likely to adjust their donations towards those of the peer (figure 3c,d; logistic GLMM: β =−0.269, 95% CI [−0.376, −0.161], p < 0.001; electronic supplementary material, table S3, model 2). The magnitude of donation adjustments also decreased with age (logistic GLMM: β = −0.136, 95% CI [−0.203, −0.070], p < 0.001; electronic supplementary material, table S3, model 3), and did not differ between conditions (same model: β = 0.001, 95% CI [−0.361, 0.364], p = 0.995; electronic supplementary material, table S3, model 3). These results indicate that both selfish and fair examples substantially modulate altruistic giving, and that their impact on behaviour steadily decreases during adolescence.
(d) . Domain generality
In each of our tasks, we observe a downward trend in social information use across age. This suggests that there may be a common mechanism driving peer effects across domains. Our experiments allow us to test that idea. Indeed, tendencies to use social information were positively correlated across the three experiments (electronic supplementary material, table S4). To further examine the underlying structure of the data shown in figures 1–3, we conducted a confirmatory factor analysis. This analysis supports the idea that the observed peer effects in each behavioural domain reflect a single underlying factor of social information use (figure 4; electronic supplementary material, table S5). This factor—which we dub S—steadily declines during adolescence (figure 4; effect of age: β = −0.713, 95% CI [−0.922, −0.503], p < 0.001; electronic supplementary material, table S5).
4. Discussion
Our paper can be summarized in three main points. First, we present an integrated set of incentivized experiments showing how social environments can negatively and positively impact adolescents' behaviour across different key domains of behaviour, including rule compliance and belief formation, which have hitherto been understudied in adolescence. In rule compliance, the effects of bad examples induce much disobedience, but good examples also substantially increase rule compliance. Peers also have a pronounced effect on belief formation in a setting where social values and preferences are irrelevant. Prosocial behaviour is similarly increased or decreased by exposure to good (fair-sharing) or bad (selfish) examples. Second, we show that across each of these domains, the potential of peers to impact behaviour steadily decreases throughout adolescence. Third, our results suggest that the decreasing impact of peers across adolescence reflects a developmental decline in a domain-general factor of social information use.
Our three experiments consistently show that social information use declines across adolescence (figures 1–4). Young adolescents have a high sensitivity to peer behaviour, while older adolescents prioritized personal preferences and beliefs (reinforcing results of [19,20,34,57]). In the context of rule compliance and prosociality, the marked impact of social information among early adolescents might reflect high levels of uncertainty about their personal values [8], that is, they might have a less clear notion of what is the right thing to do. Older adolescents might be less uncertain of these values, and reduced uncertainty might weaken the impact of social information on behaviour [58,59]. This uncertainty about values thus makes adolescents more sensitive to social information, and makes it easier for new social values to emerge during this developmental phase. As such, adolescents may become a driving force in the evolution of our cultural values (e.g. norms around gun ownership, or environmental issues).
However, the results of our belief formation task (figure 2) indicate that uncertainty about values cannot be the whole story. In this task, values and preferences were irrelevant but resulted in the same developmental pattern of diminishing use of social information across adolescence. Our results suggest the existence of a domain-general factor of social information use S, which is very high in early adolescence and decreases as people approach adulthood. This finding provides support for the implicit assumption in previous research that developmental trends in social information use would generalize across different domains of behaviour, and would shape behaviour in positive and negative directions in similar ways [12,60]. Future experimental and field studies could test the reliability and robustness of S beyond the three domains studied in this paper, examine its links with related constructs, such as social competence and social attention [61–63], and investigate contextual effects (e.g. of being observed by others [64], a factor that can modulate peer influence in adolescents [5,59,65]). Such studies would help strengthen the empirical foundation of this factor, delineate its psychometric structure and its significance beyond laboratory settings [66]. We believe such research would contribute to a comprehensive and more mechanistic understanding of social influence across adolescence.
One might suspect that the reported developmental trends in susceptibility for social influence (S) are due to decreasing stochasticity in decision making with age [51], that is, younger participants might behave more randomly. We believe that stochasticity does not play an important role in the reported age effects. In our rule-following task, younger participants were not more likely to switch back after updating their behaviour (electronic supplementary material, table S1, model 4). Moreover, increased stochasticity in the belief formation task would not lead to more adjustments towards social information, but to more adjustments in any direction. Similarly, in the prosociality task, randomness would not systematically bias adjustments in the direction of social information. This suggests that the decreasing trend in susceptibility for social influence (S) cannot be explained by decreased stochasticity.
Our experiments tightly controlled the provision of social information, allowing us to quantify how good and bad examples impacted behaviour, while avoiding confounding effects of individuals influencing each other, and selecting certain people as social sources [67]. In real life, however, the social information that might influence individuals depends on their position in their social network, and individuals’ social learning strategies (e.g. to preferentially heed familiar individuals, experts or prestigious sources [68–72]). This introduces asymmetries in the social environments that individuals observe, determining, for example, whether rule compliance and generosity are perceived to be common, and whether rule violations and selfishness are typically disapproved of [38,71,73]. Exploring the interplay between behaviour, social learning, and network formation is key for understanding how peers can promote and consolidate positive behaviour, and why people end up running in the wrong crowd in which socially undesirable conduct is exacerbated.
As their privileges grow, adolescents have to figure out how to relate to myriad novel social situations across many different domains of behaviour. Our results show that across domains, the behaviour of early adolescents is strongly guided by the social environment. They suggest that peer influence is a double-edged sword: it can provoke rule violations and reduce prosociality, but can also prompt rule compliance, promote prosociality, and facilitate the integration of social information to form beliefs about the world. These insights can inform the timing and design of interventions emphasizing positive examples in adolescents' social networks [74]. While many interventions are based on building individuals’ resistance to peer influence [57,75], our study highlights the potential of peers to spread prosocial norms. Both offline and on social media platforms, enhancing the salience of good examples and well-informed peers may bias norm perceptions in socially desirable ways and boost the formation of accurate beliefs. This way, social influence among adolescents can be harnessed to positively impact developmental trajectories, reducing anti-social behaviour and false beliefs, while facilitating individuals' attunement to their social environment and promoting socially desirable outcomes.
Acknowledgements
We thank Anna van Duijvenvoorde and Ralf Kurvers for helpful comments and discussions. We thank Jann Wäscher and Chantal Wysocki for data collection.
Ethics
The research has been performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical approval of the study protocol was obtained from the Institutional Review Board of the Max Planck Institute for Human Development Berlin (A 2018/23). Before starting the study, we obtained informed consent from all participants. For participants under 18, we additionally obtained informed consent from their parents or legal guardians.
Data accessibility
All data and code associated with this work are available from the public repository, accessible via https://github.com/LucasMolleman/Social_influence_in_adolescence. Experimental code is available (in editable form) on the LIONESS Lab platform (https://lioness-lab.org).
Electronic supplementary material is available online [76].
Authors' contributions
L.M.: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, project administration, resources, software, supervision, validation, visualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; S.C.: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, project administration, writing—original draft; W.v.d.B.: conceptualization, formal analysis, funding acquisition, investigation, methodology, supervision, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing.
All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.
Conflict of interest declaration
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
L.M. and W.v.d.B. are supported by an Open Research Area grant no. (ID 176). L.M. is further supported by an Amsterdam Brain and Cognition Project grant no. 2018. W.v.d.B. is further supported by the Jacobs Foundation European Research Council grant no. (ERC-2018-StG-803338) and the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research grant no. (NWO-VIDI 016.Vidi.185.068). S.C. is supported by the International Max Planck Research School on Computational Methods in Psychiatry and Ageing Research (IMPRS COMP2PSYCH).
References
- 1.Lerner R, Steinberg L. 2009. Handbook of adolescent psychology, volume 1: individual bases of adolescent development. New York, NY: John Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gardner M, Steinberg L. 2005. Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood: an experimental study. Dev. Psychol. 41, 625-635. ( 10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.625) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Smith AR, Chein J, Steinberg L. 2014. Peers increase adolescent risk taking even when the probabilities of negative outcomes are known. Dev. Psychol. 50, 1564. ( 10.1037/a0035696) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Boyer TW. 2006. The development of risk-taking: a multi-perspective review. Dev. Rev. 26, 291-345. ( 10.1016/j.dr.2006.05.002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Chein J, Albert D, O'Brien L, Uckert K, Steinberg L. 2011. Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain's reward circuitry. Dev. Sci. 14, F1-F10. ( 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01035.x) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Reniers RL, Beavan A, Keogan L, Furneaux A, Mayhew S, Wood SJ. 2017. Is it all in the reward? Peers influence risk-taking behaviour in young adulthood. Br. J. Psychol. 108, 276-295. ( 10.1111/bjop.12195) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Defoe IN, Khurana A, Betancourt LM, Hurt H, Romer D. 2019. Disentangling longitudinal relations between youth cannabis use, peer cannabis use, and conduct problems: developmental cascading links to cannabis use disorder. Addiction 114, 485-493. ( 10.1111/add.14456) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Reiter AMF, Moutoussis M, Vanes L, Kievit R, Bullmore ET, Goodyer IM, Fonagy P, Jones PB, Dolan RJ. 2019. Taste uncertainty explains developmental effects on susceptibility to peer influence in adolescence. Nat. Commun. 12, 3823. ( 10.31234/osf.io/7u4xd) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Zimring FE. 2014. 1. American youth violence. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Dishion TJ, Tipsord JM. 2011. Peer contagion in child and adolescent social and emotional development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 189-214. ( 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100412) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Reyna VF, Farley F. 2006. Risk and rationality in adolescent decision making: implications for theory, practice, and public policy. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 7, 1-44. ( 10.1111/j.1529-1006.2006.00026.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Crone EA, Dahl RE. 2012. Understanding adolescence as a period of social–affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 636-650. ( 10.1038/nrn3313) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Dahl RE, Allen NB, Wilbrecht L, Suleiman AB. 2018. Importance of investing in adolescence from a developmental science perspective. Nature 554, 441-450. ( 10.1038/nature25770) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Dishion TJ, Spracklen KM, Andrews DW, Patterson GR. 1996. Deviancy training in male adolescent friendships. Behav. Ther. 27, 373-390. ( 10.1016/S0005-7894(96)80023-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.van Hoorn J, Fuligni AJ, Crone EA, Galván A.. 2016. Peer influence effects on risk-taking and prosocial decision-making in adolescence: insights from neuroimaging studies. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 10, 59-64. ( 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.05.007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Telzer EH, van Hoorn J, Rogers CR, Do KT. 2018. Social influence on positive youth development: a developmental neuroscience perspective. Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 54, 215-258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Chung D, Orloff MA, Lauharatanahirun N, Chiu PH, King-Casas B. 2020. Valuation of peers' safe choices is associated with substance-naïveté in adolescents. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 117, 31 729-31 737. ( 10.1073/pnas.1919111117) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Silva K, Shulman EP, Chein J, Steinberg L. 2016. Peers Increase Late Adolescents’ Exploratory behavior and sensitivity to positive and negative feedback. J. Res. Adolesc. 26, 696-705. ( 10.1111/jora.12219) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Ahmed S, et al. 2020. Susceptibility to prosocial and antisocial influence in adolescence. J. Adolesc. 84, 56-68. ( 10.1016/j.adolescence.2020.07.012) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Chierchia G, Piera Pi-Sunyer B, Blakemore S-J. 2020. Prosocial influence and opportunistic conformity in adolescents and young adults. Psychol. Sci. 31, 1585-1601. ( 10.1177/0956797620957625) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Braams BR, Davidow JY, Somerville LH. 2019. Developmental patterns of change in the influence of safe and risky peer choices on risky decision-making. Dev. Sci. 22, e12717. ( 10.1111/desc.12717) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Cialdini RB, Kallgren CA, Reno RR. 1991. A focus theory of normative conduct: A theoretical refinement and reevaluation of the role of norms in human behavior. In Advances in experimental social psychology, vol. 24 (ed. Zanna MP), pp. 201-234. Cambridge, MA: Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Keizer K, Lindenberg S, Steg L. 2008. The spreading of disorder. Science 322, 1681-1685. ( 10.1126/science.1161405) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Blakemore S-J. 2018. Avoiding social risk in adolescence. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 27, 116-122. ( 10.1177/0963721417738144) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Gullone E, Moore S, Moss S, Boyd C. 2000. The adolescent risk-taking questionnaire: development and psychometric evaluation. J. Adolesc. Res. 15, 231-250. ( 10.1177/0743558400152003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Knoll LJ, Magis-Weinberg L, Speekenbrink M, Blakemore S-J. 2015. Social influence on risk perception during adolescence. Psychol. Sci. 26, 583-592. ( 10.1177/0956797615569578) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Blankenstein NE, Crone EA, van den Bos W, van Duijvenvoorde AC. 2016. Dealing with uncertainty: testing risk-and ambiguity-attitude across adolescence. Dev. Neuropsychol. 41, 77-92. ( 10.1080/87565641.2016.1158265) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Moffitt TE. 2018. Male antisocial behaviour in adolescence and beyond. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2, 177-186. ( 10.1038/s41562-018-0309-4) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Boyd R, Richerson PJ, Henrich J. 2011. The cultural niche: why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 10 918-10 925. ( 10.1073/pnas.1100290108) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Surowiecki J. 2004. The wisdom of crowds: Why the many are smarter than the few and how collective wisdom shapes business, economies, societies, and nations. New York, NY: Doubleday & Co. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Tump AN, Pleskac TJ, Kurvers RHJM. 2020. Wise or mad crowds? The cognitive mechanisms underlying information cascades. Sci. Adv. 6, eabb0266. ( 10.1126/sciadv.abb0266) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Pennycook G, Rand D. In press. The cognitive science of fake news. In Routledge handbook of political epistemology (eds Hannon M, de Ridder J). Abingdon, UK: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Molleman L, Tump AN, Gradassi A, Herzog S, Jayles B, Kurvers RHJM, van den Bos W. 2020. Strategies for integrating disparate social information. Proc. R. Soc. B 287, 20202413. ( 10.1098/rspb.2020.2413) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Moutsiana C, Garrett N, Clarke RC, Lotto RB, Blakemore S-J, Sharot T. 2013. Human development of the ability to learn from bad news. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 110, 16 396-16 401. ( 10.1073/pnas.1305631110) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Axelrod R, Hamilton WD. 1981. The evolution of cooperation. Science 211, 1390-1396. ( 10.1126/science.7466396) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hertwig R, Ortmann A. 2001. Experimental practices in economics: a methodological challenge for psychologists? Behav. Brain Sci. 24, 383-403. ( 10.1017/S0140525X01004149) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Giamattei M, Yahosseini KS, Gächter S, Molleman L. 2020. LIONESS Lab: a free web-based platform for conducting interactive experiments online. J. Econ. Sci. Assoc. 6, 95-111. ( 10.1007/s40881-020-00087-0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Gächter S, Molleman L, Nosenzo D. 2018. The behavioral logic of rule following and social norm compliance. See https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=GzWCeDylQO463.
- 39.Molleman L, Kurvers RH, van den Bos W. 2019. Unleashing the BEAST: a brief measure of human social information use. Evol. Hum. Behav. 40, 492-499. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2019.06.005) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Molleman L, Kanngiesser P, van den Bos W. 2019. Social information use in adolescents: the impact of adults, peers and household composition. PLoS ONE 14, e0225498. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0225498) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Gaule A, Bevilacqua L, Molleman L, Roberts R, van Duijvenvoorde AC, van den Bos W, McCrory EJ, Viding E. In press. Social information use in adolescents with conduct problems and varying levels of callous‐unemotional traits. JCPP Advances 2. ( 10.1002/jcv2.v2.1) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Moussaïd M, Kämmer JE, Analytis PP, Neth H. 2013. Social influence and the collective dynamics of opinion formation. PLoS ONE 8, e78433. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0078433) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Jayles B, Sire C, Kurvers RHJM. 2021. Crowd control: reducing individual estimation bias by sharing biased social information. PLOS Comput. Biol. 17, e1009590. ( 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009590) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Engel C. 2011. Dictator games: a meta study. Exp. Econ. 14, 583-610. ( 10.1007/s10683-011-9283-7) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Gummerum M, Hanoch Y, Keller M, Parsons K, Hummel A. 2010. Preschoolers' allocations in the dictator game: the role of moral emotions. J. Econ. Psychol. 31, 25-34. ( 10.1016/j.joep.2009.09.002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Benenson JF, Pascoe J, Radmore N. 2007. Children's altruistic behavior in the dictator game. Evol. Hum. Behav. 28, 168-175. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.10.003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Crone EA, Achterberg M. 2022. Prosocial development in adolescence. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 44, 220-225. ( 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.09.020) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Rosseel Y. 2012. Lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling and more. Version 0.5-12 (BETA). J. Stat. Softw. 48, 1-36. ( 10.18637/jss.v048.i02) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.R Core Team. 2020. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. See https://www.R-project.org/. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Rodman AM, Powers KE, Somerville LH. 2017. Development of self-protective biases in response to social evaluative feedback. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 114, 13 158-13 163. ( 10.1073/pnas.1712398114) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Rodriguez BJM, Heekeren HR, van den Bos W. 2019. The computational basis of following advice in adolescents. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 180, 39-54. ( 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.11.019) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Yaniv I, Kleinberger E. 2000. Advice taking in decision making: egocentric discounting and reputation formation. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 83, 260-281. ( 10.1006/obhd.2000.2909) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Morin O, Jacquet PO, Vaesen K, Acerbi A. 2021. Social information use and social information waste. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 376, 20200052. ( 10.1098/rstb.2020.0052) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Gummerum M, Keller M, Takezawa M, Mata J. 2008. To give or not to give: children's and adolescents’ sharing and moral negotiations in economic decision situations. Child Dev. 79, 562-576. ( 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01143.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Almaas I, Cappelen AW, Sørensen EØ, Tungodden B. 2010. Fairness and the development of inequality acceptance. Science 328, 1176-1178. ( 10.1126/science.1187300) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Fehr E, Glätzle-Rützler D, Sutter M. 2013. The development of egalitarianism, altruism, spite and parochialism in childhood and adolescence. Eur. Econ. Rev. 64, 369-383. ( 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2013.09.006) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Steinberg L, Monahan KC. 2007. Age differences in resistance to peer influence. Dev. Psychol. 43, 1531-1543. ( 10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1531) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Morgan TJH, Rendell LE, Ehn M, Hoppitt W, Laland KN. 2012. The evolutionary basis of human social learning. Proc. R. Soc. B 279, 653-662. ( 10.1098/rspb.2011.1172) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Ciranka SK, van den Bos W. 2019. Social influence in adolescent decision making: a formal framework. Front. Psychol. 10, 1915. ( 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01915) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH. 2007. For better and for worse: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 300-304. ( 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00525.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Rothenberg BB. 1970. Children's social sensitivity and the relationship to interpersonal competence, intrapersonal comfort, and intellectual level. Dev. Psychol. 2, 335-350. ( 10.1037/h0029175) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Way BM, Lieberman MD. 2010. Is there a genetic contribution to cultural differences? Collectivism, individualism and genetic markers of social sensitivity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 5, 203-211. ( 10.1093/scan/nsq059) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Taborsky B, Oliveira RF. 2012. Social competence: an evolutionary approach. Trends Ecol. Evol. 27, 679-688. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2012.09.003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Zajonc RB. 1965. Social facilitation. Science 149, 269-274. ( 10.1126/science.149.3681.269) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Somerville LH. 2013. The teenage brain: sensitivity to social evaluation. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22, 121-127. ( 10.1177/0963721413476512) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Pedroni A, Frey R, Bruhin A, Dutilh G, Hertwig R, Rieskamp J. 2017. The risk elicitation puzzle. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 803-809. ( 10.1038/s41562-017-0219-x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Manski CF. 1993. Identification of endogenous social effects: the reflection problem. Rev. Econ. Stud. 60, 531-542. ( 10.2307/2298123) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Kendal RL, Boogert NJ, Rendell L, Laland KN, Webster M, Jones PL. 2018. Social learning strategies: bridge-building between fields. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 651-665. ( 10.1016/j.tics.2018.04.003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Molleman L, van den Berg P, Weissing FJ. 2014. Consistent individual differences in human social learning strategies. Nat. Commun. 5, 3570. ( 10.1038/ncomms4570) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Brand CO, Heap S, Morgan TJH, Mesoudi A. 2020. The emergence and adaptive use of prestige in an online social learning task. Sci. Rep. 10, 12095. ( 10.1038/s41598-020-68982-4) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.da Pinho AS, Molleman L, Braams BR, van den Bos W. 2021. Majority and popularity effects on norm formation in adolescence. Sci. Rep. 11, 12884. ( 10.1038/s41598-021-92482-8) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Gradassi A, Slagter SK, da Pinho AS, Molleman L, van den Bos W. In press. Network distance and centrality shape social learning in the classroom. Sch. Psychol. ( 10.1037/spq0000490) [DOI] [PubMed]
- 73.Dimant E. 2019. Contagion of pro-and anti-social behavior among peers and the role of social proximity. J. Econ. Psychol. 73, 66-88. ( 10.1016/j.joep.2019.04.009) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Paluck EL, Shepherd H. 2012. The salience of social referents: a field experiment on collective norms and harassment behavior in a school social network. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 103, 899. ( 10.1037/a0030015) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Gifford-Smith M, Dodge KA, Dishion TJ, McCord J. 2005. Peer influence in children and adolescents: crossing the bridge from developmental to intervention science. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 33, 255-265. ( 10.1007/s10802-005-3563-7) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Molleman L, Ciranka S, van den Bos W. 2022. Social influence in adolescence as a double-edged sword. Figshare. ( 10.6084/m9.figshare.c.6049033) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All data and code associated with this work are available from the public repository, accessible via https://github.com/LucasMolleman/Social_influence_in_adolescence. Experimental code is available (in editable form) on the LIONESS Lab platform (https://lioness-lab.org).
Electronic supplementary material is available online [76].